The User's Mental Model of an Information Retrieval System: an Experiment on a Prototype Online Catalog

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The User's Mental Model of an Information Retrieval System: an Experiment on a Prototype Online Catalog UCLA Publications Title The user's mental model of an information retrieval system: An experiment on a prototype online catalog Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/2k3386nz Journal International Journal of Human-Computer Studies, 51(2) ISSN 1071-5819 Author Borgman, Christine L. Publication Date 1999 DOI 10.1006/ijhc.1985.0318 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California THE USER'S MENTAL MODEL OF AN INFORMATION RETRIEVAL SYSTEM Christine L. Borgman Graduate School of Library and Information Science University of California, Los Angeles ABSTRACT INTRODUCTION An empirical study was performed In the search to understand how to train naive subjects in the use of a naive user learns to comprehend, a prototype Boolean logic-based in- reason about, and utilize an interac- formation retrieval system on a bib- tive computer system, a number of liographic database. Subjects were researchers have begun to explore the undergraduates with little or no nature of the user's mental model of prior computing experience. Subjects trained with a conceptual model of a system. Among the claims are that the system performed better than a mental model is useful for deter- mining methods of interaction [1,2], subjects trained with procedural in- problem solving [2,3], and debugging structions, but only on complex, problem-solving tasks. Performance errors [4]; that model-based training was equal on simple tasks. Differ- is superior to procedural training ences in patterns of interaction with [2,5,6]; that users build models the system (based on a stochastic spontaneously, in spite of training process model) showed parallel re- [1,7]; that incorrect models lead to sults. Most subjects were able to problems in interaction [4,7]; and articulate some description of the that interface design should be based system's operation, but few articu- on a mental model [8,9]. Not sur- lated a model similar to the card prisingly, these authors use a varie- catalog analogy provided intraining. ty of definitions for "mental model" Eleven of 43 subjects were unable to and the term "conceptual model" is achieve minimal competency in system often used with the same meaning. use. The failure rate was equal Young [i0] was able to identify eight between training conditions and gen- different uses of the term "concep- ders; the only differences found tual model" in the recent literature, between those passing and failing the for example. This author prefers the benchmark test were academic major distinction made by Norman [7] that a and in frequency of library use. conceptual model is a model presented to the user, usually by a designer, researcher, or trainer, which is intended to convey the workings of the system in a manner that the user can understand. A mental model is a model of the system that the user builds in his or her mind. The user's mental model may be based on the conceptual model provided, but is probably not identical to it. Permission to copy without fee all or part of this material is granted provided that the copies are not made or distributed for The first research comparing direct commercial advantage, the ACM copyright notice and the conceptual models to procedural in- title of the publication and its date appear, and notice is given structions for training sought only that copying is by pern~ission of the Association for Computing to show that the conceptual training Machinery. To copy otherwise, or to republish, requires a fee was superior [6,11]. Other recent and/or specific permission. research [1,2] has studied the inter- action between training conditions © 1985 ACM 0~89791-159-8/85/006/0268' $00.75 and tasks, finding that model-based 268 training is more beneficial for The introductory narrative pro- complex or problem solving tasks. vided to the model group described the system using an analogical model The research on mental models of the card catalog. The instruc- and training has been concentrated in tions first explained the structure the domains of text editing [ii,12] of a divided (author/title/subject) and calculators [1,2,4,10]; no such card catalog and then explained the research has yet been done in inform- system structure in terms of the ways ation retrieval. Information re- it was similar to a card catalog and trieval is an interesting domain, as the ways in which it was different. it is now undergoing a shift in user Boolean logic was described in terms population. In the last ten years, a of sets of catalog cards, showing significant population of highly- sample sets and the resulting sets trained searchers who act as inter- after specified Boolean combinations. mediaries for end users on commercial systems has developed. Although end The narrative introduction for users have been reluctant to use the the procedural group consisted of commercial systems, libraries are background information on information rapidly replacing their card catalogs retrieval that is commonly given in with online catalogs intended for system manuals. The Boolean opera- direct patron use. The online cata- tors were defined only by single- logs are typically simpler to use and sentence statements. have a more familiar record struc- ture, but still have many of the The examples provided were the difficulties associated with the use same in each conditioD, but the anno- of a complex interactive system. The tations for each reflected the dif- result is a population of naive, ferences in the introductory mater- minimally-trained, and infrequent ials. The list of searchable fields users of information retrieval sys- (16 of 25 fields were searchable) was tems [13]. The need for an efficient also identical and gave examples of form of training for this population the search elements for each field. is very great and we chose it as a domain to test the advantages of The training tasks used for the model-based training. benchmark test were all classified as simple tasks, requiring the use of only one index and no more than one EXPERIMENTAL METHOD Boolean operator. The experiment con- sisted of five simple and ten complex The experiment was structured as tasks, the latter requiring two or a two-by-two design, with two train- more indexes and one or more Boolean ing conditions (model and procedural) operators. All tasks were presented and two genders. All subjects were as narrative library reference ques- undergraduates at Stanford University tions and were designed to be within with two or fewer programming courses the scope of questions that might be and minimal, if any, additional com- asked by undergraduates in performing puter experience. course assignments. We performed the experiment on a i Subjects were given the instruc- prototype Boolean logic-based online tional materials to read and then catalog mounted on a microcomputer performed the benchmark test, which with online monitoring capabilities. consisted of completing 14 simple Two bibliographic databases were tasks on the small database in less mounted: a training database con- than 30 minutes. The testwas based sisting of 50 hand-selected records on pilot test findings that those who on the topic of "animals" and a !arg - took longest to complete the training er database of about 6,000 records tasks were least able to learn to use systematically sampled from the 10- the system (r=-0.83, p<.05). If the million record database of the OCLC subject passed the benchmark test, he Online Computer Library Center. or she was interviewed briefly, given the experimental tasks to perform, Subjects in each training condi- and then asked to perform one addi- tion received three training docu- tional search while talking aloud for ments: an introductory narrative, a the experimenter. Subjects were in- set of annotated examples of system terviewed again after completing the operation, and a table of searchable experiment. fields. 269 RESULTS If the subjects were able to describe the system's operation at Due to a high failure rate on all, it was most likely in terms of the benchmark test (ll of 43, or an abstract model bearing little 26%), we were able to gather a valid resemblance to a card catalog anal- dataset of only 28 cases. The dif- ogy. Of 28 subjects, 15 (5 model ference in time required to complete condition, i0 procedural) gave some the benchmark test was significant form of abstract model, four (3 (p<0.0001), with those failing aver- model, 1 procedural) articulated a aging 39.2 minutes and those passing card catalog-based model, only one averaged 18.2 minutes. Subjects subject (procedural condition) artic- failed equally in the two training ulated a model based on another meta- conditions and by gender. phor (robots retrieving sheets of paper from bins), and eight subjects Subjects who passed the bench- (6 model, 2 procedural) were unable mark test tended to be from science to describe the system in any model- and engineering majors rather than based manner. social science and humanities (p<0.0001), and were less frequent Only minor differences between visitors to the library (average 8.0 genders were found. Men scored high- visits per month vs. 18.4 visits for er than women (p<0.05) on the index those who passed). Major and library of describing the system, although use were not correlated. gender explained only 14% of the variance in the model index on a In task performance, we found no linear regression. Men were found to difference between training condi- make more errors on simple tasks than tions on number of simple tasks cor- women (p<0.05), but the difference rect (p>0.05). The difference" on was not significant for errors on number of complex tasks correct was complex tasks.
Recommended publications
  • Librarianship and the Philosophy of Information
    University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Library Philosophy and Practice (e-journal) Libraries at University of Nebraska-Lincoln July 2005 Librarianship and the Philosophy of Information Ken R. Herold Hamilton College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/libphilprac Part of the Library and Information Science Commons Herold, Ken R., "Librarianship and the Philosophy of Information" (2005). Library Philosophy and Practice (e-journal). 27. https://digitalcommons.unl.edu/libphilprac/27 Library Philosophy and Practice Vol. 3, No. 2 (Spring 2001) (www.uidaho.edu/~mbolin/lppv3n2.htm) ISSN 1522-0222 Librarianship and the Philosophy of Information Ken R. Herold Systems Manager Burke Library Hamilton College Clinton, NY 13323 “My purpose is to tell of bodies which have been transformed into shapes of a different kind.” Ovid, Metamorphoses Part I. Library Philosophy Provocation Information seems to be ubiquitous, diaphanous, a-categorical, discrete, a- dimensional, and knowing. · Ubiquitous. Information is ever-present and pervasive in our technology and beyond in our thinking about the world, appearing to be a generic ‘thing’ arising from all of our contacts with each other and our environment, whether thought of in terms of communication or cognition. For librarians information is a universal concept, at its greatest extent total in content and comprehensive in scope, even though we may not agree that all information is library information. · Diaphanous. Due to its virtuality, the manner in which information has the capacity to make an effect, information is freedom. In many aspects it exhibits a transparent quality, a window-like clarity as between source and patron in an ideal interface or a perfect exchange without bias.
    [Show full text]
  • What's in a Mental Model of a Dynamic System? Conceptual Structure and Model Comparison
    What’s in a mental model of a dynamic system? Conceptual structure and model comparison Martin Schaffernicht Facultad de Ciencias Empresariales Universidad de Talca Avenida Lircay s/n 3460000 Talca, CHILE [email protected] Stefan N. Groesser University of St. Gallen Institute of Management System Dynamics Group Dufourstrasse 40a CH - 9000 St. Gallen, Switzerland Tel: +41 71 224 23 82 / Fax: +41 71 224 23 55 [email protected] Contribution to the International System Dynamics Conference 2009 Albuquerque, NM, U.S.A. Abstract This paper deals with the representation of mental models of dynamic systems (MMDS). Improving ’mental models’ has always been fundamental in the field of system dynamics. Even though a specific definition exists, no conceptual model of the structure of a MMDS has been offered so far. Previous research about the learning effects of system dynamics interventions have used two methods to represent and analyze mental models. To what extend is the result of these methods comparable? Can they be used to account for a MMDS which is suitable for the system dynamics methodology? Two exemplary MMDSs are compared with both methods. We have found that the procedures and results differ significantly . In addition, neither of the methods can account for the concept of feedback loops. Based on this finding, we propose a conceptual model for the structure of a MMDS, a method to compare them, and a revised definition of MMDS. The paper concludes with a call for more substantive research. Keywords : Mental models, dynamic systems, mental model comparison, graph theory, mental model measurement Introduction Mental models have been a key concept in system dynamics from the beginning of the field (Forrester, 1961).
    [Show full text]
  • Psychology, Meaning Making and the Study of Worldviews: Beyond Religion and Non-Religion
    Psychology, Meaning Making and the Study of Worldviews: Beyond Religion and Non-Religion Ann Taves, University of California, Santa Barbara Egil Asprem, Stockholm University Elliott Ihm, University of California, Santa Barbara Abstract: To get beyond the solely negative identities signaled by atheism and agnosticism, we have to conceptualize an object of study that includes religions and non-religions. We advocate a shift from “religions” to “worldviews” and define worldviews in terms of the human ability to ask and reflect on “big questions” ([BQs], e.g., what exists? how should we live?). From a worldviews perspective, atheism, agnosticism, and theism are competing claims about one feature of reality and can be combined with various answers to the BQs to generate a wide range of worldviews. To lay a foundation for the multidisciplinary study of worldviews that includes psychology and other sciences, we ground them in humans’ evolved world-making capacities. Conceptualizing worldviews in this way allows us to identify, refine, and connect concepts that are appropriate to different levels of analysis. We argue that the language of enacted and articulated worldviews (for humans) and worldmaking and ways of life (for humans and other animals) is appropriate at the level of persons or organisms and the language of sense making, schemas, and meaning frameworks is appropriate at the cognitive level (for humans and other animals). Viewing the meaning making processes that enable humans to generate worldviews from an evolutionary perspective allows us to raise news questions for psychology with particular relevance for the study of nonreligious worldviews. Keywords: worldviews, meaning making, religion, nonreligion Acknowledgments: The authors would like to thank Raymond F.
    [Show full text]
  • Effectiveness of Holistic Mental Model Confrontation in Driving Conceptual Change
    Learning and Instruction xxx (2011) 1e15 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Learning and Instruction journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/learninstruc Effectiveness of holistic mental model confrontation in driving conceptual change Soniya Gadgil a,*, Timothy J. Nokes-Malach a, Michelene T.H. Chi b a Department of Psychology, Learning Research and Development Center, University of Pittsburgh, United States b Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, United States article info abstract Article history: Prior research on conceptual change has identified multiple kinds of misconceptions at different levels of Received 17 July 2010 representational complexity including false beliefs, flawed mental models, and incorrect ontological Received in revised form categories. We hypothesized that conceptual change of a mental model requires change in the system of 5 June 2011 relations between the features of the prior model. To test this hypothesis, we compared instruction aimed Accepted 8 June 2011 at revising knowledge at the mental model level called holistic confrontation e in which the learner compares and contrasts a diagram of his or her flawed mental model to an expert model e to instruction Keywords: aimed at revising knowledge at the false belief level e in which the learner is prompted to self-explain Learning Conceptual change the expert model alone. We found evidence that participants who engaged in holistic confrontation were Comparison more likely to acquire a correct mental model, and a deeper understanding of the systems of relations in Mental models the model than those who were prompted to self-explain the expert model. The results are discussed in Self-explanation terms of their implications for science instruction.
    [Show full text]
  • Mental Models in Expert Physics Reasoning. INSTITUTION California Univ., Berkeley
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 285 736 SE 048 324 AUTHOR Roschelle, Jeremy; Greeno, James G. TITLE Mental Models in Expert Physics Reasoning. INSTITUTION California Univ., Berkeley. SPONS AGENCY Office of Naval Research, Washington, D.C. Personnel and Training Branch. REPORT NO GA-2 PUB DATE Jul 87 CONTRACT N00014-85-K-0095 NOTE 29p.; For related documents, see SE 048 323-327. PUB TYPE Reports Research/Technical (143) EDRS PRICE MF01/PCO2 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Physics; *Problem Solving; Science Education; *Science Instruction; Sciences IDENTIFIERS *Experts; *Mental Models ABSTRACT Proposed is a relational framework for characterizing experienced physicists' representations of physics problem situations and the process of constructing these representations. A representation includes a coherent set of relations among: (1) a mental model of the objects in the situation, along with their relevant properties and relations; (2) a mental model of theoretical idealizations of objects; and (3) parameter histories based on mental simulations of both models. Evidence from protocols and a small experiment support a conclusion that experienced physicists' processes of representing problem situations (a) use informal commonsense knowledge, including envisionment of objects in the situations, and (b) are interactive, with mutual. influences between informal knowledge and their technical, theorc.t.cal knowledge. Also described are characteristics of the mental models that represent problem situations and the process of constructing them, drawing from work by artificial intelligence researchers on qualitative process models, and specifying several categories of rules that would be needed for an implementation of the system as a simulation program. (Author/RH) ********************k****2********************************************* Reproductions supplied by EDRS are the best that can be made from the original document.
    [Show full text]
  • Religious Fundamentalism in Eight Muslim‐
    JOURNAL for the SCIENTIFIC STUDY of RELIGION Religious Fundamentalism in Eight Muslim-Majority Countries: Reconceptualization and Assessment MANSOOR MOADDEL STUART A. KARABENICK Department of Sociology Combined Program in Education and Psychology University of Maryland University of Michigan To capture the common features of diverse fundamentalist movements, overcome etymological variability, and assess predictors, religious fundamentalism is conceptualized as a set of beliefs about and attitudes toward religion, expressed in a disciplinarian deity, literalism, exclusivity, and intolerance. Evidence from representative samples of over 23,000 adults in Egypt, Iraq, Jordan, Lebanon, Pakistan, Saudi Arabia, Tunisia, and Turkey supports the conclusion that fundamentalism is stronger in countries where religious liberty is lower, religion less fractionalized, state structure less fragmented, regulation of religion greater, and the national context less globalized. Among individuals within countries, fundamentalism is linked to religiosity, confidence in religious institutions, belief in religious modernity, belief in conspiracies, xenophobia, fatalism, weaker liberal values, trust in family and friends, reliance on less diverse information sources, lower socioeconomic status, and membership in an ethnic majority or dominant religion/sect. We discuss implications of these findings for understanding fundamentalism and the need for further research. Keywords: fundamentalism, Islam, Christianity, Sunni, Shia, Muslim-majority countries. INTRODUCTION
    [Show full text]
  • Constructing Mental Model Paradigms for Teaching Electronic Resources
    Volume 2, 1999 Approved January 1999 ISSN: 1523-4320 www.ala.org/aasl/slr Constructing Mental Model Paradigms for Teaching Electronic Resources Julie I. Tallman, Associate Professor, Department of Instructional Technology, The University of Georgia, and Lyn Henderson, Senior Lecturer, School of Education, James Cook University of North Queensland, Australia Library media specialists activate numerous mental models when teaching electronic information literacy database access, research, and retrieval in the context of authentic school assignments. The paper identifies these models and examines what occurred when the mental models in a study sample interconnected in the complex changing environment of a lesson. Discussion focuses on the changes to the mental models the library media specialists held with respect to the electronic database, the role of the library media specialist, the lesson goals, and their teaching strategies. Findings suggest that (a) most of the library media specialists’ mental models of teaching with electronic databases were influenced by their models of teaching access, research, and retrieval with print resources and (b) even though many library media specialists identified the necessity to incorporate mental model changes for more effective teaching, these were not sufficient to counteract their habituated teaching behaviors. New policy thrusts in education are increasing the range of electronic learning technologies for use by students, placing them as central to the core curriculum, and calling for equitable access to information via these technologies (Tallman 1998). Part of this equity includes individualized instruction to help students “develop a systematic mode of inquiry to gain physical and intellectual access to information and ideas that reflect diversity of experiences, opinions, and social and cultural perspectives” (American Association of School Librarians and Association for Educational Communications and Technology 1988, 29).
    [Show full text]
  • Models in the Mind the Mental Models Theory of Reasoning
    Models in the Mind The Mental Models Theory of Reasoning: Refinements and Exten- sions. Edited by Walter Schaeken, Andr´eVandierendonck, Walter Schroyens, and G´eryd'Ydewalle. Mahwah, New Jersey: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates. How do people reason about the what follows from certain assumptions? How do they think about implications between statements. According to one theory, people try to use a small number of mental rules of inference to construct an argument for or proof of a relevant conclusion from the assump- tions (e.g., Rips 1994). According to a competing theory, people construct one or more mental models of the situation described in the assumptions and try to determine what conclusion fits with the model or models constructed (e.g., Johnson-Laird 1983, 2006). The present collection offers eleven con- tributions to the mental models theory. Specific theories about such reasoning can be tested by asking subjects what follows from certain assumptions. Subjects can also be asked whether a particular stated conclusion follows or is implied by the assumptions. And subjects can be given problems that seem to require such reasoning, such as the familiar the Wason (1966) selection task. In this task, four cards are placed before a subject on the table. The subject is told that the cards have a letter on one side and a number on the other wide. The subject sees only one side of the cards, e.g. \A 3 B 4". The subject is asked to indicate which of the cards need to be turned over in order to determine whether a certain claim about the cards is correct: for example, the claim that, if there is a vowel on one side, there is an even number on the other side.
    [Show full text]
  • Peirce, Pragmatism, and the Right Way of Thinking
    SANDIA REPORT SAND2011-5583 Unlimited Release Printed August 2011 Peirce, Pragmatism, and The Right Way of Thinking Philip L. Campbell Prepared by Sandia National Laboratories Albuquerque, New Mexico 87185 and Livermore, California 94550 Sandia National Laboratories is a multi-program laboratory managed and operated by Sandia Corporation, a wholly owned subsidiary of Lockheed Martin Corporation, for the U.S. Department of Energy’s National Nuclear Security Administration under Contract DE-AC04-94AL85000.. Approved for public release; further dissemination unlimited. Issued by Sandia National Laboratories, operated for the United States Department of Energy by Sandia Corporation. NOTICE: This report was prepared as an account of work sponsored by an agency of the United States Government. Neither the United States Government, nor any agency thereof, nor any of their employees, nor any of their contractors, subcontractors, or their employees, make any warranty, express or implied, or assume any legal liability or responsibility for the accuracy, completeness, or usefulness of any information, apparatus, product, or process disclosed, or represent that its use would not infringe privately owned rights. Reference herein to any specific commercial product, process, or service by trade name, trademark, manufacturer, or otherwise, does not necessarily con- stitute or imply its endorsement, recommendation, or favoring by the United States Government, any agency thereof, or any of their contractors or subcontractors. The views and opinions expressed herein do not necessarily state or reflect those of the United States Government, any agency thereof, or any of their contractors. Printed in the United States of America. This report has been reproduced directly from the best available copy.
    [Show full text]
  • Citizen Science: Framing the Public, Information Exchange, and Communication in Crowdsourced Science
    University of Tennessee, Knoxville TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange Doctoral Dissertations Graduate School 8-2014 Citizen Science: Framing the Public, Information Exchange, and Communication in Crowdsourced Science Todd Ernest Suomela University of Tennessee - Knoxville, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss Part of the Communication Commons, and the Library and Information Science Commons Recommended Citation Suomela, Todd Ernest, "Citizen Science: Framing the Public, Information Exchange, and Communication in Crowdsourced Science. " PhD diss., University of Tennessee, 2014. https://trace.tennessee.edu/utk_graddiss/2864 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of TRACE: Tennessee Research and Creative Exchange. For more information, please contact [email protected]. To the Graduate Council: I am submitting herewith a dissertation written by Todd Ernest Suomela entitled "Citizen Science: Framing the Public, Information Exchange, and Communication in Crowdsourced Science." I have examined the final electronic copy of this dissertation for form and content and recommend that it be accepted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy, with a major in Communication and Information. Suzie Allard, Major Professor We have read this dissertation and recommend its acceptance: Carol Tenopir, Mark Littmann, Harry Dahms Accepted for the Council: Carolyn R. Hodges Vice Provost and Dean of the Graduate School (Original signatures are on file with official studentecor r ds.) Citizen Science: Framing the Public, Information Exchange, and Communication in Crowdsourced Science ADissertationPresentedforthe Doctor of Philosophy Degree The University of Tennessee, Knoxville Todd Ernest Suomela August 2014 c by Todd Ernest Suomela, 2014 All Rights Reserved.
    [Show full text]
  • Maintaining the Mental Model: an Exploratory Study of Dialogic Processes on Good Teaching Practice
    Pedagogy and the Human Sciences Volume 3 Issue 1 Article 1 2013 Maintaining the Mental Model: An Exploratory Study of Dialogic Processes on Good Teaching Practice Margaret Koepke Fox Suffolk University, [email protected] Debra A. Harkins Suffolk University, [email protected] Kurt W. Fischer Harvard University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.merrimack.edu/phs Recommended Citation Fox, M. K., Harkins, D. A., & Fischer, K. W. (2013). Maintaining the Mental Model: An Exploratory Study of Dialogic Processes on Good Teaching Practice. Pedagogy and the Human Sciences, 3 (1), 1-23. Retrieved from https://scholarworks.merrimack.edu/phs/vol3/iss1/1 This Feature Article is brought to you for free and open access by Merrimack ScholarWorks. It has been accepted for inclusion in Pedagogy and the Human Sciences by an authorized editor of Merrimack ScholarWorks. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Fox et al.: Dialogic Processes on Teaching Practice Pedagogy and the Human Sciences, 1, No. 3, 2013, pp. 1-23 Maintaining the Mental Model: An Exploratory Study of Dialogic Processes on Good Teaching Practice Margaret Koepke Fox,1 Debra A. Harkins,2 and Kurt W. Fischer3 Abstract. This paper presents some outcomes of an exploratory, mixed-method study that examined mental models of teaching and understanding of learning processes in 26 educators from a small suburb in the Northeastern United States. Participants, in semi- structured written interviews, were asked to rate variables contributing to their mental model of effective teaching. They were then presented with a specific educational problem and asked to provide pedagogic solutions.
    [Show full text]
  • The Theory of Everything
    The Theory of Everything R. B. Laughlin* and David Pines†‡§ *Department of Physics, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305; †Institute for Complex Adaptive Matter, University of California Office of the President, Oakland, CA 94607; ‡Science and Technology Center for Superconductivity, University of Illinois, Urbana, IL 61801; and §Los Alamos Neutron Science Center Division, Los Alamos National Laboratory, Los Alamos, NM 87545 Contributed by David Pines, November 18, 1999 We discuss recent developments in our understanding of matter, we have learned why atoms have the size they do, why chemical broadly construed, and their implications for contemporary re- bonds have the length and strength they do, why solid matter has search in fundamental physics. the elastic properties it does, why some things are transparent while others reflect or absorb light (6). With a little more he Theory of Everything is a term for the ultimate theory of experimental input for guidance it is even possible to predict Tthe universe—a set of equations capable of describing all atomic conformations of small molecules, simple chemical re- phenomena that have been observed, or that will ever be action rates, structural phase transitions, ferromagnetism, and observed (1). It is the modern incarnation of the reductionist sometimes even superconducting transition temperatures (7). ideal of the ancient Greeks, an approach to the natural world that But the schemes for approximating are not first-principles has been fabulously successful in bettering the lot of mankind deductions but are rather art keyed to experiment, and thus tend and continues in many people’s minds to be the central paradigm to be the least reliable precisely when reliability is most needed, of physics.
    [Show full text]