The 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology Or Medicine: a Spatial Model for Cognitive Neuroscience

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology Or Medicine: a Spatial Model for Cognitive Neuroscience Neuron NeuroView The 2014 Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine: A Spatial Model for Cognitive Neuroscience Neil Burgess1,* 1Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience and Institute of Neurology, University College London, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK *Correspondence: [email protected] http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.neuron.2014.12.009 Understanding how the cognitive functions of the brain arise from its basic physiological components has been an enticing final frontier in science for thousands of years. The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 2014 was awarded one half to John O’Keefe, the other half jointly to May-Britt Moser and Edvard I. Moser ‘‘for their discoveries of cells that constitute a positioning system in the brain.’’ This prize recognizes both a paradigm shift in the study of cognitive neuroscience, and some of the amazing insights that have followed from it concerning how the world is represented within the brain. Introduction persuaded of the ‘‘neuro-ethological’’ Two of the most dramatic results of the Early ideas concerning the production of approach in which, faced by a lack of neuro-ethological approach to electro- thought were varied, unconstrained by knowledge about how the brain works, physiology are honored by the Nobel direct experimental data, e.g., Hippo- the primary aim is a broad survey of how Prize: the discoveries of place cells and crates’ theory of cognition depending on the system in question is used during of grid cells. As noted in the Nobel cita- the balance of the four humors (blood, normal behavior. This approach contrasts tion, the properties of these cells, along phlegm, and two types of bile). However, with the principles of more traditional with those of other types of spatial cell, with the advance of technology, the experimental design, with its focus on most notably head direction cells, help importance of electrical signaling in the tightly controlled testing of well-specified to define ‘‘a positioning system in the brain and nervous system slowly became hypotheses, and is well described in the brain.’’ Identification of this specific sys- accepted, from the effects of electrical book written later with Lynn Nadel, his tem is indeed a great achievement in un- stimulation on muscles noted by Luigi friend and colleague from their time at derstanding the link between mind and Galvani in the 18th century, to the cogni- McGill, The Hippocampus as a Cognitive brain. But perhaps even more important tive effects in the human brain noted by Map (O’Keefe and Nadel, 1978). are the general lessons they provide for Wilder Penfield in the 1940s and 50s. O’Keefe also profited from the timely the neural coding of internal cognitive The realization of the importance of neu- advent of modern transistors, allowing constructs, and the neural mechanisms rons (the ‘‘neuron doctrine’’) by Santiago robust differential recording of electrical of learning, representation, and memory. Ramon y Cajal—recipient of the Nobel activity from freely behaving animals, in Prize in 1906 with Camillo Golgi—com- which the large variations in potential Place Cells and Cognitive Maps bined with an appreciation of the role of common to nearby electrodes could be In his first experiments, published with electricity gave rise to the modern field cancelled and the remaining signals Herman Bouma in 1969, O’Keefe re- of electrophysiology as a tool to under- boosted on the animal’s head. Along corded responses in the amygdala of stand brain function. Early successes with other early pioneers such as Jim freely moving cats to ethologically rele- included an understanding of neural re- Ranck, this allowed O’Keefe to introduce vant stimuli (birdsong, mouse shapes, sponses to light in the retina, for which a new paradigm in understanding brain etc.). They found many instances of Granit, Hartline, and Wald received the function—studying neuronal activity in neurons that appeared to be tuned Nobel Prize in 1967, leading to an under- freely behaving animals in response to to represent the presence of specific standing of the feedforward processing naturalistic stimuli or tasks. This paradigm stimuli, when presented, and some that of simple visual stimuli by neurons in the has continued to spread, slowly, over the continued to respond after the stimulus visual system of anaesthetised mammals subsequent decades, notwithstanding was withdrawn. These findings have for which David Hubel and Torsten Wiesel the intrinsic validity of more traditional been echoed recently by neurons in hu- received the Nobel Prize in 1981. approaches in narrowing down potential man amygdala that respond to pictures Amidst these exciting developments in functions as each function becomes of specific animals. the 1960s, John O’Keefe was studying more completely understood. With the Both the amygdala and the neighboring his PhD at McGill University, receiving retrospect of the subsequent 40 years of hippocampus had already been impli- instruction from, among others, Donald progress, the neuro-ethological approach cated in human memory by the finding, Hebb—one of the foremost proponents of electrophysiology in freely behaving at the nearby Montreal Neurological Insti- of looking for direct analogs of learning animals appears to be a crucial ‘‘para- tute founded by Penfield, that bilateral and experience within the brain. As he digm shift’’ for cognitive neuroscience of damage or removal of these structures began his research career, O’Keefe was the kind identified by Thomas Kuhn. and surrounding cortical tissue in the 1120 Neuron 84, December 17, 2014 ª2014 Elsevier Inc. Neuron NeuroView view of the hippocampus as part of a cir- learning and memory in humans. They cuit for emotion, as proposed by James concluded that there must be something Papez. like an innate unitary spatial framework At the invitation of Pat Wall, O’Keefe onto which experience of the world can next moved to University College London be mapped, as foreshadowed by the and into the Department of Anatomy philosopher Immanuel Kant, and that its and Developmental Biology, headed by implementation in the brain should pro- J.Z. Young—another pioneer in relating vide the animal with a ‘‘cognitive map’’ in behavior to neuronal activity (in this the sense argued for by Edward Tolman: case, in the squid). There he began to that is, an abstract and flexible represen- look at the hippocampus in freely moving tation of space allowing novel inferences rodents as they foraged for scattered food (e.g., directions of travel), beyond simple reward. The initial paper, written with representations of specific sensory stim- John Dostrovsky (an MSc student pass- uli, bodily responses, or associations be- ing through the lab on the way to tween them. Within this framework, the becoming a neurologist), describes 8 out place cells were held to represent the of 76 putative neurons responding to the animal’s current environmental location. location of the animal, with other neurons The initial place cell findings aroused responding to arousal, attention, or move- great skepticism, both regarding the ment, or responding in inconsistent or un- basic finding and potential confounds interpretable ways (O’Keefe and Dostrov- such as uncontrolled local sensory stimuli sky, 1971). The responses of many of (e.g., odours), and the bold and highly these cells were consistent with similar abstract interpretation (cognitive maps) early findings by Jim Ranck. as opposed to simpler well-studied forms Perhaps surprisingly, given the appar- of learning such as conditioned re- ently weak initial support, O’Keefe imme- sponses. These justifiable queries were diately recognized these neurons as duly addressed, in sophisticated form. relevant to a classic question in cognitive O’Keefe first showed that the orientation psychology—that of how you represent of the firing locations (‘‘fields’’) could be your own location relative to the envi- controlled by the constellation of distal Figure 1. Examples of the Four Main Types ronment—and christened them ‘‘place cues (O’Keefe and Conway, 1978) and of Spatial Firing Recorded from Neurons in the Hippocampal Formation of Freely cells.’’ In fact, this strong interpretation then that their firing was also robust to Moving Rats was aided by the last seven cells recorded the subsequent removal of the controlling Each example shows firing rate as a function of loca- all being place cells—indicating to cues (O’Keefe and Speakman, 1987). In tion or head direction (left; peak firing rate in Hz O’Keefe that they were the dominant the absence of controlling cues, the orien- above) alongside a plot of the rat’s path within a square box (black line) and the location of the rat response, once the electrodes had tation of the location of a place cell’s firing when an action potential was fired (green dots;right). reached the ‘‘right’’ brain location. Subse- within the environment was unpredictable (A) Place cells, found in areas CA3 and CA1 of the quent experiments revealed that a but remained consistent with those of hippocampus proper, typically fire in a restricted portion of the environment. majority of the principal cells in regions other place cells and with the behavioral (B) Head-direction cells, found in the presubiculum CA1 and CA3 of the hippocampus that choices of the animal. In addition, and deep layers of medial entorhinal cortex, typi- are active in a given environment are O’Keefe laid out his proposal that place- cally fire for a narrow range of allocentric heading place cells, firing action potentials at a cell firing must reflect an internally gener- directions (left). (C) Grid cells, found in medial entorhinal cortex and high rate whenever the animal enters a ated spatial signal based on path integra- pre- and parasubiculum, typically fire in a regular small portion of its environment and re- tion in combination with environmental triangular array of locations.
Recommended publications
  • Laureadas Com O Nobel Na Fisiologia Ou Medicina (1995-2015)
    No trono da ciência II: laureadas com o Nobel na Fisiologia ou Medicina (1995-2015) On the Throne of Science II: Nobel Laureates in Physiology or Medicine (1995-2015) LUZINETE SIMÕES MINELLA Universidade Federal de Santa Catarina | UFSC RESUMO O artigo dá continuidade a uma pesquisa mais ampla sobre as trajetórias das doze cientistas que receberam o Nobel na Fisiologia ou Medicina entre 1947 e 2015. Na fase anterior foram analisadas as trajetó- rias das cinco pioneiras, laureadas entre 1947 e 1988 e nesta segunda etapa, são abordadas suas sucessoras, as sete premiadas entre 1995 e 2015. A análise das suas autobiografias, discursos e palestras disponíveis no site do prêmio, além de outras fontes, se fundamenta numa perspectiva balizada pela crítica feminista à ciência bem como pelos avanços dos estudos do campo de gênero e ciências e da história da ciência. O artigo tenta identificar semelhanças e diferenças entre as pioneiras e as sucessoras na tentativa de contribuir para o debate sobre as especificidades da feminização das carreiras científicas. 85 Palavras-chave Gênero e Ciências – Nobel – Fisiologia ou Medicina. ABSTRACT The article gives continuity to a broader research on the trajectories of the twelve scientists who received the Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine between 1947 and 2015. In the previous phase, the trajectories of the five pioneers awarded between 1947 and 1988 were analyzed, and, in this second phase, their successors, the seven awarded between 1995 and 2015, were approached. The analysis of their autobiographies, speeches and lectures available on the award site, in addition to other sources, is based on a feminist critique of science as well as advances of the studies in the field of gender and science and the history of science.
    [Show full text]
  • Of Boundary Extension: Anticipatory Scene Representation Across Development and Disorder
    Received: 23 July 2016 | Revised: 14 January 2017 | Accepted: 19 January 2017 DOI: 10.1002/hipo.22728 RESEARCH ARTICLE Testing the “Boundaries” of boundary extension: Anticipatory scene representation across development and disorder G. Spano1,2 | H. Intraub3 | J. O. Edgin1,2,4 1Department of Psychology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721 Abstract 2Cognitive Science Program, University of Recent studies have suggested that Boundary Extension (BE), a scene construction error, may be Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721 linked to the function of the hippocampus. In this study, we tested BE in two groups with varia- 3Department of Psychological and Brain tions in hippocampal development and disorder: a typically developing sample ranging from Sciences, University of Delaware, Newark, preschool to adolescence and individuals with Down syndrome. We assessed BE across three dif- Delaware 19716 ferent test modalities: drawing, visual recognition, and a 3D scene boundary reconstruction task. 4Sonoran University Center for Excellence in Despite confirmed fluctuations in memory function measured through a neuropsychological Developmental Disabilities, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona 85721 assessment, the results showed consistent BE in all groups across test modalities, confirming the Correspondence near universal nature of BE. These results indicate that BE is an essential function driven by a com- Goffredina Spano, Wellcome Trust Centre plex set of processes, that occur even in the face of delayed memory development and for Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, hippocampal dysfunction in special populations. University College London, 12 Queen Square, London WC1N 3BG, UK. Email: [email protected] KEYWORDS Funding information down syndrome, memory development, hippocampus, prediction error, top-down influences LuMind Research Down Syndrome Foundation; Research Down Syndrome and the Jerome Lejeune Foundation; Molly Lawson Graduate Fellow in Down Syndrome Research (GS).
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Map Formation Through Sequence Encoding by Theta Phase Precession
    LETTER Communicated by A. David Redish Cognitive Map Formation Through Sequence Encoding by Theta Phase Precession Hiroaki Wagatsuma [email protected] Laboratory for Dynamics of Emergent Intelligence, RIKEN BSI, Wako-shi, Saitama 351-0198, Japan Yoko Yamaguchi [email protected] Laboratory for Dynamics of Emergent Intelligence, RIKEN BSI, Wako-shi, Saitama 351- 0198, Japan; College of Science and Engineering, Tokyo Denki University, Hatoyama, Saitama 350-0394, Japan; and CREST, Japan Science and Technology Corporation The rodent hippocampus has been thought to represent the spatial en- vironment as a cognitive map. The associative connections in the hip- pocampus imply that a neural entity represents the map as a geometrical network of hippocampal cells in terms of a chart. According to recent experimental observations, the cells fire successively relative to the theta oscillation of the local field potential, called theta phase precession, when the animal is running. This observation suggests the learning of temporal sequences with asymmetric connections in the hippocampus, but it also gives rather inconsistent implications on the formation of the chart that should consist of symmetric connections for space coding. In this study, we hypothesize that the chart is generated with theta phase coding through the integration of asymmetric connections. Our computer experiments use a hippocampal network model to demonstrate that a geometrical network is formed through running experiences in a few minutes. Asymmetric connections are found to remain and dis- tribute heterogeneously in the network. The obtained network exhibits the spatial localization of activities at each instance as the chart does and their propagation that represents behavioral motions with multidirec- tional properties.
    [Show full text]
  • Position–Theta-Phase Model of Hippocampal Place Cell Activity Applied to Quantification of Running Speed Modulation of Firing Rate
    Position–theta-phase model of hippocampal place cell activity applied to quantification of running speed modulation of firing rate Kathryn McClaina,b,1, David Tingleyb, David J. Heegera,c,1, and György Buzsákia,b,1 aCenter for Neural Science, New York University, New York, NY 10003; bNeuroscience Institute, New York University, New York, NY 10016; and cDepartment of Psychology, New York University, New York, NY 10003 Edited by Terrence J. Sejnowski, Salk Institute for Biological Studies, La Jolla, CA, and approved November 18, 2019 (received for review July 24, 2019) Spiking activity of place cells in the hippocampus encodes the Another challenge in understanding this system is the dynamic animal’s position as it moves through an environment. Within a interaction between the rate code and phase code. As these codes cell’s place field, both the firing rate and the phase of spiking in combine, different formats of information are conveyed simulta- the local theta oscillation contain spatial information. We propose a neously in place cell spiking. The interaction can produce unin- – position theta-phase (PTP) model that captures the simultaneous tuitive, although entirely predictable, results in traditional analyses expression of the firing-rate code and theta-phase code in place cell of place cell activity. These practical challenges have hindered the spiking. This model parametrically characterizes place fields to com- pare across cells, time, and conditions; generates realistic place cell effort to explain variability in hippocampal firing rates. A com- simulation data; and conceptualizes a framework for principled hy- putational tool is needed that accounts for the well-established pothesis testing to identify additional features of place cell activity.
    [Show full text]
  • 2 Lez Concept of Synapses
    Synapse • Gaps Between Neurons A synapse is a specialised junction between 2 neurones where the nerve impulse is passed from one neuron to another Santiago Ramón Camillo Golgi y Cajal The Nobel Prize in Physiology or Medicine 1906 was awarded jointly to Camillo Golgi and Santiago Ramón y Cajal "in recognition of their work on the structure of the nervous system Reticular vs Neuronal Doctrine The 1930 s and 1940s was a time of controversy between proponents of chemical and electrical theories of synaptic transmission. Henry Dale was a pharmacologist and a principal advocate of chemical transmission. His most prominent adversary was the neurophysiologist, JohnEccles. Otto Loewi, MD Nobel Laureate (1936) Types of Synapses • Synapses are divided into 2 groups based on zones of apposition: • Electrical • Chemical – Fast Chemical – Modulating (slow) Chemical Gap-junction Channels • Connect communicating cells at an electrical synapse • Consist of a pair of cylinders (connexons) – one in presynaptic cell, one in post – each cylinder made of 6 protein subunits – cylinders meet in the gap between the two cell membranes • Gap junctions serve to synchronize the activity of a set of neurons Electrical synapse Electrical Synapses • Common in invertebrate neurons involved with important reflex circuits. • Common in adult mammalian neurons, and in many other body tissues such as heart muscle cells. • Current generated by the action potential in pre-synaptic neuron flows through the gap junction channel into the next neuron • Send simple depolarizing
    [Show full text]
  • The Hippocampus Marion Wright* Et Al
    WikiJournal of Medicine, 2017, 4(1):3 doi: 10.15347/wjm/2017.003 Encyclopedic Review Article The Hippocampus Marion Wright* et al. Abstract The hippocampus (named after its resemblance to the seahorse, from the Greek ἱππόκαμπος, "seahorse" from ἵππος hippos, "horse" and κάμπος kampos, "sea monster") is a major component of the brains of humans and other vertebrates. Humans and other mammals have two hippocampi, one in each side of the brain. It belongs to the limbic system and plays important roles in the consolidation of information from short-term memory to long-term memory and spatial memory that enables navigation. The hippocampus is located under the cerebral cortex; (allocortical)[1][2][3] and in primates it is located in the medial temporal lobe, underneath the cortical surface. It con- tains two main interlocking parts: the hippocampus proper (also called Ammon's horn)[4] and the dentate gyrus. In Alzheimer's disease (and other forms of dementia), the hippocampus is one of the first regions of the brain to suffer damage; short-term memory loss and disorientation are included among the early symptoms. Damage to the hippocampus can also result from oxygen starvation (hypoxia), encephalitis, or medial temporal lobe epilepsy. People with extensive, bilateral hippocampal damage may experience anterograde amnesia (the inability to form and retain new memories). In rodents as model organisms, the hippocampus has been studied extensively as part of a brain system responsi- ble for spatial memory and navigation. Many neurons in the rat and mouse hippocampus respond as place cells: that is, they fire bursts of action potentials when the animal passes through a specific part of its environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Sir Charles Sherrington'sthe Integrative Action of the Nervous System: a Centenary Appreciation
    doi:10.1093/brain/awm022 Brain (2007), 130, 887^894 OCCASIONAL PAPER Sir Charles Sherrington’sThe integrative action of the nervous system: a centenary appreciation Robert E. Burke Formerly Chief of the Laboratory of Neural Control, National Institute of Neurological Disorders, National Institutes of Health, Bethesda, MD, USA Present address: P.O. Box 1722, El Prado, NM 87529,USA E-mail: [email protected] In 1906 Sir Charles Sherrington published The Integrative Action of the Nervous System, which was a collection of ten lectures delivered two years before at Yale University in the United States. In this monograph Sherrington summarized two decades of painstaking experimental observations and his incisive interpretation of them. It settled the then-current debate between the ‘‘Reticular Theory’’ versus ‘‘Neuron Doctrine’’ ideas about the fundamental nature of the nervous system in mammals in favor of the latter, and it changed forever the way in which subsequent generations have viewed the organization of the central nervous system. Sherrington’s magnum opus contains basic concepts and even terminology that are now second nature to every student of the subject. This brief article reviews the historical context in which the book was written, summarizes its content, and considers its impact on Neurology and Neuroscience. Keywords: Neuron Doctrine; spinal reflexes; reflex coordination; control of movement; nervous system organization Introduction The first decade of the 20th century saw two momentous The Silliman lectures events for science. The year 1905 was Albert Einstein’s Sherrington’s 1906 monograph, published simultaneously in ‘miraculous year’ during which three of his most celebrated London, New Haven and New York, was based on a series papers in theoretical physics appeared.
    [Show full text]
  • Mind-Wandering in People with Hippocampal Damage
    This Accepted Manuscript has not been copyedited and formatted. The final version may differ from this version. Research Articles: Behavioral/Cognitive Mind-wandering in people with hippocampal damage Cornelia McCormick1, Clive R. Rosenthal2, Thomas D. Miller2 and Eleanor A. Maguire1 1Wellcome Centre for Human Neuroimaging, Institute of Neurology, University College London, London, WC1N 3AR, UK 2Nuffield Department of Clinical Neurosciences, University of Oxford, Oxford, OX3 9DU, UK DOI: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1812-17.2018 Received: 29 June 2017 Revised: 21 January 2018 Accepted: 24 January 2018 Published: 12 February 2018 Author contributions: C.M., C.R.R., T.D.M., and E.A.M. designed research; C.M. performed research; C.M., C.R.R., T.D.M., and E.A.M. analyzed data; C.M., C.R.R., T.D.M., and E.A.M. wrote the paper. Conflict of Interest: The authors declare no competing financial interests. We thank all the participants, particularly the patients and their relatives, for the time and effort they contributed to this study. We also thank the consultant neurologists: Drs. M.J. Johnson, S.R. Irani, S. Jacobs and P. Maddison. We are grateful to Martina F. Callaghan for help with MRI sequence design, Trevor Chong for second scoring the Autobiographical Interview, Alice Liefgreen for second scoring the mind-wandering thoughts, and Elaine Williams for advice on hippocampal segmentation. E.A.M. and C.M. are supported by a Wellcome Principal Research Fellowship to E.A.M. (101759/Z/13/Z) and the Centre by a Centre Award from Wellcome (203147/Z/16/Z).
    [Show full text]
  • Cajal, Golgi, Nansen, Schäfer and the Neuron Doctrine
    Full text provided by www.sciencedirect.com Feature Endeavour Vol. 37 No. 4 Cajal, Golgi, Nansen, Scha¨ fer and the Neuron Doctrine 1, Ortwin Bock * 1 Park Road, Rosebank, 7700 Cape Town, South Africa The Nobel Prize for Physiology or Medicine of 1906 was interconnected with each other as well as connected with shared by the Italian Camillo Golgi and the Spaniard the radial bundle, whereby a coarsely meshed network of Santiago Ramo´ n y Cajal for their contributions to the medullated fibres is produced which can already be seen at 2 knowledge of the micro-anatomy of the central nervous 60 times magnification’. Gerlach’s work on vertebrates system. In his Nobel Lecture, Golgi defended the going- helped to consolidate the evolving Reticular Theory which out-of-favour Reticular Theory, which stated that the postulated that all the cells of the central nervous system nerve cells – or neurons – are fused together to form a were joined together like an electricity distribution net- diffuse network. Reticularists like Golgi insisted that the work. Gerlach was one of the most influential anatomists of axons physically join one nerve cell to another. In con- his day and the author of many books, not least the 1848 trast, Cajal in his lecture said that his own studies Handbuch der Allgemeinen und Speciellen Gewebelehre des confirmed the observations of others that the neurons Menschlichen Ko¨rpers: fu¨ r Aerzte und Studirende. For are independent of one another, a fact which is the twenty years he lent his considerable credibility to the anatomical basis of the now-accepted Neuron Doctrine Reticular Theory.
    [Show full text]
  • CNS 2014 Program
    Cognitive Neuroscience Society 21st Annual Meeting, April 5-8, 2014 Marriott Copley Place Hotel, Boston, Massachusetts 2014 Annual Meeting Program Contents 2014 Committees & Staff . 2 Schedule Overview . 3 . Keynotes . 5 2014 George A . Miller Awardee . 6. Distinguished Career Contributions Awardee . 7 . Young Investigator Awardees . 8 . General Information . 10 Exhibitors . 13 . Invited-Symposium Sessions . 14 Mini-Symposium Sessions . 18 Poster Schedule . 32. Poster Session A . 33 Poster Session B . 66 Poster Session C . 98 Poster Session D . 130 Poster Session E . 163 Poster Session F . 195 . Poster Session G . 227 Poster Topic Index . 259. Author Index . 261 . Boston Marriott Copley Place Floorplan . 272. A Supplement of the Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience Cognitive Neuroscience Society c/o Center for the Mind and Brain 267 Cousteau Place, Davis, CA 95616 ISSN 1096-8857 © CNS www.cogneurosociety.org 2014 Committees & Staff Governing Board Mini-Symposium Committee Roberto Cabeza, Ph.D., Duke University David Badre, Ph.D., Brown University (Chair) Marta Kutas, Ph.D., University of California, San Diego Adam Aron, Ph.D., University of California, San Diego Helen Neville, Ph.D., University of Oregon Lila Davachi, Ph.D., New York University Daniel Schacter, Ph.D., Harvard University Elizabeth Kensinger, Ph.D., Boston College Michael S. Gazzaniga, Ph.D., University of California, Gina Kuperberg, Ph.D., Harvard University Santa Barbara (ex officio) Thad Polk, Ph.D., University of Michigan George R. Mangun, Ph.D., University of California,
    [Show full text]
  • Decoding Representations of Scenes in the Medial Temporal Lobes
    HIPPOCAMPUS 22:1143–1153 (2012) Decoding Representations of Scenes in the Medial Temporal Lobes Heidi M. Bonnici,1 Dharshan Kumaran,2,3 Martin J. Chadwick,1 Nikolaus Weiskopf,1 Demis Hassabis,4 and Eleanor A. Maguire1* ABSTRACT: Recent theoretical perspectives have suggested that the (Eichenbaum, 2004; Johnson et al., 2007: Kumaran function of the human hippocampus, like its rodent counterpart, may be et al., 2009) and even visual perception (Lee et al., best characterized in terms of its information processing capacities. In this study, we use a combination of high-resolution functional magnetic 2005; Graham et al., 2006, 2010). Current perspec- resonance imaging, multivariate pattern analysis, and a simple decision tives, therefore, have emphasized that the function of making task, to test specific hypotheses concerning the role of the the hippocampus, and indeed surrounding areas within medial temporal lobe (MTL) in scene processing. We observed that the medial temporal lobe (MTL), may be best charac- while information that enabled two highly similar scenes to be distin- guished was widely distributed throughout the MTL, more distinct scene terized by understanding the nature of the information representations were present in the hippocampus, consistent with its processing they perform. role in performing pattern separation. As well as viewing the two similar The application of multivariate pattern analysis scenes, during scanning participants also viewed morphed scenes that (MVPA) techniques applied to functional magnetic spanned a continuum between the original two scenes. We found that patterns of hippocampal activity during morph trials, even when resonance imaging (fMRI) data (Haynes and Rees, perceptual inputs were held entirely constant (i.e., in 50% morph 2006; Norman et al., 2006) offers the possibility of trials), showed a robust relationship with participants’ choices in the characterizing the types of neural representations and decision task.
    [Show full text]
  • Is Neuroscience a Bigger Threat Than Artifical Intelligence
    Is Neuroscience a Bigger Threat than Artificial Intelligence? IBM’s Jeopardy winning computer Watson is a serious threat, not just to the livelihood of medical diagnosticians, but to other professionals who may find themselves going the way of welders. Besides its economic threat, the advance of AI seems to pose a cultural threat: if physical systems can do what we do without thought to give meaning to their achievements, the conscious human mind will be displaced from its unique role in the universe as a creative, responsible, rational agent. But this worry has a more powerful basis in the Nobel Prize winning discoveries of a quartet of neuroscientists—Eric Kandel, John O’Keefe, Edvard, and May-Britt Moser. For between them they have shown that the human brain doesn’t work the way conscious experience suggests at all. Instead it operates to deliver human achievements in the way IBM’s Watson does. Thoughts with meaning have no more role in the human brain than in artificial intelligence. Consciousness tells us that we employ a theory of mind, both to decide on our own actions and to predict and explain the behavior of others. According to this theory there have to be particular belief/desire pairings somewhere in our brains working together to bring about movements of the body, including speech and writing. Which beliefs and desires in particular? Roughly speaking it’s the contents of beliefs and desires—what they are about—that pair them up to drive our actions. The desires represent the ends, the beliefs record the available means to attain them.
    [Show full text]