Cerebral Achromatopsia As a Presentation of Trousseau's Syndrome

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Cerebral Achromatopsia As a Presentation of Trousseau's Syndrome 44 Orrell, James-Galton, Stevens, Rossor Postgrad Med J: first published as 10.1136/pgmj.71.831.44 on 1 January 1995. Downloaded from Cerebral achromatopsia as a presentation of Trousseau's syndrome Richard W Orrell, Merle James-Galton, John M Stevens, Martin N Rossor Summary four years previously, and had been taking A 67-year-old man developed a sudden 300 mg aspirin daily. onset of achromatopsia. Magnetic reson- On examination he appeared well, with a ance imaging showed occipital lobe regular pulse, blood pressure 160/100 mmHg infarction. Repeated episodes of neuro- normal heart sounds, and no carotid bruits. logical deficit referable to the posterior Neurological examination was normal, but for circulation initially suggested an embolic absent colour vision and a partial right source, but subsequently proved to be due hemianopia. Visual acuity was 6/18 bilaterally. to a coagulopathy related to a carcinoma Two weeks after the onset of visual distur- of the bladder. This has implications for bance he developed a sudden onset of upper the management of patients presenting motor neurone weakness in the right arm and with achromatopsia, and progressive or face, with reduced sensation in the right arm, recurrent neurological episodes, and in following a bout ofheavy coughing. Investiga- particular the use of anticoagulation in tion at this time demonstrated normal full this situation. blood count, erythrocyte sedimentation rate, prothrombin time, serum urea and electrolytes, Keywords: cerebral achromatopsia, Trousseau's syn- and glucose. Treponemal serology was drome, bladder carcinoma, disseminated intravascular negative. Chest radiograph was normal. Com- coagulation puted tomographic (CT) scan of the brain showed an area of presumed recent infarction Introduction involving the left occipital lobe and posterior part of the temporal lobe. Magnetic resonance 'Cerebral achromatopsia is a syndrome in (MR) scan the next day showed bilateral which the patient loses the ability to see colours occipital lobe infarction, mainly below the after cortical damage. This loss may be com- calcarine fissure (see figures). MR angiography plete or partial, and it may or may not be showed the posterior circulation to be normal, accompanied by other visual defects'."2 We with no evidence of basilar artery thrombosis. http://pmj.bmj.com/ describe a case of this unusual but readily Doppler examination of the carotid arteries recognisable clinical syndrome, and its associa- showed a 30% internal carotid stenosis on the tion with a paraneoplastic coagulopathy. right, and 55% on the left. There was no clinical or electrocardiographic evidence of an Case report arrhythmia, and an echocardiogram showed no evidence of a cardiac source of emboli. man was as a front seat on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. St Mary's Hospital, A 67-year-old travelling Praed Street, London, passenger in a car when he experienced a UK sudden brilliant flash of white throughout his NEUROPSYCHOLOGICAL ASSESSMENT Department of vision, followed by a kaleidoscope effect, with A detailed neuropsychological assessment was Neurology criss-cross lines and bright colours. The carried out at the time of the original visual RW Orrell experience lasted about 20 seconds, and he was deficit. On the Wechsler Adult Intelligence MN Rossor with some of and was Scale (Revised) he obtained a verbal IQ of 128.3 Department of left fogginess vision, only Neuroradiology able to see images in black and white. Three There was no evidence of generalised intellec- JM Stevens weeks previously he had experienced an epi- tual impairment. Recognition memory for ver- sode of weakness in the right hand, lasting for bal material was excellent, he scored in the Department of two days, but otherwise had no previous superior range on the Warrington Recognition Clinical neurological symptoms. Over a period of two Memory for words.4 Neuropsychology, The weeks there was some in his Test of primary visual function were per- National Hospital, improvement Queen Square, vision, but persistence of the loss of colour formed. Reading acuity on the Ffookes sym- London, UK vision. bols test was 6/18. Shape discrimination was M James-Galton Eighteen months previously he had present- within normal limits.5 Shape detection on the ed with haematuria. Cystoscopy demonstrated VOSP (Visual Object and Space Perception) Correspondence to a multifocal transitional cell carcinoma on the figure-ground was unimpaired.6 Colour dis- Dr Richard W Orrell, Academic Unit of left wall of the bladder which was in the early crimination was severely impaired. He was Neuroscience, Charing Cross invasive stage. This was being controlled by unable to pick out any numbers on the Ishihara Hospital, Fulham Palace repeated cystoscopy and fulguration. He had test. He had great difficulty on the Farnsworth Road, London W6 8RF, UK ischaemic heart disease, with occasional 100-Hue test, error score 784.78 There was no Accepted 11 August 1994 angina, had coronary artery by-pass grafting evidence ofvisual disorientation; he was able to Cerebral achromatopsia 45 Postgrad Med J: first published as 10.1136/pgmj.71.831.44 on 1 January 1995. Downloaded from Figure 1 MRI, 1.5 Tesla, with Gadolinium enhance- ment, showing features ofbilateral occipital lobe infarc- marked on the left Figure 2 MRI, coronal section of the brain, showing tion, more features of bilateral occipital lobe infarction, mainly below the calcarine fissure count scattered dots accurately, missing only those which fell within his visual field defect. Tests of higher visual processing from the period of weeks had some recovery of vision in VOSP6 were performed. He was completely the left homonymous fields, but remained unable to see any form of a letter on the drowsy, and the heparin was discontinued. Incomplete Letter task. His performance on Within two weeks he developed a deep vein the Silhouettes test was very poor (4/30). On thrombosis of the right leg, with gangrenous the Progressive Silhouettes test he was slow to areas in the toes, and as there was some identify the objects even when complete. He recovery in his conscious level, heparin was also made errors of identification of simple line recommenced. Within three months of the drawings ofobjects. He showed some degree of initial presentation of visual disturbance he prosopagnosias, being able to identify only 1/12 developed significant haematuria, with further Famous Faces and he complained of difficulty evidence of disseminated intravascular coagu- in recognising people. lation (DIC). Over a period of one week the The interpretation ofhis neuropsychological coagulopathy progressed. This failed to re- http://pmj.bmj.com/ assessment is that the only elements of cortical spond to treatment with platelets and fresh blindness present were a severe achromatopsia frozen plasma and he died peacefully. and a mild impairment ofacuity. In addition he Post-mortem examination showed the cause had an aperceptive agnosia. The degree of of death to be a pulmonary embolus, with a impairment of his primary visual processing thrombosed right external iliac vein. There was was insufficient to account for his poor perfor- a stage 4 bladder carcinoma, invading through mance on tests of visual object processing. the bladder wall into the perivesical fat, with a on September 29, 2021 by guest. Protected copyright. right hydronephrosis and hydroureter. His- PROGRESS tology showed an ulcerated poorly differenti- In view of the recurrent episodes of neuro- ated transitional cell carcinoma extending into logical deficit, which were felt to be possibly the perivesicular fat, and lymphatic channel embolic, he was started on an infusion of invasion. There was no evidence of metastatic heparin at 10 000 units over 24 hours, with spread. On examining the brain, there were caution because of the haemorrhagic nature of bilateral basal occipital infarcts and bilateral the infarct, and his previous haematuria. The parietal infarcts, all being recent, with signs of right arm weakness resolved over three days, colliquative necrosis. Additional infarcts were with a residual mild right facial weakness. He present in the kidney and spleen, with gang- was commenced on warfarin anticoagulation renous infarction of the right forefoot and toes with careful control. of the left foot. There was no evidence of an Two weeks later, having developed mild embolic source in the heart or great vessels. haematuria and having discontinued the war- farin himself, he was found wandering at night, Discussion aphasic and agitated. He had a left upper motor facial weakness, and a progressive left hemi- Cerebral achromatopsia has been recognised plegia. He had complete cortical blindness, for many years, with debate over the existence with small pupils, absent gag reflex, and brisk of a specific cortical colour centre. Clinical jaw jerk. CT scan at this stage showed extensive evidence links the anterior inferior part of the bilateral occipital infarction. He was again occipital lobe with colour perception in man. commenced on a heparin infusion, and over a Bilateral lesions at this site may cause achroma- 46 Orrell, James-Galton, Stevens, Rossor topsia with preservation of primary visual function.9 A unilateral lesion will cause Impaired colour vision hemiachromatopsia. Animal studies, and more * achromatopsia = cannot see colour Postgrad Med J: first published as 10.1136/pgmj.71.831.44 on 1 January 1995. Downloaded from recently functional positron emission tomo- * agnosia
Recommended publications
  • Cortical Mechanisms of Colour Vision
    REVIEWS CORTICAL MECHANISMS OF COLOUR VISION Karl R. Gegenfurtner The perception of colour is a central component of primate vision. Colour facilitates object perception and recognition, and has an important role in scene segmentation and visual memory. Moreover, it provides an aesthetic component to visual experiences that is fundamental to our perception of the world. Despite the long history of colour vision studies, much has still to be learned about the physiological basis of colour perception. Recent advances in our understanding of the early processing in the retina and thalamus have enabled us to take a fresh look at cortical processing of colour. These studies are beginning to indicate that colour is processed not in isolation, but together with information about luminance and visual form, by the same neural circuits, to achieve a unitary and robust representation of the visual world. SENSORY SYSTEMS It has been known since the nineteenth century that In the retinal ganglion cells, three channels convey there are three types of photoreceptor for daylight information from the eye to the brain7,8. In the L + M or vision in the human eye1,2. Since then, the shapes of the luminance channel, the signals from L- and M-cones spectral absorption functions of the different cones are added to compute the intensity of a stimulus. In the have been determined with increasing precision in L – M colour-opponent channel, the signals from L- and psychophysical experiments and verified using various M-cones are subtracted from each other to compute electrophysiological methods3. On the molecular the red–green component of a stimulus.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychophysical Investigations Into the Neural Basis of Synaesthesia V
    doi 10.1098/rspb.2000.1576 Psychophysical investigations into the neural basis of synaesthesia V. S. Ramachandran* andE.M.Hubbard Center for Brain and Cognition, University of California, San Diego, 9500 Gilman Drive, LaJolla, CA 92093-0109, USA We studied two otherwise normal, synaesthetic subjects who `saw' a speci¢c colour every time they saw a speci¢c number or letter. We conducted four experiments in order to show that this was a genuine perceptual experience rather than merely a memory association. i)The synaesthetically induced colours could lead to perceptual grouping, even though the inducing numerals or letters did not. ii)Synaesthetically induced colours were not experienced if the graphemes were presented peripherally. iii)Roman numerals were ine¡ective: the actual number grapheme was required. iv)If two graphemes were alternated the induced colours were also seen in alternation. However, colours were no longer experienced if the graphemes were alternated at more than 4 Hz. We propose that grapheme colour synaesthesia arises from `cross-wiring' between the `colour centre' area V4 or V8)and the `number area', both of which lie in the fusiform gyrus. We also suggest a similar explanation for the representation of metaphors in the brain: hence, the higher incidence of synaesthesia among artists and poets. Keywords: synaesthesia; multisensory; extrastriate visual areas; psychophysics that the visual number grapheme rather than the numer- 1. INTRODUCTION ical concept is required for evoking colours. However, they The bizarre phenomenon of synaesthesia, e.g. `seeing both noted that if they `imaged' the corresponding Arabic sounds' or seeing speci¢c colours upon seeing speci¢c numeral then the corresponding colour was evoked, but numerals Galton 1880, 1883; Cytowic 1989; Paulesu et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Cerebral Mechanisms Operating in the Presence and Absence of Inflammatory Pain
    Annals ofthe Rheumatic Diseases 1996; 55: 411-420 41 REVIEW: PAIN Series editor: Bruce L Kidd Ann Rheum Dis: first published as 10.1136/ard.55.7.411 on 1 July 1996. Downloaded from Cerebral mechanisms operating in the presence and absence of inflammatory pain Anthony K P Jones, Stuart W G Derbyshire During episodes ofinflammatory arthritic pain, surprising if such parallel processing did not the brain has the task of integrating new also exist for non-primary sensations such as information from receptors of muscle, tendon, pain. There is now growing evidence that pain and skin afferents with nociceptive information is processed by a network or 'matrix' of struc- from the joint capsule and, possibly, peri- tures in the brain (fig 1), some components of articular bone. This integration will incorpor- which could be considered to comprise parallel ate motivational, affective, discriminative, at- but integrated processing. tentional, and motor responses that may result in changes of behaviour and modification of central nervous system (CNS) processing of NEUROANATOMY OF NOCICEPTION IN ANIMALS pain. It is as yet unclear what the nature of this Studying pain in animals presents an insoluble CNS modification may be, and suggested problem. Human beings can call out and influences on pain related affective-motor express their pain in ways that are commonly responses or the inflammatory process itself understandable to other members of their remain largely speculative. species; unfortunately, there is no equivalent It is likely to be some time before we begin frame of reference when dealing with animals. to have a proper understanding of the complex Increased blood pressure, movements of integrated processes that operate during withdrawal, dilatation of the pupil, increased arthritic pain and other chronic pains.
    [Show full text]
  • Retinotopy and Color Sensitivity in Human Visual Cortical Area V8
    © 1998 Nature America Inc. • http://neurosci.nature.com articles Retinotopy and color sensitivity in human visual cortical area V8 Nouchine Hadjikhani, Arthur K. Liu, Anders M. Dale, Patrick Cavanagh and Roger B. H. Tootell Nuclear Magnetic Resonance Center, Massachusetts General Hospital, 149 13th Street, Charlestown, Massachusetts 02129, USA Correspondence should be addressed to N.H. ([email protected]) Prior studies suggest the presence of a color-selective area in the inferior occipital-temporal region of human visual cortex. It has been proposed that this human area is homologous to macaque area V4, which is arguably color selective, but this has never been tested directly. To test this model, we compared the location of the human color-selective region to the retinotopic area boundaries in the same subjects, using functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI), cortical flattening and retinotopic mapping techniques. The human color-selective region did not match the location of area V4 (neither its dorsal nor ventral subdivisions), as extrapolated from macaque maps. Instead this region coincides with a new retinotopic area that we call ‘V8’, which includes a distinct representation of the fovea and both upper and lower visual fields. We also tested the response to stimuli that produce color afterimages and found that these stimuli, like real colors, caused preferen- tial activation of V8 but not V4. In Old World primates such as macaque monkeys and humans, ing studies, a small patch of color-selective activity near the mid- visual information about color is processed in anatomically dle of the collateral sulcus has been named ‘V4’ (ref.
    [Show full text]
  • The Anatomy of Cerebral Achromatopsia: a Reappraisal and Comparison of Two Case Reports
    The anatomy of cerebral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports. Paolo Bartolomeo, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten To cite this version: Paolo Bartolomeo, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten. The anatomy of cere- bral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports.: Anatomy of cerebral achro- matopsia. Cortex, Elsevier, 2014, 56, pp.138-44. 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.013. inserm-00803853 HAL Id: inserm-00803853 https://www.hal.inserm.fr/inserm-00803853 Submitted on 23 Mar 2013 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Preprint version of the paper published on Cortex, doi: 10.1016/j.cortex.2013.01.013 The anatomy of cerebral achromatopsia: A reappraisal and comparison of two case reports Paolo Bartolomeo1-3, Anne-Catherine Bachoud-Lévi4-7, Michel Thiebaut de Schotten1,8 1 Inserm U975; UPMC-Paris6, UMR_S 975; CNRS UMR 7225, Centre de Recherche de l'Institut du Cerveau et de la Moelle épinière, Groupe Hospitalier Pitié-Salpêtrière, 75013 Paris, France 2 AP-HP,
    [Show full text]
  • Diamond for Neural Interfacing: a Review
    Carbon 102 (2016) 437e454 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Carbon journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/carbon Review article Diamond for neural interfacing: A review * David J. Garrett a, , Wei Tong a, David A. Simpson a, Hamish Meffin b a School of Physics, The University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia b National Vision Research Institute, Department of Optometry and Vision Science University of Melbourne, Victoria 3010, Australia article info abstract Article history: The variety of materials that fall in the diamond category is vast and increasing. From single, to micro to Received 9 December 2015 nano-crystalline, diamond can be highly electrically insulating or synthesized to possess varying degrees Received in revised form of conductivity. Diamond in all its forms is without peer in terms of chemical stability exhibits excellent 3 February 2016 biocompatibility and is famously mechanically robust. Diamond is however, a difficult material with Accepted 20 February 2016 which to fabricated devices owing to its extreme hardness, lack of ductility and weldability. New syn- Available online 26 February 2016 thesis and fabrication methods in recent decades have overcome some of these drawbacks and diamond has enjoyed a surge in interest as a biomedical material. In the field of neural interfaces a grand goal is permanent, high fidelity connections with neural populations. Diamond's longevity, biocompatibility and biochemical inertness make it a highly promising material with which to achieve this goal. This review covers recent uses of diamond in the three critical areas of neural interfacing: diamond as a growth substrate, as a neurochemical sensor electrode material and as a direct neural recording and stimulation material.
    [Show full text]
  • Computational Model of Visual Perception: from Colour to Form
    ADVERTIMENT. Lʼaccés als continguts dʼaquesta tesi queda condicionat a lʼacceptació de les condicions dʼús establertes per la següent llicència Creative Commons: http://cat.creativecommons.org/?page_id=184 ADVERTENCIA. El acceso a los contenidos de esta tesis queda condicionado a la aceptación de las condiciones de uso establecidas por la siguiente licencia Creative Commons: http://es.creativecommons.org/blog/licencias/ WARNING. The access to the contents of this doctoral thesis it is limited to the acceptance of the use conditions set by the following Creative Commons license: https://creativecommons.org/licenses/?lang=en Computational Model of Visual Perception: From Colour to Form A dissertation submitted by SeyedArash Akbarinia to the Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona in fulfil- ment of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Departament de Ciències de la Computació. Bellaterra, July 13, 2017 Director Dr. C. Alejandro Párraga Centre de Visió per Computador Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona Thesis Dr. Marcelo Bertalmío committee Department of Information and Communication Technologies Universitat Pompeu Fabra Dr. Nicolai Petkov Intelligent Systems University of Groningen Dr. Joost van de Weijer Centre de Visió per Computador Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona International Dr. Casimir Ludwig evaluators School of Experimental Psychology University of Bristol Dr. Thorsten Hansen Department of General and Experimental Psychology Justus-Liebig-Universität This document was typeset by the author using LATEX 2". The research described in this book was carried out at the Centre de Visió per Computador, Universitat Autònoma de Barcelona. Copyright © 2017 by SeyedArash Akbarinia. All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the author.
    [Show full text]
  • Very Early Responses to Colour Stimuli Detected in Prestriate
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/106047; this version posted February 6, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 Very Early Responses to Colour Stimuli Detected in Prestriate Visual Cortex by Magnetoencephalography (MEG) Yoshihito Shigihara, Hideyuki Hoshi and Semir Zeki Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London Authors: Yoshihito Shigihara, Hideyuki Hoshi, and Semir Zeki Affiliation: Wellcome Laboratory of Neurobiology, University College London, University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT Corresponding author: Semir Zeki ([email protected]); University College London, Gower Street, London, WC1E 6BT TEL/FAX, +44 (0) 20 7679 7316 Journal section: Research Articles Short title: Very early responses to colour Keywords: colour vision, early cortical latencies, Area V4, prestriate cortex, V1-bypassing input. bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/106047; this version posted February 6, 2017. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 2 Abstract Our previous studies with the visual motion and form systems show that visual stimuli belonging to these categories trigger much earlier latency responses from the visual cortex than previously supposed and that the source of the earliest signals can be located in either the prestriate cortex or in both the striate (V1) and prestriate cortex. This is consistent with the known anatomical connections since, in addition to the classical retino-geniculo-striate cortex input to the prestriate visual areas, there are direct anatomical inputs from both the lateral geniculate nucleus and the pulvinar that reach the prestriate visual cortex without passing through striate cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurological Basis of Synesthesia
    OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN P a r t V NEUROLOGICAL BASIS OF SYNESTHESIA 224_Simner_C24.indd4_Simner_C24.indd 447373 55/29/2013/29/2013 110:10:130:10:13 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN 224_Simner_C24.indd4_Simner_C24.indd 447474 55/29/2013/29/2013 110:10:130:10:13 PPMM OUP UNCORRECTED PROOF – FIRSTPROOFS, Wed May 29 2013, NEWGEN Chapter 24 Synesthesia and functional imaging Edward M. Hubbard Introduction Researchers have debated the neural mechanisms that give rise to synesthesia since the earliest days of synesthesia research (e.g., Flournoy 1893). However, it is only with the advent of sophisticated neuroimaging techniques like positron emission tomography (PET) and, more recently, functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) that these questions could be empirically addressed by examining patterns of brain activation in synesthetes and non-synesthetes alike. Since the fi rst attempt to measure brain activ- ity related to synesthetic experiences over 25 years ago (Cytowic and Stump 1985), our understanding of brain functions and the sophistication of neuroimaging methods has increased dramatically. Th ese advances have led to a number of neurophysiologically sophisticated models of synesthesia, and to a wealth of studies aimed at testing them. Here, I will not dis- cuss studies using methods such as electroencephalography (Brang et al. 2011; Niccolai, Wascher, and Stoerig 2012) or magnetoencephalography (Brang et al. 2010) although in many cases the fi ndings using these other methods converge with those from neuroim- aging methods (for a review of studies using these other methods, see Hubbard et al. 2011; J ä ncke, Chapter 28; Ramachandran and Brang, Chapter 48, this volume).
    [Show full text]
  • Grapheme-Colour and Tone-Colour Synaesthesia Is Associated
    1 Grapheme-Colour and Tone-Colour Synaesthesia is Associated with Structural Brain Changes in Visual Regions Implicated in Colour, Form and Motion Michael J Banissy1,2, Lauren Stewart2, Neil G Muggleton1, 3, Timothy D Griffiths4, 5, Vincent Y Walsh1, Jamie Ward6 & Ryota Kanai1 1 UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London, U.K.. 2 Department of Psychology, Goldsmiths, University of London, U. K. 3 Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, National Central University, Jhongli 320, Taiwan. 4 Wellcome Trust Centre for Neuroimaging, University College London, U.K. 5 Institute of Neuroscience, Newcastle University, Newcastle, U.K. 6 Sackler Centre for Consciousness Sciences and School of Psychology, University of Sussex, U.K. Address correspondence to :- Michael Banissy, Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, University College London, London, WC1N 3AR, U.K. Tel. : 020-7679-5438, Fax. : 020-7916-8517 E-mail : [email protected] 2 Abstract Synaesthesia is a rare condition in which stimulation in one modality leads to a secondary experience in another sensory modality. Varying accounts attribute the condition to either neuroanatomical differences between the synaesthetes and non- synaesthetes or functional differences in how sensory brain regions interact. This study employed voxel-based-morphometry to examine whether synaesthetes who experience both grapheme-colour and tone-colour synaesthesia as their evoked sensation show neuroanatomical differences in gray matter volume compared to non- synaesthetes. We observed that synaesthetes showed an increase in gray matter volume in left posterior fusiform gyrus, but a concomitant decrease in anterior regions of left fusiform gyrus and left MT / V5. These findings imply that synaesthesia for colour is linked to neuroanatomical changes between adjacent regions of the visual system.
    [Show full text]
  • 'How One Can See What Is Not There': Neural Mechanisms of Grapheme
    ‘How one can see what is not there’: Neural mechanisms of grapheme-colour synaesthesia Tessa M. van Leeuwen Front cover: My synaesthesia for letters and digits, overlaid on the surface of a brain (brain image generated with SPM, www.fil.ion.ucl.ac.uk/spm). The research reported in this thesis was carried out at the Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Centre for Cognitive Neuroimaging, of the Radboud University Nijmegen, the Netherlands, with financial support from the Volkswagen-Foundation [grant number I/80 743]. ISBN 978-94-91027-10-9 Printed by Ipskamp Drukkers, Enschede, the Netherlands © Tessa M. van Leeuwen, 2011 ‘How one can see what is not there’: Neural mechanisms of grapheme-colour synaesthesia Een wetenschappelijke proeve op het gebied van de Sociale Wetenschappen Proefschrift ter verkrijging van de graad van doctor aan de Radboud Universiteit Nijmegen op gezag van de rector magnificus prof. mr. S.C.J.J. Kortmann, volgens besluit van het College van Decanen in het openbaar te verdedigen op woensdag 14 september 2011 om 15:30 uur precies door Tessa Marije van Leeuwen geboren op 26 juli 1980 te Leiderdorp Promotor: Prof. dr. P. Hagoort Manuscriptcommissie: Prof. dr. M. Ullsperger Dr. R. Rouw (Universiteit van Amsterdam) Dr. R. Willems Contents 1. Introduction: About synaesthesia 7 2. Colour specificity in the human V4 complex 21 3. Synaesthetic colour in the brain: Beyond colour areas 37 4. Effective connectivity determines the nature of subjective 63 experience in grapheme-colour synaesthesia: A brief communication 5. Altered colour processing as indicated by alpha band oscillations can 79 provide an explanation for grapheme-colour synaesthesia 6.
    [Show full text]
  • Achromatopsia, Color Vision, and Cortex Charles A
    Neurol Clin N Am 21 (2003) 483–500 Achromatopsia, color vision, and cortex Charles A. Heywood, DPhila,*, Robert W. Kentridge, PhDb aDepartment of Psychology and Wolfson Research Institute, University of Durham, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom bDepartment of Psychology, University of Durham, Durham DH1 3LE, United Kingdom Few researchers would deny the advantages that color vision confers on those who possess it. In low-level vision, color assists in segmenting the visual scene on the basis of chromatic boundaries, when fluctuating shadows create spurious luminance borders that camouflage the contour of an object, thereby rendering it invisible to a monochromatic observer. In higher-level vision, color enhances identification and recognition. It can provide in- formation about the state of objects, for example its oft-cited role in in- dicating the ripeness of dietary fruit. In this respect, it is notable that the spectral tuning of retinal photoreceptors in African Old World monkeys and chimpanzees are exquisitely matched to maximize the discriminability of young green shoots concealed among less succulent foliage [1]. In fru- givorous platyrrhine monkeys, such tuning is optimal for the detection of dietary fruits concealed against variegated foliage [2,3]. In addition, color may be used in the identification of conspecifics or to signal sexual recep- tivity. Color also improves visual recognition memory for natural scenes by facilitating the encoding and retrieval of images, particularly when they are endowed in their natural and characteristic colors [4–6]. The ubiquity of color is reflected in the considerable amount of neural tissue devoted to the analysis of color information. Neuroanatomic and neurophysiologic studies have described the functional properties of visual pathways in the primate.
    [Show full text]