p •

CALIFORNIA STATE UNIVERSITY, NORTHRIDGE

AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE RELATIONSHIP ll BETWEEN COLLEGIATE LESSONS AND COLLEGIATE SABRE FENCING BOUTS

A thesis submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in

Physical Education

by

Edwin Knox Hurst II

January, 1980 Knox Hurst II is approved:

Advisor

Dr. Merril~ Hardy t

Dr. Adran Adams

Dr. Darrel Guthrie, Chair

California State University, Northridge

ii TABLE OF CONTENTS

Abstract iv

Chapter

I. The Problem 1

II. Review of the Literature 6

III. Method 13

IV. Results 19

V. Summary, Discussion, and Conclusions 25

Bibliography 33

Appendix C 36

Appendix D 39

iii ABSTRACT

AN INVESTIGATION INTO THE RELATIONSHIP

BETWEEN COLLEGIATE SABRE FENCING LESSONS AND

COLLEGE SABRE FENCING BOUTS

by

Edwin Knox Hurst II

Master of Arts in Physical Education

Observation of collegiate sabre fencing indicated that there might be a considerable disparity between the actions

emphasized in most sabre fencing lessons and the actions which actually score most often in college sabre bouts. As

a result of these observations, it was decided to formally

survey contemporary collegiate sabre competition as well as

contemporary collegiate sabre lessons and then attempt to

discern if there were significant differences between the

two.

Competition data were obtained by observation of the

finals of the 1979 National Collegiate Athletic Association

Sabre Championships and lesson data were obtained from the

iv answers to a questionnaire sent to college sabre coaches

nationwide. The data were classified into two general

categories: "simple" actions and "complex" actions. Using

these two categories as_parameters, a modified "Z" Test was

employed to determine if lesson data and competition data

constituted the same or significantly different statistical populations.

The results of the "Z" Test indicated that there were

significant differences between the employment of simple versus complex actions in actual competition and the empha­ sis placed on simple versus complex actions in collegiate sabre lessons. The conclusions of this study, that there were significant differences between college sabre bouts and college sabre lessons, thus raised the question whether the average lesson is providing the optimum preparation for modern collegiate competitive conditions. Further study of this question was strongly indicated.

v CHAPTER I

THE PROBLEM

Introduction

Fencing is one of this country's older inter­

collegiate sports~ yet it seems to have entirely escaped

the statistical scrutiny so prevalent in other areas of

athletics. As a result, most American coaching lessons have come about through imitation of lessons given by Euro­ pean fencing masters, rather than through an analysis of

the requirements of the modern collegiate game (lO:ix-x,

15:xi-xiii, 25:4). Since there is no such thing as col­

legiate fencing in Europe, this method could be somewhat suspect.

Fencing is traditionally taught by a fencing coach giving an individual lesson to each student. As .the student becomes more adept, the lessons become more complex and intricate. It appears, however, that as these lessons become more complicated, they seem to bear a decreasing resemblance to an actual college bout. This tendency is most noticeable in sabre, where the large target area, combined with the ability to score with the entire cutting edge of the blade, give the advantage to the and make for a preponderance of quick, explosive, yet simple actions.

1 2

Continued observation of collegiate sabre competi­

tion suggested that there might be a considerable disparity

between the actions emphasized in most sabre fencing lessons

and the actions which actually score most often in college

sabre bouts. Specifically, it appeared that, while success­

ful simple actions seemed to heavily outnumber successful

complex ones, most lessons observed seemed to strongly emphasize complex actions over simple ones.

As a result of these observations, it was decided to formally survey contemporary collegiate sabre competi­ tion as well as contemporary collegiate sabre lessons and then attempt to discern if there were significant differ­ ences between the two. To form the basis for this investi­ gation, a statement of the overall problem was formulated.

Statement of the Problem

There is a significant difference between the emphasis placed on simple versus complex actions .in an average college sabre lesson, and actual use of simple as opposed to complex actions in college sabre bouts.

Significance of the Problem

This problem is significant in that its investiga­ tion provides a basis from which to infer whether modern collegiate sabre lessons are correlating with the demands of the college game itself. Such inferences, and the ques­ tions raised by them, could be of great value to college 3

fencing coaches. In addition, the data developed by such a

study would provide a basis with which an individual coach

could compare his lessons with existing competitive condi­

tions.

Hypotheses

In order t~ proceed with the investigation of the

problem, three hypotheses were formulated:

1. There is a significant difference between the

ratio of simple to complex attacks taught in an average

college sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex

attacks which succeed in college sabre bouts.

2. There is a significant difference between the

ratio of simple to complex taught in an average

college sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex

ripostes which succeed in college sabre bouts.

3. There is a significant difference between the

ratio of simple to complex counterattacks taught _in an

average college sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to

complex counterattacks which succeed in college sabre

fencing bouts.

Three hypotheses were formulated as there are three

general categories of fencing actions, i.e. attacks,

ripostes and counterattacks. It w~s decided that, in order

to more precisely investigate the problem, it was necessary to survey and compare the three basic categories separately. Scope and Limitations of the Study

This study is limited by the following factors:

1. Only members of the National Fencing Coaches Association

of America were surveyed.

2. These coaches are currently coaching a college sabre

team or have overall head coaching responsibility for

one.

3. Data was obtained by observation of the 1979 National

Collegiate Athletic Association Fencing Cha~pionship

sabre finals.

Definition of Terms

For the purpose of clarity for the lay reader, various fencing terms will be defined below. All actions

listed under this category are defined in the Rules Manual of the Amateur Fencers' League of America (8). This book is a direct translation of the Rules Book of the Federation

Internationale d'Escrime, the governing body of world fencing.

Direct Attack: A one-tempo cut or thrust.

Direct Beat-Attack: A sharp strike against the opponent's blade followed by an immediate one-tempo cut or thrust.

Direct : A straight cut or thrust performed i~nediately after executing a . Direct Counter-Riposte: A straight cut or thrust executed

after the parry of the opponent's riposte. This is the

second action executed by the original attacker.

Simple Attack: A direct attack, direct beat-attack, or one­

two (-attack).

Simple Riposte: A direct riposte or direct counter-riposte.

Simple Counterattack: A direct stop cut or thrust, a

counter-time action, or a direct attack-on-preparation.

More Complex: Any action which requires more movements of

the sword (or hesitations in lieu of movements) than those

already lis ted.

It must be pointed out that the official definition

of a simple attack would exclude the "beat-attack" and the

"1-2", but these actions are considered so basic to sabre

fencing that they were classified as "simple" for the pur­ poses of this paper. CHAPTER II

REVIEW OF THE LITERATURE

Development of College Fencing

Fencing is one of this country's oldest inter­ collegiate sports. The original Intercollegiate Fencing

Association (comprised of Yale, Harvard, and Columbia) was founded in 1894, eleven years before the formation of the

National Collegiate Athletic Association (28:6). In the next few years, other Eastern colleges joined, as did the

U.S. Military and Naval Academies. Intercollegiate fencing remained concentrated on the East Coast through the 1930's although it gradually spread to a select nunilier of univer­ sities across the country. However, the inception of the

NCAA-sanctioned National Championships in 1941 spurred a rapid growth in ~ollegiate fencing teams.

The introduction of All-America awards in 1951 added further impetus to that growth until, in 1978, the NCAA was forced to limit the number of schools entering the

National Championships to forty (28:9-10).

However, American fencing has remained uniquely

European 1n philosophy relative to other collegiate sports

(10, 15, 25). The coaches who formed the backbone of col­ legiate fencing during the twenties, thirties, and forties were all European born, educated, and trained fencing

6 7

masters. Notable among these were Julio Castello of New

York University, Robert Grasson of Yale, and Clovis

Deladrier of the Naval Academy (28:8). Some of these early

coaches, espousing that the requirements of American col­

legiate competition were incompatible with many of the

accepted European theories of fencing and training, began

to make certain modifications.

In 1933, Castello tried to Americanize the various

fencing terms. He changed such terms as "riposte" to

"return", "coupe" to "cutover", "fleche" to "flash", etc.

in an attempt to make the sport more understandable in this

country ( 10). ·

In his 1948 book Deladrier described a system

created out of a combination of the prevailing European

systems of the time. He felt that such a "new" system was

more attuned to the requirements of college competition

(l5:xi-xii). Neither of these attempts seemed to gain

. general acceptance, however, as subsequent fencing books

neither mention a "Deladrier" system nor utilize Castello's

terminology (1, 6, 9, 12, 14, 17, 27, 35). Nevertheless,

the college teams trained by these men were remarkably

successful (28:7-10).

According to Aldo Nadi (25), the basic incongruity

between American reality and European methodology is the

factor of time. Nadi observed that European systems are

based on the assumption that the student will not enter 8

competition for three to five years after his first lesson.

Even then, he would be considered little more than a novice.

In discussing the techniques of his fencing master father

in the first decade of the 20th century he wrote:

He never allowed any pupil to start combat (bouting) until at least one year of mechan­ ical treatment. Competitions were out of the question before two years. And many of his pupils were never allowed to smell the dust of a competitive strip. ( 25: 231)

In view of the foregoing, it can be seen that a question might arise as to whether traditional theories of fencing instruction are really fitted to the exigencies of the college varsity environment, where a student sometimes must be placed into full competition within six months of his first lesson (15:xiii).

Specifically addressing that question to the sabre,

Barbesetti, one of the progenitors of modern sabre fencing, in discussing double-feint attacks wrote in 1894:

These double are of little practical value in attacks; but, on the other hand, they provide excellent exercise for the pupil in training his hand for the direction of the point and the accuracy of the parry. (5:75-76)

Sixty-four years later, in 1958, Roger Crosnier, the premier fencing master of Great Britain, stated:

As the sabre target is a large one to pro­ tect, the novice opponent is P-0ne too easy to deal with .... There is, therefore, every reason not to attack with compound movements, but to launch simple, rapid attacks which will have the right-of-way over any accidental whip. (12:191) 9

One of this country's most successful collegiate

coaches, Hugo Castello (son of Julio), looked into the

future of competitive fencing in 1962 and predicted:

It is likely that we shall see a double devel­ opment in the direction of (1) even faster and simpler actions in the attempt to score quickly, and (2) a re-emphasis on the ancient principle: hit without being hit. (9:3)

A search of the extant fencing literature revealed no authors or fencing masters who indicated any disagreement with the foregoing quotations. Therefore, a review was undertaken in order to discover if any observational data had been collected on what actions were actually most sue- cessful in collegiate fencing. No such study was uncovered, and for that matter, only one modern survey of Amateur

Fencer's League of America (AAU) competitions has been pub- lished. This study of the 1965 National Sabre Finals indicated that, at the highest level of American sabre fencing that year, one percent of the attacks and ten per- cent of the defenses met the definition of "more complex" actions as described in Chapter I (29:15-17).

The Fencing Lesson

The literature was next reviewed to discover what authorities had to say about what correlation, if any, should exist between the fencing lesson and the fencing bout. The majority of English language works are descrip- tive texts (6, 9, 10, 12, 13, 14, 17, 30) which are directed towards a beginning student rather than a coach and the 10

conceptual basis of fencing lessons is therefore not often

discussed. However, two highly regarded fencing masters

have cowmented on the matter.

Aldo Nadi, considered the best fencer of his era,

in his widely-acclaimed 1943 treatise On Fencing discussed

his conception of fencing lessons with this statement:

My main concern has been to set forth prac­ tical theories based exclusively upon the realities of combat, for he who separates theory from practice shows that he does not even begin to understand the meaning of fencing. (25:37)

And Maitre Michel Alaux, trainer of one of the greatest of modern French champions and coach of the U.S. Olympic

Fencing Team until his death in 1975 declared that the

fencer should be given three types of lessons: the "Aca-

demic", the "Training", and the "Bouting". Of the three, the "Academic" concentrates on straight fundamentals, tlie

"Training" emphasizes technical and physical conditioning under conditions similar to those of a bout, and .the "Bout- ing" lesson teaches tactics under simulated competitive conditions. In other words, two of the three types of lessons should recreate actual bout conditions (1:174-78).

In fact, there is a classical historical example of the efficacy of adapting fencing lessons to actual bout condi- tions, rather than vice-versa.

By the end of the nineteenth century, foil technique had become quite formalized and very different from the reality of the duel (1:82, 23:177). In that period from 11.

1896 to 1903, during the infamous Dreyfus Affair, over one

thousand duels were fought in France. One particular

Parisian fencing master departed from the accepted lesson

techniques of the day and advocated training students under

conditions similar to an actual duel rather than the scho­

lastic conventions of a formal foil engagement. When one

of his pupils, an unknown fencer, defeated one of the best

foilists of France in a duel, professional notice was taken

of this "new" method. In the succeeding years, his pupils won nearly all their combats (over 350 duels). From this

example, the techniques of epee fencing were established,

and the epee is now one of the three "weapons" of fencing;

the others being the foil and the sabre (15:92).

Statistical Analysis

It was realized that, for convenience, a statistical

test through which to analyze the data would assist the objectives of the study. The nature of these data provided some unique difficulties with respect to mathematical anal­ ysis. For one thing, there is no question as to whether the data is random: it is assuredly the result of a conscious process in the case of both the lesson and the bout. There­ fore, the classical Chi Square test, which demonstrates whether data could reasonably be ~he result of a random dib­ tribution, would be meaningless (19:276). At the same time, the basic question addressed by this study was whether there was any significant relationship at all between bouts and 12 lessons. The various tests of correlation, such as the Phi

Coefficient, "Q" test, Regressions, or Factor Analysis are designed to test the strength of a relationship that has already been established (19:196). Thus, correlational pro­ cedures were inappropriate for the needs of this study. A brief review of the literature (2, 18, 19, 24, 32), plus a personal interview with Professor Fadil Zuwaylif, Ph.D., instructor in Statistics for the School of Business at Cali­ fornia State University, Northridge, and author of Applied

Business Statistics (34), determined that the proper method by which to investigate the existence of a relationship would be a modified "Z" Test. This test is described in detail in Chapter III, but its basic purpose can be stated as a method by wh-ich to determine if two numbers could reasonably come from the same population. Most inferential statistical methods are based on testing the differences between samples from the same population. By definition, if samples come from two different populations, it is assumed that there are significant differences between them (19:63).

Thus, the first step in an investigation of fencing lessons and fencing bouts is to determine if they belong to the same or different statistical populations. The "Z" Test was deemed the appropriate statistical procedure by which to make that determination. CHAPTER III

HETHOD

Survey Design and Procedures

It was determined that the optimum procedure by which to obtain data of a national nature would be to

observe and record performances at the national collegiate

fencing championships, while information on coaching les­ sons would be garnered by a prepared questionnaire. The data on scoring actions were obtained from personal obser­ vation by the researcher at the 1979 Sabre Finals of the

National Collegiate Athletic Association (NCAA) Fencing

Championships. These finals consist of twenty-four fencers who qualified from a preliminary round of forty. These initial forty fencers reached the championship by qualify­ ing through Regional tournaments. They thus represent the best college sabremen from each region of the country.

It is customary for the directors of sabre bouts

(presiding officials) to describe the actions to their judges before awarding points. The determination as to what type of action resulted in a score was made by record­ ing the analysis of the director of the bout being observeu.

The qualifications of the author to make such observations are:

13 14

1. Class II Director's Rating (2nd highest national rating)

2. Class A Competitor's Rating (highest national rating)

3. Twenty years competing in and directing sabre

4. ~vo-and-a-half years experience coaching collegiate sabre

The data on fencing lessons themselves were subjec­

tive in nature in that they were obtained from answers to a

questionnaire sent to coaches whose names were obtained from

the current membership list of the National Fencing Coaches

Association of America (the only fencing coaches organiza­

tion in the United States). Only those questionnaires

returned by coaches who are currently responsible for col­

legiate sabre teams were included in the survey. In this

manner it was intended to insure that competitive data

. gathered during 1978-79 would be matched against the type

of lessons given during that same period.

In order to estimate the amount of emphasis placed

on various actions in a lesson, the coaches were·asked to

estimate t~e percentage of total lesson time spent on par­

ticular movements. The questionnaire and covering letter

(Appendix C) were designed so as not to indicate any bias

in the quest for information. Therefore, many more cate­

gories of actions were listed than were actually necessary

for the purposes of the study. By reference to" the defini­

tion of terms in Chapter I, it can be seen which actions on

the questionnaire are defined as simple and which are

defined as complex. Upon receipt of the completed form, 15 the data were combined to arrive at a percentage of total attacks, total ripostes, and total counterattacks for simple as opposed to complex actions as taught in fencing lessons. Thus, each questionnaire was reduced to a set of ratio numbers which indicate the percentage of total les­ son time devoted to simple as well as complex actions by each responding coach. Each category was then arithmetic­ ally combined to arrive at a mean (v) for simple and complex attacks, ripostes, and counterattacks of all the lessons surveyed. These means provided one of the parameters for the statistical comparison. The data obtained from observations were analyzed in the same fashion. The figures for simple as opposed to complex attacks, ripostes, and counterattacks were compared with the total number of actions in each category to arrive at a ratio number for simple and complex actions in each category. Reference to the covering letter (see Appendix) will demonstrate that coaches were specifically asked to report on their varsity sabre team only. It is intuitively obvious that a lesson given to a rank beginner must consist almost entirely of drills designed to teach basics and can­ not, by its very nature, have any more than a coincidental relation to an actual bout. The thrust of this project was to investigate the types of lessons given to sabre fencers who are already in competition. 16

Analysis of Data

An investigation into various methods of statistical analysis concluded that the best procedure through which to analyze the data was a modified "Z" Test.

In order to employ this test, one assumption had to be made. This assumption has no effect on the value of the data; it merely allows a basis from which to conduct a mathe- matical test to determine if the two sources of data could derive from the same statistical population. The assumption is as follows:

A coach intends a fencing lesson to be a close

approximation of the actual conditions of a fenc-

ing bout. Thus for example, if he devotes sixty

percent of the attack portion of his lesson plan

to simple actions, he is predicting that approxi-

mately sixty percent of the scoring attacks will

be simple ones.

Through use of this assumption, the "Z" Test becomes:

z = p- if / if (1 - if) n if = Predicted percent p = Observed percent n = Number of observations if will be the mean of all lessons surveyed. 17

It was decided to test the findings at the traditional 0.05

Level of Significance as no serious damage could be envi­

sioned from the one chance out of twenty that a Type I error

could be committed. Therefore, testing at a = 0.05, a "Z"

value ~ 1.64 indicates that rr and p came from the same

population; a "Z" > 1.64 indicates that rr and p came from

significantly different populations.

The conventions for use of the "Z" Test do require

an expansion of the hypotheses of this paper. It is estab­

lished practice to use this procedure to directly test a null hypothesis and to use an alternate hypothesis in con­

junction with it (24:127). Therefore, the hypotheses were expanded as follows:

1. There is no significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex attacks taught in an average college

sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex attacks

which sncceed in college sabre bouts.

1a. There is a significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex attacks taught in an average college

sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex attacks

which succeed in college sabre bouts.

2. There is no significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex ripostes taught in an average college

sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex

ripostes which succeed in a college sabre bout. 18

2a. There is a significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex ripostes taught in an average sabre

lesson and the ratio of simple to complex ripostes which

succeed in a college sabre bout.

3. There is no significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex counterattacks taught in an average

college sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex

counterattacks which succeed in a college sabre bout.

3a. There is a significant difference between the ratio of

simple to complex attacks taught in an average sabre

lesson and the ratio of simple to complex counterattacks

which succeed in a college sabre bout.

Finally, two pilot studies were conducted in prepar­ ation for this project. Observations were made at the 1978

NCAA Fencing Championships at the University of Wisconsin­

Parkside and the 1979 Western Regional Championships held at the University of California, San Diego. These observa­ tions were used to confirm the reliability of the observa­ tions made for this study. They appear in Appendix (D). CHAPTER IV

RESULTS

Of sixty questionnaires sent to college coaches,

twenty-six were returned initially, with twenty-one being

completed in a manner from which data could be taken. Two

coaches reported that they had no general lesson plan, and three stated that they did not coach sabre teams. A second mailing was effected, and six more questionnaires were returned for a total of thirty-two, with twenty-seven being completed in a usable manner. It should be noted that of the total, seventeen of the top twenty coaches (based on team finish at the NCAA's) are represented.

The various percentages reported were combined arithmetically to obtain a percentage of time devoted to simple attacks as compared 'to complex attacks, simple ripostes as compared to complex ripostes, and simple counter­ attacks as opposed to complex counterattacks. These percent­ ages were assembled and a mean derived from each category as indicated in Table A on the following page.

19 20

TABLE A

RESULTS OF FENCING LESSON SURVEY

Quest. Attacks Ripostes Counterattacks No. Sim2le Com}2lex Sim}2le Complex Simple Comnlex (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) (%) 1. 62.5 37.5 75 25 100 0 2. 100 0 50 50 100 0 3. 77 23 50 50 54 46 4. 71 29 74 26 79 21 5. 89 11 37.5 62.5 33 67 6. 70 30 33.3 66.6 50 50 7. 46.8 53.2 14.3 85.7 40 60 8. 90 10 66.6 33.3 60 40 9. 85 15 60 40 80 20 10. 50 50 25 75 40 60 11. 66.6 33.3 33.3 66.6 40 60 12. 67.5 32.5 28.6 71.4 56 44 13. 87.5 12.5 50 50 65 35 14. 50 50 16.7 83.3 48 52 15. 75 25 70 30 80 20 16. 95 5 70 30 85 15 17. 95 5 62 38 55.2 44.8 18. 75 25 62.5 37.5 40 60 19. 87 13 40 60 52.4 47.6 20. 75 25 65 35 45 55 21. 75 25 70 30 80 20 22. 60 40 50 50 50 50 23. 70 30 65 35 60 40 24. 50 50 30 70 40 60 25. 80 20 80 20 90 10 26. 75 25 65 35 45 55 27. 80 20 55 45 --65 35 rr::74.1 7l':w 25. 86 71::52.46 7T:::47.89 '11:60.36 ?r::-40.3

To determine the responses from each individual coach read horizontally. To read individual responses for each category, read vertically. 21

As described in the previous chapter, observations were made at the NCAA Fencing Championships held on March 23 and 24, 1979, at Princeton University, Princeton, New Jersey.

This information was collated and is presented in Table B, along with the percentages calculated from the raw numbers.

TABLE B

OBSERVATION OF 1979 NCAA's

Attacks No. %

Simple: 120 83.30

Complex: 24 16.67

Total: 144 99.97

Ripostes

Simple: 74 93.67

Complex: 5 6.33

Total: 79 100.00

Counterattacks

Simple: 46 79.31

Complex: 12 20.69

Total: 58 100.00

The applicable ratios were taken from Tables A and

B to compute the "Z" value. It was deemed redundant to cal­ culate the "Z" values for both complex and simple Means since if the Mean for simple attacks was (for example) 75 22

percent the Mean for complex attacks had to be 25 percent and the resulting "Z" calculation would produce an identi- cal value. Therefore, only the Means. for simple actions were matched against the observed percentages for simple actions to compute the "Z" value. The equations and the resulting computations are presented in Table C below:

TABLE C COMPUTATION OF THE "Z" VALUE

p - 'IT z = j 'IT(1 - 'IT) n

ATTACKS

p = . 833 'IT= . 741 .833 - .741 z . 741( 1 - .741) = 2.52 n = 144 =j 144

RIPOSTES

p = .9367 . 9367 - .5246 7.35 'IT= .5246 z .5246(1 - .5246) = =j 79 n = 79

COUNTERATTACKS

p = . 7931 . 7931 - . G036 'IT = . 6036 z =JJ .6036(1 - .6036) = 2.95 58 n = 58 23

As stated in Chapter III, a test at the 0.05 Level of Significance requires a "Z" value of 1.64 or less to confirm the null hypothesis; a value higher than 1.64 results in rejection. The findings, as shown in Table C, are as follows:

1. The "Z" value for simple attacks in bouts com­ pared to lessons is 2.52. Therefore, Hypothesis No. 1, that there is no significant difference between the ratio of simple to complex attacks taught in an average college sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex attacks which succeed in a college sabre bout was rejected. The alternate hypothesis, that there is a significant differ­ ence, was accepted.

2. The "Z" value for simple ripostes in bouts compared to simple ripostes in lessons is 7.35. Therefore,

Hypothesis No. 2, that there is no significant difference between the ratio of simple to complex ripostes taught in an average sabre lesson and the ratio of simple to complex ripostes which succeed in college sabre bouts was rejected.

The alternate hypothesis, that there is a significant dif­ ference, was ~ccepted.

3. The "Z" value for simple counterattacks in bouts compared to simple counterattacks"in lessons is 2.95.

Therefore, Hypothesis No. 3, that there ts no significant difference between the ratio of simple to complex counter­ attacks taught in an average college sabre lesson and the 24 ratio of simple to complex counterattacks which succeed in a bout was rejected. The alternate hypothesis, that there is a significant difference, was accepted.

Although it is statistically more precise to dis­ cuss the results in terms of the "Z" Test, comparisons are more easily comprehended if percentage ratios are dealt with. Thus while 83.3 percent of the scoring attacks in the NCAA Finals were simple actions, the mean time spent on simple attacks in lessons was 74.1 percent. And, while

93.67 percent of the scoring ripostes were simple, mean lesson time devoted to simple ripostes was only 52.46 per­ cent. In the same vein, although 79.31 percent of scoring counterattacks were simple, 60.36 percent of mean lesson time was spent on simple counterattacks.

As a parenthetical addition, it might be of value to point out that, for overall observed scoring actions, attacks constituted 51.25 percent, ripostes 28.11 percent, and counterattacks 20.64 percent. The pilot study of the

1978 NCAA's yielded figures as follows: attacks--54.4 per­ cent, ripostes--27.5 percent, and counterattacks--18.1 per­ cent. The comparable figures for the 1979 Western Regional

Championships were attacks--54.05 percent, ripostes--20.61 percent, ~nd counterattacks--25.34 percent. It was thus deemed legitimate to infer that the observed data are con­ sistent with a general pattern, and not the result of the chance selection of an aberrant tournament. CHAPTER V

SUMMARY, DISCUSSION, AND CONCLUSIONS

Summary

As demonstrated by the results obtained and pre­

sented in Chapter IV, the alternate hypotheses that there

are significant differences between the types of actions

taught most often in college sabre lessons and the types

of actions which actually score most often in college

sabre bouts were accepted. The results of the "Z" Test

indicate that the Mean ratios for simple actions in lessons

derive from significantly different populations than the

Mean ratios for simple actions actually occurring in bouts.

It must be remembered that the "Z" Test is a statistical method by which it can be determined if two numbers could

reasonably derive from the same population. A high value

for "Z" does not necessarily indicate the complete absence of the possibility that both numbers were selected from the same population, but it does demonstrate that the possibil­ ity, if it exists, is very slight.

Discussion

It should be reiterated at this point that a key assumption was made in order to apply a statistical test: a coach's lesson plan was intended to approximate actual bout conditions.

25 26

It must be pointed out that some questionnaires were returned uncompleted, with the comment that the coach tailored each lesson to the individual fencer, and thus followed no general pattern or lesson plan. Since one of these particular respondents had established an enviable collegiate record, and had been twice named "Fencing Coach of the Year", this type of reply must be considered and discussed.

In effect, this theory of designing fencing lessons advocates that the particular strengths of the individual student (as the coach perceives them) be the determinate of the structure of the lesson, and, therefore, it is not necessary that the lessons resemble actual bout conditions.

To be sure, the lessons must conform to the rules of fenc­ ing, but they are not geared to competitive conditions per se. It is felt that the fencer, by emphasizing his strengths and minimizing his weaknesses, will dominate and set the pattern for each bout, regardless of some generally observed pattern. In short, the nature of the weapon and the rules does not inherently set a structure on the bout; only the individual fencer does that. Obviously then, to say that one has no overall lesson plan, or if so, to say that it does not bear a relationship to general competitive condi­ tions, is a statement of no significance.

One can see a certain existential logic in this approach, but it does not seem to be supported either by 27 the literature or by history as indicated in the Review of the Literature in Chapter II. Both Aldo Nadi and Michel

Alaux clearly stated their belief that there should be a close relationship between fencing lessons and actual com­ petitive conditions (25:37, 1:174-78), and history, judging by the development of epee fencing (15:92), appears to support their views.

The question then arises: In view of the foregoing, how can a coach tailor lessons to each individual without regard to overall bout patterns and still be successful?

No definitive answer is forthcoming, but some answers may be suggested.

1. A coach uses many tools with his students. Technical

exactitude is one of them, but psychology, intelli­

gence, personality, character, ability to communicate,

etc. also play roles. Thus, a coach's lesson might be

tactically dubious from the standpoint of hard data,

yet the overall approach that coach takes with his

fencers may more than make up for any weaknesses.

2. In actual fact,· the respondents said that they had no

overall lesson plan, and that each lesson was geared to

the individual. They did not indicate one way or the

other whether these lessons ap~roached the observed

bout percentages. It is quite likely than an experi­

enced coach would have an intuitive grasp of the general

distribution of actions and subconsciously fit his

lessons into that framework. 28

Further support for the theory that the lesson should resemble the bout can be found in the general prin- ciples of motor learning, such as Specificity, Sensory Set, and Perception. In order to obtain definitions for these principles the works of four authorities from different sections of the country were consulted. These works were all textbooks which were summarizations of current theories in the field and all agreed on the definitions of the prin- ciples listed above. These authors were Dr. John N.

Drowatzky, Professor of Physical Education at the University of Toledo (16); Dr. John D. Lawther, Professor of Physical

Education at Pennsylvania State University (22); and Dr.

George H. Sage, Professor of Physical Education at the

University of Northern Colorado (26). Their definitions follow below:

. The principle of Specificity basically states that the best way to teach an activity is to have the student perform that specific activity as best he can rather than performing some analogous action. According to Sage, writ- ing in 1977:

. . . there is strong support in the motor skills literature for the specificity of motor learning principle . . . which is that motor skills are not based on a few general factors but are based upon factors which are speciftc to the task in question. (26:439)

As for Sensory Set, Drowatzky defines it as the expectations of the subject regarding the type of stimulus and its appearance. Observing that a subject's reaction 29 time under a sensory set was faster than under a motor set

(concentration on the task rather than the stimulus), he concluded that:

Conditions that enhance the performer's under­ standing of the task and aid in clarifying his expectancies will improve reaction time . . . ( 16:140)

Finally, Lawther's definition of Perception is succinct and supported by the others; "Perceptions, once learned, are automatic responses to specific types of stimuli." ( 22:43)

Fencing, as in any other one-against-one sport, requires of the individual the ability to perceive movements, tactical opportunities, and dangers, and to react in an appropriate manner to them. The idea behind a fencing les- son is to teach a student how to respond appropriately to what he perceives. It would follow that a fencing lesson teaches a student to respond to certain types of stimuli.

If the lesson is not predicated on the same stimuli that the student will encounter in a bout, then the reactions taught in a lesson (no matter at what length) may not appear in a bout since the studentjcompetitor is being subjected to unfamiliar stimuli. One out of a myriad of "classical" fencing lessons should serve to illustrate the point.

A training action described in a list of suggested lessons in 1948 was as follows: Instructor attacks with a beat, feint flank, feint head, disengage head (a four- movement attack). Pupil responds with parries of 2nd, 5th, 30 p '

and counter-5th, and ripostes with feint to right flank/

cut to head (a three-movement combination parry with a two­ movement riposte). (15:263)

Yet, what the pupil is most likely to encounter in a bout is a one- or two-movement attack to which (to be successful) he should respond with a one-movement riposte.

Since it takes roughly twice as long to execute a four­ movement attack as opposed to a two-movement attack, and consequently requires a greater distance as well, it seems reasonable to assume that the stimulus provided in this lesson differs considerably from the stimulus occurring in a bout. If the principle of "Sensory Set" is accepted

(26:249), then the lesson just described would probably have "set" the student to perceive complex movements and the time required for them. If the stimulus in a bout is of a speed or pattern for which the "sensory set" is unpre­ pared, the response may be erratic or delayed.

In summary then, it appears that historical example, the body of modern fencing literature, and the generally acknowledged principles of motor learning all tend to confirm the belief that the complex drill does not necessarily prepare the student for the simple action. At the same time, the well-known principle of "Specificity"

(26:439) would indicate that the complex drill will prepare the student for the complex action, but the simple drill will best prepare the student for the simple action. If, 31 then, the majority of bouting actions at the collegiate level are simple ones, it would seem logical to suggest that the major portion of collegiate fencing lessons be devoted to training the student in those simple actions.

Conclusions

Within the limits of this study, the conclusion that there are significant differences between the types of actions taught in the average collegiate sabre lesson and the types of actions that are most successful in collegiate sabre bouts is supported. In particular, the large dispari­ ties between lessons and bout conditions in the categories of parries and counterattacks.must raise the question as to whether the average college fencing lesson is providing the optimum preparation for modern competitive conditions.

It is believed that the findings of this project are not significant as an end to themselves but for the questions that they raise. As mentioned in the introduc­ tion, no published statistical studies of either collegiate bouting or collegiate lessons were uncovered. It thus appears that this is the first study to confirm that a con­ siderable difference does exist between the average college sabre lesson and collegiate sabre bouts. It therefore raises the following basic questions:

I. Will college sabremen trained only by simple actions

be more successful in competition than college sabre­

men trained only by complex actions? 32

II. Will college sabre fencers trained by a combination

of simple and complex actions be more successful in

competition than either of the above?

III. If so, what would the optimum mix of simple to complex

actions be for a general sabre lesson plan to produce

the most effective competitor?

In addition, the role of perception in fencing actions was touched on in the discussion. It appears at this time that this sport has been left virtually untouched by specialists in the field of Perceptual Motor Learning.

It is believed that there are implications in the literature and the findings of this study which indicate that extant motor learning theories may provide the best framework around which to design fencing lessons. More research in this area seems to be strongly indicated.

The goal of this study, which was to determine if there were significant differences between college sabre lessons and college sabre bouts, has been achieved. The results indicate that much more emphasis is placed on the teaching of complex actions than would seem to be warranted by the requirements of modern collegiate competition. It is left to the work of future researchers to determine what specific correlation should exist between college sabre lessons and actual bout conditions. BIBLIOGRAPHY

1. Alaux, Michel. Modern Fencing. New York: Charles Scribner~s Sons, 1975.

2. Anderson, T. W. An Introduction to Multivariate Statistical Analysis. New York: John Wiley & Sons, Inc., 1958.

3. Angelo, Domenico and Harry. The School of Fencing. London: 1787. Reprinted New York: Land's End Press, 1971.

4. Baldick, RobArt. The Duel. New York: Clarkson N. Potter, Inc., 1965.

5. Barbasetti, Luigi. The Art of the Sabre and the Epee. New York: E. P. Dutton & Co., 1936. (First American:·;;;,?:;';!fr edition--originally published in Italian in 1894.) · ·... ·.

6. Bower, Muriel. Fencing. Third edition. Dubuque: William C. Brown & Co., 1976. 7. Bryson, Frederick M. The Sixteenth Century Italian Duel. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, 1938.

8. Byrnes, J. A. for Competitions. West­ field: Amateur Fencers' League of America, 1974, rev. 1976.

9. Castello, Hugo and James. Fencing. New York: The Roland Press, 1962.

10. Castello, Julio. The Theory and Practice of Fencing. New York: Charles Scribner's Sons, 1933. 1i. Cratty, Bryant J. Perceptual Motor Development in Infants and Children. New York: Macmillan Co., 1970.

12. Crosnier, Roger. Fencing with the Sabre. London: Faber & Faber, Ltd., 1957. 13. Crosnier; Roger. Fencing with the Epee. London: Faber & Faber, Ltd., 1958. 14. Curry, Nancy L. Fencing. Pacific Palisades: Good­ year Publishing Co., 1969.

-33- 34

15. Deladrier, Clovis. Modern Fencing. Annapolis: U.S. Naval Institute, 1948.

16. Drowatzky, John N. Motor Learning: Principles and Practices. Minneapolis: Burgess Publishing Co., 1975. 17. Garret, M. R. and Heinecke, M. F. Fencing. Boston: Allyn & Bacon, Inc., 1971.

18. Ehrenfeld, S. and Littauer, S. Introduction to Statis­ tical Method. New York: McGraw-Hill, 1964. 19. Fox, Donald J. The Research Process in Education. New York: Holt, Rinehart, Winston, Inc., 1969. 20. Grombach, John V. Olympic Calvalcade of Sports. New York: Ballantine Books, 1956. 21. Hutton, Alfred. The Sword and the Centuries. London: Grant Richards, 1901. 22. Lawther, J. D. The Learning of Physical Skills. Englewood Cliffs: Prentice-Hall, 1968. 23. Lidstone, R. A. Fencing. London: H.F.&G. Witherby, Ltd. , 1952.

24. Lovejoy, E. P. Statistics for Math Haters. New York: Harper & Row, 1975. 25. Nadi, Aldo. On Fencing. New York: G. B. Putnam's Sons, 1943. 26. Sage, George H. Introduction to Motor Behavior: A Neuropsychological Approach. Reading: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., 1977. 27. Selberg, Charles A. Foil. Menlo Park: Addison­ Wesley Publishing Co.~76. 28. Tishman, Jeffrey R. "History of American Fencing", Portland: U. S. National Fencing Championship Pro­ gram, 1977. 29. Toth, Nicholas G. "Sabre Finals--1965", American Fencing, 17:15-17, January 1966. 30. Vince, Joseph. Fencing. New York: The Roland Press Co., 1962. 31. Wagner, Edward. Cut and Thrust Weapons. London: The Hamlyn Publishing Group, 1967. 35

32. Walker, H. M. and Lev, J. Elementary Statistical Methods. New York: Henry Holt & Co., 1958. 33. Wilson, John Lyde. The Code of Honor. Charleston: James Phinney, 1858.

34. Zuwaylif, Fadil H. Applied Business Statistics. Reading: Addison-Wesley Publishing Co., 1974. APPENDIX C

COPIES OF THE COVERING LETTER AND QUESTIONNAIRE SENT TO COLLEGE COACHES

36 37

18561 Prairie St., #23 Northridge, Ca. 91324 Dear Coach I· am in the process of writing my thesis for completion of a Master's Degree in Physical Education. My area of inter­ est is collegiate sabre fencing, and my research revolves around the construction of collegiate sabre lessons. In order ·to obtain some of the necessary data, I have prepared a quest­ tionnaire which I request that you complete. Please review the lessons you give to your first-string varsity sabre team over the course of a season and then est­ imate the percentage of total lesson time that you spend on each of the various categories o: actions listed. I

realize that your lessons will vary be~Neen your individual fencers, but plec>.se try to estirr.8.te the percentages for your team as a whole. I know that this request· reaches you in the heavy part of your season, but I would be grateful for your assistance.

I will be collating my data in r.~arch and therefore request you return the questionnaire at your earliest convenience. I

h~ve enclosed a self-addressed, stamped envelope for this purpose. Thanking you for your cooperation, I am

Sincerely,

Edwin K. Hurst 38

.Are You A Eead Co;1ch 0 Sabre Coach Q other ACTION % TI'-;E S?zt!T

A. .;.TT \Ci'.S

1. S:~.:m;)le

2.

). 1-2 h. Prise de Fer 1-2

r' :;)• 1-2-3 6. Prise de Fer 1-2-3

7. ~!ore comnlex

B. Ri')o'::tes

1. Direct

2. Counter

3· Co'J.nter-counter h. 2nd counter Com:>ound 6.'· more com:1lex c. Sto:>s 1. Direct st<1p

2. Feint-stop 3· counter-time action 4. D. Cornter-Attacks

1. Attack on preparation

2. Pride de fer attack on "9rep. 3. Finta in teJ:t>o h. 1-lore complex

NAME ------SCHOOL ------APPENDIX D

RESULTS OF THE PILOT STUDIES PERFORMED IN PREPARATION FOR THIS STUDY

39 40

OBSERVATIONS OF 1978 NCAA's

Attacks No. %

Simple: 59 72.8

Complex: 22 27.2

Total: 81 100.0

Ripostes

Simple: 47 92.2

Complex: 4 7.8

Total: 51 100.0

Counterattacks

Simple: 27

Complex: 41

OBSERVATIONS OF THE 1979 WIFC's

Attacks No. % Simple: 143 89.38

Complex: 17 10.63

Ripostes

Simple: 61 100.00

Complex: 0 0.00

Counterattacks

Simple: 75

Complex: