Lingual Articulation of the Sūzhōu Chinese Labial Fricative Vowels

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Lingual Articulation of the Sūzhōu Chinese Labial Fricative Vowels LINGUAL ARTICULATION OF THE SŪZHŌU CHINESE LABIAL FRICATIVE VOWELS Matthew Faytak1, Jennifer Kuo1, and Shunjie Wang1 1University of California, Los Angeles [email protected], [email protected] ABSTRACT Figure 1: Frontal lip positions at vowel midpoint for the three vowels at issue for S44. In this paper, we describe lingual activity for three back vowels with different labial constrictions in [u] [ɯβ] [ɯv] Suzhou Chinese: a rounded high back vowel [u] and two more unusual vowels, vertically compressed [ɯβ] and labiodental [ɯv]. The latter two vowels are so-called fricative vowels, consistently produced with slight fricative noise; [ɯβ] also sporadically ex- hibits bilabial trilling. Smoothing-spline ANOVA sion in a vowel with a similar but distinct acous- models of tongue surface contours extracted from ul- tic quality that we transcribe as [ɯβ]; and fricative- trasound recordings suggest that [ɯβ] and [ɯv] have like labiodental constriction in a vowel we transcribe a tongue position lower and fronter than [u]. Linear as [ɯv]. Typical lip positions for each vowel are mixed effects modeling of contour shape parameters shown in Figure 1. The latter two vowels can be also suggests that [u] has a less flat and more com- said to continue the constriction type of the conso- plex, back-raised tongue shape than [ɯβ] and [ɯv]. nants that obligatorily precede them: [ɯβ] occurs We relate the lowered tongue body of [ɯβ] and [ɯv] only after bilabial stops /p, ph, b/, and [ɯv] occurs to the trading relations between lingual and labial ac- only after labiodental fricatives /f, v/ [25, 16]. On tivity that characterize rounded vowels, as well as to distributional grounds, [u] and [ɯβ] are in fact con- aeroacoustic properties of labial fricatives and bil- trastive (though [u] and [ɯv] are allophonic): al- abial trills. though [ɯβ] is restricted in its occurrence, [u] may Keywords: Ultrasound, Wu Chinese, trading rela- occur in the same environment, and minimal pairs tion, fricative vowels are easily found (see Table 1). The two vowels [ɯβ] and [ɯv] have been called 1. INTRODUCTION syllabic labial fricatives or labial fricative vowels, in part due to the acoustic consequences of their The labial component of vowel articulation is known labial constrictions [25, 27, 16]. For [ɯv], labioden- to involve several different types of constriction, in- tal frication from the preceding [f] or [v] onset con- cluding in-rounding, out-rounding, and vertical bil- tinues through the entire vowel. Any fricative noise abial compression [15, 17]. While usage of differ- present in [ɯβ] is more spectrally diffuse and sub- ent labial constriction types most often systemati- tle, but it is also sporadically produced with bilabial cally varies with the backness of the vowel—front trilling. The aerodynamic conditions for trilling do rounded vowels tending to be out-rounded and back not seem to be met in all tokens of [ɯβ]; when this rounded vowels tending to be in-rounded—different is the case, the resulting vowel quality is not easily behaviors can be found cross-linguistically [17, 26]. distinguished from [u], but the position of the lips is A distinction between out-rounding and compres- still visually distinct from [u]. Vowels with similar sion also minimally contrasts vowel categories in a constrictions to these have been attested in a handful handful of languages, notably in a pair of Swedish of languages of southwestern China [7, 3, 4, 9] and high vowels ranging from central to front [13, 22] Cameroon [10, 20]. and in a pair of back vowels in Shanghainese [2]. While the labial articulations of [ɯβ] and [ɯv] Like Shanghainese, Sūzhōu Chinese exhibits a have been remarked upon in various languages, to three-way contrast in labial activity in a set of back our knowledge, only Ling [16] has collected data vowels: rounding in [u] that varies between in- on their lingual articulation; this data is somewhat rounding and out-rounding; vertical lip compres- limited, however, both in speaker population (n=3) 1440 and spatial resolution (three fleshpoints tracked us- ing EMA). We thus aim to characterize the lingual Table 1: Stimuli with the simplified Chinese char- β v acters used for display. Notation for each vowel as activity of [ɯ ] and [ɯ ] relative to [u] in Sūzhōu used in [16] is provided for reference. Chinese with a larger data set and a richer spatial rep- resentation using ultrasound tongue imaging. Sev- Vowel As in [16] Stimulus Gloss eral factors lead us to hypothesize that the tongue 44 will exhibit a greater back-raising excursion for [u] [u] o 疤 [pu] ‘scar’ than the other two vowels, which have more substan- [ɯβ] u 播 [pɯβ]44 ‘spread, sow’ tial lip constrictions with fewer articulatory degrees [ɯv] u 夫 [fɯv]44 ‘husband’ of freedom. In particular, rounded vowels such as [i] ɪ 边 [pi]44 ‘side’ [u] are known to exhibit trading relations in realizing [æ] æ 包 [pæ]44 ‘package’ their low F2 targets [21], which all three Sūzhōu Chi- nese vowels share [16]. A prediction of this model is that [ɯβ] and [ɯv], having more consistent and crophone attached to the stabilization headset’s right more constricted lip settings, should have less of a cheekpad arm. Audio was digitized using a Focus- need for lingual activity in service of a back vowel rite Scarlett 2i2 USB audio interface, which was also quality (i.e., low F2), and may be produced with a configured to accept the synchronization pulse train lower or centralized tongue position relative to [u]. generated by the ultrasound device for time align- This study can also be seen as a test of whether ment of the articulatory and acoustic signals. some lingual articulatory properties of similar con- sonants extend to the segments at issue. Known 2.2. Materials aerodynamic requirements for producing labioden- tal fricatives and bilabial trills, which may be shared Stimuli, shown in Table 1, were presented as sim- by [ɯv] and [ɯβ], respectively, make different pre- plified Chinese characters in randomized order. dictions. Active lowering of the tongue dorsum has The stimuli were interspersed with other characters been observed during the production of labioden- which have readings containing different consonan- tal fricative consonants [24], which should extend tal onsets and vowels not relevant to the present straightforwardly to [ɯv]. However, known lingual study. Stimuli were displayed on a computer screen, articulatory strategies for bilabial trills, which pref- and participants were instructed to produce them in a erentially occur before high back vowels [8], predict frame sentence with a reading appropriate to Sūzhōu tongue dorsum height similar to [u]. Chinese rather than Standard Chinese. Participants produced 9–13 tokens of each stimulus. 2. METHODS 2.3. Analysis 2.1. Data collection The series of ultrasound frames recorded during the Participants were 15 native speakers of Sūzhōu Chi- production of each vowel token was extracted from nese (13 F, 2 M) who took part in the larger study the ultrasound video. For each vowel token, tongue described in [11]. Participants have similar residen- surface contours consisting of 100 sampling points tial and linguistic histories: all are long-term resi- were extracted from the frames in this series using dents of Sūzhōu, and all report native proficiency in EdgeTrak [14]. The frame closest to the acoustic Sūzhōu Chinese and high or native-like proficiency midpoint of each target vowel was selected. Each in Standard Chinese. participant’s set of midpoint tongue surface contours Ultrasound video was recorded using an was submitted to a smoothing-spline analysis of vari- Echo Blaster ultrasound device equipped with ance (SSANOVA) [5]; splines are generated in polar a PV6.5/10/128 Z-3 microconvex probe, with a coordinates and then converted to Cartesian coordi- typical frame rate of 54 Hz. The ultrasound probe nates to avoid distortion in the tongue tip and root was held in place under the chin using an Articulate regions [19]. The resulting models are not directly Instruments, Ltd. stabilization headset [23]. The comparable across participants due to variation in probe angle relative to the occlusal plane varies vocal tract morphology and probe orientation. from participant to participant given the need to A discrete Fourier transform (DFT) was also per- accommodate differences in participant jaw and formed on the contour for each midpoint frame for chin morphology. [u], [ɯβ], and [ɯv], following the method imple- Audio was recorded at a sampling rate of 44.1 mented in [6]. DFT converts contour data into a kHz using a Sony ECM-77B electret condenser mi- smaller set of numerical coefficients representing the 1441 frequency and magnitude of sine and cosine func- Figure 2: SSANOVA splines for four representa- tions that can be combined to model the basis data. tive speakers; anterior is right. The DFT used here is computed from the tangent an- S03 S07 gle of each point in the contour and makes no refer- ence to a fixed coordinate system. As such, the co- efficients analyzed here do not reflect contour posi- tion or rotation, but rather solely contour shape. This property is desirable for the present study, given that the direction of speaker-specific articulatory lines u u ɯβ ɯβ may differ in physical space owing to variation in ɯv ɯv vocal tract morphology. S37 S44 DFT coefficients have real and imaginary por- tions, which correspond to the phase and magni- tude of the sinusoidal basis functions, respectively [6]. Coefficients’ real parts model the anterior- posterior position of the bulk of the tongue, and co- u u efficients’ imaginary parts the extent of bunching or ɯβ ɯβ ɯv ɯv raising. The order n of coefficients corresponds to a wavelength of 1/n contour arc lengths for the si- but we hypothesize that both will point to a larger nusoidal functions represented, such that higher co- back-raising excursion for rounded [u] than for the efficients represent greater spatial frequencies.
Recommended publications
  • Part 1: Introduction to The
    PREVIEW OF THE IPA HANDBOOK Handbook of the International Phonetic Association: A guide to the use of the International Phonetic Alphabet PARTI Introduction to the IPA 1. What is the International Phonetic Alphabet? The aim of the International Phonetic Association is to promote the scientific study of phonetics and the various practical applications of that science. For both these it is necessary to have a consistent way of representing the sounds of language in written form. From its foundation in 1886 the Association has been concerned to develop a system of notation which would be convenient to use, but comprehensive enough to cope with the wide variety of sounds found in the languages of the world; and to encourage the use of thjs notation as widely as possible among those concerned with language. The system is generally known as the International Phonetic Alphabet. Both the Association and its Alphabet are widely referred to by the abbreviation IPA, but here 'IPA' will be used only for the Alphabet. The IPA is based on the Roman alphabet, which has the advantage of being widely familiar, but also includes letters and additional symbols from a variety of other sources. These additions are necessary because the variety of sounds in languages is much greater than the number of letters in the Roman alphabet. The use of sequences of phonetic symbols to represent speech is known as transcription. The IPA can be used for many different purposes. For instance, it can be used as a way to show pronunciation in a dictionary, to record a language in linguistic fieldwork, to form the basis of a writing system for a language, or to annotate acoustic and other displays in the analysis of speech.
    [Show full text]
  • Methods for Quantifying Tongue Shape and Complexity Using Ultrasound Imaging 1960 Katherine M
    CLINICAL LINGUISTICS & PHONETICS 2016, VOL. 30, NOS. 3–5, 328–344 http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/02699206.2015.1099164 Methods for quantifying tongue shape and complexity using ultrasound imaging 1960 Katherine M. Dawsona,b, Mark K. Tiedeb, and D. H. Whalena,b,c aDepartment of Speech-Language-Hearing Sciences, City University of New York Graduate Center, New York, NY, USA; bHaskins Laboratories, New Haven, CT, USA; cDepartment of Linguistics, Yale University, New Haven, CT, USA ABSTRACT ARTICLE HISTORY Quantification of tongue shape is potentially useful for indexing Received 3 February 2015 articulatory strategies arising from intervention, therapy and devel- Revised 18 September 2015 opment. Tongue shape complexity is a parameter that can be used Accepted 20 September to reflect regional functional independence of the tongue muscu- 2015 lature. This paper considers three different shape quantification KEYWORDS methods – based on Procrustes analysis, curvature inflections and Articulation; complexity; Fourier coefficients – and uses a linear discriminant analysis to test speech production how well each method is able to classify tongue shapes from measurement; tongue different phonemes. Test data are taken from six native speakers shape; ultrasound of American English producing 15 phoneme types. Results classify tongue shapes accurately when combined across quantification methods. These methods hold promise for extending the use of ultrasound in clinical assessments of speech deficits. Introduction The shaping of the tongue during speech is a finely controlled motor activity involving the coordination of a complex array of intrinsic and extrinsic agonist–antagonist muscles (Takemoto, 2001). However, measuring and quantifying tongue shape has proved challen- ging, as the tongue, a muscular hydrostat, is a difficult structure to image.
    [Show full text]
  • The Articulatory Modeling of German Coronal Consonants Using TADA
    The articulatory modeling of German coronal consonants using TADA Manfred Pastätter, Marianne Pouplier Institute of Phonetics and Speech Processing, Ludwig-Maximilians-University, Munich, Germany [email protected], [email protected] Abstract spring equations, that is, each gesture is specified for a rest po- sition (i.e., target), as well as a stiffness and damping parameter. We present a modeling study on German coronals using the task The stiffness parameter serves to distinguish between classes dynamic synthesizer TADA to provide new aspects to the dis- of sounds with vowel gestures having a lower stiffness setting cussion whether coronals should be generally specified for a than consonant gestures (e.g. Fowler 1980, Perkell 1969, Roon tongue body target to control the tongue-shape globally. We ar- et al. 2007). Each gesture hierarchically controls an ensemble gue that all coronals must be specified for a tongue body target of articulators which are yoked in a task-specific fashion in or- in order to avoid inappropriate dominance of the vowel dur- der to achieve a particular constriction goal. For instance, a ing the consonant. This target must be weighted more strongly lip closure gesture is associated with the articulators upper lip, compared to the temporally co-existing vowel gesture. We fur- lower lip, jaw. If temporally overlapping gestures call on the ther study the effects of changing the consonant:vowel stiffness same articulators with conflicting demands, weighting param- ratio on CV and VC transitions. The stiffness settings employed eters specify the degree to which one gesture may dominate in TADA for American English lead to general vowel diphthon- in its control over a given articulator.
    [Show full text]
  • Influences of Tongue Biomechanics on Speech Movements During The
    Influences of tongue biomechanics on speech movements during the production of velar stop consonants: A modeling study Pascal Perrier1, Yohan Payan2, Majid Zandipour3 and Joseph Perkell3 1Institut de la Communication Parlée, UMR CNRS 5009, INPG, Grenoble, France 2 Laboratoire TIMC, CNRS, Université Joseph Fourier, Grenoble, France 3Speech Communication Group, R.L.E., Massachusetts Institute of Technology, Cambridge, Massachusetts, USA Received: Revision: April 15, 2003 Running Title: On loops and tongue biomechanics Abbreviated Title: On loops and tongue biomechanics Contact : Pascal Perrier ICP, INPG 46 Avenue Félix Viallet 38031 Grenoble Cédex 01 France e-mail : [email protected] Phone 33 + 476 574 825 Fax : 33 + 476 5704 710 1 Abstract This study explores the following hypothesis: forward looping movements of the tongue that are observed in VCV sequences are due partly to the anatomical arrangement of the tongue muscles, how they are used to produce a velar closure and how tongue interacts with the palate during consonantal closure. The study uses an anatomically based two-dimensional biomechanical tongue model. Tissue elastic properties are accounted for in finite-element modeling, and movement is controlled by constant-rate control parameter shifts. Tongue raising and lowering movements are produced by the model mainly with the combined actions of the genioglossus, styloglossus, and hyoglossus. Simulations of V1CV2 movements were made, where C is a velar consonant and V is [a], [i] or [u]. Both vowels and consonants are specified in terms of targets, but for the consonant the target is virtual, and cannot be reached because it is beyond the surface of the palate.
    [Show full text]
  • Uu Clicks Correlate with Phonotactic Patterns Amanda L. Miller
    Tongue body and tongue root shape differences in N|uu clicks correlate with phonotactic patterns Amanda L. Miller 1. Introduction There is a phonological constraint, known as the Back Vowel Constraint (BVC), found in most Khoesan languages, which provides information as to the phonological patterning of clicks. BVC patterns found in N|uu, the last remaining member of the !Ui branch of the Tuu family spoken in South Africa, have never been described, as the language only had very preliminary documentation undertaken by Doke (1936) and Westphal (1953–1957). In this paper, I provide a description of the BVC in N|uu, based on lexico-statistical patterns found in a database that I developed. I also provide results of an ultrasound study designed to investigate posterior place of articulation differences among clicks. Click consonants have two constrictions, one anterior, and one posterior. Thus, they have two places of articulation. Phoneticians since Doke (1923) and Beach (1938) have described the posterior place of articulation of plain clicks as velar, and the airstream involved in their production as velaric. Thus, the anterior place of articulation was thought to be the only phonetic property that differed among the various clicks. The ultrasound results reported here and in Miller, Brugman et al. (2009) show that there are differences in the posterior constrictions as well. Namely, tongue body and tongue root shape differences are found among clicks. I propose that differences in tongue body and tongue root shape may be the phonetic bases of the BVC. The airstream involved in click production is described as velaric airstream by earlier researchers.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Berkeley UC Berkeley Phonlab Annual Report
    UC Berkeley UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report Title Articulatory Variation of the Alveolar Tap and Implications for Sound Change Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/9n00618c Journal UC Berkeley PhonLab Annual Report, 8(8) ISSN 2768-5047 Author Cathcart, Chundra Publication Date 2012 DOI 10.5070/P79n00618c eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UC Berkeley Phonology Lab Annual Report (2012) Articulatory variation of the alveolar tap and implications for sound change∗ Chundra Cathcart 1 Introduction As a natural class, the label \rhotic" comprises an extremely heterogeneous set of sounds which often show little in common phonetically (Ladefoged and Maddieson, 1996). In the same way, individual rhotic phonemes in the world's languages have been shown to exhibit highly variable articulatory behavior. Several phonetic studies have revealed much subphonemic variation in the realization of individual rhotic phonemes, with different articulatory configurations corresponding to one categorical acoustic signature. An old aphorism says that the seeds of sound change can be found in the details of synchronic variation. If this insight is correct, then it follows that among variants of a phone or phoneme, possible seeds of of attested changes should be identifiable. The aim of this paper is to make a tentative connection between rhotic articulatory variability and a particular sound change seen in unrelated language families. Observing lingual ultrasound data, I analyze features of the alveolar tap that vary within and across speakers. I then propose a provisional link between some articulatory patterns seen in the data and the development of progressive rhotic postalveolarization or retroflexion, a sound change that has come about independently in some dialects of Swedish and Norwegian, in some English dialects, in Sanskrit, in some East Iranian languages, and elsewhere.
    [Show full text]
  • A Mimicry Study of Adaptation Towards Socially-Salient Tongue Shape Variants
    University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics Volume 20 Issue 2 Selected Papers from NWAV 42 Article 12 10-2014 A mimicry study of adaptation towards socially-salient tongue shape variants Eleanor Lawson Jane Stuart-Smith James M. Scobbie Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl Recommended Citation Lawson, Eleanor; Stuart-Smith, Jane; and Scobbie, James M. (2014) "A mimicry study of adaptation towards socially-salient tongue shape variants," University of Pennsylvania Working Papers in Linguistics: Vol. 20 : Iss. 2 , Article 12. Available at: https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol20/iss2/12 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/pwpl/vol20/iss2/12 For more information, please contact [email protected]. A mimicry study of adaptation towards socially-salient tongue shape variants Abstract We know that fine phonetic ariationv is exploited by speakers to construct and index social identity (Hay and Drager 2007). Sociophonetic work to date has tended to focus on acoustic analysis, e.g. Docherty and Foulkes (1999); however, some aspects of speech production are not readily recoverable from an acoustic analysis. New articulatory analysis techniques, such as ultrasound tongue imaging (UTI), have helped to identify seemingly covert aspects of speech articulation, which pattern consistently with indexical factors, e.g. underlyingly, Scottish English middle-class and working-class coda /r/, have radically different tongue shapes and tongue gesture timings (Lawson, Scobbie and Stuart-Smith, 2011). This articulatory variation has gone unidentified, despite decades of auditory and acoustic analysis (Romaine, 1979; Speitel and Johnston, 1983; Stuart-Smith, 2007). UTI revealed that middle-class speakers tend to produce bunched variants of postvocalic /r/, while working-class speakers tend to produce tongue-tip raised variants (Lawson, Stuart-Smith and Scobbie 2011).
    [Show full text]
  • Labial-Coronal Vs. Labial-Velar Consonant Sequences: an Articulatory Study
    Labial-Coronal vs. Labial-Velar Consonant Sequences: An Articulatory Study Manon Carrissimo-Bertola1, Nathalie Vallée1, Ioana Chitoran2 (1) Gipsa-Lab, (2) CLLILAC-ARP [email protected], nathalie.vallee@gipsa- lab.grenoble-inp.fr, [email protected] Studies on universal trends in languages, both from different genetic and geographic origins, have shown favored combinations of non-adjacent consonants in words across various syllabic structures. Regarding place of articulation, sound sequences are more often organized with a labial consonant followed by a coronal one, rather than the opposite order. McNeilage & Davis (2000) called this trend the Labial Coronal (LC) Effect, and since then, it has been observed in twenty or so language lexicons (Vallée & al. 2009, G-ULSID database). This preferred organization is attested (i) between heterosyllabic onsets of two consecutive open syllables - CVCV, (ii) between tautosyllabic onset and coda - CVC (Table1), and (iii) in children productions at the first stage of words (McNeilage & Davis, 2000). Carrissimo- Bertola (2010) looked for other favored consonant place orders, in particular a potential labial-velar (LV) trend (Table2). A labial-velar effect was hypothesized because it shares with the labial-coronal order an anterior-posterior order of constrictions, and the use of two independent articulators. No labial- velar preference was found. We therefore propose to compare the articulatory strategies for both consonant sequences according to the places of articulation in order to understand why some of them are more widespread than others. Table 1: Mean LC/CL ratios estimated in four studies (* means no data). Table 2: Mean LV/VL ratios for the 19 G-ULSID languages, and number of languages by trend (* means no data).
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonetics of English Pronunciation - Week 3
    Version SS 2008 The Phonetics of English Pronunciation - Week 3 W.Barry Institut für Phonetik Universität des Saarlandes IPUS 1 How far have we got, and what did I ask you to do? 1. I suggested that we need to be able to describe sounds if we are going to be able to deal with differences! - We looked briefly at consonants acccording to their place & manner of articulation and their voicing. And the homework question was: - What are the dimensions used for classifying consonants? Answer: No problem (for MOST of you!) - For each dimension, give example German word pairs with two consonants that are different ONLY because of a switch in that dimension. Some of you clearly didn't consult the script: It is corrct that we distingish obstruents and sonorants, but they are not the basis for classifying consonants in general. Some of you started at a finer level than place, manner and voice, giving the different places of articulation (without actually saying that "place" was the dimension they belong to). 2 Example word-pairs Place: mein vs nein; Laus vs. Lauch; kicken vs. kippen /x/ (Why aren't "fad" vs. "Bad" or "Hut" vs. "gut" good examples?) Manner: mein vs Bein; Laus vs. laut; lacken vs. lachen /x/ (Why isn't "Saat" vs. "Tat" a good example?) Voicing: Bein vs. Pein; Wein vs. fein; reisen vs. reißen /v/ /z/ /s/ (Why isn't "Sand" vs. "Hans" a good example?) We started with German words to make it clear that there is nothing very special about the descriptive categories we are talking about.
    [Show full text]
  • Consonants ROACH
    THE PHONEME 1. The phoneme When we speak, we produce a continuous stream of sounds. In studying speech we divide this stream into small pieces that we call segments . The word ‘man’ is pronounced with a first segment mmm, a second segment æææ and a third segment nnn. Just as there is an abstract alphabet as the basis of our writing, so there is an abstract set of units as the basis of our speech. These units are called phonemes , and the complete set of these units is called the phonemic system of the language. Free variation & complementary distribution The phonemes themselves are abstract, but there are many slightly different ways in which we make the sounds that represent these phonemes, just as there are many ways in which we may make a mark on a piece of paper to represent a particular (abstract) letter of the alphabet. Whenever we hear, for example, two different ways of making a bbb, we speak of two different realisations of the phoneme. Since one can be substituted for the other without changing the meaning; the two realisations are said to be in free variation . On another hand, when we talk about different realisations of phonemes, we sometimes call these realisations allophones . For example, we find that the realisation of ttt in the word ‘tea’ is aspirated (as are all voiceless plosives when they occur before stressed vowels at the beginning of syllables). In the word ‘eat’, the realisation of ttt is unaspirated (as are all voiceless plosives when they occur at the end of a syllable and are not followed by a vowel).
    [Show full text]
  • A Kinematic Study of Coarticulation of Cantonese Fricative /S/ Title Using Electromagnetic Articulography (EMA)
    CORE Metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk Provided by HKU Scholars Hub A kinematic study of coarticulation of Cantonese fricative /s/ Title using electromagnetic articulography (EMA) Author(s) Lai, Ho-ning, Lydia; 黎浩寧 Citation Issued Date 2009 URL http://hdl.handle.net/10722/173669 Rights Creative Commons: Attribution 3.0 Hong Kong License 1 A kinematic study of coarticulation of Cantonese fricative /s/ using electromagnetic articulography (EMA) Lydia Lai Ho Ning 2005002716 A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Bachelor of Science (Speech and Hearing Sciences), The University of Hong Kong, June 30, 2009 2 A kinematic study of coarticulation of Cantonese fricative /s/ using electromagnetic articulography (EMA) Abstract The present study investigated the coarticulation during production of Cantonese alveolar fricative /s/ in /sV/ syllables, and the effects of vowel contexts on the coarticulatory behavior. A total of 10 native speakers of Hong Kong Cantonese with ages between 20 and 25 years participated in the study. Kinematic information of tongue and lip movement during production of /s/ in various phonetic contexts (/sa/, /si/, /su/ and /sy/) was obtained using a three-dimensional electromagnetic articulography (EMA) system. Results indicated the presence of coarticulation during /sV/ production and differential effects of coarticulation in different vowel contexts. For both male and female speakers, the upper lip showed no coarticulatory activity regardless of the vowel context. The findings of the present study provided new kinematic information of the tongue and lips during the production of Cantonese fricative /s/ in different vowel contexts. 3 Introduction Among the 19 consonants in Cantonese, three are classified as fricatives: the labiodental fricative /f/, alveolar fricative /s/, and the glottal fricative /h/ (Cheung & Abberton, 2000).
    [Show full text]
  • Articulatory Characterization of English Liquid-Final Rimes
    1 2 3 4 5 6 Articulatory Characterization of 7 8 English Liquid-Final Rimes 9 10 Michael Proctor 11 Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University 12 13 Rachel Walker, Caitlin Smith 14 Department of Linguistics, University of Southern California 15 16 T¨undeSzalay 17 Department of Linguistics, Macquarie University 18 19 Louis Goldstein, Shrikanth Narayanan 20 Department of Linguistics, University of Southern California 21 22 23 24 25 Abstract 26 27 Articulation of liquid consonants in onsets and codas by four speakers of General American English was 28 examined using real-time MRI. Midsagittal tongue posture was compared for laterals and rhotics produced 29 30 in each syllable margin, adjacent to 13 different vowels and diphthongs. Vowel articulation was examined in 31 words without liquids, before each liquid, and after each liquid, to assess the coarticulatory influence of each 32 segment on the others. Overall, nuclear vocalic postures were more influenced by coda rhotics than onset 33 34 rhotics or laterals in either syllable margin. Laterals exhibited greater temporal and spatial independence 35 between coronal and dorsal gestures. Rhotics were produced with a variety of speaker-specific postures, 36 37 but were united by a greater degree of coarticulatory resistance to vowel context, patterns consistent with 38 greater coarticulatory influence on adjacent vowels, and less allophonic variation across syllable positions 39 than laterals. 40 41 Keywords: liquid consonant, rhotic, lateral, coarticulation, syllable structure 42 43 44 1. Introduction 45 46 Liquid consonants in American English exhibit asymmetries with respect to the range of contrasts in co- 47 48 occurring vowels in the rime.
    [Show full text]