Feeling Numbers: the Rhetoric of Pathos in Visualization

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Feeling Numbers: the Rhetoric of Pathos in Visualization Feeling Numbers The Rhetoric of Pathos in Visualization SARAH CAMPBELL Feeling Numbers The Rhetoric of Pathos in Visualization Thesis presented by Sarah Campbell To The Department of Art + Design In Partial Fulfillment of The Requirements for The Degree of Master of Fine Arts in Information Design + Visualization Advisor: Dietmar Offenhuber Reader: Alberto Cairo Respondent: Pedro Cruz Chair: Nathan Felde Northeastern University Boston, Massachusetts May, 2018 Acknowledgements To my mom and dad, for their never-ending support. To Alex, for being my rock through this journey. To my cohort, for the critiques, laughs, shared pain, and inspiration. To Phelps, for being such an entertaining turtle. Abstract Rhetoric is a powerful tool used to influence and persuade. Due to their inherent subjectivity, data visualizations are a form of communication that employ persuasion techniques. Therefore, the rhetoric of visualizations deserves deeper investigation. Drawing from Aristotle’s three modes of persuasion, this thesis explores the rhetoric of pathos, or appeals to emotion, within data visualizations. In this thesis, I develop a taxonomy of pathos techniques applied to visualizations and empirically measure the emotional effect of pathos techniques that relate to data proximity. This research improves the visualization community’s understanding on how certain design decisions can add meaning and relevance to data. Contents 11 17 37 Introduction Rhetoric and Techniques for Emotional Appeals Appealing to Emotion: in Visualization A Taxonomy Rhetoric Engage Emotional Appeals Humanize in Visualization Personalize 49 73 77/83 Evaluating the Conclusion References Emotional Effect of Proximity Techniques Appendix The Value Proximity Emotion Study Methodology Analysis & Results Discussion Introduction Data and Emotions. These two words are an unlikely pair. Data are typically described as rational, objective, cold, black and white. Emotions, on the other hand, are subjective, colorful, and expressive. It may seem like the two do not have a common meeting place, but they potentially do: in the visual representation of the data. Today data are ubiquitous, so it is not surprising that visualizations of data are increasing in ubiquity as well. With that, visualizations have broadened their scope to act as a communication tool to convey powerful messages. Data journalism has popularized visualizing information for the general audience. Newspapers like The New York Times, The Guardian, and The Wall Street Journal employ their own graphics teams that visually convey information on issues we care about: politics, the environment, health care, education, and so on. Advocacy groups are also taking advantage of increased accessibility of data, harnessing the power of information and technology for activism by appealing to audiences rationally, ethically, or emotionally (Tactical Technology Collective 2014). Appeals to logic, ethics, and emotion lie at the foundation of Aristotle’s Figure 1.1 system of rhetoric (2004). The existence of persuasion in visual Items: Is Fashion Modern? A representations of data is not consensually accepted in the visualization fashion exhibition visualized community. When persuasion is discussed, it is typically discussed by Accurat for MoMa through negatively connoted terminology such as bias (Hullman and (Bassan et al. 2017). 12 INTRODUCTION Diakopoulos 2011) or deception (Pandey et al. 2015). The study of rhetoric in this way has tremendous value for developing a visual literacy to recognize forms of deception, but I argue that rhetoric has other facets to consider with respect to data visualization. If we look at persuasion as an umbrella term for influence, whether it is to influence one’s beliefs, attitudes, or behaviors, the concept is not completely negative. However, efforts to influence by visualizing data have been traditionally looked down upon due to visualizations being considered as objective, neutral representations of the data. This viewpoint is fueled by the scientific, analytical lens that visualization has typically been looked at through. Additionally, influence in visualizations is typically focused on in terms of unethical design decisions that lead to deceptive representations of the data, where the message of the data does not match the message of the visualization. In an effort to minimize deceptive uses of data, the visualization community has emphasized data-centric designs, where clarity of the data is stressed. Therefore, any visual elements that do not aid in maximizing the data shown are considered unnecessary. However, in several ways, this has led to false thinking that there is an objective way to visualize data, when indeed there is not (Dörk et al. 2013; Hullman and Diakopoulos 2011; Viegas and Wattenberg 2007). If this were true, designers given the same data would visualize it in the exact same way, making identical design decisions. In reality, one dataset can be visualized in many different ways. By merely going through the process of visualizing data, the designer is creating a persuasive message, since the act indicates that the designer believes the data can be influential in some way. In this light, it is far more ethical to acknowledge the subjective nature visualizations have instead of masking it under the ideals of neutrality. Visualization practitioners first need to acknowledge the subjective, persuasive nature of visualizations and look at the practice as a form of communication. By better understanding the subjective and influential aspects of a visualization, we can better scrutinize design decisions made that deviate from the message of the data. We can go a step beyond that and embrace the subjective nature of visualizations. In doing so, we can craft visualizations that are more human-centric for our audience: people. A closer look at persuasive efforts in this way focuses on pathos: the appeal to emotions of the audience. In Aristotle’s three established appeals, pathos is the mode that is the least agreed upon in the visualization community. It is understandable how an accord was found in the use of visualizations to speak to reason and ethics. But to admit, or even celebrate, the potential for visualizations to bring an emotional capacity to data? This is a concept the community as a whole has only recently begun to talk about. With the growth of using visualizations INTRODUCTION 13 as a form of communication about important issues relevant to humanity, designers are seeking ways to add the human element to the data and impact how the audience feels about the information the visualization conveys. Through recent literature, conference talks, blog posts, and podcasts, practitioners are engaging in a conversation about emotions in visualizations. Some even offer techniques that may appeal to the emotions of the audience. However, little research has evaluated the emotive impact these techniques have in visualizations. Do they evoke emotion at all? If so, how powerful are they? This thesis builds upon existing discourse and research on the role of pathos in visualizations. Through collecting and categorizing recommended techniques, I propose a working taxonomy of techniques that are said to appeal to emotions. More importantly, I present an empirical evaluation of three specific techniques: proximity of interests, temporal proximity, and spatial proximity. These techniques rise from the concept that we care more about people and events that are near us in time, space, and importance (Campbell 1776). Through this evaluation, I begin to identify the emotive impact these techniques have when implemented in visualizations. The goal of this work is to further the research on how visualizing data can appeal to emotions. By improving the community’s understanding of emotional appeals, we improve our understanding on the communicative power of visualizations as a whole and, therefore, enhance our ability to visualize data to connect with the people who explore it. 14 INTRODUCTION INTRODUCTION 15 If I look at the mass I will never act. If I look at the one, I will. Mother Teresa Rhetoric and Emotional Appeals in Visualization This chapter serves to highlight related work around rhetoric and emotional appeals in visualization. While rhetoric has established theories throughout history, the visualization community as a whole has yet to cozy up to the idea of visualization as rhetorical communication. However, visualization literature points to the subjective and rhetorical nature of visualizations. When it comes to appealing to emotions, psychology research validates the struggle to affect people with numbers. Data visualization practitioners acknowledge this difficulty and, at the same time, many see value in arousing the audience’s emotions through visualizing data. A few practitioners identify techniques that are suspected to appeal to emotions in visualization, but very little research evaluates the emotional impact of these techniques. RHETORIC Aristotle is credited with developing the foundational system of rhetoric and Figure 2.1 defines it as the study of means of persuasion (2004). Within this system are Off the Staff. a series three modes of persuasion. The first is logos, which appeals to reason. Since of scores visualized by rhetorical efforts were primarily delivered in the form of speeches in his time, Nicholas Rougeux for Aristotle refers to logos as the substance of the speech itself “when we have the OpenScore initiative proved a truth or an apparent truth”
Recommended publications
  • Moral Framing and Charitable Donation: Integrating Exploratory Social Media Analyses and Confirmatory Experimentation
    Hoover, J., et al. (2018). Moral Framing and Charitable Donation: Integrating Exploratory Social Media Analyses and Confirmatory Experimentation. Collabra: Psychology, 4(1): 9. DOI: https://doi.org/10.1525/collabra.129 ORIGINAL RESEARCH REPORT Moral Framing and Charitable Donation: Integrating Exploratory Social Media Analyses and Confirmatory Experimentation Joe Hoover*, Kate Johnson*, Reihane Boghrati†, Jesse Graham* and Morteza Dehghani‡ Do appeals to moral values promote charitable donation during natural disasters? Using Distributed Dictionary Representation, we analyze tweets posted during Hurricane Sandy to explore associations between moral values and charitable donation sentiment. We then derive hypotheses from the observed associations and test these hypotheses across a series of preregistered experiments that investigate the effects of moral framing on perceived donation motivation (Studies 2 & 3), hypothetical donation (Study 4), and real donation behavior (Study 5). Overall, we find consistent positive associations between moral care and loyalty framing with donation sentiment and donation motivation. However, in contrast with people’s perceptions, we also find that moral frames may not actually have reliable effects on charitable donation, as measured by hypothetical indications of donation and real donation behavior. Overall, this work demonstrates that theoretically constrained, exploratory social media analyses can be used to generate viable hypotheses, but also that such approaches should be paired with rigorous controlled experiments. Keywords: Moral Psychology; Charitable Donation; Natural Language Processing; Social Media The 900-mile-wide Hurricane Sandy hit the Atlantic Coast charitable donation in public discourse by modeling the of the United States on October 29th, 2012 with record- language used to frame charitable donation in the wake of breaking rainfall and 80-mile-per-hour winds.
    [Show full text]
  • Ezra M. Markowitz [email protected] |
    Ezra M. Markowitz [email protected] | www.ezramarkowitz.com Assistant Professor of Environmental Decision-Making Department of Environmental Conservation 303 Holdsworth Hall University of Massachusetts Amherst Amherst, MA 01003 Education 2012 Ph.D. Environmental Sciences, Studies & Policy. University of Oregon. Dissertation: Affective and moral roots of environmental stewardship: The role of obligation, gratitude and compassion Committee: Sara Hodges, Azim Shariff, Paul Slovic, Ron Mitchell, Kari Norgaard 2008 M.S. Psychology. University of Oregon. Thesis: Did you just see that? Making sense of environmentally relevant behavior Advisor: Bertram Malle 2007 B.A. Psychology. Vassar College. Departmental and general honors. Advisors: Randy Cornelius, Susan Trumbetta Appointments 2014- Assistant professor. University of Massachusetts Amherst. 2013-2014 Postdoctoral research fellow. Earth Institute. Columbia University. 2013-2014 Visiting postdoctoral research associate. PIIRS. Princeton University. 2013-2014 Fellow. FrameWorks Institute. Washington, DC. 2012-2013 Postdoctoral research associate. PIIRS. Princeton University. 2011-2012 Scholar-in-residence. School of Communication. American University. Professional and Teaching Experience 2012 Consultant. FrameWorks Institute. Washington, DC. 2012 Consultant. ecoAmerica. Washington, DC. 2010-2012 Gallup research scholar. Gallup. Princeton, NJ. 1 2010-2011 Director. Campus Conservation Corps. University of Oregon. 2010 Instructor. Psychology of Climate Change, Univ. of Oregon. 2009-2010 Graduate teaching fellow. Intro to Env. Studies; Statistics. Univ. of Oregon. 2008-2009 Graduate intern. Institute for a Sustainable Environment. Eugene, OR. 2008-2013 Senior staff. PolicyInteractive. Eugene, OR. Awards, Grants, Honors, Scholarships 2013 DISCCRS VIII Symposium (invited, attended). DISCCRS.org. 2012 Outstanding student paper award. Amer. Psych. Assoc. Divisions 9 & 34. 2011 Best paper award. 9th Biennial Conference on Environmental Psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Impossible Grief:· Motivation and Intention in 1Macheth
    IMPOSSIBLE GRIEF:· MOTIVATION AND INTENTION IN 1MACHETH KER WELLS A THESIS SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF GRADUATE STUDIES IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE RE~UIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF MASTER OF FINE ARTS GRADUATE PROGRAM IN THEATRE YORK UNIVERSITY, TORONTO, ONTARIO SEPTEMBER 2013 ©KER WELLS, 2013 ii Abstract This paper considers the issues of identification and motivation in Shakespeare's Macbeth. Proceeding from the proposal that the play allows for a remarkable level of audience identification for the characters of Macbeth and Lady Macbeth, considering their heinous acts, reasons for this level of identification are explored. Two events referred to in the text of Macbeth, but preceding the time of the play, are identified as potentially significant motivating factors in the couple's actions: Macbeth's recent experience of intense battle, and the couple's apparent past loss of a child. Supporting textual evidence is presented in addition to third party research into the effects of close combat and Post-Traumatic Stress Disorder, and the psychological effects on parents of losing a child. Various staging and design implications, opportunities, and ideas that result from these proposals are explored with direct reference to the author's production of Macbeth in Toronto's High Park. iii Dedicated to Marilyn Wells, to L.L.M, and to all the others who have taught me. iv TABLE OF CONTENTS Abstract ................................................................................. ii Introduction ..........................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Pathos & Ethos: Emotions and Willingness to Pay for Tobacco
    Francesco Bogliacino, Cristiano Codagnone, Giuseppe Alessandro Veltri, Amitav Chakravarti, Pietro Ortoleva, George Gaskell, Andriy Ivchenko, Francisco Lupiáñez- Villanueva, Francesco Mureddu, Caroline Rudisill Pathos & ethos: emotions and willingness to pay for tobacco products Article (Published version) (Refereed) Original citation: Bogliacino, Francesco, Codagnone, Cristiano, Veltri, Giuseppe Alessandro, Chakravarti, Amitav, Ortoleva, Pietro, Gaskell, George, Ivchenko, Andriy, Lupiáñez-Villanueva, Francisco, Mureddu, Francesco and Rudisill, Caroline (2015) Pathos & ethos: emotions and willingness to pay for tobacco products. PLOS One, 10 (10). e0139542. ISSN 1932-6203 DOI: 10.1371/journal.pone.0139542 Reuse of this item is permitted through licensing under the Creative Commons: © 2015 The Authors CC-BY This version available at: http://eprints.lse.ac.uk/64127/ Available in LSE Research Online: November 2015 LSE has developed LSE Research Online so that users may access research output of the School. Copyright © and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. You may freely distribute the URL (http://eprints.lse.ac.uk) of the LSE Research Online website. RESEARCH ARTICLE Pathos & Ethos: Emotions and Willingness to Pay for Tobacco Products Francesco Bogliacino1☯*, Cristiano Codagnone2☯, Giuseppe Alessandro Veltri3☯, Amitav Chakravarti4,5‡, Pietro Ortoleva6‡, George Gaskell4, Andriy Ivchenko7, Francisco Lupiáñez-Villanueva8, Francesco Mureddu8,9, Caroline Rudisill4 1 Universidad
    [Show full text]
  • Rhetoric and Informal Logic
    UNIVERSITY OF WINDSOR Department of Philosophy Philosophy 561 Rhetoric and Informal Logic Chris Tindale Fall 2015 Chrysler Hall North 2183 M : 7:00-9:50 [email protected] CHN 2193 Office Hours: Wednesday Afternoon. Course description: The relationship between rhetoric and logic is fraught with tension, in spite of their once companionable places (along with grammar) in the trivium of the mediaeval university. Rhetoric is often dismissed as mere adornment to the substance of logic. In the early stages of the revolution in logic that gave birth to informal logic a similar disregard of rhetoric persisted. But that attitude has more recently been revised as informal logicians have entered into fruitful dialogues with contemporary rhetoricians. Still, there is a sense that rhetorical concerns can remain an afterthought for the logician, and the serious question needs still to be asked about the role that rhetoric can and should play in the development of informal modes of reasoning. In this course, we will ask that question, exploring the nature of ancient and contemporary rhetoric, the emergence of informal logic, and the ways in which the interests of the two can be seen to converge. Course Structure: The course will be conducted as a seminar/discussion. Members of the class will be expected to have read the material each week and be prepared to engage in discussion. Required Texts: All readings are listed week by week in the syllabus, and will be made available in the department, online, or through the reserve desk at the library. Requirements: 1. Two short papers responding to specific questions that arise in the first half of the term, worth 20% in each case, for a total of 40%.
    [Show full text]
  • Empathy Through Data Loneliness Through the Lens of Data Visualization
    Empathy Through Data Loneliness through the lens of data visualization By Sananda Dutta A thesis presented to OCAD University in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Design in Digital Futures. Toronto, Ontario, Canada, 2021. Empathy Through Data Sananda Dutta Creative Commons Copyright Notice Copyright Notice This document is licensed under the Creative Commons CC BY 4.0 2.5 Canada License. https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/legalcode You are free to: Share - copy and redistribute the material in any medium or format Under the following conditions: Attribution - You must give appropriate credit, provide a link to the license, and indicate if changes were made. You may do so in any reasonable manner, but not in any way that suggests the licensor endorses you or your use. No additional restrictions - You may not apply legal terms or technological measures that legally restrict others from doing anything the license permits. 1 Empathy Through Data Sananda Dutta ABSTRACT This research examines strategies aimed at fostering empathy through data visualizations. Loneliness experienced during the COVID-19 pandemic (during May, July and September 2020) is used as a case study to explore alternate ways of representing data. Along with ways to humanize data representation and curb Statistical numbing, this research uses metaphors to encode sensitive data to help visually represent people suffering loneliness in Ontario during the first wave of the COVID-19 pandemic. The research amalgamates ‘affect theory’ with concepts of ‘arithmetic of emotions’ and ‘compassion fade’ to try and create a solution for issues related to the ways in which we respond to sensitive issues.
    [Show full text]
  • This Is Your Brain on Rhetoric”: Research Directions for Neurorhetorics Jordynn Jacka; L
    This article was downloaded by: [Appelbaum, L. Gregory] On: 17 November 2010 Access details: Access Details: [subscription number 929642599] Publisher Routledge Informa Ltd Registered in England and Wales Registered Number: 1072954 Registered office: Mortimer House, 37- 41 Mortimer Street, London W1T 3JH, UK Rhetoric Society Quarterly Publication details, including instructions for authors and subscription information: http://www.informaworld.com/smpp/title~content=t716100769 “This is Your Brain on Rhetoric”: Research Directions for Neurorhetorics Jordynn Jacka; L. Gregory Appelbaumb a Department of English, University of North Carolina, b Center for Cognitive, Neuroscience at Duke University, Online publication date: 15 November 2010 To cite this Article Jack, Jordynn and Appelbaum, L. Gregory(2010) '“This is Your Brain on Rhetoric”: Research Directions for Neurorhetorics', Rhetoric Society Quarterly, 40: 5, 411 — 437 To link to this Article: DOI: 10.1080/02773945.2010.516303 URL: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/02773945.2010.516303 PLEASE SCROLL DOWN FOR ARTICLE Full terms and conditions of use: http://www.informaworld.com/terms-and-conditions-of-access.pdf This article may be used for research, teaching and private study purposes. Any substantial or systematic reproduction, re-distribution, re-selling, loan or sub-licensing, systematic supply or distribution in any form to anyone is expressly forbidden. The publisher does not give any warranty express or implied or make any representation that the contents will be complete or accurate or up to date. The accuracy of any instructions, formulae and drug doses should be independently verified with primary sources. The publisher shall not be liable for any loss, actions, claims, proceedings, demand or costs or damages whatsoever or howsoever caused arising directly or indirectly in connection with or arising out of the use of this material.
    [Show full text]
  • Suffering, Pity and Friendship: an Aristotelian Reading of Book 24 of Homer’S Iliad Marjolein Oele University of San Francisco, [email protected]
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by University of San Francisco The University of San Francisco USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library | Geschke Center Philosophy College of Arts and Sciences 11-2010 Suffering, Pity and Friendship: An Aristotelian Reading of Book 24 of Homer’s Iliad Marjolein Oele University of San Francisco, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://repository.usfca.edu/phil Part of the Classics Commons, and the Philosophy Commons Recommended Citation Oele, Marjolein, "Suffering, Pity and Friendship: An Aristotelian Reading of Book 24 of Homer’s Iliad" (2010). Philosophy. Paper 17. http://repository.usfca.edu/phil/17 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Arts and Sciences at USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library | Geschke Center. It has been accepted for inclusion in Philosophy by an authorized administrator of USF Scholarship: a digital repository @ Gleeson Library | Geschke Center. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Suffering, Pity and Friendship: An Aristotelian Reading of Book 24 of Homer’s Iliad Marjolein Oele University of San Francisco [email protected] Book 24 of Homer’s Iliad presents us with one of the most beautiful and chilling scenes of the epic: the scene where Achilles and Priam directly face one another at the point when the suffering (pathos) of each seems to have reached its pinnacle. Achilles’ suffering is centered on the loss of his best friend Patroclus, while the suffering of Priam – although long in the making due to the attack on his city and his family – has reached a new level of despair with the loss of his dearest son Hector.
    [Show full text]
  • Self-Other Similarity and Its Effects on Insensitivity to Mass Suffering
    Self-­‐Other Similarity and Its Effects on Insensitivity to Mass Suffering Ka Ho Tam Thesis for Honors Research Project in Psychology Northeastern University April 2016 Tam 2 Abstract Past studies show that our compassion toward suffering others when decreases we are exposed to numerous suffering individuals as opposed to a single sufferer. This phenomenon is called the “collapse of compassion”. The present research examines the effects of self-­‐other similarity on our insensitivity to suffering. mass Based on existing evidence that self-­‐other similarity can promote compassion, we e xpected the collapse of compassion to disappear diminish or when similarity is experimentally induced. In a single experiment involving 242 undergraduate participants , we manipulated the level of self-­‐other similarity priming by participants with Shared Human xperiences E (SHE) that highlighted commonalities between participants the and children in Ethiopia. Participants in the control condition were primed with cultural-­‐specific experiences that were unique to the Ethiopian population. We then presented participants with a description a of poverty-­‐stricken village in Ethiopia, followed by images of either one or eight suffering children from that village. Results of the experiment supported the opposite orig of our inal hypothesis. Participants in the SHE exhibited condition the collapse of compassion, whereas participants in the reported control condition sig nificantly more compassion for a group of suffering children than for a single child. Our research demonstrates that although self-­‐other similarity can promote compassion in a single victim context, this effect is reversed when multiple victims present, are leading to the collapse of compassion. Future studies further may explore the factors that mediate the relationship between self -­‐other similarity and the collapse of compassion.
    [Show full text]
  • Philosophies of Happiness Appendix 4: Aristotle: Pleasure Without
    Philosophies of Happiness Appendix 4: Aristotle: Pleasure Without Sensation In Book 3.10, Aristotle attributes pleasure to thinking (dianoia); he writes that though the lover of honor or learning enjoys or takes pleasure (khairei) in the thing he loves, his body is not affected at all, only his thought.i As contemporary Aristotelian scholar J.O. Urmson notes, “this remark puzzled the commentator Aspasius to the point of exasperation: ‘What does he mean,’ he protests, ‘when he says that the enjoyment of lovers of learning or honour involves a condition of the intelligence? For enjoyment and the pleasures are not in the intelligence but in the affective (pathetiko) part of the soul.’” Urmson then comments: “Aspasius never understands Aristotle's view that enjoyment of learning is exhibited in the effortless concentration of the intelligence on its problems rather than in getting some feeling as a result or concomitant of one's study.”ii In other words, Aristotle held a “modal” rather than a “sensate” view of pleasure; pleasure is a mode of engaging in activity, rather than a feeling experienced as a result of activity. When the activity is a sensory one, Aristotle regards pleasure as the enjoyment of sensation, rather than as itself a sensation.iii Thus, although in 1.8, he does say that pleasure is among the things “of the soul,”iv the passage in 3.10 suggests that a thinking being without soul can take pleasure in whatever way it can think without an organ of thought, through understanding (nous) alone. Pleasure does not require sensation; we may take pleasure in a physical sensation, but Aristotle does not seem to regard physical sensation itself as pleasure.
    [Show full text]
  • Changing the Subject: a Theory of Rhetoricalempathy
    Community Literacy Journal Volume 14 Issue 2 Spring Article 17 Spring 2020 Changing the Subject: A Theory of RhetoricalEmpathy Anita Voorhees University of Tennessee Knoxville Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/communityliteracy Recommended Citation Voorhees, Anita. “Changing the Subject: A Theory of RhetoricalEmpathy.” Community Literacy Journal, vol. 14, no. 2, 2020. pp.193 -196. doi:10.25148/14.2.009049. This work is brought to you for free and open access by FIU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Community Literacy Journal by an authorized administrator of FIU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. spring 2020 Changing the Subject: A Theory of Rhetorical Empathy Lisa Blankenship Utah State University Press, 2019, pp. 159 Reviewed by Anita Voorhees The University of Tennessee Knoxville n Changing the Subject: A Theory of Rhe- torical Empathy, Lisa Blankenship poses a new theory for interacting ethically with Iother human beings by underscoring the role pathos and empathy hold in understanding differences. She explains how rhetorical empa- thy helps us connect with one another. Blan- kenship, citing Krista Ratcliffe’s Rhetorical Lis- tening, continues the important movement in rhetoric and composition toward storytelling and listening as a means of understanding. She emphasizes the current polarization happening in the United States as her exigence for writing. The basis for this project is the notion that “pathos . is one of the most power- ful forms of persuasion and change” (5). As Blankenship writes, “[m]y purpose is to frame pathos in new ways and make a case for rhetorical empathy as a means of ethi- cal rhetorical engagement” (5).
    [Show full text]
  • ARISTOTLE's Introduction to Rhetoric and the Art of PERSUASION
    ARISTOTLE’s introduction to Rhetoric and the art of PERSUASION. Early in my studies, I recognized that there are three main types of argument that people use when trying to persuade others. I called them . ETHOS, PATHOS, and LOGOS. The first is ETHOS. Consider this. Before you can convince an audience to accept anything you say, they have to accept you as credible. There are many aspects to building your credibility: Do people respect you? Do people believe you are of good character? Do people believe you are generally trustworthy? Do people believe you are an authority on this speech topic? Keep in mind that it is not enough for you to know that you are a credible source. This is not about your confidence, experience, or expertise. Others must also know this. Ethos is affected by your level of credibility, your reputation, as perceived by others. Keep in mind that . WE ARE WHAT WE REPEATEDLY DO. Excellence, then, is not an ACT but a HABIT. ETHOS. -> similar English words: ETHICAL; ETHICS -> quotes with authority -> quotes from authority -> takes advantage of reputation and status The second is PATHOS. Pathos is the quality of a persuasive presentation which appeals to the EMOTIONS of the audience. Consider these qualities in your words and actions. Do your words evoke feelings of love? sympathy? fear? Do your words evoke feelings of compassion? envy? Does your characterization of the competition evoke feelings of hate? contempt? An emotional connection can be created in many ways by a speaker, perhaps most notably by stories. The goal of a story, anecdote, analogy, simile, and metaphor is often to link an aspect of our primary message with a triggered emotional response from the audience.
    [Show full text]