Mere Presence and Conformity Effects in Social Facilitation

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Mere Presence and Conformity Effects in Social Facilitation MERE PRESENCE AND CONFORMITY EFFECTS IN SOCIAL FACILITATION Bernard Guerfn B.A. (Hons) (Adelalde) Thesls submlÈÈed 1n fulfflmenÈ of Èhe requlrements for the Degree of DocÈor of Phllosophy ûlr/ûazQ / 7- ,1 ¿i/¡- Department of Psychology The Unlverslty of Adelalde August I 984 (1) TABLE OF EONTENTS PAGE SI¡MMARY (x) STATEMENT (xrf) NOTE (xitf) ACKNOTLEDGEMENTS (x1v ) CHAPTER I. OVERVIEW I CHAPTER II. THE HISTOR OF SOCIAL FACILITATION RESEARCTI 5 II. l. THE EARLY hIORK OF TRIPLETT 5 II. 2. THE EXPERIMENTAL STUDIES OF THE 192OIS ll II. 3. CONCEPTUAL AND EXPERIMENTAL ADVANCES uP TO 1964 2T T.T. 4. CONCLUSION 29 rr. 5. THE DRrVE MODEL OF ZAJONC (1965) 30 II. 5.1. An outllne of ZaJonc (1965) 30 II. 5.2. Crltical dlecussÍon of ZaJonc ( 1e65) 34 II. 5.2.1. The drlve construct and the domlnance hferachy of reaponse9 34 II. 5.2.2. The aroueal construct 37 II. 5.2.3. The conatruct of mere pre6ence 42 II. 5.3. Conclusfon 44 CHAPTER III. THEORIES OF SOCIAL FACILITATION 4t III. 1. INTRODUCTION 45 III. 2. DRIVE AND AROUSAL THEORIES 47 III. 2.1. Evaluatlon apprehenefon 47 III. 2.2. lúere presence effecte, fear and alertnese 49 (ff) Ghapter III (conttnued) PAGE III. 2.2.1. Alertneea to eocfal and non-soclal stimull 49 III. 2.2.2. Monit,orabfllÈy and alertneee 32 III. 2.2.3. Predlctlons of mere presence effecte 54 III. 3. SOCTAL CONFORMITY THEORIES 56 III. 3.1. Evaluatlon apprehenefon 57 IfI. 3.2. ObJecÈ1ve Self-Awarenege 58 III. 3.3. Control Syetems Model 59 III. 3.3.1. The model of Carver & Schefer ( 1978) 59 III. 3.3.2. Crltlcal dlecuesfon 61 III. 3.4. Self-Presentatfon and other functlonal models of soclal facf11Èatlon 63 rrr.3.5. Social conformlty and evaluatfon 66 III. 4. ATTENTTON PROCESSES 68 rrr. 4. I . Physical dlstractlon 68 rrt.4.2. Cognitlve dfstraction 69 rrr.4.3. Diatract fon-Conf llct 69 frr.4.4. Attentfonal proceee model 7l rrr.4.5. Arouaal effects uPon attentÍon 72 rrr.4.6. Manetead & Semln (198O) 73 III. 4.7. An lnformatlon proceaelng vlew 73 rrr. 5. DRTVE, SOCTAL CONFORI{rTY AND ATTENTTONAL EFFECTS 75 III. 6. CONCLUSIONS 77 CHAPTER IV. REVIEI.J OF EXPERIMENTAL STUDTES 78 IV. I. INT-N.ODUCTION 78 (f11) Chapter IV (contlnued) PAGE IV. 2. INITIAL REVIEI., OF STUDIES 79 IV. 3. MERE PRESENCE STUDIES 86 IV. 4. THE EFFECTS OF THE PRESENCE OF THE EXPERIMENTER 92 IV. 5. THE EFFECT OF AN OBSERVER 99 IV. 5.1. The SÈudies 99 IV. 5.2. The Theorfes l05 rv. 6. coNcl.usroNs LO7 CHAPTER V. THE EFFECT OF MERE PRESENCE ON MOTOR PERFORMANCE. AND WORD ASSOCIATTONS l09 V. I. INTRODUCTION l09 v.2 EXPERIMENT l: SOCIAL FACTLITATION OF A MOTOR RESPONSE ttl V. 2.1. Method tlr V. 2.1.1. SubJects ltt V. 2.L.2. Apparatus lll V. 2.1.3. Procedure Lt2 V. 2.2. Results 113 V. 2.2.1. Experlmenter Effects 113 V. 2.2.2. Taak Perforrnance 114 V. 2.2.3. Self-Report Measurea tl4 V.2.3. Dfecuaslon r15 V. 3. EXPERIMENT 2¡ WORD ASSOCIATION TEST 117 V. 3.1. Method L22 V.3.1.1. SubJecta r22 V. 3.1.2. Apparatua t22 V. 3.1.3. Procedure 123 V.3.2. Reeulte 124 ( rv) Chapter IV (Contlnued) PAGE v 3.2.1. Commonnese Scores L25 v 3.2.2. Latency Scores L27 v 3.2.3. Self-Report Measuree t29 V.3.3. DÍscueglon 130 V. 4. GENERAL DISCUSSTON 133 CHAPTER VI. MONITORABILITY AND F ARITY AS MEDIATORS OF MERE PRES ENCE EFFECTS 135 VI. 1. INTRODUCTION 135 vr. 2. EXPERIMENT 3: TTTE ROLE OF MONITORABILITY rN AN AUDIENCE SETTING 136 VI. 2.1. Introductfon 136 VI.2.2. Method 137 VI. 2.2.1. SubJects L37 VI. 2.2.2. Apparatus L37 VI. 2.2.3. Procedure 140 VI. 2.3. Results 143 VI. 2.3.1. Practlce Measures L43 VI. 2.3.2. Task Performance Meaeures 143 VI. 2.3.3. Self-Report Meaaures r46 VI. 2.3.4. Vldeo Measures L49 VI.2.4. Dfecusslon 155 VI. 3. EXPERIMEMT 4: THE ROLE oF FAI'IILIARITY IN COACTION AND AUDIENCE SETTINGS 158 VI. 3.1. Introductlon r58 vI. 3.2. Method r59 VI. 3.2.L. SubJecte 159 VI . 3.2.2. Apparatus 159 vI. 3.2.3. Procedure 160 (v) Chapter VI (Continued) PAGE VI. 3.3. Results and Diecusalon 163 VI. 3.3.1. Prellmfnary CornmenÈs 163 VI. 3.3.2. Task Perfornance r65 VI. 3.3.3. Self-Report Measures r69 VI. 4. GENERAL DISCUSSION t72 VI. 4.I Mere Preeence Effects L72 VT. 4.2. Self-Report Meaeures 174 VI. 4.3. Behavlour Inhlbftfon 174 CHAPTER VIT. BEHAVIOUR INHIBITION IN THE PRESENCE OF ANOTHER PERSOI¡ t76 VII. 1. INTRODUCTION 176 VII. 2. EXPERIMENT 5: TIIE EFFECT OF EVALUATION AND MERE PRESENCE ON TASK PERFORMANCE AND BEHAVIOUR INHIBITION t78 VfI.2.l. Introductfon 178 VII. 2.2. MeÈhod 179 VII.2.2.L. SubJects 179 VII. 2.2.2. Apparatus L79 VII. 2.2.3. Procedure 180 vII . 2.3. Reeult,e ra2 VII. 2.3.1. Task Performance Meaguree 182 VII. 2.3.2. Vldeo Measures 182 VII. 2.3.3. Self-Report Measuree 186 VfI. 2.4. Dlecussfon L87 VII.3 EXPERIMENT 6: A FIETD TEST OF BEHAVIOUR INHIBITION 189 vII. 3.1. IntroductLon 189 VII. 3.2. Method 190 (vi ) Chapter VII (Contlnued) PAGE VfI. 3 .2.1. SubJecta 190 VII. 3.2.2. Procedure 190 VII.3.3. Results 191 VTI. 4. GENERAL DISCUSSION OF BEHAVIOUR INHIBITION I92 CHAPTER VIII. lHE MEDIATION OF MERE PRESENCE EFF'ECTS BY MONTTORABILITY t97 VIII. l. EXPERIMENT 7: INTRODUCTION t97 VIII. 2. METHOD 199 VIII. 2.1. Subjects 199 VIII. 2.2. Procedure 199 VIII. 3. RESULTS 202 VIII. 3.I. Taek Performance Reeults 202 VIII. 3.2. Self-Report Measuree 202 VIII. 4. DISCUSSION 207 CHAPTER IX. SI.JMMARY AND CONCLUSTONS 209 IX. I. THE THEORIES 209 rx. 2 THE EXPERIMENTAL EVIDENCE 2t3 IX. 2.1 Mere Preeence effecte 2L3 IX. 2.2 Self-reporÈ mea€urea 2L7 IX. 2.3 Behavlour Inhlbltfon 2t9 rx. 3 CONCLUSIONS 220 APPENDICES 223 REFERENCES 235 (vf1) LISÎ OF TABLES PAGE Table I SEudfes rejecÈed from revfew. 82 Table 2 StudÍes lncluded 1n revlew. 88 Table 3. Mere presence sEudfes. 90 Table 4 Effects of the presence of the experlmenter. 93 Table 5 Effects of the presence of observers. too Table 6 Means and standard devlatlons of all measures across soclal conditions for Experiment 2. r26 Table 7 Means and sEandard devÍatfons of errors fn pracÈice trials of Experiment 3. r44 Tab1e I Means and st,andard devfaElons of errors per word paír 1n test trlal of Experiment 3. t44 Table 9 Means for body movemenEs, smalI movemenEs and vocallzatlons, for practice trials combinlng slmple and complex tasks 1n Experiment 3. r52 Table lO. Means for body movements, small movemenÈ,s and vocallzatlons, for the test trlals fn ExperlmenÈ 3. 152 Table 11. Means, standard deviatlons and sample sizes of the number of letEers copied 1n Experiment 4. t64 Table 12. Means (logs) and sample sfzes of t.he number of letters copied fn Experiment 4. 166 Table 13. Means of self-reporÈed arousal and dlsÈractlon 1n Experiment 4. r66 Table t4. Means and standard devfations of the number of correctly recalled dlgits 1n Experlment 5. t84 Table 15. Means for body movemenEs, small movementa and vocallzatlons Ín Experiment 5. 184 Table 16. Mean number of body movements and small movements 1n Experiment 6. 193 Table 17. Means and standard devfat,iona of Ehe number of lett.ers copied ln ExperÍment 7. 203 Table 18. Means of self-reporÈed disturbance, threat and evaluation 1n Experfment 7 203 ( v11f ) LIST OF FIGURES Page Figure I Tfme in seconds per trfal for turnfng reels alone (A) or 1n competitLon (C) for groups A and B (daÈa from Trlplet,t, 1898' p.521). I Flgure 2 Average number of assocfaÈlons worklng alone or t,ogether for each of 3 mlnuÈes (data from Allport, l920r p.165). t2 Ffgure 3. Cumulatlve graph of the number of social facllitation experimental studles from 1898 Èo 1983. Astertsks mark the lnfluentlal papers of Allport (1920) and ZaJonc ( 1965). l8 Figure 4. Graph of the number of dtglts recalled after 2 mfnuÈes or 45 minut,es uslng data from three studfes whfch used Èhe Bame procedure ( l- Deffenbacher et al., L974i 2-Geenr 1973¡ l- Geen, L974. Dat,a used by permlsslon of authors). 97 Flgure 5 Plan of experimenEal and recordfng rooms for Experfment 3. 139 Figure 6 Mean errors per word pair across socfal condftlon for the simple and complex tasks for ExperlmenÈ 3. t45 Flgure 7. Means for body movements across socfal condition wlth slmple or complex task for ExperÍment 3. r45 ( Íx) LIST OF FIGURES (conÈlnued) PAGE FÍgure I Means for emall movements across socfal conditlons with slmple or complex tasks for Experfment, 3. 153 Flgure 9 Mean vocallzatfons across social condltÍons wÍth slrnple or complex tasks for ExperfmenÈ 3 ts3 Figure lO. Mean number of letters copled (logs) by type of presence and famllfarlty for Experfment 4 168 Figure I 1. Mean self-report of arousal by taek complexfEy and famlllarlty for ExperfmenE 4 168 Figure 12. Means for body movements, small movements and vocalfzations across eocial condltfons for ExperÍment 5. 185 Flgure 13. Mean body movement,s slone and with presence for the FronE and Behind condltlons for Experiment 6. L94 Figure 14. Mean smalÌ movements alone and wfth presence for the FronE and Behl-nd conditions for Experfment 6 L94 Ffgure t5.
Recommended publications
  • Social Facilitation: Salience and Mediated, Anticipatory, And
    SOCIAL FACILITATION: SALIENCE AND MEDIATED, ANTICIPATORY, AND RESIDUAL PRESENCE By JASON MATTHEW GLUSHAKOW A dissertation submitted to the Graduate School- New Brunswick Rutgers, the State University of New Jersey in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Program in Psychology written under the direction of John R. Aiello, Ph.D. and approved by ___________________________________ ___________________________________ ___________________________________ ___________________________________ New Brunswick, New Jersey October, 2011 Abstract of the Dissertation Social Facilitation: Salience and Mediated, Anticipatory, and Residual Presence by Jason M. Glushakow Dissertation director: Dr. John R. Aiello Social facilitation is the oldest experimental concept in social psychology. Throughout decades of social facilitation research, social presence has typically been viewed as a dichotomous variable which affects individuals while performing a task. However, this dissertation attempts to investigate whether social presence may be viewed as continuous variable, differing on the salience of presence. In addition, it seeks to determine whether changes in performance can be elicited by prior social presence (residual presence) and the expectation of future social presence (anticipatory presence). Study 1 compared the effects of 6 different levels of presence (absence, artificial presence, passive presence, implied presence, embodied presence, and active presence) on simple and complex task performance. It provides evidence that the various levels of social presence did have a differential impact on participants. From the questionnaire responses, social presence was able to be classified into three distinct categories: low (absence, artificial and passive presence), intermediate (implied presence), and high salience (embodied and active presence). Study 1 also provides some support for the expectation that presence results in simple task facilitation.
    [Show full text]
  • DESIGNING PERSUASIVE SYSTEMS for USER ENGAGEMENT in COLLABORATIVE INTERACTION Agnis Stibe University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland, [email protected]
    Association for Information Systems AIS Electronic Library (AISeL) ECIS 2014 Proceedings DESIGNING PERSUASIVE SYSTEMS FOR USER ENGAGEMENT IN COLLABORATIVE INTERACTION Agnis Stibe University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland, [email protected] Harri Oinas-Kukkonen University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://aisel.aisnet.org/ecis2014 Agnis Stibe and Harri Oinas-Kukkonen, 2014, "DESIGNING PERSUASIVE SYSTEMS FOR USER ENGAGEMENT IN COLLABORATIVE INTERACTION", Proceedings of the European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS) 2014, Tel Aviv, Israel, June 9-11, 2014, ISBN 978-0-9915567-0-0 http://aisel.aisnet.org/ecis2014/proceedings/track07/3 This material is brought to you by the European Conference on Information Systems (ECIS) at AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). It has been accepted for inclusion in ECIS 2014 Proceedings by an authorized administrator of AIS Electronic Library (AISeL). For more information, please contact [email protected]. DESIGNING PERSUASIVE SYSTEMS FOR USER ENGAGEMENT IN COLLABORATIVE INTERACTION Stibe, Agnis, Oulu Advanced Research on Software and Information Systems, Department of Information Processing Science, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland, [email protected] Oinas-Kukkonen, Harri, Oulu Advanced Research on Software and Information Systems, Department of Information Processing Science, University of Oulu, Oulu, Finland, [email protected] Abstract Social influence concepts have great potential to positively affect the behaviors and attitudes of individuals. Drawing on socio-psychological theories, this study explores how social influence design principles alter user engagement in collaborative interaction during public events. Based on a theory- driven research model, a persuasive information system comprising social influence design principles of cooperation, social learning, and social facilitation was implemented and examined with a sample of 101 participants.
    [Show full text]
  • 11 Social Influence
    9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 216 Social Influence 11 Miles Hewstone and Robin Martin KEY CONCEPTS autokinetic effect compliance consistency conversion deindividuation door-in-the-face technique evaluation apprehension foot-in-the-door technique group polarization groupthink informational influence lowballing technique majority influence (conformity) minority influence (innovation) norms normative influence obedience to authority referent informational influence self-categorization theory social comparison social facilitation social influence whistleblowing 9781405124003_4_011.qxd 10/31/07 3:09 PM Page 217 CHAPTER OUTLINE This chapter considers two main types of social influence, both of which can be understood in terms of fundamental motives. First, we discuss ‘incidental’ social influence, where people are influenced by the presence or implied presence of others, although there has been no explicit attempt to influence them. We consider the impact of the mere presence of other people on task performance, and the impact of social norms. In the second part of the chapter, we ask why people succumb to social influence, highlighting types of social influence and motives underlying influence on the part of the target of influence. In the third part of the chapter, we turn to ‘deliberate’ social influence. We introduce theory and research on compliance, the influence of numerical majorities and minorities, group decision-making and obedience. Throughout we will see that social influence is an ambivalent concept. On the one hand, it is the glue of society: it makes things work, and society would be utterly chaotic without it. But on the other hand it can be a dark force, underlying some of the most extreme, even immoral, forms of human social behaviour.
    [Show full text]
  • Women's Stereotype Threat‐Based Performance Motivation And
    This is a repository copy of Women's stereotype threat‐ based performance motivation and prepotent inhibitory ability. White Rose Research Online URL for this paper: http://eprints.whiterose.ac.uk/138164/ Version: Accepted Version Article: Hutter, RRC orcid.org/0000-0001-7622-2273, Davies, LC, Sedikides, C et al. (1 more author) (2019) Women's stereotype threat‐ based performance motivation and prepotent inhibitory ability. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58 (3). pp. 691-713. ISSN 0144-6665 https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12298 © 2018 The British Psychological Society. This is the pre-peer reviewed version of the following article: Hutter, RRC , Davies, LC, Sedikides, C et al. (1 more author) (2019) Women's stereotype threat‐ based performance motivation and prepotent inhibitory ability. British Journal of Social Psychology, 58 (3). pp. 691-713, which has been published in final form at https://doi.org/10.1111/bjso.12298. This article may be used for non-commercial purposes in accordance with Wiley Terms and Conditions for Use of Self-Archived Versions. Reuse Items deposited in White Rose Research Online are protected by copyright, with all rights reserved unless indicated otherwise. They may be downloaded and/or printed for private study, or other acts as permitted by national copyright laws. The publisher or other rights holders may allow further reproduction and re-use of the full text version. This is indicated by the licence information on the White Rose Research Online record for the item. Takedown If you consider content in White Rose Research Online to be in breach of UK law, please notify us by emailing [email protected] including the URL of the record and the reason for the withdrawal request.
    [Show full text]
  • Religiosity, Prosocial Values, and Adjustment Among Students in Mennonite High Schools in Winnipeg
    Religiosity, Prosocial Values, and Adjustment among Students in Mennonite High Schools in Winnipeg Eduard Schludermanm, Shirin SchPudermann, and Cam-loi Huynh, University of Manitoba The Consequences of Religious Commitment Religious groups, such as the Mennonites, have established religious schools in order to promote religious commitment1 on the premise that the Christian religion is not only true, but also beneficial to the individual and to society (e.g., Friesen, 1983; Kraybill, 1978). Even though this premise may seem almost self evident to Mennonite religious and educational leaders, such a premise has been rejected by many educators, social scientists and psychologists who have argued that religious commitment has bad effects and therefore harms both students and society. For instance: Dittes (1969:637-641) argued that compared to nonreligious persons, religious persons are characterized by more personal inadequacy (e.g., low intelligence, high suggestibility, wealc egos, fewer friends). Wulff (1991:309) argued that religious commitment tends to be associated with poor mental health and poor personal adjustment. Others have argued that compared to nonreligious 192 Journal ofMennonite Studies persons, religious persons endorse harmful social values, such as lack of social compassion (Rokeach, 1969) or right-wing authoritarianism (Altemeyer, 1988:230). These views have been widely accepted and quoted in surveys of the psychology of religion (e.g., Batson, Schoenrade and Ventis, 1993:234;Wulff, 1991:309,225-226).Needless to say, such persons see little value in schools which promote religious commitment in their students (see Thiessen, 1993). There is thus a wide spectrum of opinion as to whether religious commitment promotes or hinders good social and personal adjustment.
    [Show full text]
  • Stereotype Threat • Explore Related Articles • Search Keywords Steven J
    PS67CH17-Spencer ARI 14 November 2015 8:18 ANNUAL REVIEWS Further Click here to view this article's online features: • Download figures as PPT slides • Navigate linked references • Download citations Stereotype Threat • Explore related articles • Search keywords Steven J. Spencer,1 Christine Logel,2 and Paul G. Davies3 1Department of Psychology, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1; email: [email protected] 2Renison University College, University of Waterloo, Waterloo, Ontario, Canada, N2L 3G1; email: [email protected] 3Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Kelowna, British Columbia, Canada, V1V 1V7; email: [email protected] Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016. 67:415–37 Keywords First published online as a Review in Advance on stereotype threat, social identity, identity safety, stereotypes, prejudice, September 10, 2015 discrimination The Annual Review of Psychology is online at psych.annualreviews.org Abstract This article’s doi: When members of a stigmatized group find themselves in a situation where 10.1146/annurev-psych-073115-103235 Annu. Rev. Psychol. 2016.67:415-437. Downloaded from www.annualreviews.org negative stereotypes provide a possible framework for interpreting their be- Access provided by Ohio State University Library on 02/14/17. For personal use only. Copyright c 2016 by Annual Reviews. havior, the risk of being judged in light of those stereotypes can elicit a dis- All rights reserved ruptive state that undermines performance and aspirations in that domain. This situational predicament, termed stereotype threat, continues to be an intensely debated and researched topic in educational, social, and organiza- tional psychology. In this review, we explore the various sources of stereotype threat, the mechanisms underlying stereotype-threat effects (both mediators and moderators), and the consequences of this situational predicament, as well as the means through which society and stigmatized individuals can overcome the insidious effects of stereotype threat.
    [Show full text]
  • Intelligent Social Learning Rosaria Conte National Research Council, Institute of Psychology, V.LE Marx 15, 00137 Roma
    Intelligent Social Learning Rosaria Conte National Research Council, Institute of Psychology, V.LE Marx 15, 00137 Roma. Division "AI, Cognitive and Interaction Modelling" & University of Siena - Communication Sciences - "Social Psychology" PSS (Project on Social Simulation) - voice:+39+06+86090210;fax:+39+06+824737 email: [email protected] - http://ip.rm.cnr.it Abstract One of the cognitive processes responsible for social propagation is social learning, broadly meant as the process by means of which agents' acquisition of new information is caused or favoured by their being exposed to one another in a common environment. Social learning results from one or other of a number of social phenomena, the most important of which are social facilitation and imitation. In this paper, a general notion of social learning will be defined and the main processes which are responsible for it, namely social facilitation and imitation, will be analysed in terms of the social mental processes they require. A brief analysis of classical definitions of social learning is carried on, showing that a systematic and consistent treatment of this notion is still missing. A general notion of social learning is then introduced and the two main processes which may lead to it, social facilitation and imitation, will be defined as different steps on a continuum of cognitive complexity. Finally, the utility of the present approach is discussed. The analysis presented in this paper draws upon a cognitive model of social action (cf. Conte & Castelfranchi, 1995; Conte, 1999). The agent model which will be referred to throughout the paper is a cognitive model, endowed with mental properties for pursuing goals and intentions, and for knowledge-based action.
    [Show full text]
  • The Audience Effect in Adolescence Depends on Who's Looking Over Your Shoulder
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Birkbeck Institutional Research Online Journal of Adolescence 43 (2015) 5e14 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Adolescence journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jado The audience effect in adolescence depends on who's looking over your shoulder * Laura K. Wolf a, , Narges Bazargani b, Emma J. Kilford a, Iroise Dumontheil a, c, Sarah-Jayne Blakemore a a UCL Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, 17 Queen Square, London WC1N 3AR, UK b UCL Department of Neuroscience, Physiology and Pharmacology, Gower St, London WC1E 6BT, UK c Department of Psychological Sciences, Birkbeck, University of London, Malet Street, London WC1E 7HX, UK article info abstract Article history: Adolescents have been shown to be particularly sensitive to peer influence. However, the Available online data supporting these findings have been mostly limited to the impact of peers on risk- taking behaviours. Here, we investigated the influence of peers on performance of a Keywords: high-level cognitive task (relational reasoning) during adolescence. We further Adolescence assessed whether this effect on performance was dependent on the identity of the fl Peer in uence audience, either a friend (peer) or the experimenter (non-peer). We tested 24 younger Audience effect adolescent (10.6e14.2 years), 20 older adolescent (14.9e17.8 years) and 20 adult (21.8 Reasoning e Development 34.9 years) female participants. The presence of an audience affected adolescent, but not adult, relational reasoning performance. This audience effect on adolescent performance was influenced by the participants' age, task difficulty and the identity of the audience.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Stereotype Threat on the Solving of Quantitative GRE Problems: a Mere Effort Interpretation
    Pers Soc Psychol Bull OnlineFirst, published on May 6, 2009 as doi:10.1177/0146167209335165 The Effect of Stereotype Threat on the Solving of Quantitative GRE Problems: A Mere Effort Interpretation Jeremy P. Jamieson Stephen G. Harkins Northeastern University The mere effort account argues that stereotype threat ways—cognitively, affectively, and motivationally” (p. 397). motivates participants to want to perform well, which Consistent with this view, Schmader, Johns, and Forbes potentiates prepotent responses. If the prepotent response (2008) have identified working memory “as a core cog- is correct, performance is facilitated. If incorrect and nitive faculty that is implicated in cognitive and social participants do not know, or lack the knowledge or time stereotype threat effects” (p. 337). Research has also required for correction, performance is debilitated. The demonstrated the role of affective processes (e.g., anxi- Graduate Record Examinations (GRE) quantitative test ety; Bosson, Haymovitz, & Pinel 2004). In the current is made up of two problem types: (a) solve problems, work, we focus on the contribution of motivation to which require the solution of an equation, and (b) com- threat effects, specifically on Jamieson and Harkins’s parison problems, which require the use of logic and esti- (2007) mere effort account. mation. Previous research shows that the prepotent The mere effort account was suggested by Harkins’s tendency is to attempt to solve the equations. Consistent (2006) analysis of the effect of evaluation on performance. with mere effort predictions, Experiment 1 demonstrates This account argues that evaluation motivates partici- that threatened participants perform better than con- pants to want to perform well, which potentiates whatever trols on solve problems (prepotent response correct) but response is prepotent, or most likely to be produced in a worse than controls on comparison problems (prepotent given situation.
    [Show full text]
  • Components of Social Facilitation, Home Field Advantage, and Home Field Disadvantage and Their Effects on Sport Performance
    University of Montana ScholarWorks at University of Montana Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers Graduate School 1994 Components of social facilitation, home field advantage, and home field disadvantage and their effects on sport performance Scott Meissner The University of Montana Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd Let us know how access to this document benefits ou.y Recommended Citation Meissner, Scott, "Components of social facilitation, home field advantage, and home field disadvantage and their effects on sport performance" (1994). Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers. 1876. https://scholarworks.umt.edu/etd/1876 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at ScholarWorks at University of Montana. It has been accepted for inclusion in Graduate Student Theses, Dissertations, & Professional Papers by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks at University of Montana. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Dav\0 SCbTT MGISSaJBR^ Maureen and Mike MANSFIELD LIBRARY TheMontana University of Permission is granted by the author to reproduce this material in its entirety, provided that this material is used for scholarly purposes and is properly cited in published works and reports. ** Please check "Yes " or "No " and provide signature^"^ Yes, I grant permission _y^ No, I do not grant permission Author's Signature _ Date: \'Z\- Any f-r commercial purposes or financial gain may be undertaken explicit consent. COMPONENTS OF SOCIAL FACILITATION, HOME FIELD ADVANTAGE, AND HOME FIELD DISADVANTAGE AND THEIR EFFECTS ON SPORT PERFORMANCE by Scott Meissner Bachelor of Science, Western Montana College, 1988 Presented in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science University of Montana 1993 Approved by Chairman Board Examiners ^ Dean, Graduate School Date UMI Number: EP35163 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted.
    [Show full text]
  • The Dynamic of Infaq and Sadaqah: According to the Social Facilitation Theory
    The Dynamic of Infaq and Sadaqah: According to the Social Facilitation Theory Nelly Marhayati [email protected] State Islamic Institute of Bengkulu Abstract. The present study purpose to examine the individual dynamic of infaq and sadaqah according to social facilitation theory. Infaq and sadaqah referred to charity that was considered as vertical relationship between God and human, and it was Sunnah for both terms. Some Muslims find difficulties and do without sincerity when perform these deeds and acts, which is also similar to others worship. According to social facilitation theory, individual performance will enhance as another individual gets involved together. Phenomenology approach and analysis of interview data employed in this study revealed that social dynamics in individual behavior was increasing to perform infaq and sadaqah as people with same behavior emerged. However, individuals who considered infaq and sadaqah as worship, the presence of others did not affect to individual’s behavior. Keywords : Infaq and Sadaqah, Social Facilitation Theory ICRMH 2019, September 18-19, Jakarta, Indonesia Copyright © 2020 EAI DOI 10.4108/eai.18-9-2019.2293319 1. INTRODUCTION Basically, human as social being since always been social creatures which need help from others, especially in meeting personal and public need. Islam ideology objective to bring prosperity, grace, and peace for human being in the world let people to help each other. Allah SWT has delivered enormous natural sources in case human being takes the benefit wisely. Based on survey and a study conducted by CAF (Charities, Aid, Fondation), non-profit organization based in Great Britain, Indonesia is in the first the most generous country in ther world.
    [Show full text]
  • Are Groups More Or Less Than the Sum of Their Members? the Moderating Role of Individual Identification
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2016), Page 1 of 56 doi:10.1017/S0140525X15000618, e137 Are groups more or less than the sum of their members? The moderating role of individual identification Roy F. Baumeister Department of Psychology, Florida State University, Tallahassee, FL 32306- 4301 [email protected] Sarah E. Ainsworth Department of Psychology, University of North Florida, Jacksonville, FL 32224 [email protected] Kathleen D. Vohs Carlson School of Management, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN 55455 [email protected] Abstract: This paper seeks to make a theoretical and empirical case for the importance of differentiated identities for group function. Research on groups has found that groups sometimes perform better and other times perform worse than the sum of their individual members. Differentiation of selves is a crucial moderator. We propose a heuristic framework that divides formation of work or task groups into two steps. One step emphasizes shared common identity and promotes emotional bonds. In the other step, which we emphasize, group members take increasingly differentiated roles that improve performance through specialization, moral responsibility, and efficiency. Pathologies of groups (e.g., social loafing, depletion of shared resources/commons dilemmas, failure to pool information, groupthink) are linked to submerging the individual self in the group. These pathologies are decreased when selves are differentiated, such as by individual rewards, individual competition, accountability, responsibility, and public identification. Differentiating individual selves contributes to many of the best outcomes of groups, such as with social facilitation, wisdom- of-crowds effects, and division of labor. Anonymous confidentiality may hamper differentiation by allowing people to blend into the group (so that selfish or lazy efforts are not punished), but it may also facilitate differentiation by enabling people to think and judge without pressure to conform.
    [Show full text]