<<

Virginia Commonwealth University VCU Scholars Compass

Theses and Dissertations Graduate School

2011

SPECIES LEVEL DIFFERENCES IN THE ECOLOGY OF TWO NEOTROPICAL TADPOLE SPECIES: RESPONSES TO NONLETHAL PREDATORS AND THE ROLES OF COMPETITION AND RESOURCE USE

Zacharia Costa Virginia Commonwealth University

Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd

Part of the Biology Commons

© The Author

Downloaded from https://scholarscompass.vcu.edu/etd/2635

This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at VCU Scholars Compass. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses and Dissertations by an authorized administrator of VCU Scholars Compass. For more information, please contact [email protected].

© Zacharia J. Costa

All Rights Reserved

SPECIES LEVEL DIFFERENCES IN THE ECOLOGY OF TWO NEOTROPICAL TADPOLE

SPECIES: RESPONSES TO NONLETHAL PREDATORS AND THE ROLES OF COMPETITION

AND RESOURCE USE

A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIRMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF SCIENCE AT VIRGINIA COMMONWEALTH UNIVERSITY

by

ZACHARIA J. COSTA Bachelor of Science, University of California, Davis 2007

Director DR. JAMES VONESH DEPARTMENT OF BIOLOGY

Virginia Commonwealth University Richmond, Virginia December 13, 2011

Acknowledgments

This would not have been possible if not for the support of my parents Kim and

Marty Costa. They have always encouraged my pursuit of knowledge and love of the natural world, and are thus directly responsible for my career as a biologist. They have supported me in all of my endeavors often at their own expense, financially and emotionally, and for that I will forever be grateful.

I also want to express my deep gratitude and thanks to my advisor Dr. James

Vonesh, who has gone above and beyond the call of duty as an advisor to help me succeed as a scientist. His support as a mentor, boss, and friend has helped me learn a tremendous amount, and I am a better scientist because of it.

I would also like to thank Dr. Tobias Landberg and Dr. Justin Touchon for helping me in the field, giving me advice about science and life, and for usually having cold drinks. I would also like to thank everyone who helped me in the field including B.

Willink, R. Perez, S. Abinette, R. Jimenez and M. Palmer. I would also like to thank Dr.

Johanna Kraus for her advice and support, in addition to my committee members and B.

Colteaux, D. Johnson and the Vonesh lab group for comments that improved the manuscript. This work was funded by the National Science Foundation (DEB-0717200 to JRV) and the Sigma Xi Grants-in-Aid of Research (ZJC).

ii

Table of Contents

Acknowledgements ...... ii

Table of Contents ...... iii

List of Tables ...... v

List of Figures ...... vi

Abstract………...... vii

Chapter 1: Species specific effects of two Neotropical hylid tadpoles on aquatic resources and other primary consumers

Introduction ...... 1

Materials and Methods ...... 4

Results ...... 10

Discussion ...... 16

Chapter 2: Nonlethal effects of predators change interactions between two Neotropical tadpoles through ontogeny

Introduction ...... 23

Materials and Methods ...... 28

Results ...... 34

iii

Discussion ...... 38

References ...... 47

Tables and Figures ...... 57

Vita ...... 70

iv

List of Tables

Experiment 1

Table 1: Results of two-way ANOVAs on the effects of tadpole species identity and density on the abundances of zooplankton morphospecies ...... 57

Experiment 2

Table 2: Main effects and the significant interactions of time, predators and competitors on the total length, growth rates and final biomass of A. callidryas ...... 58

Table 3: Main effects and the significant interactions of time, predators and competitors on the total length, growth rates and final biomass of D. ebraccatus ...... 59

Table 4: Main effects and significant interactions among time, predators, and tadpole treatments on the abundances of zooplankton morphospecies ...... 60

v

List of Figures

Experiment 1

Figure 1: Mean (±SE) of tadpole size (A), growth rates (B), and total tank biomass (C) in which we manipulated the abundances, high (50) and low (25), of Ac (A. callidryas) and

De (D. ebraccatus) tadpoles...... 61

Figure 2: Mean (±SE) of water column chlorophyll a (A) and dried periphyton biomass

(B) in experimental treatments...... 62

Figure 3: Mean (±SE) of zooplankton morphospecies abundances in experimental

treatments...... 63

Figure 4: A) Mean (±SE) of water column chlorophyll a after being filtered through

different sized Nitex, (B) periphyton chlorophyll a abundances inside and

outside of tadpole exclosures, and (C) zooplankton abundances in which we

manipulated the presence and absence of 50 A. callidryas tadpoles...... 64

Experiment 2

Figure 5: Mean (±SE) of phytoplankton chlorophyll a for A. Predator presence and B.

Tadpole composition over time ...... 65

Figure 6: Mean (±SE) of periphyton chlorophyll a for A. Predator presence and B.

Tadpole composition over time ...... 66

Figure 7: Mean (±SE) of tadpole length for A. (Agalychnis callidryas) and B.

( ebraccatus) by treatment over time ...... 67

vi

Figure 8: Mean (±SE) of tadpole growth rates for A. (Agalychnis callidryas) and B.

(Dendropsophus ebraccatus) by treatment over time. Growth rates are

calculated as Final: log(Final TL/Mid TL) and Mid: (Mid TL/Initial TL) ...... 68

Figure 9: Mean (±SE) of zooplankton numbers per liter for A. Total zooplankton B.

Copepods and C. Rotifers ...... 69

vii

ABSTRACT

SPECIES LEVEL DIFFERENCES IN THE ECOLOGY OF TWO NEOTROPICAL TADPOLE

SPECIES: RESPONSES TO NONLETHAL PREDATORS AND THE ROLES OF COMPETITION

AND RESOURCE USE

By Zacharia J. Costa, M.S.

A THESIS SUBMITTED IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIRMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF

MASTER OF SCIENCE AT VIRGINIA COMMONWEALTH UNIVERSITY

Virginia Commonwealth University, 2011 Major Director: Dr. James Vonesh Assistant Professor, Department of Biology

Closely related species at the same trophic level are often considered to be ecologically equivalent. However, it is clear that individuals species can have unique functional roles that drive community and ecosystem processes. In this study we examine the growth responses of two Neotropical hylid tadpole species, Agalychnis callidryas and Dendropsophus ebraccatus, to intraspecific and interspecific competition.

We also look at density-dependent effects of each on phytoplankton, periphyton and zooplankton, as well as their responses to a caged predator through ontogeny.

viii

Intraspecific competition affected both species similarly, and their effects on resources were qualitatively similar but quantitatively different. Predators affected resource levels and interspecific competition. Predator effects on tadpole size varied in both magnitude and direction through ontogeny for both species. This study shows that closely related species at the same trophic level can have different ecological roles and that tadpoles are more functionally unique than previously thought.

ix

CHAPTER 1: SPECIES SPECIFIC EFFECTS OF TWO NEOTROPICAL HYLID TADPOLES ON

AQUATIC RESOURCES AND OTHER PRIMARY CONSUMERS

Introduction

Natural communities are often too complex to study on a species-by-species basis.

Therefore, ecologists often simplify food webs by grouping species together based on shared characteristics such as phylogeny and/or trophic status. Species within such groups are assumed to be functionally redundant, or to have similar effects on population, community or ecological processes (Chalcraft & Resetarits 2003a,b, Harris 1995, Lawton

& Brown 1993). In fact, functional redundancy often serves as a null hypothesis when looking at the consequences of biodiversity for ecosystem functioning and is the main assumption underlying neutral theory (Loreau 2004, Holoyoak & Loreay 2006, Hubbell

2001). It is increasingly clear however, that even closely related species at the same trophic level can vary greatly in their functional roles. This is often attributed to a suite of traits that influence consumers’ interactions with their resources, such as habitat use, metabolic requirements, and/or foraging modes, often making it difficult to make generalizations about species and their functional roles based on individual traits

(Beckerman et al. 2010, Fox et al. 2009, Resetarits & Chalcraft 2007, Schmitz & Suttle

2001). Thus, it is likely that investigations into qualitative and quantitative differences in species’ interactions will give insight into their specific impacts on communities,

1

something phylogeny and trophic status may not do (Lazzaro et al. 2009, Schmitz et al.

2008, Schmitz 2009, Vanni et al. 2002).

The functional roles of aquatic herbivores often involve various direct and indirect interactions with primary producers. For example, besides direct consumptive effects, herbivores can both regulate and facilitate their resources indirectly through their interactions with other factors that control primary productivity such as nutrient cycling

(Vanni 2002). Thus, aquatic herbivores can have dramatic effects on communities by simultaneously exerting consumptive top-down effects and bottom-up effects (e.g. facilitation of primary producers) on food web structure and dynamics, making it difficult to predict their functional roles a priori (Flecker et al. 2002, Knoll et al. 2009,

Kupferberg 1997, Power 1990). In the tropics, the drivers of food web dynamics in freshwater systems remain unclear (Danger et al. 2009, Lazarro et al. 2003, Menezes et al. 2010). However, in a few cases single species have found to be functionally unique

(Flecker 1992, 1996, Taylor et al. 2006, Vanni et al. 2002), suggesting the need to evaluate the assumption that species are functionally redundant in these systems if we want to understand the role of these herbivores in food web dynamics.

Larval anurans are some of the least understood consumers in regards to their functional roles, especially in the tropics where they are more diverse and exhibit more trophic variation than in temperate systems (Wells 2007). Traditionally, diverse groups of herbivorous tadpoles have been lumped together as redundant primary consumers, although their effects on aquatic ecosystems can be highly species-specific and involve

2

direct and indirect impacts on primary producer and invertebrate communities (Connelly et al. 2008, Flecker et al. 1999, McDiarmid & Altig 1999, Whiles et al. 2006). In order to assess the role of these important consumers in a tropical lentic food web, we manipulated the identity and density of two larval Neotropical hylid species.

Agalychnis callidryas is a mid-water suspension feeder thought to primarily feed on phytoplankton, and Dendropsophus ebraccatus is a smaller benthic grazer that feeds on filamentous algae and periphyton (Duellman & Treub 1986, McDiarmid & Altig 1999,

Wassersug & Rosenberg 1979, Wells 2007). Because of their different resource and habitat use, we expected a priori that these species would have different density dependent direct and indirect effects on a simplified food web. Specifically, we expected

D. ebraccatus to have strong density-dependent consumptive effects on periphyton and

A. callidryas would not. Since phytoplankton and periphyton compete for nutrients

(Confer 1972, Hansson 1988, 1990), by reducing periphyton levels these tadpoles could indirectly facilitate phytoplankton. We also expected A. callidryas to have a density dependent impact on phytoplankton levels, which would indirectly benefit periphyton and would indirectly decrease zooplankton through exploitative competition. Thus, we investigated how species identity and two different intraspecific densities affected tadpole size and relative growth rates, their resource levels, and zooplankton.

3

Methods

Experiment 1

This study was conducted at the Smithsonian Tropical Research Institute in

Gamboa, (9°7′17″ N,79°42′11″W) between 18 June and 10 July 2010.

Agalychnis callidryas and Dendropsophus ebraccatus are Neotropical treefrogs that commonly co-occur in lowland ponds throughout their ranges from southern Mexico to

Panama. At the study site they are locally abundant, and eggs, tadpoles and adults commonly co-occur throughout the rainy season (May-December) in Gamboa area ponds

(J.C. Touchon & J.R. Vonesh unpubl. data).

To examine the effects of intraspecific competition on the growth and survival of

A. callidryas and D. ebraccatus and the effects of these species on resources and other aquatic primary consumers I conducted an experiment using five different treatments.

These were a control with no tadpoles and each species crossed with two densities, twenty five and fifty tadpoles per tank. Each treatment was replicated five times. Initial tadpole (0.125 and 0.0625 tadpole/L) densities were within the natural range that were observed during field estimates across five breeding ponds in the Gamboa area (J.C.

Touchon & J.R. Vonesh unpubl. data). Treatments were randomly assigned to 400L mesocosms (0.75 x 0.8 m) with screened drain holes at 0.75 m height, arranged in five blocks of five tanks each in an open field. Tanks were filled with a mix of filtered aged rain and tap water, 75-85g of dried leaves confined to a mesh bag and 1.5 g of Sera

4

Micron® powdered algae on 6 June. Each tank received 100 ml of standardized pond inoculate on 8 June and again on 17 June. Pond inoculate was collected from an artificial pond adjacent to secondary forest, by repeatedly sweeping a net through the water column and filtering it through a 1 mm mesh filter to exclude large invertebrates but to allow communities of phytoplankton, periphyton and zooplankton to establish.

Tanks were covered with fine nylon mesh secured with rubber to prevent the colonization by non-experimental organisms.

On 11 June I collected 12 egg clutches of D. ebraccatus from Ocelot Pond and

16 clutches of A. callidryas from vegetation surrounding Experimental Pond. Eggs were kept in an ambient temperature laboratory and misted often to prevent desiccation. Eggs from both species were the same age but D. ebraccatus eggs hatch earlier because they are smaller and develop more rapidly (Touchon & Warkentin 2010), so they were three days post-hatching when A. callidryas eggs hatched on 16 June. Tadpoles were dorsally photographed in a white tray with a Nikon D40x digital camera and added to mesocosms on 18 June. All tadpoles were dip-netted again on 9 July and re-photographed. All photographs included a ruler for calibration in order to use ImageJ

(http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/) to obtain measurements of tadpole length.

To measure the effects of tadpoles on their resources and other potential competitors, phytoplankton and zooplankton were sampled on 8 July. I used a 1 L integrated tube sampler to collect four samples from the water column of each tank. To measure water column chlorophyll a to indicate phytoplankton levels, 100 ml were sub-

5

sampled and put on ice before vacuum filtered on Whatman GF/A glass filters. Filters were extracted in ethanol for 24 hours under refrigeration in the dark. Samples were then read in Aquafluor fluorimeter following standard protocol to estimate μg of chlorophyll a per unit volume (Welschmeyer 1994).

Periphyton samples were collected by hanging a piece of flagging tape (625 x 29 mm) in the center of the tank at the water level (Austin et al. 1981). Periphyton was sampled on 10 July. The flagging tape was scraped onto a filter with a razor blade, as were two additional 70 x 70mm squares on the north and south walls of the tanks, approximately 30 cm from the top. These solutions were vacuum filtered on pre-weighed

Whatman GF/A glass filters. Filters were then put in a drying oven at 30° C for 48 hours and weighed on a digital balance and then converted to μg of periphyton per cm2 (Aloi

1990).

Zooplankton samples were obtained from water from the tanks that had previously been collected with the tube sampler. Four liters were filtered on a 20 μm

Nitex® filter and rinsed with tap water to form a more concentrated solution (~15ml).

Four to six drops of Lugol’s solution were added as a preservative and to stain the zooplankton (Stoecker et al. 1994). Zooplankton were then quantified in the lab at 30X magnification with a stereo microscope, and individuals were classified into morphospecies.

6

Experiment 2

In 2011 a follow up experiment was performed to investigate the effects of A. callidryas on the size structure of the phytoplankton community and their indirect effects on periphyton and zooplankton. Following the procedure from 2010, ten mesocosms were filled on 7 July with a mix of filtered aged rain and tap water, 75-85g of dried leaves confined to a mesh bag and 1.5 g of Sera Micron®. Each tank received 100 ml of standardized pond inoculate on 7 July and again on 16 July swept from Experimental

Pond. On 17 July five tanks were randomly assigned fifty tadpoles each. Tadpoles were hatched from 8 clutches collected from Experimental Pond at ages 4-6 days.

Experimental tadpoles were introduced at seven days post-oviposition and were randomly distributed. To separate the direct consumptive and possible indirect effects of tadpoles

(e.g. due to nutrient cycling) on periphyton, tadpole exclosures (9 cm diameter plastic pots) open at the surface with mesh sides were suspended in the middle of the mesocosms at the surface with pieces of tape (approx. 3.8 x 12 cm) attached to two cards on the sides of both the outside and inside of the exclosures. On 5 August tape samples from each side of the exclosure were dried and extracted in ethanol to measure chlorophyll a with flourometry. To examine the effect of tadpoles on the size structure of the phytoplankton community three additional 200 ml samples from each tank were taken on 4 August and analyzed with flourometry as in 2010, after being filtered through 20 μm, 5 μm and 1 μm

Nitex to look at the relative abundances of these different plankton size classes. Before vacuum filtering, in addition to the regular (non filtered) samples. Zooplankton samples

7

were also collected on this date by filtering 2 L through a 20 μm filter and then quantified to morphospecies. Two tadpole tanks and one control tank were colonized by other organisms and were excluded from analyses.

Analysis

All statistical analyses were conducted on tank means using R version 2.11.0 (R

Developmental Core Team 2010). We first analyzed univariate treatment effects using

ANOVA. To test for species and density effect compared to controls, we performed additional univariate tests as a priori planned contrasts. For the factorial treatments with tadpoles (i.e., excluding controls) we also performed a two-way ANOVA to examine the interaction between species identity and density. Due to size differences between the species, if species effects were significant we also performed an ANCOVA to determine if tadpole effects on their resources and zooplankton were driven by differences in biomass rather than species identity per se. Data for tadpole growth and primary production were analyzed with mixed linear models treating block as a random effect.

Periphyton biomass and phytoplankton chlorophyll a measurements were log transformed to meet assumptions of homogeneity of variance. Zooplankton count data were analyzed using a generalized linear mixed model approach to account for both block effects and non-normal error distributions. To account for non-normality in the error distribution

Poisson error structure was assumed (Pinheiro & Bates 2000). Zooplankton data from

2011 had substantial overdispersion so a quasi-Poisson error distribution was used. All

8

pair wise comparisons were tested using Tukey’s test of honestly significant differences

(HSD).

For data from 2011 we first analyzed univariate treatment effects on the four different phytoplankton measures using ANOVA. To examine possible differences between direct and indirect effects of tadpoles on periphyton (outside and inside the exclosures respectively) we used a two-way ANOVA to examine possible interactions between treatments and the side of the exclosure periphyton was grown. A mixed linear model used tank as a random factor to avoid nested sampling errors. Primary production values were log transformed to homogenize variance.

9

Results

Tadpole Growth

Initially, A. callidryas tadpoles were 12.78 ± 0.34 mm (mean ± SD) and D. ebraccatus tadpoles were 6.92 ± 0.18 in total length (TL). There was no difference in initial size within species across treatments (A. callidryas: F1, 4 = 0.169, P = 0.70; D. ebraccatus: F1, 4 = 5.61, P = 0.08). Species identity (F1, 13 = 73.54, P < 0.001) and density

(F1, 13 = 37.03, P < 0.001; Fig. 1A) both had significant influences on final tadpole TL.

Low density A. callidryas tadpoles (34.41 ± 0.50) were 18.6% larger than high density tadpoles (29.00 ± 2.11) (F1, 4 = 31.05, P = 0.005). Tadpoles of D. ebraccatus from low density treatments (26.92 ± 2.61) were 21.4% larger than high density tadpoles (22.17 ±

1.78) (F1, 4 = 11.33, P = 0.028). Comparing the two species, at low densities A. callidryas tadpoles were 27.8% larger than D. ebraccatus, and at high densities they were 30.8% larger than D. ebraccatus. Thus, the effects of increasing conspecific density were similar for both species and there was no species by density interaction (F1, 12 = 0.145, P

= 0.71).

Results for relative tadpole growth rates parallel those for TL. Species identity and density affected growth (species: F1, 13 = 108.87, P < 0.001; density: F1, 13 = 34.54, P

< 0.001; Fig. 1B), with no interaction between the two (F1, 12 = 0.40, P = 0.54). Low density A. callidryas (0.43 ± 0.01) grew 21.9% faster than high density treatment tadpoles (0.35 ± 0.05) (F1, 4 = 13.79, P = 0.021). Dendropsophus ebraccatus tadpoles in

10

low density treatments (0.60 ± 0.04) grew 19.3% faster than high density tadpoles (0.50 ±

0.03) (F1, 4 = 19.67, P = 0.011). Within density treatments, at low density treatments D. ebraccatus grew 38% faster than A. callidryas. In high density treatments D. ebraccatus grew 40.9% faster than A. callidryas. Overall, D. ebraccatus (0.55 ± 0.06) grew 39.3% faster than A. callidryas (0.39 ± 0.05).

Total tank tadpole biomass was only affected by species identity (F1, 12 = 5.42, P =

0.038; density: F1, 12 = 2.54, P = 0.14; interaction: F1, 12 = 0.58, P = 0.81; Fig. 1C), with D. ebraccatus (5.29 ± 1.26g) having 19.2% less biomass than A. callidryas (6.55 ± 1.20g).

Primary Producers

Water column chlorophyll a levels were strongly affected by treatment (F4, 16 =

8.12, P < 0.001; Fig. 2A). When pooled across densities there was a strong species effect as A. callidryas and D. ebraccatus each significantly increased chlorophyll a by 197% and 84.3% relative to controls, respectively (F2, 18 = 8.36, P = 0.003). When pooled across tadpole species, high density treatments had 39% more chlorophyll a compared to low density treatments and 180% more than controls (F2, 18 = 5.94, P = 0.011). Looking at the main and interactive effects of species type and tadpole density within treatments with tadpoles, there was a species (F1, 12 = 7.50, P = 0.018) and species-by-density interaction (F1, 12 = 11.08, P = 0.006) while density itself was not significant (F1, 12 = 1.86,

P = 0.20). This was due to the fact that high density A. callidryas’ treatments increased chlorophyll a by 120% compared to low densities (F1, 4 = 9.38, P = 0.038), but D. ebraccatus density treatments were not different from one another (F1, 4 = 1.38, P = 0.30).

11

Similarly, species identity (F1, 12 = 6.73, P = 0.017) and its interaction with biomass were both significant (F1, 12 = 10.34, P = 0.007) while biomass itself was not (F1, 12 = 0.77, P =

0.40). This is because there were biomass effects within A. callidryas treatments (F1, 4 =

11.65, P = 0.03), but not for D. ebraccatus (F1, 4 = 1.9, P = 0.24).

In the 2011 follow-up experiment with A. callidryas, total water column

2 chlorophyll a was also affected by treatment (F1, 5 = 16.95, P = 0.009, R = 0.73; Fig. 4A), as A. callidryas increased it by 123% compared to controls. All measured size classes

2 responded similarly to tadpole presence as the < 20 μm (F1, 5 = 13.36, P = 0.015, R =

2 2 0.67), < 5 μm (F1, 5 = 41.49, P = 0.001, R = 0.87) and < 1 μm (F1, 5 = 30.97, P = 0.003, R

= 0.83) size classes increased by 124%, 104% and 118%, respectively.

In 2010, periphyton dry mass was also affected by treatment (F4, 16 = 6.94, P =

0.002). When pooled across densities there was a significant species effect as both species reduced periphyton similarly as compared to controls (F2, 18 = 15.06, P < 0.001;

Fig. 2B). Pooled across species there was also a density effect as controls had 591% more periphyton than high density treatments and 625% more than low density treatments (F2, 18 = 10.16, P = 0.001). Within tadpole treatments only species identity was relevant as A. callidryas reduced periphyton by 67.7% more than D. ebraccatus (species:

F1, 12 = 4.95, P = 0.046; density: F1, 12 = 0.12, P = 0.73; interaction: F1, 12 = 0.04, P = 0.85), and this was not due to a biomass effect due to the larger size of A. callidryas (F1, 12 =

0.18, P = 0.68).

12

In 2011, periphyton chlorophyll a levels were affected by tadpoles (F1, 5 = 14.99, P

= 0.01), side of exclosure (F1, 5 = 32.78, P = 0.002), and their interaction (F1, 5 = 35.26, P

= 0.002; Fig. 4B). Post-hoc pair wise comparisons showed tadpoles decreased periphyton on the outside of exclosures by 91.2% compared to the inside of hose tanks (P

< 0.001). In addition, tadpole decreased periphyton by 89.3% compared to the outside (P

< 0.001) and 88% compared to the inside (P < 0.001) of control tanks.

Zooplankton

The total number of zooplankton varied among treatments (F4, 16 = 6.34, P =

0.003). Specifically, across density species effects were significant (F2, 18 = 12.41, P <

0.001) because A. callidryas (138.53 ± 71.67 per L) reduced zooplankton 54% compared to controls (303.2 ± 109.76) and by 61.2% compared to D. ebraccatus (363.33 ± 117.34;

Fig. 3F), but pooled across species, density had no effect (F2, 18 = 0.46, P = 0.64). Within tadpole treatments only species effects were significant, as A. callidryas reduced total zooplankton levels (Table 1). Copepod densities were affected by treatment (F4, 16 = 6.20,

P = 0.001). Pooled across species there was no density effect (F2, 18 = 0.22, P = 0.81), but across densities there was a significant species effect (F2, 18 = 13.02, P < 0.001).

Specifically, A. callidryas (1.93 ± 1.34) reduced copepods 87% relative to controls (15.85

± 7.27) and by 91% compared to D. ebraccatus (21.50 ± 14.56; Fig. 3A). When looking at only tadpole treatments and possible interactions between species identity and density, only species effects were significant (Table 1). There was no interaction between species identity and biomass, but both were significant (species: F1, 12 = 31.80, P < 0.001;

13

biomass: F1, 12 = 10.26, P = 0.008; interaction: F1, 12 = 1.18, P = 0.30). When pooled by species, biomass was marginally significant for D. ebraccatus (F1, 4 = 6.80, P = 0.06) as copepods increased with increasing tadpole biomass, but this was not the case with A. callidryas (F1, 4 = 0.04, P = 0.85). Nauplii, early copepod instars, were also affected by treatment (F4, 16 = 11.48, P < 0.001) and when pooled across densities, by species (F2, 18 =

19.63, P < 0.001). They were reduced by A. callidryas (52.10 ± 38.15) 75.8% relative to controls (215.50 ± 117.30) and by 81% compared to D. ebraccatus (273.95 ± 98.54; Fig.

2B), but density was not significant (F2, 18 = 0.34, P = 0.72). Within tadpole treatments, there was a significant species effect and a marginal species by density interaction (F1, 12

= 4.33, P = 0.06) in which low density A. callidryas (29.10 ± 8.59) reduced nauplii compared to high density treatments (75.10 ± 43.35) (F1, 4 = 10.08, P = 0.03) but overall, density had no effect (Table 1), and neither did biomass (F1, 12 = 0.03, P = 0.87). There were no predictors of cladoceran density (treatment: F4, 16 = 2.05, P = 0.14; species: F2, 18

= 1.36, P = 0.28; density: F2, 18 = 2.39, P = 0.12; Fig. 3C). For rotifers, univariate tests comparing treatment effects were not significant (F4, 16 = 1.82, P = 0.17), but pooled across densities A. callidryas (53.85 ± 70.00) increased rotifers 140% compared to D. ebraccatus (22.45 ± 25.06) and 161% compared to controls (20.65 ± 25.18) (F2, 18 = 3.80,

P = 0.04; Fig. 3D), and this was not due to tadpole biomass (F1, 12 = 0.05, P = 0.82).

There were no other predictors of rotifer abundances (Table 1). None of the treatments affected ostracod abundances (treatment: F4, 16 = 0.47, P = 0.76; density: F2, 18 = 0.99, P =

0.39; species: F2, 18 = 0.12, P = 0.89; Fig. 3E).

14

In 2011, no zooplankton groups responded to the presence of A. callidryas tadpoles: copepods (F1, 5 = 3.25, P = 0.13), nauplii (F1, 5 = 0.20, P = 0.68), ostracods (F1, 5

= 0.66, P = 0.45), and total zooplankton (F1, 5 = 0.21, P = 0.67; Fig. 4C), although cladocerans (F1, 5 = 4.93, P = 0.08) and rotifers (F1, 5 = 5.11, P = 0.07) were marginal.

Post-hoc pair wise comparisons however, showed tadpoles decreased rotifers (44.33 ±

27.27) (P = 0.03) compared to controls (108.5 ± 45.75) and increased cladocerans (40.67

± 30.72) (P < 0.05) compared to controls (8.13 ± 10.28).

15

Discussion

This experiment shows that these two tadpole species can have dramatically different roles within these lentic communities. Both species reduced periphyton and increased phytoplankton. For A. callidryas their effect on phytoplankton was density dependent, as higher densities had a stronger facilitative effect. This strong bottom-up effect on these food webs was contrasted by the fact that they also decreased the abundances of several zooplankton taxa, while D. ebraccatus did not. Thus, the assumption that closely related species at the same trophic level have similar functional roles within communities are not warranted with these tropical tadpole species.

Similar to previous studies, we show that an increased number of conspecifics reduced both the final sizes and growth rates of the two focal species, supporting the idea that intraspecific competition plays an important role in structuring communities

(Gurevitch et al. 2000, Morin 1999). Species identity and density were both important determinants of final length and growth rates, as A. callidryas tadpoles were larger and grew slower than D. ebraccatus, and tadpoles of both species from high density treatments were smaller and grew slower than those from low density treatments. The growth rates and sizes of both species were affected similarly by the number of conspecifics, so there was no interaction between these two factors. This is surprising given the initial differences in size between the two species, and that smaller tadpoles are thought to tolerate competitive effects better than larger tadpoles, and larger tadpoles

16

have a greater per capita effect on resources (Werner 1994). The lack of pair-wise differences in biomass across all tadpole treatments supports the idea that the final total tank masses of both species, at both densities, was limited by conspecifics due to possible resource limitations, and that the per unit biomass competitive effects of tadpoles were similar across species.

Both tadpole species had strong effects on primary producers. All tadpoles reduced periphyton dry mass compared to controls, but A. callidryas, the putative , consumed 67.7% more periphyton than D. ebraccatus. This was a species specific effect that was not attributable to size or biomass, suggesting A. callidryas consumes more periphyton than previously thought (McDiarmid & Altig 1999,

Wassersug & Rosenberg 1979) and is a more efficient grazer per unit biomass and per individual than D. ebraccatus. The lack of density dependent effects on periphyton by both species shows they were limited by this resource, as both densities nearly grazed it completely. Phytoplankton biomass was also not affected by density across treatments, but both species increased its levels relative to controls. Across D. ebraccatus treatments total phytoplankton was increased compared to controls, and this effect was stronger in low density treatments. Agalychnis callidryas increased phytoplankton compared to controls, and this effect was strongest in high density treatments. Given that A. callidryas has behavioral and anatomical features that strongly suggest filter feeding (McDiarmid &

Altig 1999, Wassersug & Rosenberg 1979), and they graze periphyton, there are several possible mechanisms that may explain this counterintuitive result.

17

Consumer-mediated nutrient cycling can provide substantial amounts of nutrients required for phytoplankton and periphyton growth (Knoll et al. 2009, Vanni & Layne

1997, Vanni 2002). Thus, it is possible A. callidryas’ indirect facilitation of phytoplankton due to their nutrient excretion could overcome their consumptive effects.

This is supported by the fact that their facilitation of phytoplankton was density dependent, as more individuals excrete more nutrients. Final tadpole tank biomass was not a significant predictor of phytoplankton abundance however, but species and the interaction between the two were. This was due to the fact that lower biomass D. ebraccatus treatments actually increased chlorophyll a more than higher biomass treatments, while the opposite was true for A. callidryas. It is then likely that not just the amount of nutrients, but possible differences between these two species in their body nutrient composition, excretion rates and ratios (stoichiometry) can all contribute to their indirect effects on primary producers (Elser & Urabe 1999, Vanni et al. 2002). Although we did not address any of those questions, in 2011 we attempted to separate the consumptive effects of tadpoles on periphyton from nutrient-mediated indirect effects using tadpole exclosures. The indirect effects of tadpoles did tend to benefit periphyton on the inside of exclosures as they had the highest periphyton chlorophyll a levels, but this was not statistically significant.

Filter feeders can also have dramatic effects on phytoplankton communities due to size selective feeding. By altering phytoplankton size composition, filter feeders can indirectly alter plankton biomass, primary productivity and photosynthetic efficiency

18

(Watson et al. 2003). Some filter feeding fish can actually enhance total phytoplankton levels by consuming large phytoplankton, which reduces competition between large and small size classes (Byers & Vinyard 1990, Drenner et al. 1996, Lazzaro et al. 1992). This is because nanoplankton (<20 μm) are often too small to be effectively filtered, have higher growth rates (Smith & Kalff 1983) and have higher primary productivity per unit of algal biomass relative to larger phytoplankton (Schlesinger et al. 1981). As A. callidryas increased total phytoplankton and did not affect any of the measured size classes (<1 μm, <5 μm , <20 μm) differently, this suggests they increased total phytoplankton levels by increasing nanoplankton < 1 μm.

Another mechanism by which these consumers could benefit their resources is mediated by competition between the resources. Phytoplankton and periphyton compete for nutrients (Confer 1972, Hansson 1988, 1990), so it is possible that by reducing periphyton levels these tadpoles could potentially facilitate phytoplankton, as has been proposed previously (Bronmark et al. 1991, Leibold and Wilbur 1992). This may be true however, within A. callidryas treatments there was no density effect on periphyton, but strong density dependent facilitation of phytoplankton. Similarly, pair wise comparisons show D. ebraccatus’ reduction of periphyton was not density driven, but its effect on phytoplankton was; low density treatments increased phytoplankton more than high density treatments. It is possible that we missed density effects by sampling after periphyton has already been completely grazed, but regardless, the evidence suggests other mechanisms than competition between the two resources are at work.

19

Aquatic consumers can also indirectly affect phytoplankton through their interactions with zooplankton. Changes in the abundance and/or species composition of zooplankton communities can alter their consumption and/or excretion rates and ratios, which can have strong impacts on phytoplankton communities (Brett & Goldman 1996,

Elser & Urabe 1999, Vanni & Findlay 1990, Vanni & Layne 1997). It is possible this mechanism also influenced A. callidryas’ effect on phytoplankton as they had strong effects on the zooplankton community while D. ebraccatus did not. Specifically, the abundances of Mesocyclops cyclopoid copepods, the largest sized zooplankton (0.6-

1.1mm) in our mesocosms, were strongly reduced by A. callidryas compared to all other treatments, and this was independent of biomass and density. These copepods are omnivorous (Hopp et al. 1997, Kumar and Rao 1999) and can have strong impacts on plankton communities (Blumenshine & Hambright 2003, Chang & Hanazato 2005a,

Nagata & Hanazato 2006). Therefore, by decreasing their abundances, tadpoles could indirectly interact with phytoplankton and other zooplankton taxa.

Nauplii, the younger instars of Mesocyclops, followed a similar pattern as A. callidryas reduced their abundances compared to other treatments. Within A. callidryas treatments there was a marginal density effect because low density treatments had fewer nauplii than high density treatments. Other tadpole species can feed on zooplankton

(Whiles et al. 2010), yet examination of A. callidryas’ stomach contents and feces have failed to show evidence that they do (Warkentin pers. comm.), so the mechanisms behind these reductions are unclear. Previous studies have shown tadpoles can reduce

20

zooplankton biomass (Leibold & Wilbur 1992) and cladoceran densities (Mokany 2007), but the mechanisms were also unknown. In 2010 there were no predictors of cladoceran density, but pair wise comparisons showed low densities of A. callidryas reduced them more than all other treatments. In contrast, rotifers were increased by A. callidryas compared to other treatments, regardless of density. These results suggest A. callidryas’ impact on some zooplankton taxa are not density dependent. In 2011 there were no significant treatment effects of A. callidryas on any of the zooplankton, while they had a similar impact on phytoplankton and periphyton, suggesting that their interactions with zooplankton are not driven by their effects on primary producers. However, it is possible that we lacked the statistical resolution to detect such effects since the sample size was reduced because other anurans invaded the mesocosms.

Agalychnis callidryas’ effects on zooplankton may also be explained by a trophic cascade due to their reduction of predatory Mesocyclops copepods. If this were true we would expect a subsequent increase in their zooplankton prey items, which are cladocerans and rotifers (Chang & Hanazato 2003, 2005a,b, Nagata & Hanazato 2006).

This did not happen with cladocerans in 2010 but this may be the case with rotifers as there was a marginal species effect (P = 0.056) as A. callidryas increased rotifers compared to other treatments. In 2011 there were no treatment effects of A. callidryas on any zooplankton, but pair-wise comparisons showed they increased cladocerans and reduced rotifers, suggesting other factors influence zooplankton dynamics. Previous work has shown zooplankton community responses to changes in the abundances of

21

Mesocyclops can be highly species-specific and likely involve various indirect effects within the plankton community (Chang & Hanazato 2005a, Nagata & Hanazato 2006).

Our results show that tadpole effects on aquatic food webs can be highly species specific and are not driven by biomass or trophic status per se. Besides the importance of their direct consumptive effects, the roles of these tadpoles within aquatic food webs are also driven by various indirect effects that most likely involve simultaneous processes that include their interactions with other consumers, nutrient cycling and competition among primary producers. Specifically, by both regulating and facilitating different primary producers and consumers (zooplankton) simultaneously, the indirect effects of tadpoles on aquatic food webs may be more important and complex than previously thought. Understanding the functional roles of these aquatic consumers will require further work that addresses the specific mechanisms that drive the relative importance of both their bottom-up and top-down influences on aquatic food webs.

22

CHAPTER 2: NONLETHAL EFFECTS OF PREDATORS CHANGE INTERACTIONS BETWEEN TWO

NEOTROPICAL HYLID TADPOLES THROUGH ONTOGENY

Introduction

Predators can affect the growth rates and traits of their prey (trait-mediated interactions; TMIs), independent of their influence on prey mortality, which can have strong effects on prey populations (Preisser et al. 2005). In order to lower predation risk prey must make a trade-off between the acquisition and metabolic use of resources and the expression of defensive phenotypes (Johansson et al. 2001, Steiner 2007, Werner &

Anholt 1996, Van Buskirk & Schmidt 2000). By altering prey traits such as behavior, morphology, and physiology, predators can indirectly affect how their prey interact with other species (Peacor & Werner 2001, Werner & Peacor 2003). Such trait-mediated indirect interactions (TMIIs; Abrams 1995) can have important consequences for population dynamics and the structure and functioning of ecological communities (Bolker et al. 2003, Miner et al. 2005, Schmitz et al. 2008, Werner & Peacor 2003).

Theoretical and empirical work has shown that the magnitude and even the direction of TMIs and TMIIs are context dependent, shifting with changes in prey resource use, resource responses to those changes, and their effects on intra- and interspecific competition (Peacor 2002, Peacor & Werner 2004, Relyea 2000, Werner &

23

Anholt 1996). For example, under conditions of high resources/low competition, prey growth rates can be negatively affected by the presence of predators due to reduced foraging and energy income. Further, because net herbivory is low and resources are primarily self-limiting, reduced prey foraging will only have a weak influence on resource levels. However, as competition increases and resources become limited by herbivory (e.g. prey are getting larger), predator induced reductions in prey foraging can have a strong positive effect on resources, via a trait-mediated trophic cascade

(Beckerman et al. 1997). In fact, this can indirectly increase resource availability to the extent that it compensates for reduced prey foraging, resulting in a net positive effect of predators on prey growth (Bolnick & Preisser 2005, Peacor 2002, Peacor & Werner

2004). Because the trait-mediated indirect interactions of predators with resource levels can affect their trait-mediated interactions with prey, and that these effects can change over time, it is necessary to look at resource dynamics when examining how predators influence prey growth.

Predators can also decrease the impacts of competition on prey growth

(Gureveitch et al. 2000), and can alter and even reverse competitive interactions among prey (Peacor & Werner 2000, 2001, Relyea 2000). This is attributed to predator-induced changes in prey behavior and morphology that affects their acquisition and use of resources (Reylea 2000, 2002a, Werner & Anholt 1996), thus indirectly affecting competitors. However, in the majority of these studies resource levels are not measured through time, instead the growth rates of focal organisms are used as an indirect

24

measurement of resource levels (e.g. Peacor 2002, Peacor & Werner 2004). This method is problematic because it is difficult to distinguish between trait-mediated interactions driven by prey interactions with resources vs. other factors such as costs of plasticity

(Callahan et al. 2008, Relyea 2002b), or other indirect effects being propagated through the food web (Wootton 1994). For example predators can indirectly increase resources due to nutrient excretion and egestion that is independent of their effects on prey phenotypes (Vanni 2002). In this case, predators could reduce competitive interactions among consumers because their facilitation of resources, which could be independent of effects on prey traits. Because generating predator cues usually involves feeding caged predators prey that are external to the focal system, this represents a potentially large nutrient subsidy to the system that is seldom addressed in the experimental studies of the non-lethal effects of aquatic predators. Therefore, besides monitoring resources levels over time, it is also necessary to examine nutrient mediated effects of predators on resources without the presence of prey in order to address possible mechanisms explaining predator effects on prey, their competitors and their resources.

Predicting how predators will alter prey traits and the consequences of these trait shifts for communities is further complicated by the fact that prey responses to predation risk can vary between species and even through ontogeny within a species (Benard 2004,

Hettyey et al. 2010, Reylea 2003). Predator effects on prey phenotype can change through ontogeny because prey often exhibit responses to predators that reflects their relative predation risk. For example, larger individuals often respond less or not at all to

25

the presence of gape-limited predators (Peacor & Werner 2000, Riessen & Trevett-Smith

2009, Werner & Anholt 1996). Thus, it is necessary to examine prey responses to predators through ontogeny, as their trait-mediated interactions with predators can change, making it likely the resulting indirect effects of these changes on other taxa will change through prey ontogeny as well.

Research on the community consequences of the non-lethal effects of predators has focused on the interactions between anuran larvae and their predators in temperate ponds (McDiarmid & Altig 1999, Wilbur 1997). In many of these studies resources are often not measured or measured only at the end of the study. Most tadpoles are assumed to be generalist periphyton grazers, although recent work suggests their may be substantial trophic variation between tadpole species (Altig et al. 2007, Wells 2007,

Whiles et al. 2010). This is despite the fact that competition is thought to be relatively common within tadpole assemblages (Gurevitch et al. 2000), yet little is known about how they actually compete (Alford 1999). This is especially true in tropical lentic systems where many tropical anuran species have prolonged breeding periods, increasing the opportunity for cohorts of different species and different ages to interact. The effects of predators on such interactions are also relatively unexplored, in fact, predator induced trait-mediated effects in tropical anurans been investigated only recently (Warkentin

2000, Touchon & Warkentin 2008, Vonesh & Warkentin 2006, Warkentin & Caldwell

2009), despite the fact that predation is thought to be more important structuring tropical than temperate communities (Azevedo-Ramos et al. 1999, Hero et al. 2001, Paine 1969).

26

In this experiment we examine the independent and combined effects of interspecific competition and a caged dragonfly predator on the resources and growth of tadpoles of two commonly co-occurring Neotropical hylid , the red-eyed treefrog (Agalychnis callidryas) and the hourglass tree frog (Dendropsophus ebraccatus) over their larval period. It has been shown that A. callidryas increases phytoplankton and decreases some zooplankton taxa, and both species consume periphyton (Costa, MS thesis, Chapter 1.).

Thus, it is likely both species compete for periphyton, but they interact differently with basal resources and other consumers.

In this experiment we address: (1) The independent and combined effects of predators and tadpoles on periphyton and phytoplankton levels over time. (2) The effects of predators on the growth rates of tadpoles through ontogeny. (3) The importance of interspecific competition for both species. We also examine if (4) the presence of predators affects competitive interactions between the tadpoles through time and (5) The independent and combined effects of predators and tadpoles on other consumers

(zooplankton). And finally, (6) can resource and zooplankton dynamics provide insight into the effects of predators and competitors on tadpole growth.

27

Methods

Dendropsophus ebraccatus and A. callidryas commonly co-occur in lowland ponds throughout their ranges from southern Mexico to Panama. At the study site they are locally abundant and eggs, tadpoles and adults commonly co-occur throughout the rainy season (May-December). The larvae of the Amazon darner dragonfly (Anax amazili) are also found in the breeding ponds shared by the two frog species near

Gamboa (J.C. Touchon & J.R. Vonesh unpublished data). Gonzalez et al. (2011) used lethal dragonfly predators in combination with a substitutive design to examine predation’s effect on competition between these two species. They found that predators dramatically reduced the growth rates of both species and erased the competitive asymmetry that favored A. callidryas in the absence of predators, despite the fact that A. callidryas was more vulnerable to predation. However, this experiment was performed over a short-time period (8 days), food was supplemented (i.e., limited internal resource dynamics), and because they used a substitutive design, it was not possible to separate the absolute strength of inter- and intraspecific competition between the two species.

Experimental design: This study was conducted at the Smithsonian Tropical Research

Institute in Gamboa, Panama (9°7′17″ N,79°42′11″W) between 30 September and 28

October 2010. To examine the interaction between predator presence and competition on the growth and survival of A. callidryas and D. ebraccatus through ontogeny, I conducted

28

a completely randomized 2 x 4 factorial design in which the presence or absence of a caged A. amazili was crossed with four levels of tadpole species composition (25 A. callidryas, 25 D. ebraccatus, 25 A. callidryas and 25 D. ebraccatus or no tadpoles).

Each treatment was replicated five times and the experiment ran for 28 days. I used an additive design to look at the absolute strength of interspecific competition. I also measured periphyton, phytoplankton and zooplankton periodically throughout the experiment. Treatments were randomly assigned to 400L mesocosms (0.75 m diameter x

0.8 m high, with screened drain holes at 0.75 m height) arranged in five blocks of eight tanks each. Tanks were filled with a mixture of filtered aged rain and tap water on 9

September. On 10 September 15 g of rabbit food (primarily alfalfa) pellets, fifteen large

Inga tree leaves (~250 cm2), and 100ml aliquots of concentrated pond inoculate were added to each tank. A second 100 ml pond inoculation was added on 19 September.

Pond inoculate was collected from an artificial pond by repeatedly sweeping a plankton net through the water column and then filtering it through a 1 mm mesh filter to exclude large invertebrates but allowing experimental communities of phytoplankton, periphyton and zooplankton to establish. Tanks were securely covered with fine nylon mesh to prevent colonization by non-experimental organisms. Aquatic communities were allowed to establish for twenty-one days after first inoculation before the start of the experiment.

Four additional tanks were stocked with 115 tadpoles of each species to provide prey for caged predators. These tanks were filled on 29 September and given 6.0 g of rabbit food

29

and 3.0 g of Sera Micron® powdered algar. Additional resources of 3.0 g of rabbit food and 1.5g of sera-micron were added to these tanks on 4 October.

On 22 September, I collected twelve one-day old A. callidryas clutches from

Experimental Pond to use as focal tadpoles. On 23 September thirteen newly laid D. ebraccatus clutches were collected from Ocelot Pond and twelve clutches were collected from Experimental Pond. All eggs were kept in an ambient temperature laboratory and misted frequently to prevent desiccation. Most clutches hatched on 26 September, and some A. callidryas unhatched clutches were manually stimulated to induce hatching

(Warkentin 2000). For D. ebraccatus, focal tadpoles captured from Ocelot Pond but feeder tadpoles included a combination of tadpoles from both ponds. Agalychnis callidryas feeder tadpoles were hatched on 26 September from some individuals collected from Experimental Pond on 22 Sept, in addition to 13 clutches ages 0-2 days collected from Ocelot Pond on 22 September. Thus, focal and feeder tadpoles came from egg clutches of different ages, but they were all hatched on the same day and were introduced into mesocosms on 1 October after they were haphazardly sampled from their respective groups and assigned to treatments randomly.

Anax amazili were collected from Quarry Pond and placed in mesh cages (40 cm deep x 10 cm diameter) constructed from mesh window screen (1.2 mm mesh diameter).

Cages were suspended at the top of the water column on the south side of each tank.

Predators were introduced on 1 October and fed six tadpoles every three days. Predators in single species tadpole treatments were only fed tadpoles of that species. The mixed

30

tadpole and no tadpole-predator treatments were fed three individuals of each tadpole species. The number of feeder tadpoles stayed fixed through the experiment, but they grew in a similar manner to focal tadpoles so feeder tadpole biomass increased over time.

Predators that did not eat all tadpoles during each three day period were replaced.

Feeding stopped on 21 October, and all feeder tadpoles were eaten by 25 October and no predators were replaced. For each round of feeding an equal number of tadpoles were haphazardly chosen from each feeder tank and haphazardly assigned to predator treatments.

Focal tadpoles were dorsally photographed in a white tray with a Nikon D40x digital camera and added to mesocosms on 1 October. Approximately twenty tadpoles were dip-netted from each tank on 13 October and photographed (twenty of each species in mix-species treatments), and all tadpoles were dip-netted again on 28 October and re- photographed. All photographs included a ruler for calibration to use ImageJ

(http://rsbweb.nih.gov/ij/) to obtain measurements of tadpole length.

To measure the effects of tadpoles and predators on resources and other potential competitors, I sampled phytoplankton and zooplankton from each tank on day 0 (before tadpole additions), 12, 20 and 27 using a 1 L integrated tube sampler. Three samples were collected from the water column of each tank and a 100 ml subsample was taken and put on ice and then vacuum filtered on Whatman GF/A glass filters. Filters were extracted in 95% ethanol for 24 hours under refrigeration in the dark. Samples were then

31

read in Aquafluor fluorimeter following standard protocol to estimate μg of chlorophyll a per unit volume (Welschmeyer 1994).

Zooplankton samples were obtained by filtering 2 L on a 20 μm Nitex® filter which was rinsed with 15 ml of tap water. Four to six drops of Lygol’s solution were added to stain the soft tissue and silhouette. Zooplankton were counted in the lab at 30X magnification with a stereo microscope

Periphyton was sampled on 13 October and 28 October. Before the tanks were filled with water, 38 cm2 pieces of tape were stuck to the north and south walls of the tank at both 70 and 45 cm from the bottom (Austin et al. 1981). On 13 October tape was removed with forceps, dried, and placed into 30 ml of ethanol to extract chlorophyll a as above. On 28 October two 115 cm2 sections of tank wall were scraped from both the north and south sides of the tanks (60 cm from bottom) and vacuum filtered on Whatman

GF/A glass filters to estimate μg of chlorophyll a per mm2 (Aloi 1990).

Analysis: All statistical analyses were conducted on tank means using R version 2.11.0

(R Development Core Team 2010). For phytoplankton, periphyton and for tadpole total length (TL) and growth rates, values were log transformed to homogenize variances, and analyzed using repeated measure linear mixed effects models (LMM) to examine their responses to interactions among predator presence, tadpole species composition, and time, with repeated measures on the same tank treated as the random factor. Model selection in all analyses was based on minimizing Akaike information criterion (AIC;

Burnham & Anderson 2002) but retaining significant (p < 0.05) explanatory variables.

32

For each model, if necessary, we also chose specific variance-covariance structures, and heterogeneity of variances at different time points, based on minimizing Akaike information criterion. For TL analysis, each species was considered separately and initial measurements were excluded because there were no differences between treatments for each species, and there was low variance. Because A. callidryas is a larger species than

D. ebraccatus, we analyzed relative growth rates as log (final or mid TL/ mid or initial

TL) to facilitate comparisons of species responses to competitors and predators. Total tank biomass was calculated using length-mass regressions (Vonesh & Costa unpub.) for each species from the number of tadpoles remaining at the end of the experiment. Values were log transformed and analyzed with a mixed linear model examining the interaction between tadpole composition and predator presence treating block as a random factor.

Percent mortality of each species from each tank was calculated using the number of remaining tadpoles and was analyzed with generalized linear model (GLM) with a binomial error distribution. For zooplankton data we used generalized linear mixed models (GLMM) to examine the interactions among time, predator presence and tadpole composition. We minimized overdispersion in the count data by using either a Poisson or

Gamma distribution (Pinheiro & Bates 2000), and also specifying the variance- covariance structure and treating repeated measures of tanks as a random factor. Post- hoc comparisons of treatments were conducted using Tukey’s HSD.

33

Results

Phytoplankton chlorophyll a levels were affected by time (F1,118 = 8046, P <

0.001) and tadpole composition (F3,35 = 5.09, P = 0.005; Figure 1B). Phytoplankton levels decreased in all treatments by day 12 and then increased through the end of the experiment. The main effect of predators was marginal (F1,35 = 3.78, P = 0.06), but there was a significant predator by time interaction (F1,118 = 7.67, P = 0.007) in which predators tended to increase chlorophyll a levels over time (Figure 1A). Averaged across the entire experiment phytoplankton levels were the same in the mixed and A. callidryas treatments

(35.45 μg/L-1), which represented an increase of 21.3% and 28.3% as compared to the D. ebraccatus and no tadpole treatments, respectively.

Significant predictors of periphyton chlorophyll a levels included time (F1,39 =

555, P < 0.001), predators (F1,35 = 13.47, P < 0.001; Figure 2A) and tadpole composition

(F3,35 = 3.68, P = 0.02; Figure 2B), no interactions were significant. Midway through the experiment periphyton levels were highest in the no tadpole and D. ebraccatus treatments, and across all treatments predators doubled periphyton compared to no predator treatments. By the end of the experiment overall periphyton levels were greatly reduced, but predators increased periphyton by 85.1%, compared to no predator treatments. Tadpoles decreased periphyton at both time points and this effect was more pronounced at the end of the experiment.

34

Initially, A. callidryas tadpoles were 15.69 ± 0.31 mm (mean ± SD) in total length

(TL), and there were marginal differences across treatments (F3,16 = 3.02, P = 0.061).

This was because by chance when tadpoles were assigned to tanks there were marginal differences between the two no predator treatments in which tadpoles from the single species treatment (15.89 ± 0.06) were larger than those from the mixed treatment (15.42

± 0.23; Tukey’s HSD; P = 0.07). Total length of A. callidryas was affected by time, predators, and the interactions between predators and predators and competitors (Table 1,

Figure 3A). Predators reduced tadpole TL by 21.9% and 6.4% at the midpoint and end of the experiment respectively. In the absence of predators D. ebraccatus initially reduced

A. callidryas’ size by 2.8%, but in the presence of predators D. ebraccatus actually increased their growth by 12.9%. At the end of the experiment in the absence of predators there was little difference between competitor treatments (0.6% reduction in TL by D. ebraccatus) while in the predator treatments, D. ebraccatus increased A. callidryas’ size by 7.1%. Growth rates of A. callidryas were influenced by time and the interaction between predators and time (Figure 4A), in which predators initially decreased growth rates by 32.3%, but by the end of the experiment, growth rates were increased by 128% in the presence of predators (Table 1).

Initial sizes of D. ebraccatus tadpoles were 7.74 ± 0.15 mm TL, and there were no differences in size across treatments (F3,16 = 1.98, P = 0.157). Total length of D. ebraccatus was affected by time, and the interaction between predators and time and competitors and time (Table 2, Figure 3B). Initially predators had no affect on length,

35

but at the end of the experiment predators increased length by 13.3% compared to no predator treatments. Similarly, initially, A. callidryas initially had no affect on D. ebraccatus length, but by the end of the experiment they had reduced tadpole size by

13%. Time and competitors were significant predictors of D. ebraccatus growth rates, as

A. callidryas initially had no affect, but in the second half of the experiment they reduced

D. ebraccatus growth by 44.4%. Predators also had a marginal effect on D. ebraccatus growth rates: midway through the experiment there were no differences, but in the second half of the experiment predators increased growth rates by 49.8% (Figure 4B; Table 2).

There were no interactions among these factors.

The biomass of each species was not affected by treatment (A. callidryas: F3,12 =

1.81, P = 0.20; D. ebraccatus: F3,12 = 1.42, P = 0.29). Within species, A. callidryas biomass was marginally decreased by predators, but not competitors or their interaction

(Table 1). The biomass of D. ebraccatus was not affected by predators, competitors or their interaction (Table 2). Total tank biomass was affected by tadpole composition (F2,20

= 24.76, P < 0.001), but not predators (F1,20 = 0.05, P = 0.83) or their interaction (F2,20 =

1.14, P = 0.34). This was because D. ebraccatus (3.26 ± 2.15g) had much lower biomass than A. callidryas (8.77 ± 2.66g), regardless of treatment. Overall, tadpole mortality was low for D. ebraccatus (0.058 ± 0.055) and A. callidryas (0.02 ± 0.038), but there were no differences in mortality rates between the species (χ2 = 0.38, P = 0.54). Mortality was not affected by treatment (χ2 = 0.56, P = 0.99), predator presence (χ2 < 0.01, P = 0.95), or competitors (χ2 = 0.59, P = 0.9).

36

The total number of zooplankton was affected by tadpole composition and time

(Table 4, Figure 9A). Total zooplankton numbers decreased over time and averaged across all time points, tadpole treatments decreased total zooplankton by 17.9% compared to no tadpole treatments. Copepod abundances were affected by predators and time (Figure 9B). Initially copepods were slightly higher in predator treatments, and this trend continued over time. Averaged across the entire experiment, predators increased copepods by 88.1% compared to no tadpole treatments. Nauplii (early copepod instars) did not respond to any experimental treatments but they gradually decreased through the experiment. Similarly, ostracods were also only affected by time, but they gradually increased. Rotifers were only affected by tadpole composition (Figure 9C). Averaged over the entire experiment tadpoles decreased rotifer abundances by 38.5% compared to no tadpole treatments. No treatments predicted cladoceran abundances except for the predator by tadpole composition interaction. Overall predators increased cladocerans compared to no predator treatments, but this increase depended on the tadpole treatment.

37

Discussion

In order to elucidate possible mechanism explaining tadpole growth responses to predators and competitors, we first looked at the independent and combined effects of tadpole composition and predators on periphyton and phytoplankton levels. We found that the chlorophyll a levels of phytoplankton initially decreased, and then increased toward the end of the experiment, and more so in the A. callidryas and mixed tadpole treatments. Phytoplankton has previously been shown to respond positively to the presence of A. callidryas, but the mechanisms for this remain unknown (Costa, MS thesis, Chapter 1). Although the main effect of predators on phytoplankton was marginal, there was a significant predator by time interaction in which predators increased phytoplankton as the experiment progressed. Periphyton chlorophyll a decreased in all treatments over time, but there was a main effect of predators that increased periphyton abundances. All tadpole treatments reduced periphyton and this effect was more pronounced at the end of the experiment, as periphyton reached relatively low levels (<

2.5 μg/cm2), suggesting that all tadpole treatments were limited by this resource, regardless of predators. For both periphyton and phytoplankton there were no tadpole by predator interactions, suggesting that the responses of these resources to the presence of predators was not dependent on the presence of tadpoles. Thus the main positive effect of predators on both resources was not occurring solely due to a trait-mediated trophic cascade. Instead, is likely that nutrient cycling due to the excretion and egestion of the

38

also contributed to the growth of both resources (Schmitz et al. 2010, Vanni

2002).

The fact that predators tended to facilitate both basal resources may explain how their trait-mediated effects on tadpole growth changed over time. In this experiment, A. callidryas’ TL and growth rates were initially reduced by dragonflies but this affect was ameliorated over time, as shown by the significant predator by time effects on total length and growth rates. In the second half of the experiment, predators actually increased growth rates by 128% compared to no predator treatments, which almost completely compensated for the initial decrease in growth. As predators increased phytoplankton over time, and increased periphyton as well, it is possible these tadpoles benefited from the presence of predators later in the experiment due to an increase in their resources as

Agalychnis callidryas filter feeds phytoplankton and grazes periphyton (Costa, MS thesis,

Chapter 1, McDiarmid & Altig 1999, Wassersug & Rosenberg 1979). It is unknown to what extent A. callidryas depends on both of these resources but it is likely that predators increased tadpole growth later in ontogeny because they increased phytoplankton over time, and they reduced competition for periphyton, which became more important as it became a limiting resource toward the end of the experiment.

Similarly, the size of D. ebraccatus was also affected by the interaction between predators and time. Midway through the experiment predators had no affect on their length or growth rates, but in the second half predators increased their growth rates and their final size. This result is surprising as this species has been shown to reduce their

39

growth in response to dragonflies (Gonzalez et al. 2001), and that these tadpoles respond morphologically to dragonflies and other aquatic invertebrate predators by altering the size and color of their tails (Costa unpublished data, Touchon & Warkentin 2008). So it is possible that similar to A. callidryas, these tadpoles’ responses to these predators were changing through ontogeny due to predator facilitation of their resources. The tadpoles of D. ebraccatus do not filter feed, but they do consume periphyton (Costa, MS thesis,

Chapter 1, McDiarmid & Altig 1999). As periphyton appeared to be a limiting resource in all tadpole treatments by the end of the experiment it is likely predators were able to increase tadpole size and although marginally, their growth rates as well, due to their facilitation of periphyton.

For both tadpole species it is likely that predator effects on their growth changed over time due to increased competition for resources (Bolnick & Preisser 2005, Relyea

2004). Although we were not able to distinguish between the two most plausible mechanisms, it is clear that predators can indirectly facilitate resources both through nutrient cycling (Vanni 2002) and through induced changes in the foraging of their prey

(Beckerman et al. 1997, Peacor & Werner 2001). Given the nonlinear nature between resource growth rates and total resource levels, a reduction in consumer foraging and/or an increase in limiting nutrients can potentially cause a proportionately larger increase in resource levels than one might expect. In this case the indirect negative effect of predators on prey growth will be less than the indirect positive effect due to the increase in total resource levels. This positive effect of predators on resource levels can then

40

compensate for reduced foraging by tadpoles, to the extent that predators can have a net positive effect on the growth rates of their prey (Peacor 2002). These results suggest that the trait-mediated effects of predators on tadpole growth rates can change through ontogeny and are strongly dependent on resource dynamics (Peacor & Werner 2004,

Werner & Anholt 1996).

Alternatively, for both species it is likely that tadpoles are altering their responses to predators to reflect their relative predation risks. For A. callidryas tadpoles it has been shown that they reach a size refuge from predation as they get larger, so larger tadpoles do not change their growth in response to predators (Vonesh et al. unpublished data).

The tadpoles of D.ebraccatus also approach a size refuge to most predators at they near (Costa unpublished data), so like other tadpole species, it is likely their trait-mediated interactions with predators also change through ontogeny (Hettyey et al.

2010, Relyea 2003, Werner & Anholt 1996). It is interesting to note that Gonzalez et al.

(2011) found that these same predators strongly reduced the growth of both of these tadpole species over 8 days, despite the fact that dragonflies consumed > 60% of both species. However, they also found that A. callidryas tadpoles were more vulnerable to predation, so perhaps in our experiment they initially responded to their relatively higher risk, and reduced their growth while D. ebraccatus did not.

Competition for resources and resource dynamics may have also played a role in

A. callidryas’ response to competitors and predators, as there was a significant predator by competitor interaction on their size. In the absence of predators, the presence of D.

41

ebraccatus slightly reduced A. callidryas size at both time points, while in the presence of predators D. ebraccatus had a much greater positive effect on their length. Thus the presence of caged predators reversed the sign of their interaction, for competition to facilitation. Since predators increased periphyton, regardless of tadpole treatments, it is possible they simply reduced competition for that resource. Similarly, competition is expected to reduce nonlethal effects of predators on prey growth when there is competition for resources, because prey can not “afford” to reduce their foraging

(Luttbeg et al. 2003, Peacor & Werner 2001, Relyea 2004). As previously mentioned, reduced tadpole foraging in response to predators under high competition/low resource conditions can actually increase total resource levels, which can result in a net positive effect of predators on prey growth (Peacor 2002), regardless of their nutrient based facilitation of resources. Alternatively, D. ebraccatus could have improved A. callidryas growth in the presence of predators because of the higher overall density of tadpoles.

Due to our additive design, it is possible A. callidryas is responding to their relative risk.

In the presence of more heterospecifics, we might expect their responses to predators to be reduced. Higher densities of conspecifics has been shown to reduce the negative effects of predators on the growth rates of A. callidryas and other tadpole species

(McCoy 2007, Van Buskirk et al. 2011, Vonesh et al. in press), but this has not been addressed with heterospecifics. Although it is not possible to distinguish among these mechanisms it is likely that they are all occurring simultaneously and are contributing to our results.

42

Another explanation of D. ebraccatus’ facilitation of A. callidryas’ growth in the presence of predators is the amount of predator cue concentrations in the mixed species versus the single species tadpole treatments. Predators in mixed species treatments were fed the same total number of tadpoles but half as many of each species than the single species treatments. Previous work has shown the growth response of A. callidryas to predators is an asymptotic function of prey biomass consumed, and increasing prey biomass while holding prey number constant elicits a smaller asymptotic response than increasing both the biomass and the number of prey consumed (McCoy et al. in review).

So this could explain how focal species’ responses to predators in mixed species treatments may be reduced. We did not see this with D. ebraccatus, but we did with A. callidryas. This scenario is unlikely however, given that the biomass of individual feeder tadpoles increased over time enough to surpass the biomass required to reach the asymptote of the phenotypic response of A. callidryas (~0.2g). Furthermore, tadpoles have been shown to respond similarly to chemical cues from predators feeding on other tadpole species (Schoeppner & Relyea 2005) so it is unlikely the amount of predation cues can explain our results.

For D. ebraccatus, size was affected by a competitor by time interaction in which

A. callidryas initially had no affect on their size, but by the end of the experiment their

TL was reduced by 13%. Similarly, A. callidryas had a significant negative effect on their growth rates (~44%), but only in the second half of the experiment. This is most likely due to an increase in exploitative competition for periphyton over time, as A.

43

callidryas has been shown to be a more efficient periphyton grazer per unit biomass

(Costa, MS thesis, Chapter 1). This is also similar to previous work that showed D. ebraccatus’ growth was 13% slower with A. callidryas than with the same number of conspecifics (Gonzalez et al. 2011). However, this previous experiment was much shorter in duration (8 days), supplemented food, and used a substitutive design, making straight forward comparisons between the two studies difficult.

Despite the strong effects of predators and competitors on tadpole growth, total tank biomass was only affected by tadpole composition, due to the fact that D. ebraccatus treatments were lower than A. callidryas and mixed species treatments. Given that mixed species treatments had twice as many tadpoles, per individual biomass levels then decreased in these treatments as total tadpole biomass appears to be limited by interspecific competition. Similarly, a previous study showed that the total tank biomass of both species did not change despite the doubling of intraspecific competitors (Costa,

MS thesis, Chapter 1), showing that both intra- and interspecific competition can limit total tank biomass production. Within each species however, D. ebraccatus was not affected by any explanatory variables, while A. callidryas’ total tank biomass was reduced only by the presence of predators, although this effect was marginal. For A. callidryas, it appears that the initial decrease in growth caused by predators was able to affect their final biomass, despite the positive effect of predators in the second half of the experiment. Conversely, D. ebraccatus’ biomass was not affected by competitors or

44

predators which decreased and increased their growth, respectively, but only in the second half of the experiment.

Predator and competitor effects on tadpole growth and resources could also be attributed to indirect effects due to their interactions with zooplankton. Predators tended to increase copepods while tadpoles decreased total zooplankton and rotifers. Because predators increased phytoplankton, their facilitation of copepods could be due to an increase in this resource. It is not clear exactly how tadpoles interact with these different zooplankton groups, but previous work in this system has shown A. callidryas can reduce copepods, their younger instars, nauplii, and total zooplankton levels (Costa, MS thesis,

Chapter 1). However, it appears these effects can be variable across experiments and over time, so these results may not reflect biologically relevant interactions and it is unlikely tadpole and predator interactions with zooplankton are indirectly contributing to the effects of competitors and predators on tadpole growth and basal resources.

The growth of each of these tadpole species was affected by both competitors and predators, but depending on the timing during ontogeny, both the magnitude and the direction of these effects changed. This is important because the size and growth of larval can determine their fitness because it can affect time to metamorphosis, juvenile growth, size at maturity, and future egg production (Berven &

Gill 1983, Semlitsch et al. 1988, Smith 1983, Van Allen et al. 2010). Although I did not quantify behavior in this study, previous work in the lab and field has shown that the responses of these two species to the presence of predators and competitors does not

45

appear to be attributable to changes in behavior per se (Costa unpub, Vonesh &

Warkentin 2006). Instead, differences between the species in their responses to predators and competitors are most likely due to their perception of relative predation risk, their different resource requirements, and the complex interplay among the costs of avoiding predation, resource responses to altered foraging and predator nutrient cycling, and their effects on intra- and interspecific competition. Thus, future studies examining predator induced trait-mediated interactions should monitor resources and growth responses over time in order to disentangle the effects of ontogeny and resource levels on the trait- mediated impacts of predators on prey and their communities.

46

References

Abrams PA. 1995. Implications of dynamically variable traits for identifying, classifying, and measuring direct and indirect effects in ecological communities. Am Nat 146(1):112-34.

Alford, RA. 1999. Ecology: Resource use, competition, and predation. In RW McDiarmid & R. Altig. Tadpoles: The biology of anuran larvae. pp. 240-278. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.

Aloi JE. 1990. A critical review of recent freshwater periphyton field methods. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 47(3):656-70.

Altig R, Whiles MR, Taylor CL. 2007. What do tadpoles really eat? Assessing the trophic status of an understudied and imperiled group of consumers in freshwater habitats. Freshwat Biol 52(2):386-95.

Altig, R and Johnston GF. 1989. Guilds of anuran larvae: relationships among developmental modes, morphologies, and habitats. Herpetological Monographs 3: 81-109.

Austin A, Lang S, Pomeroy M. 1981. Simple methods for sampling periphyton with observations on sampler design criteria. Hydrobiologia 85(1):33-48.

Azevedo-Ramos C, Magnusson W, Bayliss P. 1999. Predation as the key factor structuring tadpole assemblages in a savanna area in central amazonia. Copeia (1):22- 33.

Beckerman A, Petchey OL, Morin PJ. 2010. Adaptive foragers and community ecology: Linking individuals to communities and ecosystems. Funct Ecol 24(1):1-6.

Beckerman BR, Uriarte M. Schmitz OJ. 1994. Experimental evidence for a behavior- mediated trophic cascade in a terrestrial food chain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 94(20):10735-38. Benard MF. 2004. Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity in organisms with complex life histories. Annual Review of Ecology Evolution and Systematics 35:651-73.

Berven K and D. 1983. Interpreting geographic-variation in life-history traits. Am Zool 23(1):85-97.

47

Blumenshine SC and Hambright KD. 2003. Top-down control in pelagic systems: A role for invertebrate predation. Hydrobiologia 491(1-3):347-56.

Bolker B, Holyoak M, Krivan V, Rowe L, Schmitz O. 2003. Connecting theoretical and empirical studies of trait-mediated interactions. Ecology 84(5):1101-14.

Bolnick DI and Preisser EL. 2005. Resource competition modifies the strength of trait- mediated predator-prey interactions: A meta-analysis. Ecology 86(10):2771-9.

Brett MT and Goldman CR. 1996. A meta-analysis of the freshwater cascade (vol 93, pg 7723, 1996). Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A 93(26):15522-.

Bronmark C, Rundle SD, Erlandsson A. 1991. Interactions between fresh-water snails and tadpoles - competition and facilitation. Oecologia 87(1):8-18.

Burnham, KP & Anderson, DR. 2002. Model selection and multimodel inference. A practical information-theoretic approach. Springer, New York.

Byers S and Vinyard GL. 1990. The effects on the plankton community of filter-feeding sacramento blackfish, orthodon-microlepidotus. Oecologia 83(3):352-7.

Callahan HS, Maughan H, Steiner UK. 2008. Phenotypic plasticity, costs of phenotypes, and costs of plasticity toward an integrative view. Year in Evolutionary Biology 2008 1133:44-66.

Chalcraft DR and Resetarits Jr. WJ. 2004. Metabolic rate models and the substitutability of predator populations. J Anim Ecol 73(2):323-32.

Chalcraft DR and Resetarits WJ. 2003a. Predator identity and ecological impacts: Functional Redundancy or diversity? Ecology 84(9):2407-18.

Chalcraft DR and Resetarits Jr. WJ. 2003b. Mapping functional similarity of predators on the basis of trait similarities. Am Nat 162(4):390-402.

Chang KH and Hanazato T. 2003. Vulnerability of cladoceran species to predation by the copepod mesocyclops leuckarti: Laboratory observations on the behavioural interactions between predator and prey. Freshwat Biol 48(3):476-84.

Chang KH and Hanazato T. 2005a. Impact of selective predation by mesocyclops pehpeiensis on a zooplankton community: Experimental analysis using mesocosms. Ecol Res 20(6):726-32.

48

Chang KH and Hanazato T. 2005b. The predacious cladoceran leptodora kindtii as a prey for the cyclopoid copepod mesocyclops sp.: Laboratory observations of predator-prey interaction. J Freshwat Ecol 20(4):655-60.

Confer JL. 1972. Interrelations among plankton, attached algae, and the phosphorus cycle in artificial open systems. Ecol Monogr 42(1):2-23.

Connelly S, Pringle CM, Bixby RJ, Brenes R, Whiles MR, Lips KR, Kilham S, Huryn AD. 2008. Changes in stream primary producer communities resulting from large- scale catastrophic declines: Can small-scale experiments predict effects of tadpole loss? Ecosystems 11(8):1262-76.

Danger M, Lacroix G, Ka S, Ndour EH, Corbin D, Lazzaro X. 2009. Food-web structure and functioning of temperate and tropical lakes: A stoichiometric viewpoint. Annales De Limnologie-International Journal of Limnology 45(1):11-21.

Davidson DW, Inouye RS, Brown JH. 1984. Granivory in a desert ecosystem: Experimental evidence for indirect facilitation of ants by rodents. Ecology 65(6):1780-6.

Davidson DW, Samson DA, Inouye RS. 1985. Granivory in the chihuahuan desert: Interactions within and between trophic levels. Ecology 66(2):486-502.

Dethier MN and Duggins DO. 1984. An "indirect commensalism" between marine herbivores and the importance of competitive hierarchies. Am Nat 124(2):205-19.

Drenner RW, Smith JD, Threlkeld ST. 1996. Lake trophic state and the limnological effects of omnivorous fish. Hydrobiologia 319(3):213-23.

Duellman WE and Treub L. 1986. Biology of amphibians. McGraw-Hill. New York, NY.

Elser JJ and Urabe J. 1999. The stoichiometry of consumer-driven nutrient recycling: Theory, observations, and consequences. Ecology 80(3):735-51.

Flecker A. 1992. Fish trophic guilds and the structure of a tropical stream - weak direct vs strong indirect effects. Ecology 73(3):927-40.

Flecker A. 1996. Ecosystem engineering by a dominant detritivore in a diverse tropical stream. Ecology 77(6):1845-54.

49

Flecker AS, Feifarek BP, Taylor BW. 1999. Ecosystem engineering by a tropical tadpole: Density-dependent effects on habitat structure and larval growth rates. Copeia 1999(2):495-500.

Flecker AS, Taylor BW, Bernhardt ES, Hood JM, Cornwell WK, Cassatt SR, Vanni MJ, Altman NS. 2002. Interactions between herbivorous fishes and limiting nutrients in a tropical stream ecosystem. Ecology 83(7):1831-44.

Fox RJ, Sunderland TL, Hoey AS, Bellwood DR. 2009. Estimating ecosystem function: Contrasting roles of closely related herbivorous rabbitfishes (Siganidae) on coral reefs. Marine Ecology-Progress Series 385:261-9.

Gonzalez SC, Touchon JC, Vonesh JR. 2011. Interactions between competition and predation shape early growth and survival of two neotropical hylid tadpoles. Biotropica 43(5):633-9.

Gotelli NJ. 2001. A Primer of Ecology. Sinauer Associates, Inc. Sunderland, MA.

Gurevitch J, Morrison JA, Hedges LV. 2000. The interaction between competition and predation: A meta-analysis of field experiments. Am Nat 155(4):435-53.

Hansson L. 1988. Effects of competitive interactions on the biomass development of planktonic and periphytic algae in lakes. Limnol Oceanogr 33(1):121-8.

Hansson L. 1990. Quantifying the impact of periphytic algae on nutrient availability for phytoplankton. Freshwat Biol 24(2):265-73.

Harris P. 1995. Are autecologically similar species also functionally similar - a test in pond communities. Ecology 76(2):544-52.

Hero J, Magnusson W, Rocha C, Catterall C. 2001. Antipredator defenses influence the distribution of amphibian prey species in the central Amazon rain forest. Biotropica 33(1):131-41.

Hettyey A, Zsarnoczai S, Vincze K, Hoi H, Laurila A. 2010. Interactions between the information content of different chemical cues affect induced defenses in tadpoles. Oikos 119(11):1814-22.

Holyoak M and Loreau M. 2006. Reconciling empirical ecology with neutral community models. Ecology 87(6):1370-7.

50

Hopp U, Maier G, Bleher R. 1997. Reproduction and adult longevity of five species of planktonic cyclopoid copepods reared on different diets: A comparative study. Freshwat Biol 38(2):289-300.

Hubbel SP. 2001. The unified neutral theory of biodiversity and biogeography. Princeton Univ. Press.

Johansson F, Stoks R, Rowe L, De Block M. 2001. Life history plasticity in a damselfly: Effects of combined time and biotic constraints. Ecology 82(7):1857-69.

Kiffney PM and Richardson JS. 2001. Interactions among nutrients, periphyton, and invertebrate and vertebrate (ascaphus truei) grazers in experimental channels. Copeia 2001(2):422-9.

Knoll LB, McIntyre PB, Vanni MJ, Flecker AS. 2009. Feedbacks of consumer nutrient recycling on producer biomass and stoichiometry: Separating direct and indirect effects. Oikos 118(11):1732-42.

Kumar R and Rao TR. 1999. Demographic responses of adult mesocyclops thermocyclopoides (copepoda, cyclopoida) to different plant and diets. Freshwat Biol 42(3):487-501.

Kupferberg S. 1997. Facilitation of periphyton production by tadpole grazing: Functional differences between species. Freshwat Biol 37(2):427.

Lawton JH and Brown VK. 1993. Redundancy in ecosystems. Biodiveristy and Ecosystem Function. (eds E.D. Schultz & HA Mooney), pp 255-270. Springer Verlag, New York.

Lazzaro X, Bouvy M, Ribeiro RA, Oliviera VS, Sales LT, Vasconcelos ARM, Mata MR. 2003. Do fish regulate phytoplankton in shallow eutrophic northeast brazilian reservoirs? Freshwat Biol 48(4):649-68.

Lazzaro X, Drenner RW, Stein RA, Smith JD. 1992. Planktivores and plankton dynamics - effects of fish biomass and planktivore type. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 49(7):1466-73.

Lazzaro X, Lacroix G, Gauzens B, Gignoux J, Legendre S. 2009. Predator foraging behaviour drives food-web topological structure. J Anim Ecol 78(6):1307-17.

51

Lazzaro X. 1997. Do the trophic cascade hypothesis and classical biomanipulation approaches apply to tropical lakes and reservoirs? International Association of Theoretical and Applied Limnology, Vol 26, Pt 2 26:719-30.

Leibold MA and Wilbur HM. 1992. Interactions between food-web structure and nutrients on pond organisms. Nature 360(6402):341-3.

Loreau M. 2004. Does functional redundancy exist? Oikos 104(3):606-11.

Luttbeg B, Rowe L, Mangel M. 2003. Prey state and experimental design affect relative size of trait- and density-mediated indirect effects. Ecology 84(5):1140-50.

McCoy MW. 2007. Conspecific density determines the magnitude and character of predator-induced phenotype. Oecologia 153(4):871-8.

McDiarmid, RW, and Altig R. 1999. Tadpoles: The biology of anuran larvae. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.

Menezes RF, Attayde JL, Vasconcelos FR. 2010. Effects of omnivorous filter-feeding fish and nutrient enrichment on the plankton community and water transparency of a tropical reservoir. Freshwat Biol 55(4):767-79.

Miner BG, Sultan SE, Morgan SG, Padilla DK, Relyea RA. 2005. Ecological consequences of phenotypic plasticity. Trends in Ecology & Evolution 20(12):685- 92.

Mokany A. 2007. Impact of tadpoles and mosquito larvae on ephemeral pond structure and processes. Mar Freshw Res 58(5):436-44.

Morin PJ. 1999. Community Ecology. Blackwell Publishing. Malden, MA

Nagata T and Hanazato T. 2006. Different predation impacts of two cyclopoid species on a small-sized zooplankton community: An experimental analysis with mesocosms. Hydrobiologia 556:233-42.

Paine R. 1969. A note on trophic complexity and community stability. Am Nat 103(929):91-3. Peacor SD and Werner EE. 2000. Predator effects on an assemblage of consumers through induced changes in consumer foraging behavior. Ecology 81(7):1998-2010.

52

Peacor SD and Werner EE. 2001. The contribution of trait-mediated indirect effects to the net effects of a predator. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 98(7):3904-8.

Peacor SD and Werner EE. 2004. Context dependence of nonlethal effects of a predator on prey growth. Isr J Zool 50(2-3):139-67.

Peacor SD. 2002. Positive effect of predators on prey growth rate through induced modifications of prey behaviour. Ecol Lett 5(1):77-85.

Pinheiro, JC & Bates DM. 2000. Mixed Effects Models in S and S-plus. Springer, New York.

Power ME. 1990. Effects of fish in river food webs. Science 250(4982):811-4.

Preisser EL, Bolnick DI, Benard MF. 2005. Scared to death? the effects of intimidation and consumption in predator-prey interactions. Ecology 86(2):501-9. Relyea R. 2000. Trait-mediated indirect effects in larval anurans: Reversing competition with the threat of predation. Ecology 81(8):2278-89.

Relyea R. 2003. Predators come and predators go: The reversibility of predator-induced traits. Ecology 84(7):1840-8.

Relyea RA. 2002a. The many faces of predation: How induction, selection, and thinning combine to alter prey phenotypes. Ecology 83(7):1953-64.

Relyea RA. 2002b. Costs of phenotypic plasticity. Am Nat 159(3):272-82.

Relyea RA. 2004. Fine-tuned phenotypes: Tadpole plasticity under 16 combinations of predators and competitors. Ecology 85(1):172-9.

Resetarits WJ and Chalcraft DR. 2007. Functional diversity within a morphologically conservative genus of predators: Implications for functional equivalence and redundancy in ecological communities. Funct Ecol 21(4):793-804.

Riessen HP and Trevett-Smith JB. 2009. Turning inducible defenses on and off: Adaptive responses of Daphnia to a gape-limited predator. Ecology 90(12):3455-69.

Schlesinger DA, Molot LA, Shuter BJ. 1981. Specific growth-rates of fresh-water algae in relation to cell-size and light-intensity. Can J Fish Aquat Sci 38(9):1052-8.

53

Schmitz OJ and Suttle KB. 2001. Effects of top predator species on direct and indirect interactions in a food web. Ecology 82(7):2072-81.

Schmitz OJ, Grabowski JH, Peckarsky BL, Preisser EL, Trussell GC, Vonesh JR. 2008. From individuals to ecosystem function: Toward an integration of evolutionary and ecosystem ecology. Ecology 89(9):2436-45.

Schmitz OJ, Hawlena D, Trussell GC. 2010. Predator control of ecosystem nutrient dynamics. Ecol Lett 13(10):1199-209.

Schmitz OJ. 2009. Effects of predator functional diversity on grassland ecosystem function. Ecology 90(9):2339-45.

Schoeppner NM and Relyea RA. 2005. Damage, digestion, and defence: The roles of alarm cues and kairomones for inducing prey defences. Ecol Lett 8(5):505-12.

Seale DB and Wassersug RJ. 1979. Suspension feeding dynamics of anuran larvae related to their functional-morphology. Oecologia 39(3):259-72.

Seale DB. 1980. Influence of amphibian larvae on primary production, nutrient flux, and competition in a pond ecosystem. Ecology 61(6):1531-50.

Semlitsch R, Scott D, Pechmann J. 1988. Time and size at metamorphosis related to adult fitness in Ambystoma-talpoideum. Ecology 69(1):184-92.

Smith D. 1983. Factors controlling tadpole populations of the chorus frog (pseudacris- triseriata) on Isle Royale, Michigan. Ecology 64(3):501-10.

Smith REH and Kalff J. 1983. Competition for phosphorus among co-occurring fresh- water phytoplankton. Limnol Oceanogr 28(3):448-64.

Steiner UK and Van Buskirk J. 2008. Environmental stress and the costs of whole- organism phenotypic plasticity in tadpoles. J Evol Biol 21(1):97-103.

Steiner UK. 2007. Linking antipredator behaviour, ingestion, gut evacuation and costs of predator-induced responses in tadpoles. Anim Behav 74:1473-9.

Stoecker DK, Gifford DJ, Putt M. 1994. Preservation of marine planktonic ciliates - losses and cell shrinkage during fixation. Mar Ecol -Prog Ser 110(2-3):293-9.

54

Taylor BW, Flecker AS, Hall RO, Jr. 2006. Loss of a harvested fish species disrupts carbon flow in a diverse tropical river. Science 313(5788):833-6.

Touchon JC and Warkentin KM. 2008. Fish and dragonfly nymph predators induce opposite shifts in color and morphology of tadpoles. Oikos 117(4):634-40.

Touchon JC and Warkentin KM. 2010. Short- and long-term effects of the abiotic egg environment on viability, development and vulnerability to predators of a neotropical anuran. Funct Ecol 24(3):566-75.

Van Allen BG, Briggs VS, McCoy MW, Vonesh JR. 2010. Carry-over effects of the larval environment on post-metamorphic performance in two hylid frogs. Oecologia 164(4):891-8.

Van Buskirk J and Schmidt BR. 2000. Predator-induced phenotypic plasticity in larval newts: Trade-offs, selection, and variation in nature. Ecology 81(11):3009-28. Van Buskirk J, Ferrari M, Kueng D, Naepflin K, Ritter N. 2011. Prey risk assessment depends on conspecific density. Oikos 120(8):1235-9.

Vandermeer J. 1980. Indirect mutualism: Variations on a theme by Stephen Levine. Am Nat 116(3):441-8.

Vanni MJ and Findlay DL. 1990. Trophic cascades and phytoplankton community structure. Ecology 71(3):921-37.

Vanni MJ and Layne CD. 1997. Nutrient recycling and herbivory as mechanisms in the ''top-down'' effect of fish on algae in lakes. Ecology 78(1):21-40.

Vanni MJ, Flecker AS, Hood JM, Headworth JL. 2002. Stoichiometry of nutrient recycling by vertebrates in a tropical stream: Linking species identity and ecosystem processes. Ecol Lett 5(2):285-93.

Vanni MJ. 2002. Nutrient cycling by in freshwater ecosystems. Annu Rev Ecol Syst 33:341-70.

Vonesh JR and Warkentin KM. 2006. Opposite shifts in size at metamorphosis in response to larval and metamorph predators. Ecology 87(3):556-62.

Warkentin K. 2000. Hatching as a defense against egg predators: The role of vibrational cues. Am Zool 40(6):1250-1.

55

Warkentin KM and Caldwell MS. 2009. Assessing risk: embryos, information and escape hatching. In DR & RJM. Cognitive ecology II: the evolutionary ecology of information processing and decision-making. University of Chicago Press, Chicago.

Wassersug RJ and Rosenberg K. 1979. Surface anatomy of branchial food traps of tadpoles - comparative-study. J Morphol 159(3):393-425.

Watson DL, Bayne DR, DeVries DR, Williams J. 2003. Influence of gizzard shad on phytoplankton size and primary productivity in mesocosms and earthen ponds in the southeastern US. Hydrobiologia 495(1-3):17-32.

Wells, KD 2007. The ecology and behavior of amphibians. University of Chicago Press, Chicago, IL.

Welschmeyer NA. 1994. Fluorometric analysis of chlorophyll-a in the presence of chlorophyll-B and pheopigments. Limnol Oceanogr 39(8):1985-92.

Werner EE and Anholt BR. 1996. Predator-induced behavioral indirect effects: Consequences to competitive interactions in anuran larvae. Ecology 77(1):157-69.

Werner EE and Peacor SD. 2003. A review of trait-mediated indirect interactions in ecological communities. Ecology 84(5):1083-100.

Werner EE. 1994. Ontogenetic scaling of competitive relations: Size-dependent effects and responses in two anuran larvae. Ecology 75(1):197-213.

Whiles MR, Gladyshev MI, Sushchik NN, Makhutova ON, Kalachova GS, Peterson SD, Regester KJ. 2010. Fatty acid analyses reveal high degrees of omnivory and dietary plasticity in pond-dwelling tadpoles. Freshwat Biol 55(7):1533-47.

Whiles MR, Lips KR, Pringle CM, Kilham SS, Bixby RJ, Brenes R, Connelly S, Colon- Gaud JC, Hunte-Brown M, Huryn AD, and others. 2006. The effects of amphibian population declines on the structure and function of neotropical stream ecosystems. Frontiers in Ecology and the Environment 4(1):27-34.

Wilbur HM. 1997. Experimental ecology of food webs: Complex systems in temporary ponds. Ecology 78(8):2279-302.

Wootton, JT. 1994. The nature and consequences of indirect effects in ecological communities. Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 25: 443-66

56

Table 1: Results of two-way ANOVAs on the effects of tadpole species identity and density on the abundances of zooplankton morphospecies.

Tests df F P

Copepods

Species 1,12 20.6 <0.001

Density 1,12 0.3 <0.001

Species x Density 1,12 0.6 0.81

Nauplii

Species 1,12 53.3 <0.001

Density 1,12 0.4 0.53

Species x Density 1,12 4.3 0.06

Rotifers

Species 1,12 4.0 0.07

Density 1,12 0.01 0.94

Species x Density 1,12 0.1 0.72

Total zooplankton

Species 1,12 23.0 <0.001

Density 1,12 0.9 0.36

Species x Density 1,12 1.83 0.20

57

Table 2: Main effects and the significant interactions of time, predators and competitors on the total length, growth rates and final biomass of A. callidryas.

A. callidryas Tests Df F P Total Length: Time 1,18 139.8 <0.001

Predator 1,16 48.0 <0.001

Competitors 1,16 1.86 0.19

Pred*Time 1,16 21.24 <0.001

Pred*Comp 1,16 4.48 0.05 Growth Rate: Time 1,16 228.7 < 0.001

Predator 1,16 2.78 0.11

Competitors 1,16 1.29 0.27

Pred*Time 1,16 58.9 < 0.001 Biomass: Predator 1,14 4.39 0.055

Competitors 1,14 0.13 0.72

Pred*Comp 1,12 1.12 0.31

58

Table 3: Main effects and the significant interactions of time, predators and competitors on the total length, growth rates and final biomass of D. ebraccatus.

D. ebraccatus Tests Df F P Total Length: Time 1,16 119.3 <0.001

Predator 1,16 0.06 0.80

Competitors 1,16 0.2 0.66

Predator*Time 1,16 4.74 0.045

Competitor*Time 1,16 9.73 0.007 Growth Rate: Time 1,18 80.6 < 0.001

Predator 1, 16 4.08 0.061

Competitors 1,16 7.88 0.013 Biomass: Predator 1, 14 2.98 0.11

Competitors 1,14 1.06 0.26

Predator*Competitors 1,12 0.26 0.62

59

Table 4: Main effects and significant interactions among time, predators, and tadpole treatments on the abundances of zooplankton morphospecies.

Tests Zooplankton Df F P Total Time 1,112 101.8 <0.001

Predator 1,32 < 0.01 0.99

Tadpoles 3,32 4.79 0.007 Copepods Time 1,112 14.0 <0.001

Predator 1,32 7.55 0.01

Tadpoles 3,32 0.35 0.79 Nauplii Time 1,112 86.4 <0.001

Predator 1,32 1.98 0.16

Tadpoles 3,32 1.61 0.21 Ostracods Time 1,112 13.3 <0.001

Predator 1,32 0.54 0.47

Tadpoles 3,32 2.28 0.1 Rotifers Time 1,112 2.68 0.1

Predator 1,32 0.93 0.34

Tadpoles 3,32 9.02 <0.001 Cladocerans Time 1,112 0.39 0.54

Predator 1,32 3.11 0.09

Tadpoles 3,32 1.09 0.37

Predator*Tadpoles 3,32 3.6 0.02

60

Figure 1. Mean (±SE) of tadpole size (A), growth rates (B), and total tank biomass (C) in which we manipulated the abundances, high (50) and low (25), of Ac (A. callidryas) and De (D. ebraccatus) tadpoles. Results of two-way ANOVA looking at effects of tadpole species identity and density. * = p <0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001. Letters indicate statistically different treatmenrs (Tukey HSD, p < 0.05)

61

Figure 2. Mean (±SE) of water column chlorophyll a (A) and dried periphyton biomass (B) in experimental treatments. Results of two-way ANOVA looking at effects of tadpole species identity and density. * = p <0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001. Letters indicate statistically different treatments (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.05)

62

Figure 3. Mean (±SE) of zooplankton morphospecies abundances in experimental treatments. Results of two-way ANOVA looking at effects of tadpole species identity and density. * = p <0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p < 0.001. Letters indicate statistically different treatments (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.05)

63

Figure 4. (A) Mean (±SE) of water column chlorophyll a after being filtered through different sized Nitex, (B) periphyton chlorophyll a abundances inside and outside of tadpole exclosures, and (C) zooplankton abundances in which we manipulated the presence and absence of 50 A. callidryas tadpoles. Results of ANOVA looking at effects of treatment, and in the case of periphyton, results of a two-way ANOVA looking at treatment and exclosure side effects. * = p <0.05, ** = p < 0.01. Letters indicate statistically different treatments (Tukey’s HSD, p < 0.05)

64

Figure 5: Mean (±SE) of phytoplankton chlorophyll a for A. Predator presence and B. Tadpole composition over time. Main effects and interactions of time, predator presence and tadpole composition are shown * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001

65

Figure 6: Mean (±SE) of periphyton chlorophyll a for A. Predator presence and B. Tadpole composition over time. Main effects and interactions of time, predator presence and tadpole composition are shown * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001

66

Figure 7: Mean (±SE) of tadpole length for A. (Agalychnis callidryas) and B. (Dendropsophus ebraccatus) by treatment over time. Main effects and interactions of time, predator presence and competitors are shown * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001.

67

Figure 8: Mean (±SE) of tadpole growth rates for A. (Agalychnis callidryas) and B. (Dendropsophus ebraccatus) by treatment over time. Growth rates are calculated as Final: log(Final TL/Mid TL) and Mid: (Mid TL/Initial TL). Main effects and interactions of time, predator presence and competitors are shown * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001.

68

Figure 9: Mean (±SE) of zooplankton numbers per liter for A. Total zooplankton B. Copepods and C. Rotifers. Main effects and interactions of time, predator presence and tadpole composition are shown * = p < 0.05, ** = p < 0.01, *** = p <0.001

69

Vita

Zacharia J. Costa was born on 8 February 1985 in Santa Rosa, California. He graduated

from the University of California, Davis in 2007 with a Bachelor of Science in

Evolution and Ecology. After working in a plant ecology lab and then living in San

Francisco, CA, he realized only riding bikes and getting hyph was not the life he

envisioned it to be. Going to graduate school was a great opportunity to explore the

world and to keep himself mentally stimulated, so he joined Dr. James Vonesh’ lab

at Virginia Commonwealth University in 2009. Now enamored with research, he

hopes to return to Latin America to do research while pursuing a Ph.D. in aquatic

ecology.

70