University of California, San Diego

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

University of California, San Diego UC San Diego UC San Diego Electronic Theses and Dissertations Title Neural correlates of conspecific vocal recognition in the caudomedial nidopallium Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4qq4d0bc Author Thompson, Jason Venard Publication Date 2010 Peer reviewed|Thesis/dissertation eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA, SAN DIEGO Neural Correlates of Conspecific Vocal Recognition in the Caudomedial Nidopallium A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Neurosciences by Jason Venard Thompson Committee in charge: Professor Timothy Gentner, Chair Professor Henry Abarbanel, Co-Chair Professor Stephan Anagnostaras Professor Andrea Chiba Professor Rich Krauzlis 2010 The Dissertation of Jason Venard Thompson is approved, and it is acceptable in quality and form for publication on microfilm and electronically: Co-Chair Chair University of California, San Diego 2010 iii TABLE OF CONTENTS Signature Page…………………………………………………………………………………………………….iii Table of Contents………………………………………………………………………………………………...iv List of Figures……………………………………………………………………………………………………..vi List of Tables……………………………………………………………………………………………………...vii Acknowledgements……………………………………………………………………………………….......viii Vita…………………………………………………………………………………………………….........................x Abstract………………………………………………………………………………………………………………xi I. Introduction……………………………………………………………………………………………………...1 Vocal Recognition……………………………………………………………………………………...1 Songbirds………………………………………………………………………………………………….2 European starling song behavior………………………………………………........................5 Songbird auditory system………………………………………………………….......................8 Learning and the songbird auditory system……………………………………………...13 Mammalian auditory system……………………………………………………………………15 Learning and the mammalian auditory system…………………………………………19 Mechanisms of sensory plasticity……………………………………………………………..23 Inhibition and stimulus selectivity……………………………………………………………25 References……………………………………………………………………….................................27 II. Song recognition learning and stimulus-specific weakening of neural responses in the avian auditory forebrain……………………………………………………….41 Abstract……………………………………………………………………………………....................41 Introduction……………………………………………………………………………………………42 Methods……………………………………………………………………………………....................44 Results………………………………………………………………………………………...................53 Discussion…………………………………………………………………………………...................77 Supplementary Material.……………………………………………………………...................85 References………………………………………………………………………………………………94 III. Local inhibition inactivates learning-related plasticity in the songbird auditory forebrain…………………………………………………………………………100 Abstract………………………………………………………………………………………………...100 iv Introduction………………………………………………………………………………………….101 Methods………………………………………………………………………………………………..103 Results………………………………………………………………………………………………….110 Discussion……………………………………………………………………………………………..130 References…………………………………………………………………………………………….136 IV. Conclusion…………………………………………………………………………………………………..141 References…………………………………………………………………………………………….153 v LIST OF FIGURES Chapter 2: Figure 2.1 Auditory Diagram and song recognition training………….…….................54 Figure 2.2 Recording location………………….…………………………………………………….56 Figure 2.3 Preference for unfamiliar songs in single neurons………………………….58 Figure 2.4 Preference for unfamiliar songs in single neurons………………………….60 Figure 2.5 Preference for unfamiliar songs in NCM neurons.……………………. …….63 Figure 2.6 Song exposure does not cause weakened responses to learned songs in NCM.…………………………………………………………………..68 Figure 2.7 No evidence for firing rate adaptation during recording experiments………………………………………………………………….70 Figure 2.8 Motif-level contributions to song-level effects………………………………..75 Figure 2.S1 Comparison of NCM plasticity for Male and Female Starlings..………..90 Chapter 3: Figure 3.1 Song Recognition Training…………………………………………………………..111 Figure 3.2 Blocking GABA-A inhibition increases firing rates………………………...113 Figure 3.3 Blocking Inhibition Enhances Plasticity in Ventral NCM………………..115 Figure 3.4 Blocking Inhibition uncovers a preference for learned songs in dorsal NCM……………………………………………………………………118 Figure 3.5 Variable Release from Inhibition throughout songs………………………121 Figure 3.6 Blocking inhibition decreases selectivity……………………………………...124 Figure 3.7 Blocking inhibition uncovers robust responses…………………………….126 Figure 3.8 Effects of blocking inhibition on MID receptive fields……………………129 vi LIST OF TABLES Chapter 2: Table 2.S1 Behavioral performance for last 1000 trials…………………………………...91 Table 2.S2 Different threshold for spontaneous activity in ventral NCM motif analysis………………………………………………………..92 vii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to acknowledge Professor Timothy Gentner for his support as my advisor and mentor. Tim was a constant source of excitement about data and science. I appreciate that Tim truly treats his students as collaborators. I’ve learned a lot from Tim about designing experiments, thinking about data and writing about science. This work would not have been possible without his guidance and support. I would also like to acknowledge the fellow graduate students in the lab: Micah Bregman, Emily Caporello, James Jeanne and Dan Knudsen. I was fortunate to be in a lab with some of the smartest people I’ve ever met. They contributed to my scientific development and made working in the lab a lot of fun. I owe a special thanks to James Jeanne and Dan Knudsen who I collaborated with on papers. I would like to thank my committee for their continued support and guidance, Drs. Henry Abarbanel, Stephan Anagnostaras,, Andrea Chiba and Rich Krauzlis. Finally, I am grateful to my family and especially my parents for their emotional support while I was at UCSD. I am particularly grateful to my partner Hope Williams. I probably would have given up long ago without her help and support. Chapter 2, in full, is a reprint of the material as it appears in The Journal of Neurophysiology 2010. Thompson, Jason V.; Gentner, Timothy Q., The American Physiological Society, 2010; doi:10.1152/jn.00885.2009 and is used with viii permission from The American Physiological Society. The dissertation author was the primary investigator and author of this paper. Chapter 3, in full, is being prepared for submission for publication of the material. Thompson, Jason V.; Jeanne, James M.; Gentner, Timothy Q. The dissertation author was the primary investigator and author of this material. ix VITA 2005 Bachelors of Science, University of Oklahoma 2006 Teaching Assistant, Department of Psychology University of California, San Diego 2009 Teaching Assistant, School of Medicine University of California, San Diego 2005-2010 Research Assistant, University of California, San Diego 2010 Doctor of Philosophy, University of California, San Diego PUBLICATIONS Thompson, J.V., Jeanne, J.M. and Gentner, T.Q. (in preparation) Local inhibition inactivates learning-related plasticity in the songbird auditory forebrain. Jeanne, J.M., Thompson, J.V., Sharpee, T.O. and Gentner, T.Q. (submitted) Learning enhances auditory information processing in a hierarchical neural circuit. Knudsen, D. P., Thompson, J.V. and Gentner, T.Q. (in press) Distributed recognition of natural songs by European starlings. Learning and Motivation. Thompson, J.V. and Gentner, T. Q. (2010) Song Recognition Learning and Stimulus- Specific Weakening of Responses in the Avian Auditory Forebrain. Journal of Neurophysiology 103, 1785-1797. Thompson, J.V., Sullivan, R.M. and Wilson, D.A. (2008). Developmental emergence of fear learning corresponds with changes in amygdala synaptic plasticity. Brain Research 1200, 58-65. Thompson, J.V., Best, A.R. and Wilson, D.A. (2005). Ontogeny of cortical synaptic depression underlying olfactory sensory gating in the rat. Developmental Brain Research 158, 107-110. Best, A.R., Thompson, J.V., Fletcher, M.L. and Wilson, D.A. (2005). Cortical metabotropic glutamate receptors contribute to habituation of a simple odor- evoked behavior. The Journal of Neuroscience 25, 2513-2517. x ABSTRACT OF THE DISSERTATION Neural Correlates of Conspecific Vocal Recognition in the Caudomedial Nidopallium by Jason Venard Thompson Doctor of Philosophy in Neurosciences University of California, San Diego, 2010 Professor Timothy Gentner, Chair Professor Henry Abarbanel, Co-Chair Vocal recognition is important for communication in several species, including humans. Songbirds learn to recognize the vocalizations of conspecifics for social behaviors such as mate attraction and territorial defense. We examined the role of the caudomedial nidopallium (NCM), a forebrain region analogous to secondary auditory cortices, in song recognition. We trained European starlings to recognize conspecific songs and recorded the activity of single neurons in NCM. In ventral NCM, neurons responded stronger to unfamiliar songs than to songs that starlings had learned to recognize. While in dorsal NCM, neurons responded xi similarly to learned and unfamiliar songs. In a second experiment we trained starlings to recognize songs and exposed them to an equal number of songs passively. Recognition learning weakened the firing rates to the learned songs, while passively hearing songs had no effect, indicating that the response suppression in NCM requires associative learning. These results show that song recognition learning weakens the responses to learned
Recommended publications
  • Toads Have Warts... and That's Good! | Nature Detectives | Summer 2021
    Summer 2021 TOADS HAVE WARTS…AND THAT’S GOOD! Warts on your skin are not good. Warts can occur when a virus sneaks into human skin through a cut. A medicine gets rid of the virus and then it’s good-bye ugly wart. Toad warts look slightly like human warts, but toad warts and people warts are not one bit the same. Toad warts are natural bumps on a toad’s back. Toads have larger lumps behind their eyes. The bumps and lumps are glands. The glands produce a whitish goo that is a foul-tasting and smelly poison. The poison is a toad’s ultimate defense in a predator attack. It is toxic enough to kill small animals, if they swallow enough of it. The toxin can cause skin and eye irritation in humans. Some people used to think toad warts were contagious. Touching a toad can’t cause human warts, but licking a toad might make you sick! Toads have other defenses too. Their camouflage green/gray/brown colors blend perfectly into their surroundings. They can puff up with air to look bigger, and maybe less appetizing. Pull Out and Save Pull Out and Pick one up, and it might pee on your hand. Toads Travel, Frogs Swim Toads and frogs are amphibians with some similarities and quite a few differences. Amphibians spend all or part of their life in water. Frogs have moist, smooth skin that loses moisture easily. A toad’s dry, bumpy skin doesn’t lose water as easily as frog skin. Frogs are always in water or very near it, otherwise they quickly dehydrate and die.
    [Show full text]
  • Naturally Occurring Fluorescence in Frogs
    Naturally occurring fluorescence in frogs Carlos Taboadaa,b, Andrés E. Brunettic, Federico N. Pedronb,d, Fausto Carnevale Netoc, Darío A. Estrinb,d, Sara E. Barib, Lucía B. Chemese, Norberto Peporine Lopesc,1, María G. Lagoriob,d,1, and Julián Faivovicha,f,1 aDivisión Herpetología, Museo Argentino de Ciencias Naturales “Bernardino Rivadavia”, Consejo Nacional de Investigaciones Científicas y Técnicas (CONICET), Ciudad de Buenos Aires C1405DJR, Argentina; bInstituto de Química Física de los Materiales, Medio Ambiente y Energía, CONICET, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad de Buenos Aires C1428EHA, Argentina; cFaculty of Pharmaceutical Sciences of Ribeirão Preto, University of São Paulo, Ribeirão Preto SP 14040-903, Brazil; dDepartamento de Química Inorgánica, Analítica y Química Física, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad de Buenos Aires C1428EHA, Argentina; eProtein Structure-Function and Engineering Laboratory, Fundación Instituto Leloir and Instituto de Investigaciones Bioquímicas de Buenos Aires-CONICET, Ciudad de Buenos Aires C1405BWE, Argentina; and fDepartamento de Biodiversidad y Biología Experimental, Facultad de Ciencias Exactas y Naturales, Universidad de Buenos Aires, Ciudad de Buenos Aires C1428EHA, Argentina Edited by Jerrold Meinwald, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY, and approved February 9, 2017 (received for review January 19, 2017) Fluorescence, the absorption of short-wavelength electromagnetic dorsum of living adults of both sexes (Fig. 1D). Both spectral radiation reemitted at longer wavelengths, has been suggested to profiles presented excitation maxima of 390−430 nm and emission play several biological roles in metazoans. This phenomenon is maxima at 450−470 nm (blue), with a shoulder at 505−515 nm uncommon in tetrapods, being restricted mostly to parrots and (green) giving an overall cyan coloration and showing no evident marine turtles.
    [Show full text]
  • How Photons Start Vision DENIS BAYLOR Department of Neurobiology, Sherman Fairchild Science Building, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305
    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 560-565, January 1996 Colloquium Paper This paper was presented at a coUoquium entitled "Vision: From Photon to Perception," organized by John Dowling, Lubert Stryer (chair), and Torsten Wiesel, held May 20-22, 1995, at the National Academy of Sciences in Irvine, CA. How photons start vision DENIS BAYLOR Department of Neurobiology, Sherman Fairchild Science Building, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305 ABSTRACT Recent studies have elucidated how the ab- bipolar and horizontal cells. Light absorbed in the pigment acts sorption of a photon in a rod or cone cell leads to the to close cationic channels in the outer segment, causing the generation of the amplified neural signal that is transmitted surface membrane of the entire cell to hyperpolarize. The to higher-order visual neurons. Photoexcited visual pigment hyperpolarization relays visual information to the synaptic activates the GTP-binding protein transducin, which in turn terminal, where it slows ongoing transmitter release. The stimulates cGMP phosphodiesterase. This enzyme hydrolyzes cationic channels in the outer segment are controlled by the cGMP, allowing cGMP-gated cationic channels in the surface diffusible cytoplasmic ligand cGMP, which binds to channels membrane to close, hyperpolarize the cell, and modulate in darkness to hold them open. Light closes channels by transmitter release at the synaptic terminal. The kinetics of lowering the cytoplasmic concentration of cGMP. The steps reactions in the cGMP cascade limit the temporal resolution that link light absorption to channel closure in a rod are of the visual system as a whole, while statistical fluctuations illustrated schematically in Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Crypsis Decreases with Elevation in a Lizard
    diversity Article Crypsis Decreases with Elevation in a Lizard Gregorio Moreno-Rueda * , Laureano G. González-Granda, Senda Reguera, Francisco J. Zamora-Camacho and Elena Melero Departamento de Zoología, Facultad de Ciencias, Universidad de Granada, E-18071 Granada, Spain; [email protected] (L.G.G.-G.); [email protected] (S.R.); [email protected] (F.J.Z.-C.); [email protected] (E.M.) * Correspondence: [email protected] Received: 7 November 2019; Accepted: 5 December 2019; Published: 7 December 2019 Abstract: Predation usually selects for visual crypsis, the colour matching between an animal and its background. Geographic co-variation between animal and background colourations is well known, but how crypsis varies along elevational gradients remains unknown. We predict that dorsal colouration in the lizard Psammodromus algirus should covary with the colour of bare soil—where this lizard is mainly found—along a 2200 m elevational gradient in Sierra Nevada (SE Spain). Moreover, we predict that crypsis should decrease with elevation for two reasons: (1) Predation pressure typically decreases with elevation, and (2) at high elevation, dorsal colouration is under conflicting selection for both crypsis and thermoregulation. By means of standardised photographies of the substratum and colourimetric measurements of lizard dorsal skin, we tested the colour matching between lizard dorsum and background. We found that, along the gradient, lizard dorsal colouration covaried with the colouration of bare soil, but not with other background elements where the lizard is rarely detected. Moreover, supporting our prediction, the degree of crypsis against bare soil decreased with elevation. Hence, our findings suggest local adaptation for crypsis in this lizard along an elevational gradient, but this local adaptation would be hindered at high elevations.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereopsis in Animals: Evolution, Function and Mechanisms Vivek Nityananda1,2,* and Jenny C
    © 2017. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2017) 220, 2502-2512 doi:10.1242/jeb.143883 REVIEW Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms Vivek Nityananda1,2,* and Jenny C. A. Read2 ABSTRACT In humans, stereopsis has become an attractive model system for Stereopsis is the computation of depth information from views understanding the link between neural activity and perception (Roe acquired simultaneously from different points in space. For many et al., 2007; Read, 2015a). We now have a good basic understanding years, stereopsis was thought to be confined to primates and other of the different processes of primate stereopsis and the brain areas mammals with front-facing eyes. However, stereopsis has now been involved (Cumming and DeAngelis, 2001). Yet we know little demonstrated in many other animals, including lateral-eyed prey about stereopsis in other species. Remarkably, stereopsis was not mammals, birds, amphibians and invertebrates. The diversity of demonstrated behaviourally in any non-human species until ’ animals known to have stereo vision allows us to begin to investigate 130 years after Wheatstone, with Bough s (1970) proof of ideas about its evolution and the underlying selective pressures in stereopsis in macaque monkeys. We now know that many different animals. It also further prompts the question of whether all different species have evolved some form of stereoscopic vision. animals have evolved essentially the same algorithms to implement However, with the exception of a few model taxa, including stereopsis. If so, this must be the best way to do stereo vision, and macaques, cats and barn owls, we know very little about the should be implemented by engineers in machine stereopsis.
    [Show full text]
  • How Photons Start Vision DENIS BAYLOR Department of Neurobiology, Sherman Fairchild Science Building, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305
    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 93, pp. 560-565, January 1996 Colloquium Paper This paper was presented at a coUoquium entitled "Vision: From Photon to Perception," organized by John Dowling, Lubert Stryer (chair), and Torsten Wiesel, held May 20-22, 1995, at the National Academy of Sciences in Irvine, CA. How photons start vision DENIS BAYLOR Department of Neurobiology, Sherman Fairchild Science Building, Stanford University School of Medicine, Stanford, CA 94305 ABSTRACT Recent studies have elucidated how the ab- bipolar and horizontal cells. Light absorbed in the pigment acts sorption of a photon in a rod or cone cell leads to the to close cationic channels in the outer segment, causing the generation of the amplified neural signal that is transmitted surface membrane of the entire cell to hyperpolarize. The to higher-order visual neurons. Photoexcited visual pigment hyperpolarization relays visual information to the synaptic activates the GTP-binding protein transducin, which in turn terminal, where it slows ongoing transmitter release. The stimulates cGMP phosphodiesterase. This enzyme hydrolyzes cationic channels in the outer segment are controlled by the cGMP, allowing cGMP-gated cationic channels in the surface diffusible cytoplasmic ligand cGMP, which binds to channels membrane to close, hyperpolarize the cell, and modulate in darkness to hold them open. Light closes channels by transmitter release at the synaptic terminal. The kinetics of lowering the cytoplasmic concentration of cGMP. The steps reactions in the cGMP cascade limit the temporal resolution that link light absorption to channel closure in a rod are of the visual system as a whole, while statistical fluctuations illustrated schematically in Fig.
    [Show full text]
  • Interspecific and Intraspecific Views of Color Signals in the Strawberry
    The Journal of Experimental Biology 207, 2471-2485 2471 Published by The Company of Biologists 2004 doi:10.1242/jeb.01047 Interspecific and intraspecific views of color signals in the strawberry poison frog Dendrobates pumilio Afsheen Siddiqi1, Thomas W. Cronin1,*, Ellis R. Loew2, Misha Vorobyev3 and Kyle Summers4 1Department of Biological Sciences, University of Maryland Baltimore County, Baltimore, MD 21250, USA, 2Department of Biomedical Sciences, Cornell University, Ithaca, NY 14853, USA, 3Vision Touch and Hearing Research Centre, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Queensland 4072, Australia and 4Department of Biology, East Carolina University, Greenville, NC 27858, USA *Author for correspondence (e-mail: [email protected]) Accepted 21 April 2004 Summary Poison frogs in the anuran family Dendrobatidae use background colors were obtained, and discrimination bright colors on their bodies to advertise toxicity. The among spectra in natural illuminants was mathematically species Dendrobates pumilio Schmidt 1858, the strawberry modeled. The results revealed that both D. pumilio and the poison frog, shows extreme polymorphism in color and model predator discriminate most colors quite well, both pattern in Panama. It is known that females of D. pumilio from each other and from typical backgrounds, with the preferentially choose mates of their own color morph. predator generally performing somewhat better than the Nevertheless, potential predators must clearly see and conspecifics. Each color morph displayed at least one color recognize all color morphs if the aposematic signaling signal that is highly visible against backgrounds to both system is to function effectively. We examined the ability visual systems. Our results indicate that the colors of conspecifics and a model predator to discriminate a displayed by the various color morphs of D.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereopsis in Animals: Evolution, Function and Mechanisms
    Nityananda V, Read JCA. Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms. Journal of Experimental Biology 2017, 220(14), 2502-2512 Copyright: © 2017. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltdhttp://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0. This is an Open Access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/3.0), which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed. DOI link to article: http://doi.org/10.1242/jeb.143883 Date deposited: 26/07/2017 This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 3.0 Unported License Newcastle University ePrints - eprint.ncl.ac.uk © 2017. Published by The Company of Biologists Ltd | Journal of Experimental Biology (2017) 220, 2502-2512 doi:10.1242/jeb.143883 REVIEW Stereopsis in animals: evolution, function and mechanisms Vivek Nityananda1,2,* and Jenny C. A. Read2 ABSTRACT In humans, stereopsis has become an attractive model system for Stereopsis is the computation of depth information from views understanding the link between neural activity and perception (Roe acquired simultaneously from different points in space. For many et al., 2007; Read, 2015a). We now have a good basic understanding years, stereopsis was thought to be confined to primates and other of the different processes of primate stereopsis and the brain areas mammals with front-facing eyes. However, stereopsis has now been involved (Cumming and DeAngelis, 2001). Yet we know little demonstrated in many other animals, including lateral-eyed prey about stereopsis in other species. Remarkably, stereopsis was not mammals, birds, amphibians and invertebrates.
    [Show full text]
  • Instruction Manual and Frog Survey Protocols
    Instruction Manual and Frog Survey Protocols for Region 1 National Wildlife Refuges, East-side Zone (Great Basin and Great Northern LCCs). October 2012 Chris Rombough TABLE OF CONTENTS Introduction 4 Names and Terms 7 Project Zone 9 Section 2. Surveys 2a. Conducting Surveys 11 2b. Breeding Surveys 14 2c. Tadpole Surveys 21 2d. Frog Surveys 24 Section 3. Data and Equipment 3a. Survey Data 35 3b. Organizing and Interpreting Data 41 3c. Survey Equipment 45 3d. Sources of Equipment 50 Section 4. Techniques 4a. Identification 52 Frog eggs 52 Tadpoles 54 Adult frogs and toads 57 Field guides and keys 59 4b. Capture and Handling 61 Adult frogs and toads 61 Tadpoles 67 Eggs 68 4c. Containers and Transport 69 4d. Photography 71 Adult frogs and toads 72 Tadpoles 78 Eggs 82 Habitat Photos 85 4e. Raising Tadpoles 86 4f. Collection and Preservation 88 Eggs 90 Tadpoles 92 Adult frogs and toads 94 Salvaging dead stuff 96 Additional reading 97 4g. Disinfection 99 2 Section 5. References References 101 Section 6. Appendices Appendix 1: Introduction to survey protocols 107 Appendix 2: Occurrence and detectability of east-side zone frog species 109 Appendix 3: General survey protocol for frogs and toads (in east-side zone of Region 112 Appendix 4: Oregon spotted frog 116 a) Species description and survey methods b) Survey protocol Appendix 5: Columbia spotted frog 126 a) Species description and survey methods b) Survey protocol Appendix 6: Northern leopard frog 140 a) Species description and survey methods b) Survey protocol Appendix 7: Western toad 150 a) Species description and survey methods b) Survey protocol Appendix 8: Data sheets 161 3 Introduction and Document Organization This Instruction Manual and accompanying protocols are designed to provide guidance to stations wishing to conduct frog surveys.
    [Show full text]
  • Three-Dimensional Camouflage: Exploiting Photons to Conceal Form
    Penacchio, O., Lovell, P. G., Cuthill, I. C., Ruxton, G. D., & Harris, J. M. (2015). Three-Dimensional Camouflage: Exploiting Photons to Conceal Form. American Naturalist, 186(4), 553-563. https://doi.org/10.1086/682570 Publisher's PDF, also known as Version of record Link to published version (if available): 10.1086/682570 Link to publication record in Explore Bristol Research PDF-document This is the final published version of the article (version of record). It first appeared online via University of Chicago Press at http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/682570. University of Bristol - Explore Bristol Research General rights This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies. Please cite only the published version using the reference above. Full terms of use are available: http://www.bristol.ac.uk/red/research-policy/pure/user-guides/ebr-terms/ vol. 186, no. 4 the american naturalist october 2015 Three-Dimensional Camouflage: Exploiting Photons to Conceal Form Olivier Penacchio,1,*P.GeorgeLovell,1,2 Innes C. Cuthill,3 Graeme D. Ruxton,4 and Julie M. Harris1 1. School of Psychology and Neuroscience, University of St. Andrews, South Street, St. Andrews, Fife KY16 9JP, United Kingdom; 2. Division of Psychology, Social and Health Sciences, Abertay University, Dundee DD1 1HG, United Kingdom; 3. School of Biological Sciences, University of Bristol, Life Sciences Building, 24 Tyndall Avenue, Bristol BS8 1TQ, United Kingdom; 4. School of Biology, University of St. Andrews, Dyers Brae, St. Andrews, Fife KY16 9TH, United Kingdom Submitted June 28, 2014; Accepted April 1, 2015; Electronically published August 20, 2015 Online enhancement: appendixes.
    [Show full text]
  • Are the Unken Reflex and the Aposematic Colouration of Red-Bellied Toads Efficient Against Bird Predation?
    RESEARCH ARTICLE Are the unken reflex and the aposematic colouration of Red-Bellied Toads efficient against bird predation? Debora Wolff Bordignon1*, Valentina Zaffaroni Caorsi1, Patrick Colombo2, Michelle Abadie3, Ismael Verrastro Brack3, Bibiana Terra Dasoler3, MaÂrcio Borges- Martins1 1 Programa de PoÂs-GraduacËão em Biologia Animal, Departamento de Zoologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil, 2 Museu de Ciências Naturais, FundacËão ZoobotaÃnica do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil, 3 Programa a1111111111 de PoÂs-GraduacËão em Ecologia, Instituto de Biociências, Universidade Federal do Rio Grande do Sul, Porto a1111111111 Alegre, Rio Grande do Sul, Brasil a1111111111 a1111111111 * [email protected] a1111111111 Abstract Aposematic signals as well as body behaviours may be important anti-predator defences. OPEN ACCESS Species of the genus Melanophryniscus are characterised by having toxic lipophilic alka- Citation: Bordignon DW, Caorsi VZ, Colombo P, loids in the skin and for presenting a red ventral colouration, which can be observed when Abadie M, Brack IV, Dasoler BT, et al. (2018) Are they perform the behaviour called the unken reflex. Both the reflex behaviour and the col- the unken reflex and the aposematic colouration of Red-Bellied Toads efficient against bird predation? ouration pattern are described as defence mechanisms. However, there are currently no PLoS ONE 13(3): e0193551. https://doi.org/ studies testing their effectiveness against predators. This study aimed to test experimentally 10.1371/journal.pone.0193551 if both ventral conspicuous colouration and the unken reflex in Melanophryniscus cambar- Editor: Daniel Osorio, University of Sussex, aensis function as aposematic signals against visually oriented predators (birds).
    [Show full text]
  • OPTICAL PROPERTIES of the PIGMENTS from CEPHALOPOD CHROMATOPHORES: SOLUTION and PARTICLE DETERMINATION Sean Ryan Dinneen University of New Hampshire, Durham
    University of New Hampshire University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository Doctoral Dissertations Student Scholarship Fall 2018 OPTICAL PROPERTIES OF THE PIGMENTS FROM CEPHALOPOD CHROMATOPHORES: SOLUTION AND PARTICLE DETERMINATION Sean Ryan Dinneen University of New Hampshire, Durham Follow this and additional works at: https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation Recommended Citation Dinneen, Sean Ryan, "OPTICAL PROPERTIES OF THE PIGMENTS FROM CEPHALOPOD CHROMATOPHORES: SOLUTION AND PARTICLE DETERMINATION" (2018). Doctoral Dissertations. 2423. https://scholars.unh.edu/dissertation/2423 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Student Scholarship at University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations by an authorized administrator of University of New Hampshire Scholars' Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. OPTICAL PROPERTIES OF THE PIGMENTS FROM CEPHALOPOD CHROMATOPHORES: SOLUTION AND PARTICLE DETERMINATION BY SEAN R. DINNEEN B.S., Eastern Nazarene College, 2014 DISSERTATION Submitted to the University of New Hampshire in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Chemistry September, 2018 This dissertation has been examined and approved in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Chemistry by: Dr. Margaret E. Greenslade, Associate Professor of Chemistry Dr. Leila F. Deravi, Assistant Professor of Chemistry and Materials Science, Northeastern University Dr. Sterling A. Tomellini, Professor of Chemistry Dr. Arthur Greenberg, Professor of Chemistry Dr. Richard M. Osgood III, Research Physicist, US Army Natick Soldier RD&E Center Defended On May 25th, 2018 Original approval signatures are on file with the University of New Hampshire Graduate School Dinneen | ii This dissertation is dedicated to my wife, Melanie Catherine Dinneen.
    [Show full text]