Imagery and Memory : the Bizarreness Issue Reexamined

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Imagery and Memory : the Bizarreness Issue Reexamined RICE UNIVERSITY IMAGERY AND MEMORY: THE BIZARRENESS ISSUE REEXAMINED by PAMELA ANN KENNEDY A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE MASTER OF ARTS APPROVED, THESIS COMMITTEE: Assistant Professor of Psychology Chairperson William C. Howell Professor of Psychology HOUSTON, TEXAS MAY, 1979 Abstract IMAGERY AND MEMORY: THE BIZARRENESS ISSUE REEXAMINED by PAMELA ANN KENNEDY This research examined the effects of the bizarreness attribute of imagery on memory. While previous research has not generally sup¬ ported the facilitory effects of bizarreness on recall performance, there are a number of conceptual and methodological problems with this body of research. The present research attempted to overcome these problems by developing a more rigorous conceptualization and operation¬ alization of the construct of bizarreness, and utilizing a design which controls for past methodological contaminants. Half of the 64 subjects were instructed to form images while the other half rehearsed phrases in rote fashion. Within each of these conditions, half of the subjects were tested by free recall and half by frequency estimation. For all subjects, half of the phrases were bizarre and half were common, as defined by pre-ratings made by independent subjects. Frequency level of phrase presentation was varied, with frequency levels 1, 2, 4 and 6 being represented. Finally, subjects were tested immediately after list presentation and again after one week. The results indicated that for cued recall, bizarre phrases were superior to common phrases. In addition, the superiority of bizarre over common imagery increased between the immediate and delayed tests. For frequency estimation, the data did not reveal any readily inter¬ pretable differences between common and bizarre phrases. The results were discussed as they relate to previous research on bizarreness. It was concluded that bizarreness does have a facilitory effect in imagery mediation. Suggestions for future research to further clarify the effects of bizarreness were presented. ii Acknowledgments I would like to express appreciation to the members of my thesis committee: Sarah A. Burnett, who served as Chairperson, and William C. Howell. I am indebted to both of these for their guidance and assistance on all stages of my work. I would also like to express sincere gratitude to Robert D. Pritchard, who was a constant source of moral support and encouragement during my hours of writing. Table of Contents Title page Abstract .... i Acknowledgments iii Table of Contents iv Introduction . 1 Method .... 25 Results .... 30 Discussion . 37 References . 42 Appendix A . 45 Imagery has been recognized as a useful memory aid for centuries. Indeed the practical mnemonic application of imagery has existed in professional mnémotechniques since its formal inception in ancient Greece. According to several treatises (see Yates, 1966), a Greek poet named Simonodes was the first to develop and formally describe a method for using imagery to improve memory. This method, commonly known as the "method of loci," combined the use of images with rules for their orderly mental arrangement, which enabled the user to retain long speeches, lists, etc. (Yates, 1966). There are several steps involved in using this method. First, one would visualize a familiar, highly imageable set of locations (loci) and memorize them in order. After the set of loci are committed to memory, the items or passages to be learned are represented as images and stored sequentially in these locations. When recall of this material is desired one would progress mentally from one location to the next. By visualizing each location and its contents, the information stored there would be avail able for recall. Many of the systems used by professional mnemonists employ the same principles outlined in the method of loci. For example, the peg word system is a commonly used mnemonic technique (see Pavio, 1971) which is based on imagery and rules to preserve order. In this system peg words which rhyme with the numbers one through ten (1 - bun, 2 - shoe, 3 - tree, etc.) are used in place of visual locations. 1 2 Specifically, a to-be-remembered word is associated with each peg word by forming an image between the two. Order is preserved by the numbers associated with each peg word. When retrieval is desired the peg word is used to elicit the image and the to-be-remembered word should be available for recall. Although many such ideas and schemes for improving memory have been suggested by stage mnemonists down through the years, only in the last twenty years or so have these techniques been tested in a systematic fashion by psychologists. According to psychologists during the behavioristic era, imagery could not be observed and was thus not a suitable subject for study. Thus the functional role of imagery in memory, the value of imagery mediation relative to other memory systems, and specific attributes of the image responsible for its effects had not been adequately understood. Recently however, by incorporating many of the practices and assumptions derived from mnemonic strategies into testable hypotheses, considerable research effort has been directed toward these issues. As a result, fairly consistent findings and generalizations have emerged supporting imagery mediation as a memory system and specifying qualities of the image which enhance its effective¬ ness as a mediational device. If findings from imagery studies are examined, it is clear that the use of imagery has been shown to result in impressive memory performance in many situations. Although many early studies were conducted in the absence of control or comparison groups, the results have shown that by simply instructing subjects to form images using techniques such as the method of loci and peg word system, surprisingly excellent recall is produced (Bugelski, 1968; Pavio, 1970). 3 Furthermore, the use of imagery in traditional learning paradigms, (with control groups) has yielded additional evidence supporting its value in the memory system. Take for example paired-associated learning, which is the paradigm most frequently employed to assess imagery effects. Several different instructional sets have been used in P-A paradigms. In these instructional sets subjects have been 1) instructed to form images connecting the stimulus and response terms of a paired-associate list, 2) told to use variations of the peg word system, 3) given pictures, sentences, or phrases and asked to generate the specified images. In all of these variations of the P-A task, memory facilitation has been demonstrated consistently (Pavio, 1971; Reese, 1977). A third line of support for imagery comes from studies which have tested the value of imagery mediation relative to other memory systems (e.g. Delin, 1969). In general, instructing subjects to use imagery produces better memory performance than instructing them to rehearse in a rote fashion or giving them no instructions at all (Pavio, 1971; Bower, 1972; Reese, 1977; Kieras, 1978). From this line of research a number of factors critical to the effect of imagery on memory performance have begun to be studied and identified. These factors include individual difference variables, the nature of the material to be learned, how it is coded, etc. From these studies, generalizations have emerged regarding the specific qualities of the image which increase its effectiveness as a mediational device. Two such well documented generalizations have to do with con¬ creteness of the image and whether or not the image represents inter¬ action between the members of the noun pair. The conclusion that 4 images formed from concrete nouns are easier to form and recall than those from abstract nouns is now widely accepted (Pavio, 1971; Bower, 1972). Furthermore, in a paired-associate task recall is facilitated if the image connecting the stimulus and response terms is one that depicts the referents of the two terms in interaction rather than in isolation (Wollen, Weber, and Lowry, 1972; Bower, 1972). Another quality of the image which has received considerable research attention but has not produced clear results is bizarreness. The idea that bizarre images are remembered better than common images is found in recent mnemonic applications (Lorayneand Lucas, 1974; Bower, 1973), as well as in ancient treatises (Yates, 1966). In modern day theory the assumption that bizarreness produces superior retention seems to be based on the belief that making an image bizarre increases its distinctiveness and thereby reduces interference (see Pavio, 1971). Research on this particular attribute of images has been plagued with difficulties and consequently results have been inconclusive. The purpose of this paper is to explore the contributions of bizarreness in the influence of imagery on cued recall. That is, while it is clear that imagery aids recall, it is less clear what role bizarreness plays in the facilitory effects of imagery. In the section that follows, the conclusions of the existing research dealing with this issue will first be summarized briefly. Next, the major problems with this body of research will be discussed. These problems involve: 1) the conceptualization of bizarreness, 2) the operational definition of bizarreness, and 3) a variety of methodological/design issues. In a second section, the ways the present study attempts to overcome the problems described in these three areas will be presented. 5 Evaluation and Review of Existing Research The conclusions of the existing research on the relationship between bizarreness of the image and recall have been mixed. The majority of studies have concluded that bizarreness is not critical (Briggs, Hawkins, and Crovitz, 1970; Johnson, 1972; Nappe and Wollen, 1973; Hauck, Walsh, and Kroll, 1976; Wood, 1967; Collyer, Jonldes, and Bevan, 1972; Wollen, Weber, and Lowry, 1972; Senter and Hoffman, 1976). Other studies have shown support for bizarreness (Crovitz, 1969; Del in, 1968; Perensky and Senter, 1970; Andreoff and Yarmey, 1976). Careful examination of these studies, however, suggests a variety of problems that make it impossible to draw any firm conclusions about the effects of bizzareness.
Recommended publications
  • Enhancing the Production Effect in Memory: Singing, Underlying Mechanisms, and Applications
    ENHANCING THE PRODUCTION EFFECT IN MEMORY: SINGING, UNDERLYING MECHANISMS, AND APPLICATIONS by Chelsea Karmel Quinlan Submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy at Dalhousie University Halifax, Nova Scotia December 2017 © Copyright by Chelsea Karmel Quinlan, 2017 TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES .......................................................................... v LIST OF FIGURES ....................................................................... vi ABSTRACT ................................................................................ vii LIST OF ABBREVIATIONS USED .................................................. viii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS ................................................................ ix CHAPTER 1: INTRODUCTION ......................................................... 1 1.1 INTRODUCTION .................................................................................................. 2 1.2 HISTORY OF THE PRODUCTION EFFECT ........................................................... 4 1.3 A REVIEW OF THEORETICAL PERSPECTIVES .................................................. 11 1.4 MUSIC AND MEMORY ....................................................................................... 17 1.5 CHAPTER SUMMARY, RATIONALE, AND CURRENT EXPERIMENTS ................ 19 CHAPTER 2: EXTENDING THE BOUNDARIES OF THE PRODUCTION EFFECT .................................................................................... 24 2.1 ABSTRACT ........................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Elaborative Encoding, the Ancient Art of Memory, and the Hippocampus
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2013) 36, 589–659 provided by RERO DOC Digital Library doi:10.1017/S0140525X12003135 Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus Sue Llewellyn Faculty of Humanities, University of Manchester, Manchester M15 6PB, United Kingdom http://www.humanities.manchester.ac.uk [email protected] Abstract: This article argues that rapid eye movement (REM) dreaming is elaborative encoding for episodic memories. Elaborative encoding in REM can, at least partially, be understood through ancient art of memory (AAOM) principles: visualization, bizarre association, organization, narration, embodiment, and location. These principles render recent memories more distinctive through novel and meaningful association with emotionally salient, remote memories. The AAOM optimizes memory performance, suggesting that its principles may predict aspects of how episodic memory is configured in the brain. Integration and segregation are fundamental organizing principles in the cerebral cortex. Episodic memory networks interconnect profusely within the cortex, creating omnidirectional “landmark” junctions. Memories may be integrated at junctions but segregated along connecting network paths that meet at junctions. Episodic junctions may be instantiated during non–rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep after hippocampal associational function during REM dreams. Hippocampal association involves relating, binding, and integrating episodic memories into a mnemonic compositional whole. This often bizarre, composite image has not been present to the senses; it is not “real” because it hyperassociates several memories. During REM sleep, on the phenomenological level, this composite image is experienced as a dream scene.
    [Show full text]
  • Ack Baraly Kylee Tamera 202
    EXTRINSIC AND INTRINSIC FACTORS INFLUENCING THE POSITIVE MEMORY BIAS IN AGING KYLEE TAMERA ACK BARALY A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Doctorate in Philosophy degree in Experimental Psychology at the University of Ottawa; Doctorate in Philosophy degree in Cognitive Sciences, Cognitive Psychology and Neurocognition at the Communauté Université Grenoble Alpes. School of Psychology Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition Faculty of Social Sciences École Doctorale Ingénierie pour la Santé, la Cognition et l’Environnement University of Ottawa Communauté Université Grenoble Alpes ©Kylee Tamera Ack Baraly, Ottawa, Canada, 2020 K. T. ACK BARALY PH.D. DISSERTATION ii Acknowledgements To Patrick and Pascal, thank you for being two of the most incredibly supportive and knowledgeable supervisors. You helped enlighten my scientific mind, pushing me to question better, reflect deeper, and learn more. You are both inspiring as researchers and as mentors. And I am forever grateful to have gone through this journey with you. I would also like to thank all of the members of the Neuropsychology Lab (Ottawa) and Laboratoire de Psychologie et NeuroCognition (Chambéry and Grenoble). Thank you to all of the Master's students, honours thesis students, UROP students, lab volunteers, and research assistants who helped in more ways than I can count. Your enthusiasm and dedication to this work are truly appreciated. I am thankful for all the additional support provided by the INSPIRE Lab (Ottawa), which made collecting reliable data more efficient. A very special thank you to all of the research participants who so generously gave their time to this work. You are the pillar of scientific research and without you, none of this would have been possible.
    [Show full text]
  • Communication Science to the Public
    David M. Berube North Carolina State University ▪ HOW WE COMMUNICATE. In The Age of American Unreason, Jacoby posited that it trickled down from the top, fueled by faux-populist politicians striving to make themselves sound approachable rather than smart. (Jacoby, 2008). EX: The average length of a sound bite by a presidential candidate in 1968 was 42.3 seconds. Two decades later, it was 9.8 seconds. Today, it’s just a touch over seven seconds and well on its way to being supplanted by 140/280- character Twitter bursts. ▪ DATA FRAMING. ▪ When asked if they truly believe what scientists tell them, NEW ANTI- only 36 percent of respondents said yes. Just 12 percent expressed strong confidence in the press to accurately INTELLECTUALISM: report scientific findings. ▪ ROLE OF THE PUBLIC. A study by two Princeton University researchers, Martin TRENDS Gilens and Benjamin Page, released Fall 2014, tracked 1,800 U.S. policy changes between 1981 and 2002, and compared the outcome with the expressed preferences of median- income Americans, the affluent, business interests and powerful lobbies. They concluded that average citizens “have little or no independent influence” on policy in the U.S., while the rich and their hired mouthpieces routinely get their way. “The majority does not rule,” they wrote. ▪ Anti-intellectualism and suspicion (trends). ▪ Trump world – outsiders/insiders. ▪ Erasing/re-writing history – damnatio memoriae. ▪ False news. ▪ Infoxication (CC) and infobesity. ▪ Aggregators and managed reality. ▪ Affirmation and confirmation bias. ▪ Negotiating reality. ▪ New tribalism is mostly ideational not political. ▪ Unspoken – guns, birth control, sexual harassment, race… “The amount of technical information is doubling every two years.
    [Show full text]
  • Ilidigital Master Anton 2.Indd
    services are developed to be used by humans. Thus, understanding humans understanding Thus, humans. by used be to developed are services obvious than others but certainly not less complex. Most products bioengineering, and as shown in this magazine. Psychology mightbusiness world. beBe it more the comparison to relationships, game elements, or There are many non-business flieds which can betransfered to the COGNTIVE COGNTIVE is key to a succesfully develop a product orservice. is keytoasuccesfullydevelopproduct BIASES by ANTON KOGER The Power of Power The //PsychologistatILI.DIGITAL WE EDIT AND REINFORCE SOME WE DISCARD SPECIFICS TO WE REDUCE EVENTS AND LISTS WE STORE MEMORY DIFFERENTLY BASED WE NOTICE THINGS ALREADY PRIMED BIZARRE, FUNNY, OR VISUALLY WE NOTICE WHEN WE ARE DRAWN TO DETAILS THAT WE NOTICE FLAWS IN OTHERS WE FAVOR SIMPLE-LOOKING OPTIONS MEMORIES AFTER THE FACT FORM GENERALITIES TO THEIR KEY ELEMENTS ON HOW THEY WERE EXPERIENCED IN MEMORY OR REPEATED OFTEN STRIKING THINGS STICK OUT MORE SOMETHING HAS CHANGED CONFIRM OUR OWN EXISTING BELIEFS MORE EASILY THAN IN OURSELVES AND COMPLETE INFORMATION way we see situations but also the way we situationsbutalsotheway wesee way the biasesnotonlychange Furthermore, overload. cognitive avoid attention, ore situations, guide help todesign massively can This in. take people information of kind explainhowandwhat ofperception egory First,biasesinthecat andappraisal. ory, self,mem perception, into fourcategories: roughly bedivided Cognitive biasescan within thesesituations. forusers interaction andeasy in anatural situationswhichresults sible toimprove itpos and adaptingtothesebiasesmakes ingiven situations.Reacting ways certain act sively helpstounderstandwhypeople mas into consideration biases ing cognitive Tak humanbehavior. topredict likely less or andmore relevant illusionsare cognitive In each situation different every havior day.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • The Imagery Bizarreness Effect As a Function of Sentence Complexity and Presentation Time
    Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 1991. 29 (1). 25-27 The imagery bizarreness effect as a function of sentence complexity and presentation time SHERYL KLINE and LOWELL D. GRONINGER University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, Maryland Two experiments were carried out to examine the relationship between the bizarreness effect (more words recalled from bizarre images than from common images), sentence complexity, and presentation time. Experiment 1 showed that the bizarreness effect was not influenced by processing time when simple sentences were used. However, for complex sentences, an inter­ action occurred such that nouns from common sentences were recalled better with an ll-sec presen­ tation rate, and nouns from bizarre sentences were recalled better with a I5-sec presentation rate. This interaction was replicated in Experiment 2. These results show that the bizarreness effect does occur for complex sentences, provided that enough processing time is given. The results also suggest that reviews of the bizarreness literature need to consider processing time as a poten­ tially important variable. In 1972, Wollen, Weber, and Lowry showed that cued sitive to different presentation times. If common images recall for pictures of words (e.g., piano-cigar) was no can be formed more quickly than bizarre images, then better when the words were shown in a bizarre as opposed there should exist a presentation time such that a com­ to a common relationship. Research done since this time mon image relationship within a sentence could be has also shown that the formation of bizarre images of formed, but a bizarre image relationship could not be words produces no better recall of those words when formed.
    [Show full text]
  • Running Head: VALIDATING PHENOMENOLOGICAL ASPECTS 1
    Running Head: VALIDATING PHENOMENOLOGICAL ASPECTS 1 Validating Phenomenological Aspects of the Mental Imagery Experience through Meta- Analysis: Beyond Global Assessment of Imagery Ability Matthew S. Runge Carleton University 2012/2013 A THESIS PRESENTED TO THE DEPARTMENT OF NEUROSCIENCE IN PARTIAL FULLFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE M.Sc. DEGREE VALIDATING PHENOMENOLOGICAL ASPECTS 2 Abstract The phenomenological experience of mental imagery vividness is of increasing interest within the field of mental health science, yet commentators refute its empirical validity as an independent scientific construct. Two assessments of mental imagery vividness were compared through meta-analysis, the Vividness of Visual Imagery Questionnaire (VVIQ), and trial-by-trial ratings of vividness. Each vividness assessment was further divided into behavioural/cognitive or neuroscientific measures. A corpus of 965 peer-reviewed journal articles were retrieved from four major databases and relevant statistical outcomes from each paper were recorded. Effect size estimates were computed for 3579 statistical outcomes, which were categorized as into one of four comparison groups (Vividness and Behavioural/Cognitive, VVIQ and Behavioural/Cognitive, Vividness and Neuroscientific, and VVIQ and Neuroscientific). It was found that the average effect size magnitude for trial-by-trial vividness exceeded that of the VVIQ for behavioural/cognitive, but not neuroscientific measures. However, the average effect sizes magnitude for neuroscientific measures was generally greater than behavioural/cognitive ones. Additionally, the average effect sizes magnitude for trial-by-trial vividness ratings was generally greater than the VVIQ. It is suggested that trial-by-trial ratings, in conjunction with neuroscientific measurement, may provide a more precise and reliable measure of mental imagery vividness.
    [Show full text]
  • The Bizarreness Effect and Memory: Implications for Eyewitness Testimony
    The Bizarreness Effect and Memory: Implications for Eyewitness Testimony by Jennifer Wiseman A Thesis Submitted to the Faculty of The Wilkes Honors College in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Arts in Liberal Arts and Sciences with a Concentration in Psychology Wilkes Honors College of Florida Atlantic University Jupiter, Florida May 2008 The Bizarreness Effect and Memory: Implications for Eyewitness Testimony by Jennifer Wiseman This thesis was prepared under the direction of the candidate’s thesis advisor, Dr. Julie L. Earles, and has been approved by the members of her/his supervisory committee. It was submitted to the faculty of The Honors College and was accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts in Liberal Arts and Sciences. SUPERVISORY COMMITTEE: ____________________________ Dr. Julie L. Earles ____________________________ Dr. William O’Brien ______________________________ Dean, Wilkes Honors College ____________ Date ii ABSTRACT Author: Jennifer Wiseman Title: The bizarreness effect and memory: Implications for eyewitness testimony Institution: Wilkes Honors College of Florida Atlantic University Thesis Advisor: Dr. Julie L. Earles Degree: Bachelor of Arts in Liberal Arts and Sciences Concentration: Psychology Year: 2008 Mistakes in combining components of stimuli are called binding or memory conjunction errors. They occur when people mistakenly associate two previously seen stimulus features that were not previously seen together. It is hypothesized that bizarre items will be better remembered than common items. Participants saw 18 continuous events, each containing four actions performed by four different actors. One week later they returned for a recognition test and were shown more video clips.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases in Fitness to Practise Decision-Making
    ADVICE ON BIASES IN FITNESS TO PRACTISE DECISION-MAKING IN ACCEPTED OUTCOME VERSUS PANEL MODELS FOR THE PROFESSIONAL STANDARDS AUTHORITY Prepared by: LESLIE CUTHBERT Date: 31 March 2021 Contents I. Introduction ................................................................................................. 2 Background .................................................................................................. 2 Purpose of the advice.................................................................................... 3 Summary of conclusions ................................................................................ 4 Documents considered .................................................................................. 4 Technical terms and explanations ................................................................... 5 Disclaimer .................................................................................................... 5 II. How biases might affect the quality of decision-making in the AO model, as compared to the panel model .............................................................................. 6 Understanding cognitive biases in fitness to practise decision-making ............... 6 Individual Decision Maker (e.g. a Case Examiner) in AO model ......................... 8 Multiple Case Examiners .............................................................................. 17 Group Decision Maker in a Panel model of 3 or more members ....................... 22 III. Assessment of the impact of these biases,
    [Show full text]
  • Enactment and Retrieval
    Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (2), 233-243 doi:10.3758/MC.38.2.233 Enactment and retrieval DANIEL J. PETERSON AND NEIL W. M ULLIGAN University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill, North Carolina The enactment effect is one of a number of effects (e.g., bizarreness, generation, perceptual interference) that have been treated in common theoretical frameworks, most of them focusing on encoding processes. Recent results from McDaniel, Dornburg, and Guynn (2005) call into question whether bizarreness and, by associa- tion, related phenomena such as enactment are better conceptualized as arising due to retrieval processes. Four experiments investigated the degree to which retrieval processes are responsible for enhanced memory for enacted phrases. Participants were presented with two pure study lists and later recalled the lists separately (inducing pure retrieval sets) or recalled the lists together in a single test (inducing a combined or mixed re- trieval set). Across all four experiments, the combined recall condition consistently failed to enhance the size of the enactment effect. The results provide no support for the retrieval account but are generally consistent with encoding accounts. Although much of the research on human memory has (see also Mulligan & Hornstein, 2003). This reenactment focused on verbal materials, a significant portion of what effect was interpreted in terms of the encoding specificity people actually remember relates to what they have done. principle (e.g., Tulving & Thomson, 1973) and was taken Research on enactment, or action memory, is concerned as evidence that motor information was a critical part of with understanding how people recall both simple and the original memory trace for SPTs.
    [Show full text]