The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley,1 Elizabeth Phelps,1 and Mahzarin Banaji2

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley,1 Elizabeth Phelps,1 and Mahzarin Banaji2 CURRENT DIRECTIONS IN PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE The Neural Basis of Implicit Attitudes Damian Stanley,1 Elizabeth Phelps,1 and Mahzarin Banaji2 1New York University and 2Harvard University ABSTRACT—Evidence that human preferences, beliefs, and enable us to rapidly and efficiently react to simple sensory inputs behavior are influenced by sources that are outside the as well as to multifaceted experiences such as individuals, reach of conscious awareness, control, intention, and self- groups, objects, and events we encounter in our social worlds. reflection is incontrovertible. Recent advances in neu- Unlike other organisms, humans have the ability to introspec- roscience have enabled researchers to investigate the tively identify and even change the attitudes they hold. As Max neural basis of these implicit attitudes, particularly atti- Klinger of MnAnSnH said, ‘‘I used to be an atheist, but I gave it up tudes involving social groups. From this research, a model for lent.’’ This ability to examine the contents of our own minds with three identified neural components related to the and manipulate them is uniquely human. Its presence can automatic activation and regulation of implicit attitudes create the illusion that we control more about ourselves and our is beginning to emerge. The amygdala is implicated in the universe than we actually do, and that we know what our pref- automatic evaluation of socially relevant stimuli, while erences are and why we have them. Research in social psy- the anterior cingulate and dorsolateral prefrontal cortices chology has shown that, in addition to our conscious attitudes, are involved in the detection and regulation, respectively, we possess automatically triggered attitudes that can influence of implicit attitudes. Further support for this model comes behavior without our awareness (Fazio, Jackson, Dunton, & from the inclusion of these regions in current models con- Williams, 1995; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). Because these cerning the cognitive regulation of emotion and the detec- attitudes are ‘‘introspectively unidentified (or inaccurately tion of conflict. The identification of a putative neural identified)’’ (Greenwald & Banaji, 1995, p. 8), they are termed substrate for implicit attitudes has had a direct impact on implicit, thereby distinguishing them from explicit attitudes that psychological research into their nature and operational reflect our conscious thoughts and beliefs. Current models of characteristics. We discuss how this emerging neural model social cognition posit that human social behavior is the com- has influenced current research on implicit attitudes and bined result of these two related but distinct sets of attitudes. describe the importance of such models for directing future research. IMPLICIT ATTITUDES KEYWORDS—implicit attitude; implicit bias; social cogni- tion; social neuroscience; amygdala The distinction between implicit and explicit attitudes emerged only in the last 20 years, primarily motivated by findings from cognitive psychology demonstrating the influence of implicit Attitudes or preferences are a fundamental component of all memory and unconscious cognition on behavior (Greenwald & living systems. They orient the organism toward or away from Banaji, 1995; Bargh & Chartrand, 1999). While theoretically people, things, and events in the world. When plants orient to- driven, in practice the distinction between implicit and explicit ward the sun and flies flee from swatters, their behavior is based often ends up being a methodological one. Studies assessing on a relatively simple and automatic system of preferences (e.g., implicit processes use indirect measures over which the partic- tropisms, taxis) without which survival would be compromised. ipant has little control and of which they may be unaware. Many In humans, the levels of preferences and attitudes that guide such measures require participants to respond as quickly as behavior range from the simple to the highly complex. Attitudes possible, thereby limiting introspection (e.g., Fazio et al., 1995; Nosek, Greenwald, & Banaji, 2006). Others measure physiolog- ical responses, such as skin conductance or indicators of neural Address correspondence to Damian Stanley, Department of Psy- chology, New York University, 6 Washington Place, Rm 865, New activity, to assess participants’ automatic reactions to target York, NY 10003; e-mail: [email protected]. stimuli or concepts (e.g., Phelps et al., 2000). In contrast, studies 164 Copyright r 2008 Association for Psychological Science Volume 17—Number 2 Damian Stanley, Elizabeth Phelps, and Mahzarin Banaji assessing explicit processes usually require conscious delibera- nuclei located bilaterally in the anterior temporal lobe that has tion on the part of the participant and/or rely on self-report. been studied extensively with respect to implicit emotional Indirect measures have been used to assess a wide range of learning and memory, as well as with respect to the automatic implicit attitudes including, but not limited to, those toward effects of emotional stimuli upon cognition and perception (for race, gender, sexual orientation, age, religion, and political review, see Phelps & LeDoux, 2005). Structurally, the amygdala issues (Greenwald, Poehlman, Uhlmann, & Banaji, in press; is well situated to combine social and cognitive input and to Nosek et al., 2007). While informative about specific attitudes, modulate cognition and automatic aspects of behavior. Input to these data have also provided a wealth of information about the the amygdala is diverse, including contributions from low-level characteristics of implicit attitudes in general. Implicit attitudes sensory areas and from brain regions underlying memory and predict behavior such as choices, judgments, and nonverbal higher cognitive processes. Output from the amygdala is also behaviors toward members of other social groups, and in some widespread, ranging from the brain stem and hypothalamus to cases they can be more informative than explicit attitudes are early sensory cortices and cortical association areas. (Fazio et al., 1995; Greenwald et al., in press). In particular, A range of studies have demonstrated that, with its diverse measures of implicit attitudes outperform measures of explicit projections, the amygdala modulates a range of cognitive func- attitudes in socially sensitive domains (e.g., stereotyping and tions, such as memory and attention, as well as autonomic re- prejudice; Green et al., 2007). Implicit attitudes are triggered sponses, in the presence of emotionally salient stimuli (Phelps & automatically, often without awareness, in a manner that can LeDoux, 2005). In particular, the amygdala has been shown to be depend upon the particular social context (Blair, 2002; Fazio et critical for fear learning—that is, the implicit, physiological al., 1995; Greenwald & Banaji, 1995). In addition, they are expression of emotional responses to neutral stimuli that have pervasive, in that they are found across different demographic been paired with an aversive stimulus. Importantly, the amyg- groups and topics (Nosek et al., 2007). Implicit attitudes are dala is also recruited when fear learning is induced indirectly, related to explicit attitudes, but they are distinct in that neither is either through observation or instruction (Phelps & Banaji, robustly predictive of the other (Greenwald et al., in press; Nosek 2005; Phelps & LeDoux, 2005), indicating that it is sensitive et al., 2007). to the types of social cues imperative in the formation of implicit These qualities have led to interest in implicit attitudes and attitudes. The amygdala’s role in evaluative aspects of social their influence on behavior across disciplines as diverse as so- interactions is further supported by lesion and functional cial psychology, neuroscience, law, developmental psychology, imaging work in monkeys (Adolphs, 2003) and humans (Phelps and market research (Greenwald et al., in press; Nosek et al., & LeDoux, 2005). 2007; Phelps & Banaji, 2005). Accompanying this interest is a The amygdala was first linked to implicit attitudes in a study desire to better understand the underlying phenomena that give by Phelps et al. (2000). The authors used fMRI to measure blood- rise to implicit attitudes, as well as to learn how we might reg- oxygen-level dependent (BOLD) responses in White American ulate their impact on our own behavior. Neuroscientific tech- participants while they viewed faces of unfamiliar Black and niques such as functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) White males. In addition, they collected behavioral measures and electroencephalography (EEG) have enabled researchers to assessing the participants’ implicit and explicit race-related begin to elucidate the neural systems involved in the expression attitudes. The magnitude of the amygdala BOLD response when and regulation of implicit attitudes. Importantly, these tech- participants viewed Black (compared to White) faces was sig- niques make it possible not only to investigate the neural sub- nificantly correlated with measures of individuals’ implicit, but strate of implicit attitudes in humans but also to draw not explicit, race-related behavioral attitudes (see Fig. 1a). comparisons across species, allowing animal models to inform These data suggested that the amygdala plays a role in the research on implicit attitudes. The merging of neuroscience and evaluation of other social groups and that this role reflects in- psychology has influenced current theories
Recommended publications
  • The Contact Hypothesis Reevaluated1
    The Contact Hypothesis Reevaluated1 Elizabeth Levy Paluck2, Seth A. Green3, and Donald P. Green4 Abstract This paper evaluates the state of contact hypothesis research from a policy perspective. Building on Pettigrew and Tropp’s (2006) influential meta-analysis, we assemble all intergroup contact studies that feature random assignment and delayed outcome measures, of which there are 27 in total, nearly two-thirds of which were published following the original review. We find the evidence from this updated dataset to be consistent with Pettigrew and Tropp’s (2006) conclusion that contact “typically reduces prejudice." At the same time, our meta-analysis suggests that contact’s effects vary, with interventions directed at ethnic or racial prejudice generating substantially weaker effects. Moreover, our inventory of relevant studies reveals important gaps, most notably the absence of studies addressing adults’ racial or ethnic prejudice, an important limitation for both theory and policy. We also call attention to the lack of research that systematically investigates the scope conditions suggested by Allport (1954) under which contact is most influential. We conclude that these gaps in contact research must be addressed empirically before this hypothesis can reliably guide policy. What we have learned since Pettigrew and Tropp: A 10-year retrospective and update For more than a century, researchers have sought to understand what causes people to harbor and express prejudice against outgroups. Sustained attention to the topic of prejudice reflects the fact that in every era and region, stereotyping, discrimination, and 1 For invaluable research contributions, the authors thank Jason Chin and Kulani Dias. For detailed comments on their meta-analyses, we thank Linda Tropp, Thomas Pettigrew, Gunnar Lemmer, and Ulrich Wagner.
    [Show full text]
  • A Neglected Aspect of Conscience: Awareness of Implicit Attitudes
    Note: this is the pre-refereed draft version. Please find the final published version in Bioethics, Vol: 28:1, pp. 24-32 at DOI: 10.1111/bioe.12058 A neglected aspect of conscience: awareness of implicit attitudes The conception of conscience that dominates discussions in bioethics focuses narrowly on the regulation of behaviour resulting from explicit attitudes and neglects to mention implicit attitudes. But if conscience is a way of ensuring that a person’s behaviour is in line with her moral values, it must be responsive to all aspects of the mind that influence behaviour. There is a wealth of recent psychological work demonstrating the influence of implicit attitudes on behaviour. A necessary part of having a well-functioning conscience must thus be awareness and regulation of one’s implicit attitudes in addition to one’s explicit attitudes and resulting behaviour. On my revised conception of conscience, the ethical training necessary to ensure that health professionals develop and maintain a well- functioning conscience must involve cultivation of awareness of implicit attitudes and techniques to enable better control over them. This could be done through the methods of reflective practice in combination with workshops on implicit attitudes. If the function of conscience is to encourage a person to behave in accordance with her moral values, conscience must be responsive to all aspects of the mind that influence behaviour. Recent empirical work in psychology demonstrates the influence of implicit attitudes on behaviour and also emphasises the need for self-awareness relating to these aspects of one’s mind. Implicit attitudes are attitudes that are below the level of conscious awareness and thus seemingly not subject to direct conscious control nor to rational revision through reflection.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 the Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect Of
    1 The Psychology of Implicit Intergroup Bias and the Prospect of Change Calvin K. Lai1 & Mahzarin R. Banaji2 1 Washington University in St. Louis 2 Harvard University Last updated: June 24, 2020 Correspondence should be addressed to: Calvin Lai at [email protected]. Paper should be cited as: Lai, C. K., & Banaji, M. R. (2020). The psychology of implicit intergroup bias and the prospect of change. In D. Allen & R. Somanathan (Eds.), Difference without Domination: Pursuing Justice in Diverse Democracies. Chicago, IL: University of Chicago Press. 2 1. Introduction Over the course of evolution, human minds acquired the breathtaking quality of consciousness which gave our species the capacity to regulate behavior. Among the consequences of this capacity was the possibility of internal dialogue with oneself about the consistency between one’s intentions and actions. This facility to engage in the daily rituals of deliberative thought and action is so natural to our species that we hardly reflect on it or take stock of how effectively we are achieving the goal of intention-action consistency. We do not routinely ask at the end of each day how many of our actions were consistent with the values so many individuals hold: a belief in freedom and equality for all, in opportunity and access for all, in fairness in treatment and justice for all. Even if we wished to compute the extent to which we succeed at this task, how would we go about doing it? As William James (1904) pointed over a century ago, the difficulty of studying the human mind is that the knower is also the known, and this poses difficulties in accessing, in modestly objective fashion, the data from our own moral ledger.
    [Show full text]
  • A History of Implicit Social Cognition 1
    A History of Implicit Social Cognition 1 A History of Implicit Social Cognition: Where Is It Coming From? Where Is It Now? Where Is It Going? B. Keith Payne University of North Carolina, Chapel Hill Bertram Gawronski The University of Western Ontario Within the space of two decades, virtually every In this introduction, we aim to provide a context for intellectual question in social psychology, and many the chapters that follow by highlighting some of the outside of it, has been shaped by the theories and themes that keep surfacing in the field, tracing them to methods of implicit social cognition. Many of those their historical roots, and identifying emerging themes questions are pondered in this volume, involving the role that may guide future research. We hope that our of automatic/implicit/unconscious processes in attitudes historical synopsis will not only provide an orientation (Petty & Briñol, Chapter 18), social judgment and for the in-depth discussions of particular topics in the decision-making (Bodenhausen & Todd, Chapter 15), following chapters, but also circumscribe the goal pursuit (Ferguson & Porter, Chapter 17), prejudice characteristics that define implicit social cognition as a and stereotyping (Amodio & Mendoza, Chapter 19; particular way of studying, understanding, and Trawalter & Shapiro, Chapter 20), self-concepts and self- explaining human behavior. In addition, we hope that our esteem (Schnabel & Asendorpf, Chapter 22; Zeigler-Hill introduction helps illuminate the historical roots of & Jordan, Chapter 21), social-cognitive development previous and ongoing debates, which seems valuable for (Olson & Dunham, Chapter 13), romantic relationships critical appraisals of theoretical interpretations in (Baldwin, Lydon, McClure, & Etchison, Chapter 23), implicit social cognition.
    [Show full text]
  • Attitude, Inference, Association: on the Propositional Structure of Implicit Bias
    Attitude, Inference, Association Attitude, Inference, Association: On the Propositional Structure of Implicit Bias Eric Mandelbaum Department of Philosophy Baruch College, CUNY 17 Lexington Avenue New York, NY 10010 Email: [email protected]; [email protected] Phone: 908-770-0378 Homepage: http://people.fas.harvard.edu/~mandelbaum/ Keywords: Implicit Bias, Dual Process Theories, Cognitive Architecture, Implicit Attitudes, Unconscious Reasoning, Associative Learning 1 Attitude, Inference, Association Attitude, Inference, Association: On the Propositional Structure of Implicit Bias Abstract: The overwhelming majority of those who theorize about implicit biases posit that these biases are caused by some sort of association. However, what exactly this claim amounts to is rarely specified. In this paper, I distinguish between different understandings of association, and I argue that the crucial senses of association for elucidating implicit bias are the cognitive structure and mental process senses. A hypothesis is subsequently derived: if associations really underpin implicit biases, then implicit biases should be modulated by counterconditioning or extinction but should not be modulated by rational argumentation or logical interventions. This hypothesis is false; implicit biases are not predicated on any associative structures or associative processes but instead arise because of unconscious propositionally structured beliefs. I conclude by discussing how the case study of implicit bias illuminates problems with popular dual process models of cognitive architecture. 2 Attitude, Inference, Association Attitude, Inference, Association: On the Propositional Structure of Implicit Bias Implicit biases have received much attention, and for good reason: many pernicious and ubiquitous forms of prejudice are perpetuated because of them. A person with a strong implicit bias against African Americans is apt to smile less at them and to cut off conversations with them sooner (McConnell & Leibold 2001).
    [Show full text]
  • Attitudes As Global and Local Action Guides0
    ibits. inion tonal New Attitudes as Global and Local Action Guides0 ALISON LEDGERWOOD AND YAACOV THOPE \Ve often th ink of ou r att itudes ami beliefs as stahle personality chamcteristics­ wllen asked to describe ourselves, we might cite our love of a particular composer, oor support for a long-preferred political party, or perhaps a deep and ahiding hatred of Oreo cookies. Echoing this assumption, attitudes have historically been consid­ ered relatively stable individual differences that remain consistent across time and contexts, unless or until an overt persuasion attempt is encountered. However, more recently, a far more malleable pictu re of attitudes has clnerged from research sug­ gesting that evaluations can shift quite flexibly in response to the immed iate social environment (e.g., Baldwin & Holmes, 1987; Kawakami, Dovidio, & Dijksterhui s. 2003; Ledgerwood & Chaiken, 200i; Lowery, Hardin , & Sinclair, 200j ). In this chapter, we propose that these competing conceptualizations of attitudes as stable versus shifting llIay reflect two difTerent roles that attitudes play in regulat­ ing evaluative responding. On the one hand, att itudes can funct ion to gU ide action with respect to tile current social context. In order to act efTect ively and efficiently in the here and 1I0W , individuals need quick summaries of pertinent information to gUide their interactions with objects and people in the present situation. On the other hand, att itudes can function to guide action at a distance. \Vhen planning behavior in the distant future or making decisions about a faraway location, indi­ viduals need to he able to efficient ly abstract across the particularities of any one .
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes
    Psychological Review Copyright 1995 by the American Psychological Association, Inc. 1995, Vol. 102, No. 1,4-27 0033-295X/95/S3.00 Implicit Social Cognition: Attitudes, Self-Esteem, and Stereotypes Anthony G. Greenwald Mahzarin R. Banaji University of Washington Yale University Social behavior is ordinarily treated as being under conscious (if not always thoughtful) control. However, considerable evidence now supports the view that social behavior often operates in an implicit or unconscious fashion. The identifying feature of implicit cognition is that past experience influences judgment in a fashion not introspectively known by the actor. The present conclusion— that attitudes, self-esteem, and stereotypes have important implicit modes of operation—extends both the construct validity and predictive usefulness of these major theoretical constructs of social psychology. Methodologically, this review calls for increased use of indirect measures—which are imperative in studies of implicit cognition. The theorized ordinariness of implicit stereotyping is consistent with recent findings of discrimination by people who explicitly disavow prejudice. The finding that implicit cognitive effects are often reduced by focusing judges' attention on their judg- ment task provides a basis for evaluating applications (such as affirmative action) aimed at reducing such unintended discrimination. Long before they became central to other areas of psycholog- research, also has a long-established history (e.g., James, 1890; ical theory, concepts of cognitive mediation dominated the anal- see overview in Wylie, 1974, 1979). ysis of social behavior. The constructs on which this article fo- Through much of the period since the 1930s, most social psy- cuses achieved early prominence in social psychological theory chologists have assumed that attitudes, and to a lesser extent with formulations that were partly (attitude) or entirely (stereo- stereotypes, operate in a conscious mode.
    [Show full text]
  • Implicit Motivation to Control Prejudice
    Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Journal of Experimental Social Psychology 44 (2008) 164–172 www.elsevier.com/locate/jesp Implicit motivation to control prejudice a, b Jack Glaser ¤, Eric D. Knowles a Goldman School of Public Policy, University of California, 2607 Hearst Avenue, Berkeley, CA 94720-7320, USA b Department of Psychology and Social Behavior, University of California, Irvine, USA Received 16 August 2005; revised 15 December 2006 Available online 18 January 2007 Abstract This research examines whether spontaneous, unintentional discriminatory behavior can be moderated by an implicit (noncon- scious) motivation to control prejudice. We operationalize implicit motivation to control prejudice (IMCP) in terms of an implicit negative attitude toward prejudice (NAP) and an implicit belief that oneself is prejudiced (BOP). In the present experiment, an implicit stereotypic association of Blacks (vs. Whites) with weapons was positively correlated with the tendency to “shoot” armed Black men faster than armed White men (the “Shooter Bias”) in a computer simulation. However, participants relatively high in implicit negative attitude toward prejudice showed no relation between the race-weapons stereotype and the shooter bias. Implicit belief that oneself is prejudiced had no direct eVect on this relation, but the interaction of NAP and BOP did. Participants who had a strong association between self and prejudice (high BOP) but a weak association between prejudice and bad (low NAP) showed the strongest relation between the implicit
    [Show full text]
  • The Hidden Mechanisms of Prejudice: Implicit Bias & Interpersonal Fluency
    The Hidden Mechanisms of Prejudice: Implicit Bias & Interpersonal Fluency Alex Madva Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences COLUMBIA UNIVERSITY 2012 © 2012 Alex Madva All rights reserved Abstract The Hidden Mechanisms of Prejudice: Implicit Bias and Interpersonal Fluency Alex Madva This dissertation is about prejudice. In particular, it examines the theoretical and ethical questions raised by research on implicit social biases. Social biases are termed “implicit” when they are not reported, though they lie just beneath the surface of consciousness. Such biases are easy to adopt but very difficult to introspect and control. Despite this difficulty, I argue that we are personally responsible for our biases and obligated to overcome them if they can bring harm to ourselves or to others. My dissertation addresses the terms of their removal. It is grounded in a comprehensive examination of empirical research and, as such, is a contribution to social psychology. Although implicit social biases significantly influence our judgment and action, they are not reducible to beliefs or desires. Rather, they constitute a class of their own. Understanding their particular character is vital to determining how to replace them with more preferable habits of mind. I argue for a model of interpersonal fluency, a kind of ethical expertise that requires transforming our underlying dispositions of thought, feeling, and action. Table of Contents List of Figures……………………………………………………………………………………..v Acknowledgments………………………………………………………………………………..vi Dedication…………………………………………………………………………………………x Introduction………………………………………………………………………………………. 1 Chapter 1: The Structure of Implicit Social Attitudes…………………………………………...14 I. Introduction: Madeleine meets Bob…………………………………………………...14 II.
    [Show full text]
  • A Dual-Step and Dual-Process Model of Advertising Effects: Implications for Reducing
    A Dual-Step and Dual-Process Model of Advertising Effects: Implications for Reducing the Negative Impact of Advertising on Children’s Consumption Behaviour Oliver B. Büttner Arnd Florack Benjamin G. Serfas University of Vienna, Austria This manuscript is a final draft of a manuscript accepted for publication in the Journal of Consumer Policy: Büttner, O. B., Florack, A., & Serfas, B. G. (2014). A Dual-Step and Dual-Process Model of Advertising Effects: Implications for Reducing the Negative Impact of Advertising on Children’s Consumption Behaviour. Journal of Consumer Policy, 37(2), 161–182. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10603-013-9250-0 Author Note This research was supported by a Marie Curie FP7 Integration Grant within the 7th European Union Framework Programme to Oliver B. Büttner (FP7-PEOPLE-2011-CIG 293577). Correspondence should be addressed to Oliver B. Büttner, Applied Social Psychology and Consumer Research; University of Vienna, Universitätsstraße 7, 1010 Vienna, Austria. E-mail: [email protected] A Dual-Step and Dual-Process Model of Advertising Effects: Implications for Reducing the Negative Impact of Advertising on Children’s Consumption Behaviour Abstract Children are important targets of advertising campaigns from companies. However, children have been found to be particularly vulnerable to negative effects of advertising, and protecting children from these effects is an important task of consumer policy. Two important aspects have to be considered in this task. First, advertising affects judgments and behaviour not only during ad exposure, but also in delayed consumption and purchase contexts. Second, advertising thrives largely at an implicit level – during ad exposure as well as in consumption decisions.
    [Show full text]
  • A Cross Cultural Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Xenophobia 1
    A Cross Cultural Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Xenophobia 1 A Cross Cultural Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Xenophobia Lydia Keating Yale University April, 2017 Advised by John Dovidio, Carl I Hovland Professor of Psychology Submitted to the faculty of Cognitive Science in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Bachelor of Arts A Cross Cultural Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Xenophobia 2 Abstract The present research was composed of two distinct studies that examined the effects of cultural beliefs, implicit xenophobia, explicit xenophobia, and the suppression and rebound effects of implicit xenophobia in Indian and American populations. In Study 1, Indian participants revealed greater explicit xenophobia than American participants. In contrast, American and Indian participants both demonstrated comparable levels of xenophobia through the implicit association test. In Study 2, implicit xenophobia was not tested. The focus of Study 2 was to identity trends of individualist versus collectivist ideologies within cultures, the trends of differing explicit attitudes towards immigrants, and the relation of those attitudes to participants’ reactions to refined scenarios. Participants were asked to respond to more complex scenarios than were used in Study 1. The main findings of both studies suggest that Americans tend to underreport their xenophobia when explicitly asked about their feelings towards immigrants compared to Indian participants. However, Americans show their bias through in-group favoritism rather than negative out- group bias. The question of whether there are rebound effects of this underreporting remains unanswered based on the results of the present research and is an avenue for future research. A Cross Cultural Analysis of Implicit and Explicit Xenophobia 3 Contents 1 Introduction 1.1 Culture and cognition, individualism vs.
    [Show full text]
  • Conscious and Non-Conscious Bases of Social Judgment: Mindset and Implicit Attitudes in the Perception of Intergroup Conflict
    CONSCIOUS AND NON-CONSCIOUS BASES OF SOCIAL JUDGMENT: MINDSET AND IMPLICIT ATTITUDES IN THE PERCEPTION OF INTERGROUP CONFLICT by Susan D. Sullivan A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of The Charles E. Schmidt College of Science in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirement for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton, FL May 2013 CONSCIOUS AND NON-CONSCIOUS BASES OF SOCIAL JUDGMENT: MIND SET AND IMPLICIT ATTITUDES IN THE PERCEPTION OF INTERGROUP CONFLICT by Susan D. Sullivan This dissertation was prepared under the direction of the candidate's dissertation advisor, Dr. Robin R. Vallacher, Department of Psychology, and has been approved by the members of her supervision committee. It was submitted to the faculty of the Charles E. Schmidt College of Science and was accepted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. R. Vallacher, Dissertation Advisor ~ d L. lvdl,-- David L. Woigin, Ph.D. Chair, eR e of sychology - ~¢~ ~/ .".. ~,...-,- ------- College of Science Barry T. R son, Ph.D. Dean, Graduate College 11 ABSTRACT Author: Susan D. Sullivan Title: Conscious and Non-conscious Bases of Social Judgment: Mindset and Implicit Attitudes in the Perception of Intergroup Conflict Institution: Florida Atlantic University Dissertation Advisor: Dr. Robin R. Vallacher Degree: Doctor of Philosophy Year: 2013 Research on social judgment typically emphasizes one of three processes that enable unequivocal understanding of events with ambiguous causality. In the social influence perspective, people are susceptible to the interpretations offered by others. In the explicit attitudes perspective, people interpret events in line with their consciously held attitudes and values.
    [Show full text]