Morphologization in Turkish: Implications for Phonology in Grammaticalization1
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Morphologization in Turkish: Implications for Phonology in Grammaticalization1 René Schiering (University of Leipzig) 1. Introduction Grammaticalization theory assumes that the gradual progression from a content item to a grammatical marker is accompanied by a number of interdependent phonological, morphosyntactic, and functional processes. Accordingly, morphologization processes, such as cliticization and compounding, are said to be concomitant with phonological erosion and desemantization (Lehmann [1982] 1995, Heine & Reh 1984, Heine, Claudi & Hünnemeyer 1991, Hopper & Traugott 1993, Croft 2003). Some proponents of this theory even claim that the loss of autonomy and substance defines grammaticalization as opposed to other mechanisms in language change, for instance reanalysis (Haspelmath 1998). The role of phonology in grammaticalization has recently been reconsidered in the context of a cross-linguistic study on cliticization (Schiering 2006). A number of phonological processes, namely structure preservation, assimilation, weakening and strengthening, can accompany ongoing grammaticalization. The distribution of these phonological rules can be predicted by a rhythm-based typology of language which distinguishes between mora-, syllable- and stress- based languages. With respect to erosion, mora- and syllable-based languages tend to retain the phonological substance of cliticized elements which ultimately leads to disyllabic clitics and affixes. Stress-based languages, on the other hand, tend to reduce and delete 1 The research for this paper was carried out at the University of Leipzig as part of the project ‘Typology and Theories of the Word’, supported by the German Science Foundation (DFG). For comments and suggestions I am grateful to Balthasar Bickel, Ann Denwood, Geoff Haig, Barış Kabak. 2 the phonological substance of cliticized elements developing subminimal clitics and affixes in the course of morphologization. This paper will test the predictions made by the rhythm-based typology against diachronic data from Turkish. In Section 2, Turkish will be situated in the rhythm-based typology of language and the latter’s predictions concerning the phonology of grammaticalization will be summarized. In Section 3, two morphologization processes in the verbal domain, namely the cliticization of pronouns yielding agreement markers and the univerbation of verb stems resulting in new tense-aspect morphology, will be traced from Old Turkic to Modern Turkish. The phonological changes involved in these processes will be of focal interest. Finally, the actual diachronic data from Turkish will be compared with the predictions made by the rhythm-based typology and the major findings will be discussed in the context of grammaticalization theory. 2. Turkish in a Rhythm-based Typology of Language The conception of linguistic rhythm which will be adhered to in this study is based on the assumption that different phonological properties tend to cluster in the prototypes of mora-, syllable- and stress-based rhythm (Auer 2001, Dufter 2003, Schiering 2006). In what follows, we will restrict our attention to the phonetic correlates of stress, the segmental effects of stress, the degree of syllable complexity, the distribution of length contrasts and the domain for vowel harmony and their respective distribution over the rhythmic prototypes. Stress may be realized phonetically by pitch only or by a combination of pitch, duration and intensity (Beckman 1986). Phonetically weak stress is prototypical for mora- or syllable-based rhythm, whereas stress-based rhythm is characterized by phonetically strong stress. In Turkish, the realization of stress relies on pitch movement and to a certain extent on intensity.2 With respect to this parameter, Turkish behaves like a mora- or syllable-based language. 2 All information and data on Old Turkic and Modern Turkish have been taken from the following reference grammars: Gabain (1950), Erdal (2004), Kornfilt (1997) and Lewis (2000). 3 Stress-based languages further exhibit strong segmental effects of stress, i.e. vowel reduction and consonant weakening in unstressed syllables and vowel lengthening and consonant strengthening in stressed syllables (Bybee et al. 1998). Although there are some hints at word-medial vowel alternation and deletion, e.g. aγïz > aγzan ‘being said’ (Old Turkic) and burada > burda ‘here’ (Modern Turkish), Turkish lacks such segmental effects of stress and behaves like other mora- or syllable-based languages with respect to this parameter. Another phonological property which distinguishes stress-based from mora- and syllable-based languages is the degree of syllable complexity (Blevins 1995). Languages of the former class show high degrees of syllable complexity, whereas languages of the other types have simple or moderate syllable structure. Since Turkish allows only six syllable types, it belongs to the latter group of languages, cf. o ‘he/she/it’, at ‘horse’, bu ‘this’, sol ‘left’, ilk ‘beginning’, kırk ‘forty’. Mora- and syllable-based languages behave alike with respect to the phonological parameters discussed in the preceding paragraphs. The distribution of length contrasts in vowels and consonants, however, helps to distinguish the two rhythmic types. Mora-based rhythm is characterized by length contrasts with are distributed irrespective of stress placement. Although phonemic vowel length can be reconstructed for the proto-language, Turkish exhibits vowel length only in loans, beraber ‘together’. Long vowels surface phonetically after contractions, e.g. değil /deyil/ > [di:l] ‘not’, and in expressive lengthening, e.g. asla > aslaaa ‘never!’. Geminates occur only underlyingly in loans such as hak ‘the right, justice’ and word- medially at morpheme boundaries of complex words, such as bat-tı ‘it sank’. Accordingly, Turkish cannot be considered a mora-based language. Although the rhythm-based typology makes reliable predictions with respect to the prosodic and phonotactic parameters discussed above, the distribution of morphophonological rules, such as cluster simplification and coalescence, turns out to be erratic over the rhythmic prototypes. Vowel harmony, however, is restricted to disyllabic domains in stress-based languages and applies over word domains in languages of other rhythm classes. In Turkish, palatal and 4 labial assimilation are not restricted to smaller domains, but span the word domain, e.g. anla-yacak ‘s/he will understand’ and göz-lük-çü ‘optician, oculist’. The distribution of the various phonological properties over the rhythm prototypes is illustrated in Figure 1. Note that Turkish behaves like a typical representative of syllable-based rhythm. In fact, the rhythmic profile of Turkish has not changed in the course of the development from Old Turkic to Modern Turkish. Figure 1: Selected phonological properties of rhythmic prototypes Mora-based Syllable-based Stress-based Accent none/weak none/weak strong Stress effect none/weak none/weak strong Syllable types simple moderate complex Length contrasts unrestricted restricted restricted Vowel harmony word word disyllabic The rhythm-based typology of language makes a number of predictions with respect to possible phonological effects of grammaticalization. Within stress prosodies, such as Turkish, the model predicts gradual stress reduction, i.e. from primary stress to secondary stress to unstressed, and gradual integration into the word domain for stress placement, i.e. from unstressable to stressable (see Selkirk 1995 for a formal analysis). Whereas this stress reduction goes hand in hand with segmental reduction in stress-based rhythm, languages of the other rhythm classes have no reductive potential in their phonologies and retain the grammaticalized element. Accordingly, we would not expect erosion to accompany grammaticalization in a syllable-based language like Turkish. Since vowel harmony processes are sensitive to the word domain in languages of this rhythmic type, gradual integration into the word domain should also manifest itself in the inclusion of the grammaticalized element into the vowel harmony domain. Since morphophonological rules operating at the morpheme boundary occur in languages of all rhythmic classes, junctural processes such as cluster simplification and coalescence are possible and provide the 5 only context in which segments can be lost in the course of prosodic integration. 3. Morphologization in Turkish To test the predictions made by the typology outlined in the previous section, we will examine two cases of grammaticalization in Turkish, namely the cliticization of personal pronouns which led to subject agreement marking (Givón 1976) and the univerbation of verbal complexes which led to new aspect-tense markers (Lehmann [1982] 1995). In Old Turkic, sentences with pronominal subjects were formed with a postponed pronoun at the end of the non-verbal or verbal predicate. For the sake of emphasis or contrast, another personal pronoun could be placed in preverbal subject position. In thirteenth century texts, the postponed pronouns appear cliticized to the preceding word. In Modern Turkish, the cliticized subject pronouns form the back bone of the z-paradigm of subject agreement marking (cf. Adamović 1985, Kornfilt 1996, Good & Yu 2005). The various stages in this diachronic development are exemplified for the first person singular in the examples (1)-(3). (1) (ben) kelür ben ‘I am coming’ (2) (ben) gelür-ven ~ (ben) gelür-em ‘I am coming’ (3) (ben) gelír-im ‘I come’ Although