The Evolution of Religion

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Evolution of Religion The Evolution of Religion: How Cognitive By-Products, Adaptive Learning Heuristics, Ritual Displays, and Group Competition Generate Deep Commitments to Prosocial Religions Scott Atran Abstract CNRS Understanding religion requires explaining why supernatu- Institut Jean Nicod ral beliefs, devotions, and rituals are both universal and vari- Paris, France able across cultures, and why religion is so often associated & with both large-scale cooperation and enduring group conflict. John Jay College of Criminal Justice Emerging lines of research suggest that these oppositions result New York, NY, USA from the convergence of three processes. First, the interaction & of certain reliably developing cognitive processes, such as our ISR and Ford School of Public Policy University of Michigan ability to infer the presence of intentional agents, favors—as Ann Arbor, MI, USA an evolutionary by-product—the spread of certain kinds of [email protected] counterintuitive concepts. Second, participation in rituals and devotions involving costly displays exploits various aspects Joseph Henrich of our evolved psychology to deepen people’s commitment Department of Economics and Department of Psychology to both supernatural agents and religious communities. Third, University of British Columbia competition among societies and organizations with different Vancouver, BC, Canada faith-based beliefs and practices has increasingly connected re- [email protected] ligion with both within-group prosociality and between-group enmity. This connection has strengthened dramatically in re- cent millennia, as part of the evolution of complex societies, and is important to understanding cooperation and conflict in today’s world. Keywords by-product hypothesis, credibility enhancing displays, cultural transmission, cooperation, group competition, high gods, min- imally counterintuitive, morality, religion, rise of civilization March 8, 2010; revised and accepted March 29, 2010 18 Biological Theory 5(1) 2010, 18–30. c 2010 Konrad Lorenz Institute for Evolution and Cognition Research Scott Atran and Joseph Henrich [An] advancement in the standard of morality and an increase in population size, or in large-group interactions such as those the number of well-endowed men . who, from possessing in a high associated with many kinds of public goods or common dilem- degree the spirit of patriotism, fidelity, obedience, courage, and sym- mas (Boyd and Richerson 1988; Panchanathan and Boyd 2003; pathy, were always ready to give aid to each other and to sacrifice Nowak and Sigmund 2005; Mathew and Boyd 2009). Even themselves for the common good, would be victorious over other more telling is that none of these mechanisms explains the tribes. variation in cooperation among human societies, or the mas- — Charles Darwin, The Descent of Man sive expansion of cooperation in some societies over the last ten millennia (Henrich et al. 2005; Atran 2010). This synthesis integrates insights from studies of the cogni- Converging lines of field and experimental evidence sug- tive foundations of religion with evolutionary approaches to gest that cultural evolution, building on certain innate cognitive human cooperation to derive a deeper understanding of the foundations, has favored the emergence of beliefs in power- origin and development of prosocial religions. We argue that ful moralizing deities concerned with the prosocial behav- the cultural evolution of prosocial religions and the historical ior of individuals beyond kin- and reciprocity-based networks rise of large-scale civilizations involve the dynamic interaction (Norenzayan and Shariff 2008). Cross-cultural analysis of 186 of the by-products of adaptive cognitive mechanisms (e.g., societies has found that larger and more complex societies minimally counterintuitive beliefs and overextended agent were much more likely to subscribe to potent deities directly concepts), adaptive learning heuristics (e.g., emulation of concerned with morality and willing to punish norm violators successful and prestigious individuals), credibility-enhancing (Roes and Raymond 2003; Johnson 2005). Studies conducted ritual displays (e.g., self-sacrifice and costly commitments to across a diverse range of societies including foragers, farm- seemingly preposterous beliefs), and cultural group selection ers, and herders, show that professing a world religion pre- for those packages of rituals, devotions, and beliefs that best dicts greater fairness toward ephemeral interactants (Henrich sustain in-group prosocial norms (e.g., monumental undertak- et al. 2010). Experiments with North Americans show that ings, sacred values). unconsciously activating religious concepts lead to reduced Many religions pose an evolutionary enigma because they cheating and greater generosity toward strangers (Bargh and require costly commitments to beliefs that violate both core Chartrand 1999; Mazar and Ariely 2006; Shariff and Noren- aspects of logical consistency and our intuitive expectations zayan 2007), except among ardent atheists. Together, these about how the world works, both of which are otherwise crucial cross-cultural, historical, and experimental findings suggest for successfully navigating the world (Atran and Norenzayan that (1) religion—as a phenomenon with potentially deep 2004). Religious practices are often costly in terms of material roots (Klein 1989)—has not always been about high mor- sacrifice (ranging from human sacrifice to prayer time), emo- alizing gods and (2) modern world religions may have evolved tional expenditure (inciting fears and hopes), and cognitive to create a potent linkage between the supernatural and the effort (maintaining conflicting models about the nature of the prosocial. Thus, we hypothesize that cultural evolutionary pro- world). One anthropological review of religious offerings con- cesses, driven by competition among groups, have exploited cludes: “Sacrifice is giving something up at a cost. ‘Afford aspects of our evolved psychology, including certain cognitive it or not,’ the attitude seems to be” (Firth 1963). by-products, to gradually assemble packages of supernatural At the same time, the origin of large-scale cooperative beliefs, devotions, and rituals that were increasingly effective human societies is also an evolutionary puzzle because people at instilling deep commitment, galvanizing internal solidarity, frequently cooperate and trade with non-relatives in ephemeral and sustaining larger-scale cooperation. interactions (Fehr and Fischbacher 2003). Thus, while the evo- lutionary mechanisms associated with kinship, reciprocity, and Ordinary Cognition Produces Extraordinary Agents reputation clearly influence cooperation in important ways, they do not capture the fullest extent of human prosocial- Humans are purpose-seeking, cause-inferring, story-telling an- ity. Kinship cannot explain cooperation among non-relatives imals (Gazzaniga et al. 2009). As Hume noted in The Natural (Henrich and Henrich 2007), though “fictive kinship”—a cul- History of Religion, the greater the impact of events on our tural manipulation of kin psychology—may contribute to mo- lives, the greater is our drive to impose purpose and coherence bilizing larger groups (Johnson 1987; Atran 2003). Reciprocity on those events. This view is backed by a recent experiment does not suffice to explain cooperation beyond dense so- in which people were asked what patterns they could see in cial networks, small villages, or tightly knit neighborhoods arrangements of dots or stock market figures (Whitson and (Hruschka and Henrich 2006; Allen-Arave et al. 2008; Atran Galinsky 2008). Before asking, the experimenters made half 2010). Neither direct nor indirect reciprocity can explain coop- the participants feel a lack of control. Those who experienced eration in transient interactions in large populations, because a lack of control were more likely to see patterns and processes reputational information rapidly degrades as a function of underlying the randomness, suggesting that under uncertainty Biological Theory 5(1) 2010 19 The Evolution of Religion we are more likely to find preternatural explanations for the at least until supernatural agents were harnessed by cultural randomness. Both cross-cultural experiments and surveys in- evolution to begin demanding costly actions and cooperation, dicate that people more readily ascribe to the veracity of nar- under threat of divine punishment or offers of sublime rewards. ratives containing counterintuitive elements (e.g., miracles) How do our minds make agent concepts into gods? Cogni- when primed with death (Norenzayan and Hansen 2006), or tive approaches propose that supernatural concepts exploit or- when facing danger or insecurity, as with pleas of hope for dinary mental processes to construct counterintuitive concepts God’s intervention during wartime (Argyle and Beit-Hallahmi (Boyer 2001; Atran 2002; Barrett 2004). Religious beliefs are 2000). Such findings help explain both cross-national analyses counterintuitive because they violate universal expectations showing that a country’s religiosity (devotion to a world reli- about the world’s mundane structure. This includes the ba- gion) is positively related to its degree of existential insecurity sic categories of our “intuitive ontology” (i.e., the ontology (Norris and Inglehart 2004), and why certain kinds of religions of our semantic system), such as person, animal, plant, and enjoy revivals in challenging times. The issue then becomes: substance (Whythe 1993; Sperber
Recommended publications
  • Reframing Sacred Values
    In Theory Reframing Sacred Values Scott Atran and Robert Axelrod Sacred values differ from material or instrumental values in that they incorporate moral beliefs that drive action in ways dissociated from prospects for success. Across the world, people believe that devotion to essential or core values — such as the welfare of their family and country, or their commitment to religion, honor, and justice — are, or ought to be, absolute and inviolable. Counterintuitively, understanding an opponent’s sacred values, we believe, offers surprising opportunities for breakthroughs to peace. Because of the emotional unwillingness of those in conflict situations to negotiate sacred values, conventional wisdom suggests that nego- tiators should either leave sacred values for last in political negotia- tions or should try to bypass them with sufficient material incentives. Our empirical findings and historical analysis suggest that conven- tional wisdom is wrong.In fact, offering to provide material benefits in exchange for giving up a sacred value actually makes settlement more difficult because people see the offering as an insult rather than a compromise. But we also found that making symbolic concessions of no apparent material benefit might open the way to resolving seem- ingly irresolvable conflicts. Scott Atran is research director in anthropology at CNRS — Institut Jean Nicod in Paris, a presidential scholar in sociology at the John Jay College of the City University of New York, and a visiting professor of psychology and public policy at the University of Michigan in Ann Arbor. His e-mail address is [email protected]. Robert Axelrod is Walgreen Professor for the Study of Human Understanding in the department of political science and in the Ford School of Public Policy at the University of Michigan.
    [Show full text]
  • Beyond Belief: Science, Religion, Reason and Survival the Conversation Continues
    Beyond Belief: Science, Religion, Reason and Survival The Conversation Continues Criticism of Beyond Belief by RP Bird It deeply annoys me when the Big Guns of science spout off about protecting science. Like the rest of us have our thumbs up our asses? I'm one of the thousands of Kansans who wrote to the state board of education complaining about the pseudo-science of Intelligent Design. I'm always short of money, but I gave a little to SEA when they started up operations. I've written letters to my state representatives and my congressmen on the subject of science and keeping religious ideas out of science education. So imagine my dismay when I read the NY Times account of the La Jolla meeting. Remember just before the Iraq war, when Middle Eastern experts warned Bush and the rest of us that invading Iraq would be confirmation of the worst fantasies of the jihadist movement? Congratulations, you've just done for science what Bush did for the USA in the Middle East. I can already hear the Christian Right: "We told you, they're out to get us!" Not only that, but you and others want to hurt the cause of science by adopting the methods of the Right? Are you nuts? If you adopt the methods of a religion, you make science into a religion. Also, what's with this "loyalty oath" everyone at the conference had to spout before being heard? No one will listen to them unless they declare the aren't religious? Isn't that what the Christian Right does? You of all people should know that the best defense of science is to do science and teach what you have learned.
    [Show full text]
  • Folk Biology and the Anthropology of Science Page 1 of 41
    Folk biology and the anthropology of science Page 1 of 41 Folk Biology and the Anthropology of Science: Cognitive Universals and Cultural Particulars Scott Atran Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique (CREA - Ecole Polytechnique) 1 rue Descartes 75005 Paris FRANCE and Institute for Social Research The University of Michigan Ann Arbor MI48106-1248 USA [email protected] Keywords Folk biology, taxonomy, cognitive universals, modularity, evolution, culture, Maya, anthropology Abstract This essay in the "anthropology of science" is about how cognition constrains culture in producing science. The example is folk biology, whose cultural recurrence issues from the very same domain- specific cognitive universals that provide the historical backbone of systematic biology. Humans everywhere think about plants and animals in highly structured ways. People have similar folk- biological taxonomies composed of essence-based species-like groups and the ranking of species into lower- and higher-order groups. Such taxonomies are not as arbitrary in structure and content, nor as variable across cultures, as the assembly of entities into cosmologies, materials or social groups. These structures are routine products of our "habits of mind," which may be in part naturally selected to grasp relevant and recurrent "habits of the world." An experiment illustrates that the same taxonomic rank is preferred for making biological inferences in two diverse populations: Lowland Maya and Midwest Americans. These findings cannot be explained by domain-general models of similarity because such models cannot account for why both cultures prefer species-like groups, despite the fact that Americans have relatively little actual knowledge or experience at this level. This supports a modular view of folk biology as a core domain of human knowledge and as a special player, or "core meme," in the selection processes by which cultures evolve.
    [Show full text]
  • Counting Casualties: a Framework for Respectful, Useful Records
    J Risk Uncertainty DOI 10.1007/s11166-006-9001-6 Counting casualties: A framework for respectful, useful records Baruch Fischhoff · Scott Atran · Noam Fischhoff C Springer Science + Business Media, LLC 2007 Abstract Counting casualties in conflict zones faces both practical and ethical concerns. Drawing on procedures from risk analysis, we propose a general approach. It represents each death by standard features, having either essential value, for capturing the social and cultural meaning of individual casualties, or instrumental value, for relating patterns of casualties to possible causes and effects. We illustrate the approach with the choices involved in attempts to record casualties in Iraq and the Israel-Palestine conflict, and with natural disasters, as exemplified by Hurricane Katrina. We advocate institutionalizing the approach, so that recording casualties increases understanding, rather than suspicion. Keywords Risk analysis . Risk characterization . Casualties . Conflict . Forensics JEL Classification D74 . D78 . D81 . F51 . H56 . I18 . N40 . Q54 Counting casualties is part of any conflict. The parties must do it both to monitor the conflict’s progress and to give it meaning (Department of Defense, 2005; Graham, 2005; Holt, 2006). Without accurate counts, the situation is obscured and the fallen are dishonored. If these failures appear deliberate, then they may aggravate the conflict, by adding insult to injury. Political scientists need these counts for their own reasons: creating narrative accounts, providing policy advice, and testing theories of conflict and reconciliation (e.g., Daponte, 2003; Epstein, 2002; Peterson, 2002; Walzer, 1992). The work is grim and challenging. The recorders face not only the risks of work in a conflict zone, but also the wrath of those who want the story told a particular way or not told at all.
    [Show full text]
  • The Trouble with Memes: Inference Versus Imitation in Cultural Creation Scott Atran
    The Trouble with Memes: Inference versus Imitation in Cultural Creation Scott Atran To cite this version: Scott Atran. The Trouble with Memes: Inference versus Imitation in Cultural Creation. Human Nature, Springer Verlag, 2001, 12 (4), pp.351-381. ijn_00000123 HAL Id: ijn_00000123 https://jeannicod.ccsd.cnrs.fr/ijn_00000123 Submitted on 3 Sep 2002 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. 1 Appeared in: Human Nature 12(4):351-381, 2001 THE TROUBLE WITH MEMES : INFERENCE VERSUS IMITATION IN CULTURAL CREATION Scott Atran CNRS – Institut Jean Nicod, Paris and The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor Address for correspondence: CNRS, 9 rampe de l‟observatoire, 66660 Port Vendres, France Email: [email protected] 2 ABSTRACT (Word Count: 100) Memes are hypothetical cultural units passed on by imitation; although non-biological, they undergo Darwinian selection like genes. Cognitive study of multimodular human minds undermines memetics: unlike genetic replication, high fidelity transmission of cultural information is the exception, not the rule. Constant, rapid “mutation” of information during communication generates endlessly varied creations that nevertheless adhere to modular input conditions. The sort of cultural information most susceptible to modular processing is that most readily acquired by children, most easily transmitted across individuals, most apt to survive within a culture, most likely to recur in different cultures, and most disposed to cultural variation and elaboration.
    [Show full text]
  • Suicide Terrorism As Strategy: Case Studies of Hamas and the Kurdistan Workers Party
    Suicide Terrorism as Strategy: Case Studies of Hamas and the Kurdistan Workers Party Strategic Insights, Volume IV, Issue 7 (July 2005) by Ali Wyne Strategic Insights is a monthly electronic journal produced by the Center for Contemporary Conflict at the Naval Postgraduate School in Monterey, California. The views expressed here are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily represent the views of NPS, the Department of Defense, or the U.S. Government. For a PDF version of this article, click here. Introduction Why is it that certain terrorist organizations employ suicide terrorism to advance their objectives, while others do not? In order to answer this question, I will examine why Hamas, the central terrorist organization operating within the Occupied Territories, has gradually escalated its employment of suicide terrorism; and why the Kurdistan Workers Party (PKK), a separatist group operating in Turkey, gradually abandoned it as a component of its broader strategy. I will discuss the activities of these groups within the context of two competing explanations of suicide terrorism: those which focus on religious motivations, and those which focus on strategic motivations. I argue that while the first set of explanations is meritorious in some instances, the latter set of explanations is much more broadly applicable, and, as such, more legitimate. Suicide Terrorism as Strategy Hamas’ official charter, made public on August 18, 1988, articulates a harrowing commitment to the complete destruction of Israel: “Israel will exist and will continue to exist until Islam will obliterate it, just as it obliterated others before it.”[1] The charter’s most striking features lie not in its body, but, rather, in its opening and closing statements.
    [Show full text]
  • Mishandling Suicide Terrorism
    Scott Atran Mishandling Suicide Terrorism The past three years saw more suicide attacks than the last quar- ter century. Most of them were religiously motivated. Repeated suicide ac- tions show that massive counterforce alone does not diminish the frequency or intensity of suicide attack. Like pounding mercury with a hammer, this sort of top-heavy counterstrategy only seems to generate more varied and insidious forms of suicide terrorism. Even with many top Al Qaeda leaders now dead or in custody, the transnational jihadist fraternity is transforming into a hydra-headed network more difficult to fight than before. Poverty and lack of education per se are not root causes of suicide terror- ism. Nor do Muslims who have expressed support for martyr actions and trust in Osama bin Laden or the late Hamas leader Sheikh Yassin as a rule hate democratic freedoms or Western culture, although many of these Mus- lims do despise U.S. foreign policy, especially in the Middle East. Rising aspi- rations followed by dwindling expectations, particularly regarding civil liberties, are critical factors in generating support for suicide terrorism. The United States, Israel, Russia, and other nations on the frontline in the war on terrorism need to realize that military and counterinsurgency ac- tions are tactical, not strategic, responses to suicide terrorism—the most po- litically destabilizing and psychologically devastating form of terrorism. When these nations back oppressive and unpopular governments (even those deemed “partners in the war on terror”), this only generates popular resentment and support for terrorism against those governments as well as their backers. To attract potential recruits away from jihadist martyrdom— suicide terrorism’s most virulent strain—and to dry up its popular support Scott Atran is a director of research at the Centre National de la Recherche Scientifique in Paris and an adjunct professor of psychology, anthropology, and natural resources at the University of Michigan.
    [Show full text]
  • Can Fictional Superhuman Agents Have Mental States?
    METHOD Method and Theory in the Study & THEORY in the STUDY OF of Religion 30 (�0�8) 4�5-448 RELIGION brill.com/mtsr Can Fictional Superhuman Agents have Mental States? Gabriel Levy Department of Philosophy and Religious Studies Norwegian University of Science and Technology NO-7491 Trondheim, Norway [email protected] Abstract According to Deborah Tollefsen, from the analytic perspective called “interpretivism”, there is a reasonable way in which groups can be said to have mental states. She bases her argument on the every-day use of language, where people speak as if groups have states such as intentions, desires and wishes. Such propositional attitudes form the basis of any account of truth-conditional semantics, the rules by which people grasp the conditions under which an utterance is true. If groups (abstract units of people) have mental states, perhaps superhuman agents have them too. One argument that may contradict this premise is one that says that, whereas groups exist, superhuman agents do not. However, if groups exist on the basis of normative narratives about them and the institutionalized actions they carry out in the world, the same can be said for superhuman agents. They are like legal fictions: fictional but real. Superhuman agents are fictional and real in a similar sense as groups.1 Keywords religion – semantics – Davidson – fiction – truth – falsity 1 Thanks to Miriam Kyselo, Lionel Sacks, Ulrika Mårtensson, Rene van Woudenberg, the Department of Philosophy and Religious Studies at NTNU, John McGraw, Jeppe Sinding Jensen, Terry Godlove, Scott Davis, Lars Albinus, Nancy Frankenberry, and especially Mark Gardiner.
    [Show full text]
  • Why Explaining Religion Is Not Sufficient to Explain Away Religion
    Why Explaining Religion Is Not Sufficient to Explain Away Religion An argumentative exploration of the leading evolutionary explanatory accounts of religion to demonstrate the inability of science to explain away religious belief Author: Vipusaayini Sivanesanathan Edited by: Ko-Lun Liu 1. Introduction While science continues to make significant progressive strides, religion has yet to add to its historically established doctrines. Not only has the rapid expansion of science brought into question the validity and necessity of religion, part of scientific inquiry now focuses on how ‘counterintuitive’ notions of religion came to be. ‘Counterintuitive’ ideas of religion posit religious beliefs to go against or violate empirically verified facts or knowledge. Some argue that we can utilize the knowledge attained from advancements in science to explain away religion. One particular aspect of science that is used to explain away religion are evolutionary theories. In this paper, I will argue that while evolutionary accounts can explain our affinity towards religion, it has yet to explain away religion. I will explicate and refute the three different argument for evolutionary accounts of religion, including the socio-evolutionary, bio-evolutionary and cultural-evolutionary, to demonstrate how science has not succeeded in explaining away religion. 2. Why Does Science Try to Explain Away Religion? There is a prominent assumption in academia that science and religion are two separate and distinct fields which fundamentally do not, and (for some) cannot overlap. Both science and theism attempt to answer the central questions concerning the design and function of natural phenomena (i.e., evolution, questions of the universe, life etc).
    [Show full text]
  • A Commentary on Emmanuel Macron's Declaration of the Need
    New England Journal of Public Policy Volume 31 Article 8 Issue 1 Special Issue: CRIC 5-1-2019 The rC isis of Cultures and the Vitality of Values: A Commentary on Emmanuel Macron’s Declaration of the Need for Religion Scott Atran Institut Jean Nicod, Ecole Normale Supérieure; Centre for the Resolution of Intractable Conflict (Harris Manchester College, University of Oxford); Gerald Ford School of Public Policy, University of Michigan; Artis International Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umb.edu/nejpp Part of the Peace and Conflict Studies Commons, Public Policy Commons, and the Religion Commons Recommended Citation Atran, Scott (2019) "The rC isis of Cultures and the Vitality of Values: A Commentary on Emmanuel Macron’s Declaration of the Need for Religion," New England Journal of Public Policy: Vol. 31 : Iss. 1 , Article 8. Available at: https://scholarworks.umb.edu/nejpp/vol31/iss1/8 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks at UMass Boston. It has been accepted for inclusion in New England Journal of Public Policy by an authorized editor of ScholarWorks at UMass Boston. For more information, please contact [email protected]. New England Journal of Public Policy The Crisis of Cultures and the Vitality of Values: A Commentary on Emmanuel Macron’s Declaration of the Need for Religion Scott Atran Institut Jean Nicod, Ecole Normale Supérieure Centre for the Resolution of Intractable Conflict (Harris Manchester College, University of Oxford) Gerald Ford School of Public Policy, University of Michigan Artis International French president Emmanuel Macron’s claim that society needs religion is explored in the light of rising populism and illiberalism, and failures allied to the forced gamble of globalization.
    [Show full text]
  • Interview with Scott Atran, the TIMES (London)
    THE TIMES (LONDON) • Scott Atran, who says that religious terrorism came ‘out of nowhere’ in the 1970s Richard Pohle for The Times David Aaronovitch November 13 2010 12:01AM Scott Atran has spent a lot of time in the past few years talking to terrorists and would-be suicide attackers, or to the people who love and support them. Now he’s in the Waldorf Hilton in Covent Garden talking to me. It’s his book tour, so I get to be the anthropologist, asking him questions and observing his behaviour. Talking to the Enemy, subtitled Violent Extremism, Sacred Values and What it Means to be Human, is an important book, by turns fascinating, dense, scientific, debatable, illuminating and irritating. Parts really have to be read by anyone seeking to understand modern jihadism, other sections reflect Atran’s wide knowledge of anthropology, but great dollops of evolutionary psychology ‘ conjectural at best and certainly untestable for the moment ‘ send the readers on unreliable journeys. Atran is a wonderful guy to spend a morning with. Now in his late fifties but mistakable for 45, he started in the anthropology business in 1969, when he embarked on what can best be described as a gap decade. Atran had won the Westinghouse scholarship for advanced mathematics at Columbia University in New York. It was a time of student protest, and one of these brought about an encounter with the legendary anthropologist Margaret Mead. The two of them had an argument about tactics, which somehow led to the teenager being invited up to her tower office in the Museum of Natural History in New York, which was ‘the tower of Ali Baba’, Atran says, ‘With thousands of skulls.
    [Show full text]
  • Religion's Evolutionary Landscape
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2004) 27, 713–770 Printed in the United States of America Religion’s evolutionary landscape: Counterintuition, commitment, compassion, communion Scott Atran CNRS–Institut Jean Nicod, 75007 Paris, France; and Institute for Social Research–University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI 48106-1248 [email protected] http://www.institutnicod.org Ara Norenzayan Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, British Columbia V6T 1Z4 Canada [email protected] www.psych.ubc.ca/~ara Abstract: Religion is not an evolutionary adaptation per se, but a recurring cultural by-product of the complex evolutionary landscape that sets cognitive, emotional, and material conditions for ordinary human interactions. Religion exploits only ordinary cognitive processes to passionately display costly devotion to counterintuitive worlds governed by supernatural agents. The conceptual founda- tions of religion are intuitively given by task-specific panhuman cognitive domains, including folkmechanics, folkbiology, and folkpsy- chology. Core religious beliefs minimally violate ordinary notions about how the world is, with all of its inescapable problems, thus en- abling people to imagine minimally impossible supernatural worlds that solve existential problems, including death and deception. Here the focus is on folkpsychology and agency. A key feature of the supernatural agent concepts common to all religions is the triggering of an “Innate Releasing Mechanism,” or “agency detector,” whose proper (naturally selected) domain encompasses animate objects rele- vant to hominid survival – such as predators, protectors, and prey – but which actually extends to moving dots on computer screens, voices in wind, and faces on clouds. Folkpsychology also crucially involves metarepresentation, which makes deception possible and threatens any social order.
    [Show full text]