A Cognitive Model of Economics

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Cognitive Model of Economics A cognitive model of economics Lorin Friesen, mentalsymmetry.com, Abbotsford, BC, Canada doi: 10.13140/RG.2.2.17044.83847 Abstract Economics is typically regarded as a collection of partially related theories. However, if economics is analyzed from a cognitive perspective, then it becomes evident that these theories form a cognitively natural, coherent package. The theory of mental symmetry is a cognitive model based in seven interacting cognitive modules. This theory is used to analyze most of the fundamental concepts of micro- and macroeconomics, as well as behavioral economics and neuroeconomics. Economics can be explained primarily as a focus upon the interaction between the cognitive module that provides motivation and the cognitive module that emphasizes choice. Economics recognizes the existence of other cognitive modules but regards them as outside of the realm of economics. A fuller cognitive picture can be gained by including the activity of these other cognitive modules. Going further, a cognitive basis for the institutions of macroeconomics can be found by viewing these institutions from the perspective of abstract technical thought. And this cognitive basis provides a framework for applying economics to the modern world of information and technology. Introduction The theory of mental symmetry began as a system of cognitive styles (Friesen, 1986). Mental symmetry maps in detail onto current neurology (Friesen, 2019). It has been used to explain a number of aspects of language and culture (Friesen, 2013). It has been used to analyze theology from a cognitive perspective (Friesen, 2015) and to develop an integrated philosophy of science and theology (Friesen, 2020). And it has been applied to a number of additional topics at a less rigorous level, which can be accessed on the mentalsymmetry website. This paper will use the theory of mental symmetry to analyze most of the basic principles of economics. Mental symmetry is a cognitive meta-theory that interprets human thought as seven interacting high-level cognitive modules. Basic economics will be defined as the content that is taught in the typical first-year university microeconomics and macroeconomics courses. This paper shows that basic economics can be interpreted primarily as one of the seven cognitive modules (Exhorter thought) interacting with one other module (Contributor thought). Mental symmetry is a meta-theory and not a theory. A new theory typically replaces an old theory, rendering the old theory obsolete. A meta-theory acts as a general framework within which other theories can be placed. Theories that are placed within a meta-theory continue to be relevant as specialized viewpoints that provide a more detailed view of some aspect of the meta-theory. The goal of this paper is not to introduce new economic concepts, but rather to take the existing concepts of economics, explain them from a cognitive perspective, and then discuss the larger cognitive picture that emerges. Taking an entire field and placing it within a new theory is highly unusual. 2 Obviously, it is not possible to discuss an entire field within a single academic paper. But it is possible to examine the basic principles of some field within a long academic paper. The discussion of economics in this paper will be largely limited to principles that are taught in the two introductory courses of microeconomics and macroeconomics, ensuring that the economic concepts that are being analyzed are well-known and widely accepted. Two primary references were used when researching this paper: the Khan Academy courses on AP microeconomics and AP macroeconomics, and the openstax second edition textbooks on microeconomics and macroeconomics. These textbooks were chosen because they are representative and freely available, and not because they contain any unusual information. This paper will be quoting extensively from the two openstax textbooks. When facts from some specialization are placed within a new paradigm, then it is easy to twist these facts so that they fit better within the new paradigm. When facts have to be modified to fit a new paradigm, then this is a sign of an inadequate paradigm. Quoting extensively from an economics textbook makes it clear that existing economic facts are being used without being twisted or modified. These economic quotes also make it easier to distinguish between the facts of economics and the theory of mental symmetry, as well as assist in translating concepts between these two fields. Whenever a standard term of economics is mentioned, these this term will be put in bold. In most cases, these terms are also placed in bold in the original textbooks. That leads to the more technical matter of referencing. Normal practice when quoting directly from some author is to mention the first author, the publishing year, and the page number from which the quote is taken. In this case, that would be either (Greenlaw, 2018, p.xx) or (Greenlaw, 2017, p.xx). But repeating this throughout the paper might give the mistaken impression that the author is the discoverer of a vast amount of original material, which is not the case. It might also give the mistaken impression that I am focusing inappropriately upon this author, or even possibly plagiarizing from this author. But that is also not the case. Instead, I am quoting from a textbook, and the only way to make it clear that one is quoting from an entire textbook is to quote many passages from many sections of the textbook. Therefore, instead of using (Greenlaw, 2018, p.xx) or (Greenlaw, 2017, p.xx), I will be using (micro, p.xx) or (macro, p.xx). This flows better as well as making it clear which concepts are being taken from a micro-economics textbook or a macro-economics textbook. Of course, authors of a textbook still need to be acknowledged as academic experts who are presenting the established concepts of a field in a coherent and comprehensible manner. Therefore, conventional references have also been included in the reference section. Looking at this more generally, I have found over the years that the approach taken by mental symmetry does not fit into the normal methodology of academia. I have also found that attempting to understand this mismatch has proved useful for deciphering the nature of academic thought. The book that best describes my experiences is Thomas Kuhn’s well-known volume on paradigms and paradigm shifts (Kuhn, 1970), and this paper will be referring several times to that book. This paper will discuss an aspect of mental symmetry which has been largely ignored in other papers about mental symmetry. Mental symmetry began with a detailed look at personality traits. Abstract 3 technical thought and its relationship to paradigms have been discussed in other papers and books. This paper will emphasize concrete technical thought as well as the interaction between Exhorter thought and Contributor thought. Placing the limited perspective of economics within a general theory of cognition will clarify how the field of economics interacts with other fields of thought as well as point out some inherent strengths and weaknesses of economic theory. The thesis of this paper is that economics can be explained primarily as the interaction between Exhorter and Contributor thought as viewed from Contributor thought. Economics does recognize other modes of thought, but it regards them as outside of the realm of economics, and it attempts to interpret these outside effects in terms of the interaction between Exhorter and Contributor thought. The next section introduces the theory of mental symmetry and has been copied from the paper on science and theology (Friesen, 2020). It explains terms such as ‘Exhorter’, ‘Contributor’, and ‘concrete technical thought’. Mental Symmetry The theory of mental symmetry originated as a system of cognitive styles that divides people into seven different categories. This system of cognitive styles was first developed in the 1970s and is still being taught, primarily in religious circles. Lane Friesen (brother of Lorin Friesen) initially expanded this system of cognitive styles by analyzing approximately 200 biographies in the 1980s (Friesen, 1986). This system of cognitive styles turned into a cognitive model when it was realized that the fundamental traits of each cognitive style correspond to the general functioning of a specific brain region. Since everyone has a complete brain, this led to the conclusion that cognitive style relates in some way to consciousness: Everyone has all seven cognitive modules; cognitive style is determined by the cognitive module in which a person is conscious, consciousness being defined as awareness and control. Thus, one can speak either of cognitive styles or of cognitive modules. The explaining power of mental symmetry emerges when one thinks in terms of cognitive modules. The theory of mental symmetry is summarized by a simple diagram known as the diagram of mental symmetry (Figure 1). The seven cognitive modules are each given a label that describes a major cognitive trait: Teacher, Exhorter, Mercy, Server, Contributor, Perceiver, and Facilitator. Studying personality has revealed details that are not explicitly shown by the diagram of mental symmetry, but this diagram still remains the best way to summarize the traits in a simple manner, and the diagram has been arranged in a manner that maps in a general way onto neurology. Thus, the seven labels (Teacher, Exhorter, Mercy, etc.) in the diagram of mental symmetry should be viewed as large-scale cognitive modules each composed of several interacting parts. The term ‘Mercy thought’ will be used to refer to a cognitive module, while the term ‘Mercy person’ refers to a cognitive style. The diagram uses two primary axes: analytical versus associative, and emotion versus confidence. Analytical versus associative corresponds to left hemisphere versus right hemisphere. This is a crude distinction, but it remains a useful one. Many cognitive tasks involve extensive interaction between the corresponding regions in the left and right hemispheres, but the underlying principle remains that the two hemispheres are using different strategies to perform similar tasks.
Recommended publications
  • Modularity As a Concept Modern Ideas About Mental Modularity Typically Use Fodor (1983) As a Key Touchstone
    COGNITIVE PROCESSING International Quarterly of Cognitive Science Mental modularity, metaphors, and the marriage of evolutionary and cognitive sciences GARY L. BRASE University of Missouri – Columbia Abstract - As evolutionary approaches in the behavioral sciences become increasingly prominent, issues arising from the proposition that the mind is a collection of modular adaptations (the multi-modular mind thesis) become even more pressing. One purpose of this paper is to help clarify some valid issues raised by this thesis and clarify why other issues are not as critical. An aspect of the cognitive sciences that appears to both promote and impair progress on this issue (in different ways) is the use of metaphors for understand- ing the mind. Utilizing different metaphors can yield different perspectives and advancement in our understanding of the nature of the human mind. A second purpose of this paper is to outline the kindred natures of cognitive science and evolutionary psychology, both of which cut across traditional academic divisions and engage in functional analyses of problems. Key words: Evolutionary Theory, Cognitive Science, Modularity, Metaphors Evolutionary approaches in the behavioral sciences have begun to influence a wide range of fields, from cognitive neuroscience (e.g., Gazzaniga, 1998), to clini- cal psychology (e.g., Baron-Cohen, 1997; McGuire and Troisi, 1998), to literary theory (e.g., Carroll, 1999). At the same time, however, there are ongoing debates about the details of what exactly an evolutionary approach – often called evolu- tionary psychology— entails (Holcomb, 2001). Some of these debates are based on confusions of terminology, implicit arguments, or misunderstandings – things that can in principle be resolved by clarifying current ideas.
    [Show full text]
  • Emerald Jrit Jrit628638 164..182
    The current issue and full text archive of this journal is available on Emerald Insight at: www.emeraldinsight.com/2397-7604.htm JRIT&L 12,2 Facilitating success for people with mental health issues in a college through cognitive 164 remediation therapy and social Received 19 January 2019 Revised 22 April 2019 and emotional learning Accepted 6 May 2019 Jaswant Kaur Bajwa CPLS, George Brown College – Saint James Campus, Toronto, Canada Bobby Bajwa Department of Medical Sciences, University of Western Ontario, London, Canada, and Taras Gula CPLS, George Brown College – Saint James Campus, Toronto, Canada Abstract Purpose – The purpose of this paper is to describe the components, structure and theoretical underpinnings of a cognitive remediation intervention that was delivered within a supported education program for mental health survivors. Design/methodology/approach – In total, 21 participants enrolled in the course Strengthening Memory, Concentration and Learning (PREP 1033 at George Brown College (GBC)) with the diagnosis of depression, anxiety, PTSD, ED and substance use disorder were included in the research. After a baseline assessment, participants completed 14 week cognitive remediation training (CRT) protocol that included six essential components that were integrated and implemented within the course structure of the supported education program at GBC. This was followed by a post-training assessment. Findings – Analysis of the participants’ performance on CRT protocol using computerized games showed little significant progress. However, the research found a positive change in the self-esteem of the participants that was statistically significant and the findings also aligned with the social and emotional learning framework. Research limitations/implications – One of the limitations in the research was the use of computer- assisted cognitive remediation in the form of the HappyNeuron software.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Theory of Mind As a Mediator in The
    THE ROLE OF THEORY OF MIND AS A MEDIATOR IN THE RELATIONSHIP BETWEEN SOCIAL FUNCTIONING AND SCHIZOTYPY A thesis submitted to Kent State University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts by Marielle Divilbiss December 2009 Thesis written by Marielle Divilbiss B.A., DePaul University, 2007 M.A., Kent State University, 2009 Approved by Nancy Docherty, Ph.D.,_______________________ Advisor Douglas Delahanty, Ph.D.,_____________________ Chair, Department of Psychology Timothy Moerland, Ph.D.,_____________________ Dean, College of Arts and Sciences ii Table of Contents List of figures…………………………………………………………………………… v List of tables……………………………………………………………………………. vi Acknowledgments……………………………………………………………………… vii Introduction…………………………………………………………………………….. 1 Hypotheses……………………………………………………………………….......... 11 Method…………………………………………………………………………………. 13 Results………………………………………………………………………………….. 21 Table 1………………………………………………………………………………….. 22 Table 2………………………………………………………………………………….. 23 Table 3………………………………………………………………………………….. 24 Table 4………………………………………………………………………………….. 25 Table 6…………………………………………………………………………………. 26 Table 7…………………………………………………………………………………. 27 Table 8…………………………………………………………………………………. 28 Table 9…………………………………………………………………………………. 32 Table 10……………………………………………………………………………….. 33 Table 11……………………………………………………………………………….. 37 Figure 1………………………………………………………………………………... 38 Table 12………………………………………………………………………………. 40 iii Figure 2………………………………………………………………………………… 41 Discussion…………………………………………………………………………….. 42 Conclusions……………………………………………………………………………
    [Show full text]
  • Emergence Discussion
    23/10/2011 IAJS Discussion Seminar with George Hogenson – Introduction by Warren Colman NB: To access and download two papers by George Hogenson which will form the basis for the discussion, please go to the following links: ‘What are symbols symbols of’: http://bit.ly/gxKn6T ‘The self, the symbolic and synchronicity’ http://bit.ly/gj13OQ It’s a great pleasure for me to introduce George Hogenson who will be leading a seminar for the IAJS list on the theme of ‘emergence’. He has been well known in the Jungian world long before he became an analyst as the author of Jung’s Struggle with Freud (1983), an impressive and scholarly work that analysed Jung’s break from Freud in terms of the creation of a different mythological understanding of time, death and authority. At that time, George was a philosophy PhD and a teacher of political philosophy, specialising in the field of international peace and security. From this strong academic background, George became interested in pursuing the practice of psychotherapy as well as its theory and qualified as a Jungian analyst in Chicago in 1998. In 2001, George gave a plenary presentation at the IAAP Congress in Cambridge, England, debating with Anthony Stevens on the nature and origins of archetypes. This was my first introduction both to George and to the dynamic systems theory he proposed as a way of reconceptualising archetypal theory and challenging Stevens’ use of evolutionary psychology as a way of bolstering the classical ‘blueprint’ model of archetypes as a priori structures. George’s presentation of a short video from the field of robotics, illustrating the principles of self-organisation was a revelation to me: I well remember the feeling that I was seeing a vision of the future, an entirely new way of thinking that had the potential to revision and revitalise analytical psychology.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Evolutionary Psychology Without Representational Nativism
    J.Expt.Theor.Artif.Intell.15(2003)143–159 Cognitive evolutionary psychology without representational nativism DENISE CUMMINS1, ROBERT CUMMINS1 and PIERRE POIRIER2 1 Department of Philosophy, University of California-Davis, Davis, CA 95616, USA 2 University of Quebec, Montreal, Canada e-mail: [email protected];[email protected] Abstract. A viable evolutionary cognitive psychology requires that specific cognitive capacities be (a) heritable and (b) ‘quasi-independent’ from other heritable traits. They must be heritable because there can be no selection for traits that are not. They must be quasi-independent from other heritable traits, since adaptive variations in a specific cognitive capacity could have no distinctive consequences for fitness if effecting those variations required widespread changes in other unrelated traits and capacities as well. These requirements would be satisfied by innate cognitive modules, as the dominant paradigm in evolutionary cognitive psychology assumes. However, those requirements would also be satisfied by heritable learning biases, perhaps in the form of architec- tural or chronotopic constraints, that operated to increase the canalization of specific cognitive capacities in the ancestral environment (Cummins and Cummins 1999). As an organism develops, cognitive capacities that are highly canalized as the result of heritable learning biases might result in an organism that is behaviourally quite similar to an organism whose innate modules come on line as the result of various environ- mental triggers. Taking this possibility seriously is increasingly important as the case against innate cognitive modules becomes increasingly strong. Keywords: cognitive psychology, evolutionary psychology, canalization, learning biases, innate modules, modularity Received July 2002; accepted December 2002 1.
    [Show full text]
  • Mapping the Mind: Domain Specificity in Cognition and Culture
    The modularity of thought and the epidemiology of representations Dan Sperber Ten years ago, Jerry Fodor published The Modularity of Mind, a book that received much well-deserved attention. His target was the then-dominant view according to which there are no important discontinuities between per- ceptual processes and conceptual processes. Information flows freely, "up" and "down," between these two kinds of processes, and beliefs inform per- ception as much as they are informed by it. Against this view, Fodor argued that perceptual processes (and also linguistic decoding) are carried out by specialized, rather rigid mechanisms. These "modules" each have their own proprietary data base, and do not draw on information produced by concep- tual processes. Although this was probably not intended and has not been much noticed, "modularity of mind" was a paradoxical title, for, according to Fodor, modu- larity is to be found only at the periphery of the mind, in its input systems.1 In its center and bulk, Fodor's mind is decidedly nonmodular. Conceptual processes - that is, thought proper - are presented as a big holistic lump lacking joints at which to carve. Controversies have focused on the thesis that perceptual and linguistic decoding processes are modular, much more than on the alleged nonmodularity of thought.2 In this chapter, I have two aims. The first is to defend the view that thought processes might be modular too (what Fodor [1987: 27] calls "modularity theory gone mad" - oh well!). Let me however echo Fodor and say that, "when I speak of a cognitive system as modular, I shall..
    [Show full text]
  • The Case for Massively Modular Models of Mind
    CDIC01 11/25/05 2:37 PM Page 1 JUST HOW MODULAR IS THE MIND? CDIC01 11/25/05 2:37 PM Page 2 CDIC01 11/25/05 2:37 PM Page 3 CHAPTER ONE The Case for Massively Modular Models of Mind Peter Carruthers My charge in this chapter is to set out the positive case supporting massively modular models of the human mind.1 Unfortunately, there is no generally accepted under- standing of what a massively modular model of the mind is. So at least some of our discussion will have to be terminological. I shall begin by laying out the range of things that can be meant by “modularity.” I shall then adopt a pair of strategies. One will be to distinguish some things that “modularity” definitely can’t mean, if the thesis of massive modularity is to be even remotely plausible. The other will be to look at some of the arguments that have been offered in support of massive modularity, discussing what notion of “module” they might warrant. It will turn out that there is, indeed, a strong case in support of massively modular models of the mind on one reasonably natural understanding of “module.” But what really matters in the end, of course, is the substantive question of what sorts of structure are adequate to account for the organization and operations of the human mind, not whether or not the com- ponents appealed to in that account get described as “modules.” So the more inter- esting question before us is what the arguments that have been offered in support of massive modularity can succeed in showing us about those structures, whatever they get called.
    [Show full text]
  • Is the Acquisition of New Behavior Through Conditioning
    1 www.onlineeducation.bharatsevaksamaj.net www.bssskillmission.in SOCIAL PHYCHOLOGY OF ADULT LEARNING Topic Objective: At the end of this topic student would be able to: Introduce learning theory. Give the importance of operant conditioning. Discuss the terminology related to psychology. Definition/Overview: Learning Theory: In psychology and education, a learning theory is an attempt to describe how people and animals learn, thereby helping us understand the inherently complex process of learning. There are three main categories or philosophical frameworks under which learning theories fall: behaviorism, cognitivism, and constructivism. Behaviorism focuses only on the objectively observable aspects of learning. Cognitive theories look beyond behavior to explain brain-based learning. And constructivism views learning as a process in which the learner actively constructs or builds new ideas or concepts. It is also important to take account of informal learning theories, and to consider the philosophical anthropology implied by any theory. Behaviorism focuses only on the objectively observable aspects of learning, and discounts the internal processing that might be associated with the activity. For behaviorism, learningWWW.BSSVE.IN is the acquisition of new behavior through conditioning. Key Points: 1. Problem Conditioning There are two types of possible conditioning: www.bsscommunitycollege.in www.bssnewgeneration.in www.bsslifeskillscollege.in 2 www.onlineeducation.bharatsevaksamaj.net www.bssskillmission.in SOCIAL PHYCHOLOGY OF ADULT LEARNING Classical conditioning, where the behavior becomes a reflex response to stimulus as in the case of Pavlov's Dogs Operant conditioning where there is reinforcement of the behavior by a reward or a punishment. The theory of operant conditioning was developed by B.F.
    [Show full text]
  • Modularity, Development and 'Theory of Mind'
    Blackwell Publishers Ltd. 1999, 108 Cowley Road, Oxford, OX4 1JF, UK and 350 Main Street, Malden, MA 02148, USA. Mind & Language, ISSN: 0268-1064 Vol. 14. No. 1 March 1999, pp 131–153. Modularity, Development and ‘Theory of Mind’ BRIAN J. SCHOLL AND ALAN M. LESLIE Abstract: Psychologists and philosophers have recently been exploring whether the mechanisms which underlie the acquisition of ‘theory of mind’ (ToM) are best charac- terized as cognitive modules or as developing theories. In this paper, we attempt to clarify what a modular account of ToM entails, and why it is an attractive type of explanation. Intuitions and arguments in this debate often turn on the role of develop- ment: traditional research on ToM focuses on various developmental sequences, whereas cognitive modules are thought to be static and ‘anti-developmental’. We suggest that this mistaken view relies on an overly limited notion of modularity, and we explore how ToM might be grounded in a cognitive module and yet still afford development. Modules must ‘come on-line’, and even fully developed modules may still develop internally, based on their constrained input. We make these points con- crete by focusing on a recent proposal to capture the development of ToM in a module via parameterization. 1. Introduction The currency of our mental lives consists largely of propositional attitudes, even when we are interpreting the behaviours of others. If you see a person running to catch up with a just-departing train, for example, you interpret the person as an intentional agent, who believes that there is a just-departing train, and who wants to get on it.
    [Show full text]
  • Developmental Psychology
    Developmental psychology Psychology History · Subfields Basic science Abnormal · Biological Cognitive · Comparative Cultural · Developmental Evolutionary · Experimental Mathematical · Personality Positive · Social Applied science Clinical · Consumer Educational · Health Industrial and organizational Law · Military Occupational health · Political Religion · School · Sport Lists Disciplines · Organizations Outline · Psychologists Psychotherapies · Publications Research methods · Theories Timeline · Topics Portal v · d · e Developmental psychology, also known as human development, is the scientific study of systematic psychological changes that occur in human beings over the course of their life span. Originally concerned with infants and children, the field has expanded to include adolescence, adult development, aging, and the entire life span. This field examines change across a broad range of topics including motor skills and other psycho-physiological processes; cognitive development involving areas such as problem solving, moral understanding, and conceptual understanding; language acquisition; social, personality, and emotional development; and self-concept and identity formation. Developmental psychology includes issues such as the extent to which development occurs through the gradual accumulation of knowledge versus stage-like development, or the extent to which children are born with innate mental structures versus learning through experience. Many researchers are interested in the interaction between personal characteristics, the
    [Show full text]
  • Darwinian Domain-Generality: the Role of Evolutionary Psychology in the Modularity Debate
    Georgia State University ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University Philosophy Theses Department of Philosophy 5-3-2017 Darwinian Domain-Generality: The Role of Evolutionary Psychology in the Modularity Debate Michael Lundie Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/philosophy_theses Recommended Citation Lundie, Michael, "Darwinian Domain-Generality: The Role of Evolutionary Psychology in the Modularity Debate." Thesis, Georgia State University, 2017. https://scholarworks.gsu.edu/philosophy_theses/205 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Department of Philosophy at ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. It has been accepted for inclusion in Philosophy Theses by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks @ Georgia State University. For more information, please contact [email protected]. DARWINIAN DOMAIN-GENERALITY: THE ROLE OF EVOLUTIONARY PSYCHOLOGY IN THE MODULARITY DEBATE by MICHAEL LUNDIE Under the Direction of Daniel A. Weiskopf, Ph.D. ABSTRACT Evolutionary Psychology (EP) tends to be associated with a Massively Modular (MM) cognitive architecture. I argue that EP favors a non-MM cognitive architecture. The main point of dispute is whether central cognition, such as abstract reasoning, exhibits domain-general properties. Partisans of EP argue that domain-specific modules govern central cognition, for it is unclear how the cognitive mind could have evolved domain-generality. In response, I defend a distinction between exogenous and endogenous selection pressures, according to which exogenous pressures tend to select for domain-specificity, whereas the latter, endogenous pressures, select in favor of domain-generality. I draw on models from brain network theory to motivate this distinction, and also to establish that a domain-general, non-MM cognitive architecture is the more parsimonious adaptive solution to endogenous pressures.
    [Show full text]
  • Media Naturalness and the Ability to Predict Generosity in a Give-Some – Get-Some Interaction1
    Proceedings of the 50th Hawaii International Conference on System Sciences | 2017 Media Naturalness and the Ability to Predict Generosity in a Give-Some – Get-Some Interaction1 Roderick E. White Derrick J. Neufeld Mahdi Roghanizad Western University Western University University of Waterloo [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] Abstract to greatly facilitate exchange by reducing transaction costs, thus enhancing the performance of both organizations and markets. Although the origin and role Evolutionary psychologists believe the human mind of generosity, cooperation, sharing and trust is a widely evolved to solve adaptive problems present in our studied and much debated topic [8,25,42,45], ancestral environment. Our hominid ancestors survived researchers generally agree that the ability to reliably in face-to-face groups by assessing the cooperative detect, ex post, how partners have behaved is essential intentions of other group members. Media naturalness for cooperation to have evolved [13,39,40,31]. theory postulates face-to-face is the most ‘natural’ Establishing exchange relationships with novel partners communication medium. This paper reports results from can be highly advantageous, but the absence of prior a laboratory experiment examining the ability of student experience with, or reputational information about these subjects to predict the generosity of a counter-party partners also makes these relationships especially risky. under two media conditions: Face-to-Face (FtF), the The risk is that the prospective partner will not be more natural condition; and Video-to-Video (VtV), the cooperative, and will act opportunistically to their own less natural, technology-mediated condition. After a advantage.
    [Show full text]