Estimating Grammeme Redundancy by Measuring Their Importance for Syntactic Parser Performance

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Estimating Grammeme Redundancy by Measuring Their Importance for Syntactic Parser Performance Estimating Grammeme Redundancy by Measuring Their Importance for Syntactic Parser Performance Aleksandrs Berdicevskis UiT The Arctic University of Norway Department of Language and Linguistics [email protected] which are often described as “complex” and “re- Abstract dundant”. It is, however, often difficult to determine (and Redundancy is an important psycholinguistic provide empirical evidence in favour of the cho- concept which is often used for explanations sen decision) whether a certain feature is indeed of language change, but is notoriously diffi- redundant, or to what extent it is redundant and cult to operationalize and measure. Assuming to what extent it is functional. Some conclusions that the reconstruction of a syntactic structure can be drawn from indirect evidence, e.g. typo- by a parser can be used as a rough model of the understanding of a sentence by a human logical (cf. Dahl’s (2004) notion of cross- hearer, I propose a method for estimating re- linguistically dispensable phenomena). For mod- dundancy. The key idea is to compare per- ern languages, redundancy can be studied and formances of a parser on a given treebank be- measured by means of psycholinguistic experi- fore and after artificially removing all infor- ments (e.g. Caballero and Kapatsinski, 2014), but mation about a certain grammeme from the this approach is not applicable to older language morphological annotation. The change in per- stages and extinct languages. formance can be used as an estimate for the I propose a computational method to estimate redundancy of the grammeme. I perform an the functionality (and, conversely, redundancy) experiment, applying MaltParser to an Old of a grammeme (that is, a value of a grammati- Church Slavonic treebank to estimate gram- meme redundancy in Proto-Slavic. The re- cal/morphological category) that can potentially sults show that those Old Church Slavonic work for any language for which written sources grammemes within the case, number and are available or can be collected. tense categories that were estimated as most I describe the philosophy behind the proposed redundant are those that disappeared in mod- method and its relevance to cognitive aspects of ern Russian. Moreover, redundancy estimates language evolution in section 2. Section 3 pro- serve as a good predictor of case grammeme vides the necessary background for a particular frequencies in modern Russian. The small instance of language change that will be used as sizes of the samples do not allow to make de- a case study. Section 4 describes how the exper- finitive conclusions for number and tense. iment was performed, section 5 provides the re- sults. Section 6 discusses possible interpretations 1 Introduction of the results, and section 7 concludes. Explanations of historical language change often involve the concept of redundancy, especially 2 Using parsers to measure morpholog- grammatical (morphological) redundancy. ical redundancy One important example is a family of recent In the most general terms, morphological redun- theories about linguistic complexity (Sampson et dancy can be described as follows: if a message al., 2009), including those known under the la- contains certain morphological markers that are bels “sociolinguistic typology” (Trudgill, 2011) not necessary to understand the message fully and “Linguistic Niche Hypothesis” (Lupyan and and correctly, then these markers can be consid- Dale, 2010). The key idea behind these theories ered (at least to some extent) redundant. is that certain sociocultural factors, such as large The problem with operationalizing this intui- population size or a large share of adult learners tion is that it is unclear how to model under- in the population can facilitate morphological standing (that is, the reconstruction of the seman- simplification, i.e. increase the likelihood that the tic structure) of a message by human beings. language will lose some morphological features, 65 Proceedings of the Sixth Workshop on Cognitive Aspects of Computational Language Learning, pages 65–73, Lisbon, Portugal, 18 September 2015. c 2015 Association for Computational Linguistics. In the method I propose, syntactic structure is 2013). Thus, if it is shown that there is a dia- taken as a proxy for semantic structure, and a chronic bias towards eliminating redundant reconstruction of syntactic structure by an auto- grammemes, it will be possible to hypothesize matic parser is taken as a model of how a human that this bias stems from individual speakers' hearer understands the meaning. preference to avoid overloading their speech with The assumption that these processes have excessive complexity. enough in common to make the model adequate Importantly for diachronic studies, the method is bold, but not unwarranted. It is generally can be applied to extinct languages, provided that agreed that a correct interpretation of syntactic large enough treebanks exist. structure is necessary to understand the meaning In the following sections, I will exemplify the of a message, and that humans use morphologi- method by applying it to a particular case of lan- cal cues to reconstruct syntactic structure. guage change (Proto-Slavic —> Contemporary Parsers, obviously, do the latter, too. Crucially, Standard Russian). I also use the case study to the model does not require the assumption that test whether the resulting redundancy estimates parsers necessarily process the information in are plausible. Following a common assumption exactly the same way as humans. It is enough that more redundant grammemes are in general that they, using the same input, can approximate more likely to be lost (Kiparsky 1982: 88–99, see the output (i.e. syntactic structures) well enough, also references above), and that Russian has been and modern parsers usually can. Furthermore, under considerable pressure to shed excessive parsers also rely heavily on the morphological complexity (see section 3), I make the prediction information, not unlike humans. that the grammemes that did disappear were on The key idea is then to take a morphologically average more redundant than those that were tagged treebank of a language in question and kept, and that the “remove-and-reparse” method parse it with an efficient parser, artificially re- should be able to capture the difference. moving morphological features (either gram- In order to be explicit about the assumptions memes or categories) one by one. Changes in the behind the current study and its limitations I parser’s performance caused by the removal of a want to highlight that the study attempts to test feature can serve as a measure of its redundancy. two independent hypotheses at once: first, that In other words, if the removal of a feature causes redundant grammemes are more likely to disap- a significant decrease in parsing accuracy, the pear or become less frequent; second, that pars- feature can be considered important for extract- ing is an adequate model of human language per- ing syntactic information and thus functional. If, ception, since what is redundant for a parser is however, the decrease is small (or absent), the redundant for a human as well. This can be prob- feature can be considered redundant. lematic, since we do not really know whether Obviously, it is not necessary that this ap- either of these hypotheses is true. proach will provide an exact and comprehensive Let us look at the experiment from the follow- measure of morphological redundancy; there are ing perspective: if it turns out that there is a numerous potential sources of noise and errors. strong correlation between importance of the We can, however, expect that at least some real grammeme for parser performance and gram- redundancy will be captured. The method can meme survivability, then this fact has to be ex- then be applied to make rough estimates and thus plained. A plausible explanation which fits well be useful, for instance, in large-scale typological with the existing linguistic theories would be the studies, or in language change studies, or any one outlined above in the form of the two hy- studies aiming at understanding why languages potheses: under certain sociocultural conditions need (or do not need) redundancy. Understand- speakers tend to abandon redundant grammemes; ing that, in turn, will help to reveal the cognitive grammemes that are not important for the parser biases that influence language learning. are redundant. If there is no correlation, however, It has been shown by means of computational this absence would not tell us whether both hy- modelling and laboratory experiments that strong potheses are false or only one of them (and biases which affect the course of language which one) is. change can stem from weak individual cognitive In addition to the main prediction, I make a biases, amplified by iterated learning over gener- secondary one: assuming that more redundant ations (Kirby et al., 2007; Reali and Griffiths, grammemes will tend to become less frequent, 2009; Smith and Wonnacott, 2010) and commu- and more functional grammemes will tend to nication within populations (Fay and Ellison, become more frequent, we can expect that the 66 functionality of grammemes in Proto-Slavic CSR: the plural is used instead (the dual, howev- should serve as a good predictor of their frequen- er, left visible traces in the morphosyntax of the cy in modern Russian. I will test this prediction numerals and the formation of plural forms). as well, though the possibilities for this test of- Proto-Slavic had five basic verbal tenses: pre- fered by the current study are limited. In addi- sent (also called non-past), aorist, imperfect, per- tion, the prediction itself relies on stronger as- fect and pluperfect.1 The perfect and pluperfect sumptions (redundancy is not necessarily the were analytical forms, consisting of resp. present only, nor even the most important predictor of and imperfect2 forms of an auxiliary (‘be’) and a frequency). so-called resultative participle. Later, the aorist, imperfect and pluperfect went out of use, while 3 From Proto-Slavic to Russian the former perfect gradually lost the auxiliary verb.
Recommended publications
  • The Strategy of Case-Marking
    Case marking strategies Helen de Hoop & Andrej Malchukov1 Radboud University Nijmegen DRAFT January 2006 Abstract Two strategies of case marking in natural languages are discussed. These are defined as two violable constraints whose effects are shown to converge in the case of differential object marking but diverge in the case of differential subject marking. The strength of the case bearing arguments will be shown to be of utmost importance for case marking as well as voice alternations. The strength of arguments can be viewed as a function of their discourse prominence. The analysis of the case marking patterns we find cross-linguistically is couched in a bidirectional OT analysis. 1. Assumptions In this section we wish to put forward our three basic assumptions: (1) In ergative-absolutive systems ergative case is assigned to the first argument x of a two-place relation R(x,y). (2) In nominative-accusative systems accusative case is assigned to the second argument y of a two-place relation R(x,y). (3) Morphologically unmarked case can be the absence of case. The first two assumptions deal with the linking between the first (highest) and second (lowest) argument in a transitive sentence and the type of case marking. For reasons of convenience, we will refer to these arguments quite sloppily as the subject and the object respectively, although we are aware of the fact that the labels subject and object may not be appropriate in all contexts, dependent on how they are actually defined. In many languages, ergative and accusative case are assigned only or mainly in transitive sentences, while in intransitive sentences ergative and accusative case are usually not assigned.
    [Show full text]
  • Definiteness and Determinacy
    Linguistics and Philosophy manuscript No. (will be inserted by the editor) Definiteness and Determinacy Elizabeth Coppock · David Beaver the date of receipt and acceptance should be inserted later Abstract This paper distinguishes between definiteness and determinacy. Defi- niteness is seen as a morphological category which, in English, marks a (weak) uniqueness presupposition, while determinacy consists in denoting an individual. Definite descriptions are argued to be fundamentally predicative, presupposing uniqueness but not existence, and to acquire existential import through general type-shifting operations that apply not only to definites, but also indefinites and possessives. Through these shifts, argumental definite descriptions may become either determinate (and thus denote an individual) or indeterminate (functioning as an existential quantifier). The latter option is observed in examples like `Anna didn't give the only invited talk at the conference', which, on its indeterminate reading, implies that there is nothing in the extension of `only invited talk at the conference'. The paper also offers a resolution of the issue of whether posses- sives are inherently indefinite or definite, suggesting that, like indefinites, they do not mark definiteness lexically, but like definites, they typically yield determinate readings due to a general preference for the shifting operation that produces them. Keywords definiteness · descriptions · possessives · predicates · type-shifting We thank Dag Haug, Reinhard Muskens, Luca Crniˇc,Cleo Condoravdi, Lucas
    [Show full text]
  • PARASITIC MIRATIVITY of ENGLISH USE in COLIN TREVORROWS MOVIE “JURASSIC WORLD” Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of Th
    PARASITIC MIRATIVITY OF ENGLISH USE IN COLIN TREVORROWS MOVIE “JURASSIC WORLD” Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Sarjana Humaniora in English and Literature Department of Faculty of Adab and Humanities of UIN Alauddin Makassar By MUJI RETNO Reg. No. 40300111080 ENGLISH AND LITERATURE DEPARTMENT ADAB AND HUMANITIES FACULTY ALAUDDIN STATE ISLAMIC UNIVERSITY MAKASSAR 2016 PARASITIC MIRATIVITY OF ENGLISH USE IN COLIN TREVORROW’S MOVIE “JURASSIC WORLD” Thesis Submitted in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Sarjana Humaniora in English and Literature Department of Faculty of Adab and Humanities of UIN Alauddin Makassar By MUJI RETNO Reg. No. 40300111080 ENGLISH AND LITERATURE DEPARTMENT ADAB AND HUMANITIES FACULTY ALAUDDIN STATE ISLAMIC UNIVERSITY MAKASSAR 2016 i MOTTO “EDUCATION IS WHAT REMAINS AFTER ONE HAS FORGOTTEN WHAT ONE HAS LEARNED IN SCHOOL.” (Albert Eistein) “EDUCATION IS A PROGRESSIVE DISCOVERY OF OUR OWN IGNORENCE.” (Charlie Chaplin) “EVERY THE LAST STEP INEVITABLY HAS THE FIRST STEP” (Muji Retno) ii ACKNOWLEDGE All praises to Allah who has blessed, guided and given the health to the researcherduring writing this thesis. Then, the researcherr would like to send invocation and peace to Prophet Muhammad SAW peace be upon him, who has guided the people from the bad condition to the better life. The researcher realizes that in writing and finishing this thesis, there are many people that have provided their suggestion, advice, help and motivation. Therefore, the researcher would like to express thanks and highest appreciation to all of them. For the first, the researcher gives special gratitude to her parents, Masir Hadis and Jumariah Yaha who have given their loves, cares, supports and prayers in every single time.
    [Show full text]
  • Calculus of Possibilities As a Technique in Linguistic Typology
    Calculus of possibilities as a technique in linguistic typology Igor Mel’uk 1. The problem stated: A unified conceptual system in linguistics A central problem in the relationship between typology and the writing of individual grammars is that of developing a cross-linguistically viable con- ceptual system and a corresponding terminological framework. I will deal with this problem in three consecutive steps: First, I state the problem and sketch a conceptual system that I have put forward for typological explora- tions in morphology (Sections 1 and 2). Second, I propose a detailed illus- tration of this system: a calculus of grammatical voices in natural languages (Section 3). And third, I apply this calculus (that is, the corresponding con- cepts) in two particular case studies: an inflectional category known as an- tipassive and the grammatical voice in French (Sections 4 and 5). In the latter case, the investigation shows that even for a language as well de- scribed as French a rigorously standardized typological framework can force us to answer questions that previous descriptions have failed to re- solve. I start with the following three assumptions: 1) One of the most pressing tasks of today’s linguistics is description of particular languages, the essential core of this work being the writing of grammars and lexicons. A linguist sets out to describe a language as pre- cisely and exhaustively as possible; this includes its semantics, syntax, morphology and phonology plus (within the limits of time and funds avail- able) its lexicon. 2) Such a description is necessarily carried out in terms of some prede- fined concepts – such as lexical unit, semantic actant, syntactic role, voice, case, phoneme, etc.
    [Show full text]
  • Context and Negation 1 the Role of Context in Young Children's Comprehension of Negation Ann E. Nordmeyer and Michael C. Frank
    Context and negation 1 The role of context in young children’s comprehension of negation Ann E. Nordmeyer and Michael C. Frank Department of Psychology, Stanford University Thanks especially to the staff and families at the San Jose Children’s Discovery Museum. This work was supported by an NSF Graduate Research Fellowship to AEN and a John Merck Scholars Fellowship to MCF. An earlier version of this work was presented to the Cognitive Science Society in Nordmeyer and Frank (2013). Address all correspondence to Ann E. Nordmeyer, Stanford University, Department of Psychology, Jordan Hall, 450 Serra Mall (Bldg. 420), Stanford, CA, 94305. Phone: 650-721-9270. E-mail: [email protected] Context and negation 2 Abstract Negation is an important concept in human language, yet little is known about children’s ability to comprehend negative sentences. In this paper, we explore how 2–5-year-old children’s comprehension of negation changes depending on the context in which a negative sentence occurs. We collected eye-tracking data while children watched a video in which they heard positive and negative sentences. Negative sentences, such as “look at the boy with no apples,” referred to a boy with nothing (Experiment 1) or a boy with an alternative object (Experiment 2). All children showed greater difficulty in resolving the referent when negative sentences referred to the boy with nothing, despite suggestions that nonexistence negations of this type are produced early. In addition, 3- and 4-year-old children showed an initial tendency to look away from the target and towards the named noun when the referent of the negative utterance was an alternative object.
    [Show full text]
  • Public Policy for Thee, but Not for Me: Varying the Grammatical Person of Public Policy Justifications Influences Their Support
    Judgment and Decision Making, Vol. 9, No. 5, September 2014, pp. 433–444 Public policy for thee, but not for me: Varying the grammatical person of public policy justifications influences their support James F. M. Cornwell∗ David H. Krantz† Abstract Past research has shown that people consistently believe that others are more easily manipulated by external influences than they themselves are—a phenomenon called the “third-person effect” (Davison, 1983). The present research inves- tigates whether support for public policies aimed at changing behavior using incentives and other decision “nudges” is affected by this bias. Across two studies, we phrased justification for public policy initiatives using either the second- or third-person plural. In Study 1, we found that support for policies is higher when their justification points to people in general rather than the general “you”, and in Study 2 we found that this former phrasing also improves support compared to a no-justification control condition. Policy support is mediated by beliefs about the likelihood of success of the policies (as opposed to beliefs about the policies’ unintended consequences), and, in the second-person condition, is inversely related to a sense of personal agency. These effects suggest that the third-person effect holds true for nudge-type and incentive-based public policies, with implications for their popular support. Keywords: third-person effect, public policy, decision architecture, incentives, attitudes. 1 Introduction whether through the application of disincentive or incen- tive structures, have for some time now attempted to make In recent years, interest has grown in “nudge” economics use of self-interest in order to shift choices toward those (Thaler & Sunstein, 2009) that make use of research on that are considered more pro-social or desirable over the decision making to structure choice sets so that the most long term (Oliver, 1980).
    [Show full text]
  • (In)Definiteness, Polarity, and the Role of Wh-Morphology in Free Choice
    Journal of Semantics 23: 135–183 doi:10.1093/jos/ffl001 Advance Access publication April 4, 2006 (In)Definiteness, Polarity, and the Role of wh-morphology in Free Choice ANASTASIA GIANNAKIDOU University of Chicago LISA LAI-SHEN CHENG Leiden University Abstract In this paper we reconsider the issue of free choice and the role of the wh- morphology employed in it. We show that the property of being an interrogative wh- word alone is not sufficient for free choice, and that semantic and sometimes even morphological definiteness is a pre-requisite for some free choice items (FCIs) in certain languages, e.g. in Greek and Mandarin Chinese. We propose a theory that explains the polarity behaviour of FCIs cross-linguistically, and allows indefinite (Giannakidou 2001) as well as definite-like FCIs. The difference is manifested as a lexical distinction in English between any (indefinite) and wh-ever (definite); in Greek it appears as a choice between a FCI nominal modifier (taking an NP argument), which illustrates the indefinite option, and a FC free relative illustrating the definite one. We provide a compositional analysis of Greek FCIs in both incarnations, and derive in a parallel manner the Chinese FCIs. Here the definite versus indefinite alternation is manifested in the presence or absence of dou , which we take to express the maximality operator. It is thus shown that what we see in the morphology of FCIs in Greek is reflected in syntax in Chinese. Our analysis has important consequences for the class of so-called wh- indeterminates. In the context of current proposals, free choiceness is taken to come routinely from interrogative semantics, and wh-indeterminates are treated as question words which can freely become FCIs (Kratzer and Shimoyama 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • Evidentiality and Mood: Grammatical Expressions of Epistemic Modality in Bulgarian
    Evidentiality and mood: Grammatical expressions of epistemic modality in Bulgarian DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements o the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Anastasia Smirnova, M.A. Graduate Program in Linguistics The Ohio State University 2011 Dissertation Committee: Brian Joseph, co-advisor Judith Tonhauser, co-advisor Craige Roberts Copyright by Anastasia Smirnova 2011 ABSTRACT This dissertation is a case study of two grammatical categories, evidentiality and mood. I argue that evidentiality and mood are grammatical expressions of epistemic modality and have an epistemic modal component as part of their meanings. While the empirical foundation for this work is data from Bulgarian, my analysis has a number of empirical and theoretical consequences for the previous work on evidentiality and mood in the formal semantics literature. Evidentiality is traditionally analyzed as a grammatical category that encodes information sources (Aikhenvald 2004). I show that the Bulgarian evidential has richer meaning: not only does it express information source, but also it has a temporal and a modal component. With respect to the information source, the Bulgarian evidential is compatible with a variety of evidential meanings, i.e. direct, inferential, and reportative, as long as the speaker has concrete perceivable evidence (as opposed to evidence based on a mental activity). With respect to epistemic commitment, the construction has different felicity conditions depending on the context: the speaker must be committed to the truth of the proposition in the scope of the evidential in a direct/inferential evidential context, but not in a reportative context.
    [Show full text]
  • The Influence of Animacy and Context on Word Order Processing: Neurophysiological Evidence from Mandarin Chinese
    Impressum Max Planck Institute for Human Cognitive and Brain Sciences, 2011 Diese Arbeit ist unter folgender Creative Commons-Lizenz lizenziert: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by-nc/3.0 Druck: Sächsisches Druck- und Verlagshaus Direct World, Dresden ISBN 978-3-941504-13-4 The Influence of Animacy and Context on Word Order Processing: Neurophysiological Evidence from Mandarin Chinese Von der Philologischen Fakultät der Universität Leipzig genehmigte DISSERTATION Zur Erlangung des akademischen Grades doctor philosophiae Dr. Phil. vorgelegt von Luming Wang geboren am 16. Februar 1981 in Zhoushan, China Dekan: Prof. Dr. Wolfgang Lörscher Gutachter: Prof. Dr. Balthasar Bickel Prof. Dr. Ina Bornkessel-Schlesewsky Prof. Dr. Kaoru Horie For my mother tongue, one of the many languages in this world. Acknowledgements This thesis could not have been written without the support and friendship of many people. If life is like online sentence processing in the sense that one must make a decision even without being sure of where it will lead, those people are definitely “prominent” characters that have greatly influenced my decisions, especially when I was experiencing “ambiguities” during different periods. I would like to thank them in chronological order. I thank my parents for giving me the initial processing preference, which has driven me to become closer to those things and people that I like, but unexpectedly lead me farther from them. I thank them from the bottom of my heart for their continuing selfless love. I am lucky for having been a student of Prof. Kaoru Horie and Prashant Pardesh during my MA studies at Tohoku University in Japan.
    [Show full text]
  • EVIDENTIALITY and FLEXIBILITY of SOURCE REPORTING 1 Evidentiality and Flexibility of Source Reporting Castelain, Thomas Floyd, S
    EVIDENTIALITY AND FLEXIBILITY OF SOURCE REPORTING Evidentiality and flexibility of source reporting Castelain, Thomas Instituto de Investigaciones Psicológicas, Instituto de Investigaciones Lingüísticas, Universidad de Costa Rica, San José Costa Rica [email protected] https://orcid.org/0000-0001-9415-1771 Floyd, Simeon Universidad San Francisco de Quito, Quito Ecuador [email protected] https://orcid.org/0000-0002-9739-2914 Mercier, Hugo* Institut Jean Nicod, Département d’études cognitives, ENS, EHESS, PSL University, CNRS, Paris France [email protected] https://orcid.org/0000-0002-0575-7913 *Corresponding author: Hugo Mercier, [email protected] 1 EVIDENTIALITY AND FLEXIBILITY OF SOURCE REPORTING Abstract The reporting of the sources of our statements—whether we have, e.g., seen something or been told something—plays an important role in communication. The inaccurate reporting of sources is often associated with the spread of inaccurate beliefs, as when rumors are attributed to a non-existent credible source. Experiments have revealed that participants display a substantial amount of flexibility in the reporting of sources, in particular by attributing to themselves ideas they have acquired from others. However, these participants belonged to large-scale societies, and they spoke languages without grammatical evidential marking. Both factors might increase flexibility in source reporting. To test whether members of small-scale societies speaking languages with evidential marking also display flexibility in source reporting, we conducted two experiments with Quichua and Cha’palaa speakers in Ecuador. In both experiments, some participants acquired a belief through social transmission, and then had to communicate this belief to another participant. In doing so, very few participants mentioned the social source of their belief, whether explicitly, or by using the relevant evidential markers.
    [Show full text]
  • How Does the Mother Tongue Affect Second Language Acquisition?
    SOCIAL SCIENCES How does the mother tongue affect second language acquisition? Learning a new language can be a great learners develop LW & HG: When a learner acquires a new (known as the inflectional morpheme such as the English past challenge; here, Professor Lydia White and interlanguage tense –ed, which occurs on regular verbs like Dr Heather Goad discuss their innovative work grammar); my ‘dance’, ‘jump’, ‘laugh’, its phonological shape, main focus has its morphological status, the role it plays in towards understanding the obstacles second been on syntax and syntax and its meaning must all be acquired. language learners often face morphology and the The focus of our explanation of learners’ interplay of linguistic difficulties relates to how several of these universals with components fit together. We argue that, rather Can you discuss how you became interested mother tongue influence (L1 transfer) on the than having problems with the morphology in linguistics? development of the interlanguage. as such, learners may have difficulties determining how to combine the morphological LW & HG: Linguistics involves the HG: My research has mostly focused form of the inflection with the verb to which investigation of universal and language- on phonological theory with the goal of it attaches. If the L1 prosodic representation specific patterns in language, in an attempt trying to determine the role that linguistic for inflection is inappropriate for the L2, there to reach an understanding of the abstract representations play in explaining the will be no suitable position for the past tense grammatical system that underlies the similarities and differences between morpheme in the interlanguage grammar.
    [Show full text]
  • A Grammar of Kunbarlang
    A Grammar of Kunbarlang Ivan Kapitonov ORCID: 0000-0002-1603-6265 Submitted in total fulfilment of the requirements ofthe degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Languages and Linguistics The University of Melbourne July 2019 Copyright 2019 Ivan Kapitonov This work is licensed under the Creative Commons Attribution-NonCommercial-NoDerivs 3.0 Unported License. To view a copy of this license, visit http://creativecommons. org/licenses/by-nc-nd/3.0/ or send a letter to Creative Commons, PO Box 1866, Mountain View, CA 94042, USA. Abstract This thesis is a comprehensive description of Kunbarlang, an Aboriginal language from northern Australia. The description and analysis are based on my original field work, as well as build on the preceding body of work by other scholars. Between 2015 and 2018 I have done field work in Warruwi (South Goulburn Island), Maningrida, and Darwin. The data elicited in those trips and the recordings of narratives andsemi- spontaneous conversation constitute the foundation of the present grammar. However, I was fortunate in that I was not working from scratch. Carolin Coleman did foundational work on Kunbarlang in central-western Arnhem Land from 1981, which resulted in the first grammar of the language (Coleman 1982). In her subsequent work in the area in the 1990’s, she carried on with lexicographic research in Kunbarlang, Mawng and Maningrida languages. More recently, Dr. Aung Si (Universität zu Köln), Dr. Isabel O’Keeffe (University of Sydney), and Dr. Ruth Singer (University of Melbourne / Australian National University) made a number of recordings of Kunbarlang speakers at Maningrida, Warruwi, Minjilang and Darwin.
    [Show full text]