Human

Human Ethology

IRENAUS EIBL-EIBESFELDT

Routledge Taylor & Francis Group

LONDON AND NEW YORK First published 1989 by Transaction Publishers

Published 2017 by Routledge 2 Park Square, Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4RN 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017, USA

Routledge is an imprint of the Taylor & Francis Group, an informa business

Copyright © 1989 by Irenaus Eibl-Eibesfeldt.

All rights reserved. No part of this book may be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any form or by any electronic, mechanical, or other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publishers.

Notice: Product or corporate names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used only for identification and explanation without intent to infringe.

Library of Congress Catalog Number: 2006048043

Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data

Eibl-Eibesfeldt, Irenaus. [Biologie des menschlichen Verhaltens. English] Human ethology / Irenaus Eible-Ebesfeldt. p. cm. Originally published: New York: Aldine De Gruyter, c1989. (Foundations of ) Includes bibliographical references (p. ) and indexes. ISBN 978-0-202-30970-5 (alk. paper) 1. Genetic . 2. Human behavior. 3. Psychobiology. 4. . I. Title.

BF70 l.E4313 2007 304.5—dc22 006048043 2006048043

ISBN 13: 978-0-202-30970-5 (pbk) Contents

Preface ix Acknowledgments xiii Mirror-Image Illustrations xv

1 OBJECTIVES AND THEORETICAL BASES OF HUMAN ETHOLOGY 2

1.1. Inquiry Formulation and Definition 3 Summary 1.1 6 1.2. Phylogenetic and Cultural Adaptation 7 Summary 1.2 16

BASIC CONCEPTS OF ETHOLOGY 18

2.1. The Concept of the "Innate" 19 Summary 2.1 23 2.2. Phylogenetic Adaptations in Behavior 23 Summary 2.2 84 2.3. Decoupling of Actions from Drives and Conscious Self-Control: The of Human Freedom 86 Summary 2.3 90 2.4. The Units of Selection—A Critical Evaluation of Sociobiology 90 Summary 2.4 102

METHODOLOGY 104

3.1. Gestalt Perception and Cognition 105 Summary 3.1 107 3.2. Methods of Data Gathering, Observation, and Description 107 Summary 3.2 112 3.3. Film and Sound Documentation 112 Summary 3.3 132 3.4. The Comparative Approach 132 Summary 3.4 147 3.5. Quantifying Ethology 148 Summary 3.5 161

3.6. Models 161 Summary 3.6 165

4 SOCIAL BEHAVIOR 166

4.1. Origins of Sociability 167 Summary 4.1 169 4.2. The Ambivalence of Approach and Withdrawal in Human Interactions 170 Summary 4.2 184 4.3. The Human Family as the Nucleus of Society 184 Summary 4.3 234 4.4. Family and Marriage 235 Summary 4.4 238 4.5. Pair Formation, Courtship, Sexual Love 238 Summary 4.5 260 4.6. Incest Taboos and Incest Avoidance 261 Summary 4.6 264 4.7. Sex Roles and Their Differentiation 265 Summary 4.7 288 4.8. The Individualized Group: Family, Kin, and Alliances 289 Summary 4.8 296 4.9. Rank Order, Dominance 297 Summary 4.9 314 4.10. Group Identity Maintenance 314 Summary 4.10 320 4.11. Territoriality 321 Summary 4.11 339 4.12. Origin and Social Function of Possession 339 Summary 4.12 358

5 INTRASPECIFIC AGGRESSION: CONFLICT AND WAR 360

5.1. Concepts 361 Summary 5.1 364 5.2. Aggression Theories 365 vi Summary 5.2 386 5.3. Functional Aspects of Aggressive Behavior 391 Summary 5.3 396 5.4. Socialization of Aggressive Behavior 396 Summary 5.4 399 5.5. Duels 399 Summary 5.5 402 5.6. Intergroup Aggression—War 402 Summary 5.6 421

6 COMMUNICATION 424

6.1. Olfactory Communication 426 Summary 6.1 429 6.2. Tactile Communication 430

6.3. Visual Communication 437 Summary 6.3 492 6.4. Interaction Strategies—The Universal Grammar of Human Social Behavior 493 Summary 6.4 522 6.5. On the Ethology of Verbal Communication 523 Summary 6.5 546

7 BEHAVIOR DEVELOPMENT () 548

7.1. Developmental Theories 549 Summary 7.1 580 7.2. Curiosity and Play 580 Summary 7.2 593 7.3. The Development of Interpersonal Relationships 594

7.4. Child Groups—Child Culture 600 Summary 7.4 605

8 MAN AND HIS HABITAT: ECOLOGICAL CONSIDERATIONS 606

8.1. Ecotype Homo sapiens: Hominization and Behavior 607 Summary 8.1 616 8.2. From the Individualized Society to Industrial Society 616 Summary 8.2 628 8.3. On the Ethology of Settlement and Residence 629 Summary 8.3 648 vii 8.4. Social Order and Human Behavior 649 Summary 8.4 662 8.5. Concluding Thoughts 663

9 THE BEAUTIFUL AND TRUE: THE ETHOLOGICAL CONTRIBUTION TO AESTHETICS 664

9.1. Aesthetics and Fine Arts 665 Summary 9.1 673 9.2. Species-Specific Biases of Perception of Aesthetic Relevance 673 Summary 9.2 677 9.3. Art as Communication 677 Summary 9.3 685 9.4. Cultural Manifestations: An Observation on Style and Stylization 685 Summary 9.4 690 9.5. On the Ethology of Music, Dance, and Poetry 690 Summary 9.5 702

10 'S CONTRIBUTION TO ETHICS 704

Summary 10 717 Conclusion 718

Bibliography 720 Film Publications 789 Author Index 805 Subject Index 824

viii Preface

With the discovery of conditioned reflexes by I. P. Pavlov, the possibilities for experimenting, following the example set by the classical, exact sciences, were made available to the behavioral sciences. Many psychologists hoped that the component parts of behavior had also been found from which the entire, multi- faceted cosmos of behavior could then be constructed. An experimentally oriented psychology subsequently developed including the influential school of behaviorism. Motivated by pedagogical idealism, psychologists indulged in the hope that ed­ ucation could achieve every desired result. Among behaviorists, the Lockean conviction that humans come into the world with "clean slates" crystallized into a doctrine. It seems certain that this conceptual approach restricted research in­ terest. Although behaviorists carefully investigated the various aspects of behavior, and extracted some fundamental laws of learning, their theoretical fix­ ation prevented their perceiving other possible approaches. The possibility that some aspects of behavior might, through phylogenetic adaptation, be genetically programmed was not even considered. Anyone referring to a possible antecedent innate human nature was, until recently, dismissed as a biological determinist or reductionist. As late as the 1970s, the majority of behaviorists and sociologists viewed humans as primarily passive beings subject to the formative influences of their surroundings. It was believed that through rewards and punishments almost all human behavior could be "conditioned" (B. F. Skinner, 1938, 1971). Such an extreme environmentalist approach would consign all behavior to learning, relegate all power to the educators, and ultimately, legitimize them to establish behavior norms as well. Parallel to the development of behaviorism in psychology there developed in a pronounced cultural relativism (discussed critically by W. Rudolph, 1968) according to which culture is a construct on which the laws of biology have no influence. This school of thought is traceable to Franz Boas (1911, 1928, 1938) and its tenets must have reflected the spirit of the times since leading anthropologists acknowledged them (A. L. Kroeber, 1915; M. J. Her- skovits, 1950, among others). Margaret Mead can be regarded as one of the victims of this doctrinal teaching; she was so convinced of the relativism of cultures that she saw and recorded only that which fit those theories. Her reports on Samoa, so often cited, could not stand the test of critical review (D. Freeman, 1983). The decisive impetus for revising this extreme environmental approach came from behavioral biology (ethology). The work of (1935, 1937, 1961) and (1948, 1951) clarified the notion of , a concept which, until this time, was a philosophical speculation of the vitalists. There were, of course, exceptions, such as W. James (1890) who defined "instinct" in fairly modern terms, as capacities of organisms based on an innate neuronal organization. Lorenz and Tinbergen demonstrated that phylogenetic adaptations determine be­ havior in well-defined ways, and also voiced the opinion that comparable phy­ logenetic adaptations influence human behavior. By introducing the dimension of phylogeny into the science of behavior their work incorporated 's findings; behaviorists had apparently forgotten to consider the evolutionary aspects of behavior in their theories. About the same time that Lorenz formulated his theories, Erich von Hoist (1935, 1939) refuted the classical reflex theory, according to which all behavior is a re­ sponse to external stimuli. Ethologists can look back over the last decade with a certain sense of satis­ faction, for within a short span of 10 years, six biological researchers have received the Nobel Prize: Konrad Lorenz, Niko Tinbergen, and in 1973 (as founders of ethology) and Roger W. Sperry, David Hubel, and Torsten Wiesel in 1981 (as pioneers of neuroethology).1 The discovery that even the behavior of higher vertebrates is preprogrammed in well-definable ways through phylogenetic adaptation has initiated a process of reconsideration in the human sciences, to date so heavily influenced by behav­ iorism. In 1971, R. Sperry wrote that the new discoveries on the self-organization of the up to the point of functional maturity has hardly been noticed in disciplines beyond the fields of biology and ethology. Currently, however, the biological approach is well acknowledged by most fields of science dealing with humans. Konrad Lorenz indicated in his earlier works that the findings of animal behavior research could contribute to the understanding of human behavior. He devoted a number of important chapters to human behavior in Die angeborenen Formen moglicher Erfahrung (1943) and in 1950 he designated as one of the most important objectives of ethology the testing of the hypotheses derived from the study of animal behavior through the study of human behavior. Clearly, this objective consists of more than simply applying to humans the findings and models of animal behavior research. Ethological research on man was needed, and it truly began in the mid-1960s. I have been able to contribute to the development of this field, bringing to it a considerable background of prac­ tical field and laboratory experience in animal behavior on a broad comparative basis. As a student, beginning in 1946, I had the opportunity to study the behavior of amphibians and at the Wilhelminenberg Biological Station (Vienna). A badger that I hand-raised helped me learn a great deal about animal play behavior. It occurred to me that during the course of its play behavior the badger freely combined behavior patterns from a number of different functional systems. Indeed, in its play behavior we can observe the first manifestations of that freedom to perform derived from voluntary motor control and the ability to detach behavior patterns from emotions which also lies at the root of man's behavioral freedom (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1950). In 1949-1950, I joined Lorenz at the newly established Research Center for Behavioral Physiology of the Max Planck Society. In addition to my interest in behavioral development, questions regarding communication caught my attention. How do signals guide the course of social interactions? How do they originate, and how does the understanding of signals develop? In this area I was particularly concerned with the ritualization of aggressive behavior (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955a, 1959).

x 'Erich von Hoist is only absent from this list since his death took him from us too soon. On a 10-month expedition led by Hans Hass to the Caribbean Sea and the Galapagos Islands, I had the opportunity to study the of fishes and other interspecific relationships of coral fishes (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1955b). I also observed the ritualized fights of marine iguanas, and became acquainted with adaptive radiation from first-hand observation of Darwin's finches. On another diving expedition (1957-1958), again with Hans Hass, I spend 1 year in the Maldive and Nicobar Islands. My main interest was the behavior and ecology of coral fishes. I assembled a large collection of these fishes and learned much about sys- tematics and functional morphology. On these two research voyages21 sharpened my appreciation for interrelationships, especially those concerning ecological fac­ tors. After returning from this expedition I became increasingly involved in the nature- nurture debate. I experimentally refuted a number of assertions about the ontogeny of certain behavior patterns and showed how innate and acquired elements in mammalian behavior intercalate (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1963). In the early 1960s I began to include human behavior in my research. Through investigations with children deaf and blind from birth, I was able to demonstrate phylogenetic adaptations in human behavior (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, 1973). Between 1963 and 1965 I began with the documentation of unstaged ("natural") human social interactions. The impulse came from my friend Hans Hass, who provided the technical requisites through the development of a specialized reflex lens (p. 112). We tested this technique in 1964 in Africa and in 1965 on a world-wide expedition (I. Eibl-Eibesfeldt and H. Hass, 1966, 1967). From this experience, we developed a program of cross-cultural documentation of human social inter­ actions and rituals on film and tape. For the longitudinal studies, I selected a number of cultures with different sub­ sistence strategies and which, within certain limitations, could also serve as models for different stages of cultural -from Paleolithic hunters and gatherers to farming societies. In 1970, I was entrusted with an Institute for Human Ethology within the organization of the Max Planck Society. Cross-cultural documentation formed a principal part of our efforts.3 We in­ vestigated phylogenetic and cultural adaptations, the development of rituals and their function, and, in general, the behavior patterns of communication. In studying strategies of social interactions, I found that certain verbal and nonverbal inter­ actions are structured according to the same universal rules. The exploration of this universal system of rules that structure social behavior is at present one key aspect of our research efforts. To achieve this goal, we are working in conjunction with linguists. In 1967, I concluded my animal ethological studies with the pub­ lication of Ethology: The Biology of Behavior.4

2 A summary is found in the more recently published books Galapagos (7th ed., Piper, 1984) and Die Maldiven (Piper, 1982). 3I spent over 4 years in the field gathering comparative cultural documentation: 1568 days at the target sites (excluding travel time). During this time I produced approximately 200 km of 16 mm film. Many of my findings are based on this large body of material. My zoological field experience comprises 1488 days—in total over 8 years of expedition ex­ perience. 4Grundrip der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung, Piper, Munich. The 6th edition of this work appeared in 1980. xi As a supplement to my textbook on animal behavior, this new text on human ethology is based on the latter half of my life's work.5 The findings on animal ethology are only occasionally cited in this work and then only to the extent that they contribute to the understanding of ethological concepts. I hope that this first text on human ethology presents itself as a unified work, even though not every area could be treated with equal depth. For example, a branch of ethology has developed in the past decades which places particular emphasis on ecology and population genetics. This field, known as sociobiology, has enriched discussion beyond the boundaries of behavioral biology through its stimulating, and often provocative, theses. I will discuss some of its contributions when appropriate in this volume, but, of course, cannot cover this new field in depth. After vigorous debates between behaviorists, anthropologists, and sociologists, we have entered a period of exchange of thoughts and a mutual approach, which in many instances has led to cooperative projects of researchers from different disciplines. This work offers a biological point of view for discussion and includes data from my own cross-cultural work and research from the staff of our institute. It confirms, above all else, the astonishing unity of mankind and paints a basically positive picture of how we are moved by the same passions, jealousies, friendliness, and active curiosity. It is hoped that it encourages further discussion and thought across traditional academic boundaries. Before turning to the text, I would like to add that the need to understand our­ selves has never been so great as it is today. An ideologically torn humanity strug­ gles for its survival. Our species, who is in a position to send satellites to Mars and Venus and to broadcast photographs of Saturn and Jupiter from space, is helpless when confronted with its social problems. It does not know how it should compensate its workers, and it experiments with various economic systems, con­ stitutions, and forms of government. It struggles for freedom and stumbles into newer conflicts. Population growth is apparently completely out of hand, and at the same time many resources are being depleted. With the continuation of the present exponential growth of the world population looms the catastrophe of a global breakdown of populations. In his book Mutations of Mankind, Pierre Ber- taux (1963) writes: "Assessing contemporary human existence as a biological phenomenon is not only permissible but mandatory. Anyone rejecting this approach fails to recognize the significance of our existence and thus the extent of human responsibility. ..." Our hope is to develop an ethic for human survival through gaining insight into the biological processes governing our lives. We must consider our existence rationally in order to understand it, but certainly not with cold, calculating reason but with the warm feeling of a heart concerned for the welfare of later generations. May this book contribute to such an outlook.

Professor Dr. Irendus Eibl-Eibesfeldt

5My prior books on this subject include: Love and Hate. The Natural History of Ele­ mentary Behavioral Patterns (10th ed., 1982); The !Ko Bushman Society. Group Formation and Aggression Control in a Hunting and Gathering People (1973); Pre-programmed Man xii (4th ed., 1982); New Pathways of Human Research (2nd ed., 1984). Acknowledgments

In the past 20 years, I have spent nearly one-fourth of my time in societies whose members still follow traditional living patterns. Few of them will read the lines in this book, but perhaps some of their children will, which would make me happy indeed. It would be one of the ways in which my gratitude to them could be expressed—for their hospitality and for what they taught me. I am especially indebted to my wife Lorle and my children Bernolf and Roswitha for their courage and good spirits during the extended absences necessitated by my expeditions and field studies. Konrad Lorenz, whom I have regarded as my fatherly friend for the past 40 years, promoted my work in many ways. We engaged in endless conversations and he was open to my ideas. His openness always remained a model for me. I also express my special thanks to another close friend: Hans Hass, who, 35 years ago, led me through the reefs of the Caribbean Sea, and who developed in the early 1960s the reflex lens, the key to our cross-cultural documentary work. The unforgettable diving expeditions on the "Xarifa" were followed by no less fas­ cinating expeditions to human communities where we tested new methods of doc­ umentation. Some of the thoughts in this volume were developed on these shared journeys. We have also worked together as editors of the human ethology film archives of the Max Planck Society. I thank my expedition companions of many years. Quite aside from their as­ sistance, they have enhanced, with their human presence, the pleasure of con­ ducting field work. Thus, I thank from my heart: Kuno Budack, Volker Heeschen, Hans Joachim Heinz, Harald Herzog, Dieter Heunemann, Wulf Schiefenhovel, and Heide Sbreszny-Klein. I thank my co-workers at the Institute and my many friends in Europe and overseas, especially my colleagues Jiirgen Aschoff, Ingrid Bell-Krannhals, Norbert Bischof, William Charlesworth, Mario von Cranach, De­ rek Freeman, Elke and Karl-Friedrich Fuhrmeister, Inga Goetz, Karl Grammer, Anna Guggenberger, Bernhard Hassenstein, Klaus Helfrich, Eckhardt Hess, Les Hiatt, Barbara Hold, Franz Huber, Hermann Kacher, Erich Klinghammer, Gerd Koch, Otto Koenig, Renate Krell, Cornelia von Laue-Canady, Ernie Reese, Mar­ garet and Wolfgang Schleidt, Reinhard Schropp, Gunter Senft, Christa Siitterlin, Pauline Wiessner-Larsen and . Special thanks are due to the governments of Australia, Botswana, Indonesia, Namibia, Papua New Guinea, the Philippines, South Africa, and Venezuela, who fostered our work through granting our work permits and who also supported us to the best of their ability. In the field we had continuous help from the local missions. Individual examples include the help of the Salesian Mission in Vene­ zuela; in the early 1970s, the New Tribes Mission in Serra Parima was also a friendly host. Missions in Irian Jaya and Papua New Guinea also helped with their hospitality, by providing transportation assistance and a well-developed infra­ structure. I would like to mention especially the assistance of the German and Austrian embassies, without whom many plans would not have been realized. The scientific organizations of the Federal Republic of Germany have consis­ tently fostered our work most generously. The Max Planck Society established an independent research center for human ethology and also made possible the creation of a film documentation program. For this sustained support, I thank the Max Planck Society and its President. Without the financial assistance of the German Research Society (Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft) a number of important interdisciplinary projects would not have been carried out. Individual projects were also supported by the Fritz von Thyssen Fund, the Werner von Reimers Fund, and the Maison de l'Homme. The A. von Gwinner Fund provided me with the decisive initial assistance during the early years of my human ethology field research. I sincerely appreciate the support of Inter Nationes for subsidizing this trans­ lation. It is, furthermore, my great pleasure to thank the Institute for Scientific Film in Gottingen and its director, Hans Karl Galle, for many years of fruitful coop­ eration. My heartfelt thanks to Pauline Wiessner-Larsen and Anette Heunemann for their faithful translation of this book. In addition, I wish to express my deepest appreciation to William Charlesworth, Tom Pitcairn, Anne Rasa, and Wulf Schie- fenhovel, for consulting and assisting with specific sections of the text. Last, but not least, I want to thank Melvin Konner, and the Editors of the Foundation of Human Behavior series for their interest in my work. I also wish to thank the publisher, and, in particular, Ms. Sheila Johnston, who invested so much work in the final editing of the book.

xiv Mirror-1 mage Illustrations

The illustrations listed below appear as mirror images when printed since the film sequences from which they were copied were taken with the mirror lens.

2.10, 2.11, 2.15, 2.17, 3.18, 3.19, 3.20c, 3.21A, 3.21B, 3.21C, 3.22, 3.23, 4.1, 4.2, 4.3, 4.5, 4.24, 4.25, 4.26, 4.27, 4.28, 4.30, 4.31, 4.32, 4.33, 4.34, 4.36b,d,e, 4.37, 4.38, 4.40A, 4.40B, 4.41A, 4.41B, 4.46, 4.48, 4.49, 4.50B, 4.67b, 4.68, 4.69, 4.70, 4.77, 4.78, 4.80, 4.81, 4.82, 4.83, 5.1, 5.3, 5.4, 5.5, 5.6, 5.7, 5.8, 5.9, 5.12, 5.13, 5.15, 5.16, 5.18, 5.23, 5.24, 5.26, 5.29, 6.5, 6.13, 6.19, 6.22, 6.23, 6.24A, 6.33, 6.34, 6.35, 6.36, 6.37, 6.38, 6.39a-c, 6.40, 6.45A, 6.45B, 6.47, 6.48, 6.49, 6.54, 6.55b, 6.56, 6.62, 6.64, 6.65, 6.70, 6.71, 6.72, 6.73, 6.74, 6.76, 7.2, 7.3, 7.4, 7.5, 7.6, 7.8, 7.9, 7.10, 7.11, 7.12, 7.13, 7.14A, 7.14B, 7.15, 7.17b, 7.20, 7.21, 7.22, 7.24B, 7.25, 7.29, 7.30, 7.31, 7.32, 7.33, 7.34. FOUNDATIONS OF HUMAN BEHAVIOR An Aldine de Gruyter Series of Texts and Monographs

Series Editors Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, University of California, Davis Richard W. Wrangham, University of Michigan

Human Ethology. 1989 Irendus Eibl-Eibesfeldt Evolutionary History of the "Robust" Australopithecines. 1988 Frederick E. Grine, Editor Early Hominid Activities at Olduvai. 1988 Richard Potts Homicide. 1988 Martin Daly and Margo Wilson The Biology of Moral Systems. 1987 Richard D. Alexander Child Abuse and Neglect: Biosocial Dimensions. 1987 Richard J. Gelles and Jane B. Lancaster, Editors Parenting Across the Life Span: Biosocial Dimensions. 1987 Jane B. Lancaster, Jeanne Altmann, Alice S. Rossi, and Lonnie R. Sherrod, Eds. Evolution and Human Origins. 1987 Russell L. Ciochon and John G. Fleagle, Editors Human Birth: An Evolutionary Perspective. 1987 Wenda R. Trevathan School-Age Pregnancy and Parenthood: Biosocial Dimensions. 1986 Jane B. Lancaster and Beatrix A. Hamburg, Editors Despotism and Differential Reproduction: A Darwinian View of History. 1986 Laura L. Betzig Sex and Friendship in Baboons. 1985 Barbara Boardman Smuts : Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives. 1984 Glenn Hausfater and Sarah Blaffer Hrdy, Editors Navajo Infancy: An Ethological Study of Child Development. 1983 James S. C his holm

In Preparation Inujjuamiut Foraging Strategies Eric Alden Smith Human Reproductive Ecology James W. Wood Ecology, Evolution, and Human Behavior Eric A. Smith and Bruce Winterhalder, Editors To the Memory of

My Beloved Friend and Teacher

KONRAD LORENZ (1903 - 1989) 1

Objectives and Theoretical Bases of Human Ethology The animal is taught by its organs; man teaches his and controls them. Johann Wolfgang von Goethe

Whether mankind considers himself the son of God or a successful ape will make a clear difference in his behavior towards actual facts; in both cases he will also hear very different commands from within himself. Arnold Gehlen (1970, p. 1)

1.1. Inquiry Formulation and Definition

There has never been a shortage of attempts to define man and, by interpretation, attribute meaning to his life. For millennia, priests, artists, and philosophers have considered these questions. Religious revelation has often been contrasted with the efforts to gain insight into human nature through observation and introspection, that is, through experience and with the aid of reason. To this empirical-rational approach, biology gave new impetus with the theory of evolution, a theory that shook our anthropocentric perception of the world. Through evolutionary theory, man became aware not only of his animal heritage, but also of the reality of his incompleteness—man could no longer consider himself as the final "crowning achievement" of creation, but rather, at best, as an intermediate link on the path toward a higher level of humanity. Of course, this new insight led to a new perspective of man, one of a progres­ sively developing creature, setting ever higher goals and striving toward these objectives. But, at the same time, man became painfully aware that he travels on a narrow ridge, risks toppling off, and is burdened with responsibility, in as much as he accepts his further development as his mission. If, for some, man was simply one animal species among others, a "naked ape" as provocatively expressed it, others, in contrast, felt that man had progressed so far above animals in his evolution that he no longer had anything in common with them; that he had gone beyond the biological process of evolution by virtue of his culture. Man, therefore, was considered free to arrange his life in accordance with reason, without any limitations on constraints. These alternate viewpoints became unnecessarily polarized, for both approaches have validity. Biological inheritance determines human behavior, as we will show, in precisely definable parameters. But it is equally true that only man possesses a , with which he can creatively formulate new statements and thus pass on his cultural heritage by tradition. Only Man can be defined as a cultural being even though some show some modest cultural beginnings. Art, reason, and responsible morality, as well as open-mindedness and adaptability, are further distinctive hu­ man traits, and no reasonable biologist doubts their special role.1 It is important, however, that we are also aware of the more primitive action and reaction patterns that determine our behavior, and to not pretend as if they did not exist. It is especially in the area of social behavior that we are less free to act than we generally assume. This is poignantly demonstrated by the astonishing

'Konrad Lorenz wrote (1971, p. 509): "Far from underestimating the differences between the previously described behavioral patterns of higher animals and human achievements, molded by reason and morality, I maintain: no one is able to appreciate the uniqueness of these specific human attributes as clearly as those who can perceive them against the back­ ground of those much more primitive actions and reactions, which we also share with the higher animals." 3 discrepancy between our ability to control the external environment and our in­ ability to shape our social life satisfactorily. At the same time that we enthusi­ astically view the space photographs of Jupiter from the Voyager space probe, we read in our newspapers about executions in Iran, terrorist acts in Ireland, and mass murder in Kampuchea. Of course we should be hopeful for our future based on our potential for responsible morality, but this is possible only when we also recognize the inherited basis of our actions and take them into consideration. In this sense biology can contribute to our enlightenment and emancipation. Human ethology can be defined as the biology of human behavior. Like its parent discipline ethology, it too is divided into specialties, since the question as to why humans behave one way rather than another can be defined and answered in different ways. If one's interest lies in the function of the underlying physiological mechanisms, then human ethology becomes allied with traditional behavioral physiology. Here we are dealing with an explanation of the proximate cause of a behavior pattern. Inquiry is directed, for example, toward the stimuli that trigger specific responses, how the coordination for muscle action is achieved, what mo­ tivates and terminates behavior, as well as other questions. One can also ask how and why specific behavioral patterns evolved. In order to answer these questions, which aim at the ultimate causes, one must first understand in which way the behavior in question contributes to fitness, as measured by the number of surviving offspring. In short, one must establish its function, or in this sense its task. Ex­ perimental and ecological designs, including the study of the ontogeny, are used to answer this question. The observation of behavior in the natural context is an important starting point for such an investigation. Whenever we encounter a structure or a behavioral pattern on a regular basis it is common sense to ask what that pattern's function is, in other words to begin with the presumption that the behavior fulfills some task. It can, of course, be the case that the particular behavior under observation from a selectionistic point of view is either neutral or even a disadvantageous heritage, dragged along as a burden of history, or as a by-product of other ad­ aptations. This is, however, only rarely the case. The various cost-benefit cal­ culations used by sociobiologists constitute a promising new way to study adap- tiveness; we shall explore this topic in a special section later in this volume. Some functions are immediately obvious. If a paleontologist finds a petrified wing impression he needs no experimental procedure to state that this is a flight organ. Should one find the fossil imprint of a structure looking like a camera eye, i.e., with lens, glass body, accommodation structures, and a projection surface like the retina, the investigator can state that he found an eye, an organ used for visual perception. This statement can be made even if the organ was found in a meteorite and it was thus impossible to make any experimental verification of the structure's function. Questions about function and development can be asked for cultural behavior patterns just as they can for phylogenetically evolved characteristics. We em­ phasize this because one occasionally comes across the point of view that human ethologists deal only with the basic "animal" heritage in human behavior. This is false. We also investigate man's cultural behavior. Thus questions as to specific adaptiveness can be posed both for phylogenetically and culturally evolved patterns and achievements from the biological point of view mentioned above. O. Koenig (1970) even coined the term "cultural ethology." Excellent examples of the eth- 4 ological approach to the study of cultural behavior include Koenig's investigations on the biology of uniforms and the ethological analysis of soccer by D. Morris (1981). We must also correct the frequent equating of phylogenetic adaptation with animal heritage. This is unwarranted because there are many phylogenetic adaptations that are specific to Homo sapiens alone. Consider, for example, our innate propensity for speech or the expression of crying. Human ethologists investigate complex behavioral sequences of individuals and of interactions among people and groups of people. They thus work on higher integration levels than physiologists, who are concerned with the elemental life processes such as stimulus perception, muscle contraction, and the conduction of nerve impulses. Although these processes are an important prerequisite to the understanding of behavioral events, one cannot deduce all the laws underlying any given social interaction from these elemental processes. Each higher level of integration has its own laws that cannot be derived from those of the levels below. Special emphasis must therefore be placed on the necessity of formulating questions appropriate to each level of behavioral organization. Human ethology makes use of experimental and analytical methods of behav­ ioral research developed in related disciplines, including techniques of data sam­ pling developed in anthropology and psychology, such as, the interviewing of informants. From animal ethology we take the methods of nonparticipant obser­ vation, techniques of recording and documenting, and the comparative approach. Human ethology also places value in studying behavioral patterns in their natural context (Section 3.3) and thereafter proceeding to experimental analysis. The theoretical basis of human ethology is critical realism (K. R. Popper, 1973; K. Lorenz, 1973, 1983). Hence, the basic assumption is that every "adaptation" reflects features of a reality outside the subject (p. 8). In order to make objective statements about the real world, our perception must be capable of reconstructing this world from the sensory data perceived. In order to reconstruct the real world we must be able to recognize constancy, even under changing conditions. Special mechanisms of constancy perception as well as our ability to recognize configurations (Gestalt perception, Section 2.2.2) enable us to do this. Since these are phylogenetic adaptations, we can say that the con­ stancy hypothesis is innate. The relationships between reality, perception, and knowledge can be demon­ strated by using a graphic projection model (G. Vollmer, 1983). If a cube is optically projected onto a screen, we can reconstruct it if we know its structure, the type of the projection, and the characteristics of the screen. In this manner we can accurately reproduce three-dimensional objects from two-dimensional projections on our retina. Even when we look at pictures, we interpret the objects portrayed on them in three dimensions. In the reconstruction of three-dimensional objects, the information lost in the projection process must be recovered. This takes place relatively reliably during the course of the perception process, with the exception of special cases, such as optical illusions. Cognition can be seen as an effort to reconstruct the real, "true" structures of reality outside the subject from the sensory impressions perceived as projections of these structures. Thus we interpret the cosmic signals perceived by our sensory organs as projections of astronomical objects. That the interpretation of these projections fits the outer world is proved by the success of interplanetary space travel. The ability to reconstruct a real world from sensory data presupposes a knowl­ edge about this world. This knowledge is based in part on individual experience 5 and, in part, on the achievements of data processing mechanisms, which we in­ herited as phylogenetic adaptations. Knowledge about the world in the latter in­ stance was acquired during the course of evolution. It is, so to speak, a priori knowledge—prior to all individual experience—but certainly not prior to any ex­ perience (Section 2.1).

The process of cognition consists of a step-by-step reconstruction of an hypothetically postulated reality, a stepwise liberation from the limitations of our sensory organs (the "screen" . . . ). This reconstruction process works counter to the chain of projections. While each projection effectively reduces information, in the process of cognition we attempt a reconstruction. Naturally, such a reconstruction must remain partial and hypothetical" (G. Vollmer, 1983, p. 64, translation of the German original).

In recent years, promising relationships have developed with other disciplines of human behavior and human culture. In this book we will discuss the work of many researchers who would not regard themselves as ethologists, but whose findings are ethologically relevant. This holds true for psychologists as well as ethnologists, sociologists, political and legal scientists, and art historians. A particularly close relationship exists with ethnologists and social anthropol­ ogists (E. Goffman, 1963, 1967; K. Jettmar, 1973; M. Godelier, 1978; H. Schindler, 1980). Common interests arise from comparative cultural work and from a mutual interest in the general, universal laws of human behavior, as demonstrated, for example, by the structuralism of C. Levi-Strauss (p. 517). These relationships are so varied that they cannot all be cited here. Our common interests touch on ques­ tions of early childhood development and socialization, societal structure, hier­ archical organization, aggression, ethical norms, and many more. Ties also exist with psychology and sociology. We will cite specific examples of this in the dis­ cussion of these themes. There are further common interests with linguistics, both at the level of concept formation and that of speech itself (p. 523). There have long been mutual relationships with medicine, especially psychiatry and psycho­ analysis (D. Ploog, 1964, 1966, 1969; J. Bowlby, 1969, 1973). The growing interest in ethological findings by political scientists is noteworthy. In America, biopolitics has developed as a new discipline (R. D. Alexander, 1979; C. Barner-Barry, 1983; P. A. Corning, 1981, 1983b; H. Flohr and W. Tdnnesmann, 1983; R. D. Masters, 1976, 1981b, c; G. Schubert, 1973, 1975, 1983b; A. Somit, 1976; A. Somit and R. Slagter, 1983). Art history and archeology have also been inspired by findings from ethology (D. Fehling, 1974; G. C. Rump, 1978, 1980; M. Schuster and H. Beisl, 1978).

Summary 1.1

Human ethology can be defined as the biology of human behavior. The research objectives are the elucidation of the physiological mechanisms underlying behavior, the discovery of the functions fulfilled by behavioral patterns and thus the unveiling of the selective pressures to which the behavior in question owes its existence, and, finally, the investigation of behavioral development in ontogeny, phylogeny, and cultural history, with emphasis on the question as to how and to what extent Man became programmed to act through phylogenetically acquired adaptations or by learning during individual ontogeny. Human ethology makes use of the con­ cepts and methods developed in animal ethology, but adapts them to the special 6 requirements associated with the unique position of man within the Animal King- dom. Methodological techniques are also utilized from the related disciplines of psychology, anthropology, and sociology. Through points of mutual interest human ethology thus endeavors to bridge the gap between the different human sciences. Human ethologists study not only the phylogenetically evolved behavior of man but also its individual and cultural modifiability. Critical realism is the episte- mological basis of human ethology.

Oh, great star! What would you be, had you not those on which to shine? Friedrich Nietzsche (from Zarathustra's prologue)

1.2. Phylogenetic and Cultural Adaptation

Life is defined today as an energetic process during which organisms, bearers of this process, extract more potential energy from their environment than they must expend in the acquisition of this energy. In other words, organisms are energy- acquiring systems with a positive energy balance. H. Hass (1970), the first to clearly formulate this concept to my knowledge, originated the term "Energon" for such energy-acquiring systems. The life processes are maintained by the mul­ tiplicity of organisms and by those energy-acquiring systems developed by them. Although we can trace the development of the process throughout evolution, we have yet to understand its ultimate causes. The energetic process presupposes structures at its disposal that are "adapted" to the appropriate energy transformations. Each organism must possess structures with which it can extract energy from the environment. These structures are adapted to the appropriate energy sources, that is, they are so constructed that they can tap the specific energy sources and thus aid in the maintenance of the energy-acquiring system. The environment is not only the source of energy but also the source of a multitude of interfering and even harmful influences against which the organism must protect itself. Organisms must also repair damage and be able to transform their positive energy balance into the procreation of their species. In short, there are a multitude of adaptations that require energy investment, such as reproduction, growth, protection, and similar tasks, which enter the balance as costs (H. Hass, 1970). Since organisms live in an ever-changing environment, they must be able to modify their adaptations in response to these changes. This may even require changes in their basic construction. Furthermore, there are a variety of changes to which an organism must continually adapt during its lifetime. Thus it must be able to institute short-term, reversible changes in response to temporary envi­ ronmental changes. The musculature and circulatory systems must be able to adapt to new stresses, and calluses must be able to form on the skin in response to pressure. Finally, an animal must be able to learn from experience in such a way that it modifies its behavior adaptively. Adaptive here refers to fitness as measured by reproductive success or, ultimately, the propagation of the genes of the indi­ vidual in question (see inclusive fitness, p. 92). Adaptation is thus a central problem for all organisms. Since the entire nature- nurture controversy centers around the question of the source of adaptation, we will add a few basic remarks about this subject here. Adaptations depict features of the environment relevant to survival. They reflect facets of the external reality, such as, characteristics of the energy source they 7 tap or the environment in which they move. Thus, certain characteristics of water are reflected in the anatomy of fishes and dolphins, by adaptations that are func­ tionally related to the movement of these organisms in this medium. This is what is meant when we say that an adaptation reflects features of the environment. The degree of detail and exactness in which an adaptation depicts the outer world varies from case to case since it is determined by function. The organism only reflects characteristics of the environment relative to fitness and not every detail of the environment. However, the match can be amazingly complete, as for ex­ ample in the case of mimicry, in which a mimic imitates its model even down to the smallest details. The leaf insects, for example, mimic the leaves of the shrubs on which they live. Interesting examples of mimicry are reviewed in W. Wickler (1968). K. Lorenz (1941, 1973) has made a significant contribution to the question of what constitutes reality in his biological epistemology. In our adaptations we do not depict the external world—pictorially (iconically), but our thought processes and our perceptual capabilities fit as adaptations in the same way as do our physical characteristics. "Our cognitive and perceptual categories, given to us prior to individual experience, are adapted to the environment for the same reasons that the horse's hoof is suited for the plains before the horse is born, and the fin of a fish is adapted for water before the fish hatches from its egg" (K. Lorenz, 1941, p. 99, translation of the German original). The adaptations of an organisms, whether these are bodily structures or ad­ aptations in behavior, represent assumptions (hypothesis) about the world in which it is going to live—assumptions which have been tested by selection. Our perceptual organizations come into disarray when we move into other en­ vironments to which we are not phylogenetically adapted, as for example in the atomic or astronomical environments. Our causal thought processes are proof of this. We associate events by means of rigidly determined programs and extrapolate from the coincidence of events: When-then (coincidence) to causes (because— then). We can only think in causal sequences, and this becomes a problem when we use these intellectual tools to inquire into the origin of the universe. David Hume states that causality may not actually exist in nature, but that it is just a "necessity of the soul." The remarkable essay by R. Riedl (1981) contains a chapter on the consequences of causal thought: "The Superstition of Causes." This chapter title is misleading and more appropriately should have been "The Established Faith in Causes," since for the middle level—the "mesocosmos"—causal thinking is an appropriate adaptation. Accurate knowledge based on an iconic depiction of reality cannot be accomplished and is not really necessary for research. We perceive the world with preconceived perceptual and thought processes that strongly influence the postulation of our hypotheses (p. 105). Our perceptive and cognitive mechanisms, nonetheless, fit well enough to an outer world to enable us to send satellites into outer space, which will eventually arrive at their goal even after years of travel. That our cognition is capable of this is a matter of interest and it is more reasonable to assume that our cognition corresponds to external reality than to take the standpoint which disavows the existence of a world independent of a perceiving subject. In P. Watzlawick's (1981) Die erfundene Wirklichkeit ("Invented Reality"), these problems are precisely and intelligibly discussed by the various authors. Other notable contributions have been made by R. Riedl (1979), G. Vollmer (1975, 1983), D. Campbell (1974), and G. Radnitzky 8 and W. W. Bartley (1987). In their organization, organisms mirror facets of the extrasubjective environ­ ment. Even though the postulates of critical realism are neither demonstrable nor refutable, as Karl R. Popper states, we can "argue in their favor, and the arguments are convincing" (K. R. Popper, 1973, p. 50, translated from the original German text). This is the position any sensible scientist must take. Whether this is "critical realism" or "hypothetical realism" is, from my point of view, of little significance. G. Vollmer (1983) is of the opinion that one must differentiate between these, since critical realism at least considers the existence of the world as evident, ret­ rospectively unquestioned and intuitively guaranteed, while hypothetical realism differentiates between psychological certainty (hypothesis about the world) and epistemological uncertainty. But critical realists like Popper have a clear under­ standing of the epistemological uncertainty, and the hypothetical realists proceed from the postulation of an extrasubjective reality, which we reflect incompletely but nevertheless fittingly in our adaptations. This is not surprising, for, using an illustration from Ernst Mayr: the monkey without a realistic perception of the branch on which he wants to jump would soon be a dead monkey—and thus would not be one of our ancestors. From the above it is evident that every adaptive system must have acquired "information" from its environment at some time relevant to the organism's ad­ aptations, which reflect these environmental features. For this to occur, the or­ ganism has had to have interacted with the environment (K. Lorenz, 1961). Such an interaction can occur during phylogeny through the mechanisms of mutation, recombination, and selection. Information relative to survival has thus been col­ lected each generation and became codified genetically, providing the develop­ mental instructions that guide the process of self-differentiation by which the in­ dividual organisms or phenotypes develop. This developmental scheme is so strongly controlled by the genes that each individual develops the various species- specific structures during ontogeny. Special feedback mechanisms prevent ex­ cessive deviations from the blueprint. This is why sparrows hatch from sparrow eggs and chaffinches from chaffinch eggs and not, accidentally, a duck. Each characteristic develops within a specified range of variation. This range— the reaction norm—is genetically determined and thus the result of phylogenetical adaptation. The modifications of plants in different habitats illustrate this point. The mountain forms of many plants often have feltlike hairs and are generally shorter-stemmed and more compact than conspecifics growing in valleys. Such variability is clearly adaptive in most cases. However, it can also be an epiphe- nomenon. There are plants that develop red flowers in acid soil and blue in alkaline soil. The anthocyanins (flower pigments) turn red in acidic cell fluids and blue in weakly alkaline ones. We have not been able to deduce the selective advantage for certain plants to possess both color morphs depending on habitat. Perhaps this characteristic of the range of variation is neutral in terms of selection and may even be an epiphenomenon. The range of variation during development is limited and the environmental pressures cannot force modifications to occur in any arbitrary direction. However, the genetically determined prospective potential is often greater than what is normally observed. Gall wasps possess chemical means of stimulating plant tissue to grow galls, which they would not normally do. Generally the range of modifiability of genetically determined processes of differentiation is rather limited, and the prospective potential decreases with each further step in differentiation. Phylogenetic development is not directed toward specific goals. The direction 9 taken is dictated more by selection contingencies. In a process analogous to trial- and-error learning, an exploration of possibilities takes place at the level of the species. Many writers have been disturbed by the decisive role of "chance" in the development of the amazing diversity of organisms. Once, however, it is made clear that basically it is a question of species maintaining themselves in an en­ vironment of unforeseen changes, then it is understandable that such adaptation only works when a continual testing of all possibilities of constructive change takes place. This is the only way that "promising monsters" (R. B. Goldschmidt, 1940) can be created and open new pathways in evolution.2 These can be ethological (p. 14) or morphological monsters. Thus in every generation of flies there are wing­ less mutants that are weeded out from the population as being malformed. How­ ever, that even such monsters of evolution have a chance can be seen in the subarctic Kerguelen Islands, where only wingless insects live (beetles, butterflies, and flies). The winged forms are continually blown off the island by storms. From generation to generation, new genetic variants are thus created and tested by selection. They are, in a way, set into the world to test for new changes that might have come about. Extreme specialization limits the potential for further evolution. Whereas the pentadactylid foreleg of the vertebrate had the potential to evolve into the fin of the whale, the wing of a bat or the hooves of a horse, the potential of these latter specialized structures is greatly limited. A fin cannot become reconstructed into an extremity with five fingers or a wing. Specialists, therefore, run a greater risk of extinction when their environment changes drast­ ically. The future lies with the generalists. During the daily life of organisms there are numerous situations in which a rapid adaptability is advantageous. An insect seeking flowers would benefit from remembering where a plant had just blossomed. It is also advantageous for animals to remember where predators are or which pathway is a safe escape route. It is useful when an animal can learn from individual experience, i.e., is able to retain information about specific data or experiences, to store this information, and to adapt its behavior accordingly. During the course of phylogeny, structures of the central nervous system evolved that permitted individual experiences to be stored in recallable engrams so that future behavior could be adjusted accordingly on the basis of these experiences. Here, as well, phylogenetic adaptations determine whether what is learned is obligatory or optional. Animals learn what contributes to their fitness and, since this varies from species to species, innate learning dis­ positions vary accordingly (p. 74). Even though learning is based on phylogenetic adaptations and channeled by

2Chance is creative in this way. H. Markl (1980) has provided a pertinent definition: "I have dealt with these problems so exhaustively because chance stands in such poor stead with learned people: chance, it is said, is never creative. It is merely known as the un­ imaginative shepherd pushing his herd of events toward the gate of average expectation, lacking any originality. Rarely has something been so badly misjudged. For it is only through chance that something new can arise, for novel value only exists in terms of that which does not follow the undeviating path of the standard laws. If we termed it 'spontaneity,1 we would recognize the unpredictable character that only freedom of behavior can produce. However, it is important from the molecular level to that of behavior: chance, left to itself, can create nothing lasting, for it destroys as it builds. A mechanism that evaluates chance, retaining the useful and rejecting the unusable, is required to put to use the innovative potential of chance. Chance produces nothing of value without a selection process occurring, 10 but selection has nothing to select without chance" (translated from the German original). them, new perspectives of evolution were opened with learning, which, among others, is one of the prerequisites for cultural evolution. In general, each animal learns from experience for itself only. However, some species can also learn by imitation. In a free-living group of Japanese macaques on the Japanese island of Koshima, a female discovered the washing of sweet potatoes before eating them. The others saw this and imitated her. Today this behavior has become established as a group-specific characteristic and is a tradition passed on from one generation to the next. Such behavior is sometimes described as "protocultural," but it was only with the evolution of speech and the subsequent development of writing and other information storage systems that true culture could arise. In the protocultural stage, one individual has to show another the behavior to be learned. With the development of speech it became possible to carry on a tradition independently of an object by verbal instruction alone. One Japanese macaque must always see another washing potatoes, but one person can instruct another by saying, "We wash potatoes before we eat them." He could also per­ manently record this information in writing so that it would be available for cen­ turies to literate individuals. Thus, with the introduction of writing, a new, more powerful data bank becomes available to assist the memory enabling knowledge to be passed on without the presence of another society member. Thus accu­ mulative culture develops. With cultural evolution a new level of existence is achieved, since culturally acquired knowledge can maintain existence, to a large extent, independently of its inventors. If the inventor of the radio or another technical innovation died without heirs, the invention could still be perpetuated. Theoretically it could even be utilized by genetically unrelated inhabitants of another planet. Ideologies can survive in the same way and thus have an existence of their own. K. R. Popper (1973) speaks of a world of objective thought content, which exists even though it is a product of our mental activity. The products of this world (theories, books, and others) can be studied without knowledge of the ways in which they originated, in the same way as we can investigate spider webs and bird nests without having seen the animals that made them. Books can outlive us and be studied by other intelligent beings. Popper categorizes this world of objective ideas as World 3, with the world of states of consciousness and behavioral dispositions (World 2), and the physical world (World l).3 All our "artificial organs" (H. Hass, 1970), from tools to books and from fac­ tories to expressways, belong to Popper's World 3. They demonstrate effectively how much and in which ways our accumulating culture dominates human evolution. New ideas, theories, and inventions act like mutations in the biological realm, and they have to stand the test of selection. The problem to be solved is the survival in offspring and this is achieved by the elimination of mistakes. Popper mentions that anatomical and behavioral adaptations of plants and animals are biological analogs of theories: "Theories correspond (like many external objects such as honeycombs . . . ) to bodily organs and their functions. Theories and organs alike are experimental adaptations to the world in which we live" (K. Popper, 1973, p. 125).

3One can also describe World 2 in broader terms as the world of the organic, which differs from World 1 (physical) since its organization is derived from selective pressure (N. Bischof, 1981). 11 Progress in knowledge is the result of a process quite similar to natural selection but consists of the selection of competing hypotheses. "This interpretation applies to what animals know, prescientific knowledge and also scientific cognition. While the animal and prescientific knowledge grow mainly because those with unsuitable hypotheses are themselves selected out, scientific critics often let our theories die instead of us; they select against our wrong ideas before we ourselves are eliminated because of them (Popper, 1973, p. 289). Cultural evolution is by no means always a product of rational planning. F. A. von Hayek (1979) pointed out clearly that mankind, with the ability to imitate and to carry on traditions, develops traditions of learned rules of behavior, whose purposes are, in most instances, not understood. Von Hayek continues that in the past, Man has often learned appropriate behavior without understanding why it is correct, and even today many habits usually serve him better than does rational understanding. The system of traditional rules was tested and developed in a process of screen­ ing, which is analogous to selection on at the biological level. Cultural evolution, therefore, is mainly a result of the forces of selection and is, to a lesser extent, the result of conscious rational planning. In a competitive process, the more suc­ cessful characteristics prevail. Von Hayek is undoubtedly correct when he states that cultural evolution could proceed without any kind of insight and also when he expresses the opinion it has also done so for the majority of mankind's existence. If we consider culture to be the totality of traditional adaptations, then his statement that we were not even capable of designing culture is also true. Even today we can only understand partial aspects of our own culture.4 Nonetheless we must come to terms mentally with the problems of our existence, and through reason come up with solutions. Man has had great success with rational planning in many realms of science, economics, and technology. Furthermore, we can set goals for ourselves and thus significantly influence our further cultural develop­ ment. The developments initiated by founders of states and ideologists testify to such developments. Ideologies can reverse developmental trends completely by setting new goals and thus by altering contingencies of selection. Whether this is of advantage or not from the point of view of the fitness of the society would not be determined until later. For those Russian sects that forbade their members to engage in sexual intercourse—and who disappeared soon afterward because of so few new recruits— ideology was shown to be deleterious. The experiments of modern mass societies, whose ideologies stress the equality of all of its members, are fascinating. With few exceptions, in the majority of cultures, successful men, those controlling the natural resources, also produced more descendants. They could afford to have more women, and the women of their subordinates also stood at their disposal. Successful Yanomami chieftains have more wives and children than others in the tribe (N. A. Chagnon, 1979). In

4Cause and effect are so intertwined here that we cannot usually use our linear causal reasoning to understand all the relationships operating here. D. Dorner and R. Reither (1978) contributed to this concept in their experiment giving twelve intelligent students the task of improving the living conditions on the fictitious developing country "Tana." The conditions in the country were known and could be varied. In almost all cases, the student destroyed the originally stable structure and often produced catastrophic consequences for 12 the country. China the wealthy not only had supplemental wives and concubines but also access to the wives of their subordinates. The reigning ideology of the Chou Dynasty (1100-222 B.C.) stated that this was their subjects' obligation and fostered among subordinates the wish to comply since they believed that only the ruling class possessed certain magical powers, which they transmitted for the common good through reproduction. In the above instances the ideology supported those controlling the society's resources and led to growth in the population of the ruling class at the expense of the lower classes (further examples in K. MacDonald, 1983). With Christianity, which interestingly enough spread first among the slaves and the poor of Rome, the ideology of equality—of "leveling" as Nietzsche expressed it—came into ex­ istence. It was directed against those controlling the resources and attempted to construct an ethic of sharing and equalization as well as of enforced monogamy. A papal ban against princes was one means of stopping polygamy in the ruling European classes. The ideology of monogamy with its prohibition against birth control was the most decisive step since it removed the privilege of reproduction from the upper classes. Anyone who was capable of providing nourishment for a family could sire offspring. The division of property was less important, and such differences were even­ tually tolerated by Christianity. Such an ideology leads to the situation in which there is not a limited number of privileged families reproducing at a higher rate relative to other classes, but one in which all levels of society reproduce. It remains to be established whether the single wife in a monogamous society without birth control has more children than those in polygamous societies. I believe it is possible but cannot cite any figures for this. Perhaps this was a factor responsible for the great success of Europeans throughout the world. The Hutterites of Canada can act as a model as to how a group with a strict monogamy and no birth control achieves a higher birth rate than other groups in the Canadian population. The question here is what sort of effect this pattern of behavior will have in the long term. Populations who obey an ideology of reproductive egalitarity may experience a rapid increase in number over other groups following other prescriptions. At the same time, however, the further evolution of certain traits may be slowed down.5 Man is certainly the first being who consciously set goals. Some went as far as to argue that Man, therefore, overpowered nature—that he emancipated himself from the contraints of biology. But whatever man does will be measured by the yardstick of selection. By striving toward a particular goal set by an ideology man directs his future evolution and secures its course within certain limits against the unpredictabilities of chance. However, it will always be selection that will decide whether his choice was correct. Any kind of concept, even the ideas of an insane person, can influence the fate of a people, for better or worse, just as mutations can in the realm of biology. Today we are experiencing how selection is sifting out the newly established

5In this context some recent developments are of interest. D. R. Vining (1986) drew attention to the fact that in modern society the less economically successful produce more children than those better endowed. This leads to a decline of the IQ from one generation to the next in modern societies. Presently this does not need to bea^natter of grave concern, since such a trend might be temporary. However, it might be useful to observe further developments. 13 social and economic systems of the world according to their adaptiveness. We cannot know the results of this experiment, but without doubt various goals have selective consequences, and they may ultimately even influence biological evo­ lution. In the animal world, behavioral changes are the pacemakers of phylogenetic development (E. Mayr, 1950, 1970). If an insect larva shifts to a plant species not yet utilized by the species and becomes imprinted on the new host plant so that the imago henceforth selects this host plant, this new preference will certainly result in other adaptations in the insects' life habits and anatomy. In choosing this new host plant, the insect was placed under new selective conditions. For example, if the epidermis of the new host plant is firmer, the insect may develop stronger mandibles for biting. K. R. Popper (1973), in his "speartip theory" of behavioral evolution, inde­ pendently developed the same ideas as Ernst Mayr. He presumes that specific innate tendencies (the avoidance of danger, search for food, etc.) are subject to mutations without organs of the body being simultaneously affected. Any changes affect only the apparatus controlling behavior—in vertebrates it is the central ner­ vous system and not the morphological structures that carry out the actions. These can be independently altered by other mutations. Mutative changes in the mech­ anism's guiding behavior (Popper speaks of a "central tendency structure"6) gen­ erally disturb the functions of an organism less than mutations in structures ex­ ecuting behavior. It is easier to correct maladaptive changes in the central tendency structure with compensating behaviors. New behavioral tendencies can determine further development. Popper cites changes in feeding habits as an example and mentions in this context the classic Lamarckian example: the giraffe. He is of the opinion that changes in the eating habits must have preceded the anatomical neck modifications. A Darwinian explanation can thus be found for the much discussed topic of orthogenesis (goal-directed evolution).

If a new goal, tendency, disposition, capability or behavior pattern develops in the central tendency structure, this would influence the effects of natural selection in such a way that previously maladaptive (but potentially favorable) mutations now become adaptive ones if they support the newly developed tendency. This means, however, that the development of the executing organs will be guided by this new tendency or objective and is thus 'goal-directed' (K. R. Popper, 1973, p. 305).

Popper (1973) speaks of "promising ethological monsters" (see also p. 10), which deviate markedly from their parents in their behavior and thus introduce new developments. Certainly a new development is more likely to be preceded by a new behavior pattern than it is by a change in structure, since a new behavior is less likely to fatally disturb the functioning of an organism as a whole and is also less likely to prove lethal than a monstrous anatomical change. A new behavior pattern that takes advantage of previously existing light-sensitive structures could significantly enhance their selective value. Thus an "interest" in vision could

This "central tendency structure" consists of, on the one hand, built-in instructions, for example, to flee from one situation or to attack in another (Popper speaks of goal structures), and, on the other hand, of situations enabling one to interpret external stimuli (ability structures). These two, however, cannot be distinguished from one another, so 14 Popper introduces the concept of "central tendency structure" to unite both structures. become one of the key factors in the orthogenesis of the eye.7 In this way, according to Popper, changes in the goal structure lead and the development of the anatom­ ical-physiological structures follow. "As the vitalists claim, the direction can ac­ tually be determined by a tendency similar to consciousness—by a goal structure or the capability structure of the organism, which can develop a tendency or a 'wish' to use an eye, and an ability to interpret the perceptions which it perceives from it" (K. R. Popper, 1973, p. 307). I have referred to these concepts in some depth because I feel they are highly significant for Man. What has been stated here does not only apply to genetically controlled behavioral mutations. In the human sphere, new ideas, ideologies, and other mental concepts, as stated earlier, can also become evolutionary pacemakers and determine its future course. Developments like these lead to cultural delin­ eations and thus initiate separate developments, termed aptly "cultural pseudo- speciation" by E. H. Erikson (1966). To what extent innate values channel the course of evolution will be discussed later. There are indications that our concepts of "differentiated" and "higher" levels are universal goal structures and thus determine our behavior (p. 659). Cul­ tural evolution is also strongly influenced by , which does not appear to be exclusively a product of culture. With cultural evolution, mankind developed an adaptive mechanism that in historical times has no doubt exceeded the biological ones in importance. It is probable that cultural changes in life style are followed by subsequent genetic modifications and there are sound indications for this. Lactose tolerance is ob­ served in the majority of adults in peoples living on milk products—a trait lacking in hunter/gatherer societies. Presumably with the discovery of agriculture and the growth of the habit of drinking milk as an adult, a corresponding genetic adaptation was selected for (L. L. Cavalli-Sforza, 1981; further citations therein). In general we have not changed much in our anatomy in the last 20,000 years. And from the way we handle our culturally acquired knowledge we may even assume that our basic behavioral outfit has remained the same. People with the motivations and intellectual capacity of late upper paleolithic man are now flying jet bombers and confronted with the task of surviving in anonymous society. Since phylogenetic and cultural evolution are shaped by similar selection pres­ sures, cultural evolution phenocopies the biological evolution in many ways. Ex­ amples of this are found in the comparison of cultural and phylogenetic ritualization (p. 439). The same functional laws apply to both areas. Thus the rate of change or of mutation should in neither case be excessive, for an excessive mutation rate is dangerous. Progress depends on the balance achieved between the preserving "conservative" forces and those promoting change. We stick to the proven, but experiment with change in small doses. This is certainly "adaptive," because it is improbable that the entire store of cultural tradition should have lost its adaptive value from one generation to the next. Our need for security makes us cling passionately to our "beloved" customs. It is from this secure base that we experiment with new ideas and insights.

7"Thus an interest in seeing could be fixed and may become a significant factor in the orthogenesis of the eye; even the smallest improvements in its anatomy could have a high survival value if they were utilized by the organism's goal and capability structure" (K. R. Popper, 1973, p. 311). 15 Summary 1.2 Living systems owe their organization to processes of adaptation through se­ lection. Adaptations are all structures which contribute to the survival of the in­ dividual in question and ultimately to its survival in offspring or in short to its fitness. In their adaptations, organisms mirror features of their outer subjective world relevant to fitness. They represent sets of hypotheses about this world which were tested by selection. Thus a fin of a fish, as well as the motor program for its use, both develop prior to hatching within the egg, represent assumptions about the environment in which the fish is going to live. As a prerequisite for adaptation to occur, an interaction between the adapted organismic system and its environ­ ment must take place. During phylogeny, variants of the phenotype, brought about by genetic changes, are tested by selection as to their adaptedness and the geno­ types for adaptive traits retained. This way the organisms can be said to inform themselves in a process analogous to a trial and error learning about its environment and that information is coded and stored genetically. Phylogenetic adaptation is supplemented by adaptation via learning and indi­ vidual experience, and, in humans, also via traditions. It is in the latter case that information transfer occurs. Cultural evolution phenocopies phylogenesis in many respects, since in both cases the shape of the resulting pattern is determined by selection. Customs must prove themselves in order to be retained. We may, how­ ever, learn from failures by eliminating customs without sacrificing the life of the individual at the same time. Behavior patterns are often the pacemakers of further development in both cultural and phylogenetic evolution. Cultural evolution has certainly proceeded in an unplanned manner for most of human existence, and we tend to observe certain customs without insight into their rationale since they have proved themselves beneficial. Culture, because of its complexities, is difficult to plan, but by the setting of goals its development can be directed.

16 References 721 Abbott, L. (1975). Paternal Touch of the Newborn: Its Role in Paternal Attachment. Thesis, Boston University, Boston, MA. Abramovitch, R. (1976). The relation of attention and proximity to dominance in pre-school children. In: Chance, M. R. A. and Larsen, R. R. (eds.), The Social Structure of Attention. London: Wiley, pp. 153–177. Abramovitch, R. (1980). Attention structures in hierarchically organized groups. In: Omark, D. R. , Strayer, F. F. , and Freedman, D. G. (eds.), Dominance Relations. New York/London: Garland STPM Press, pp. 381–396. Adams, J. P. (1964). Adolescent personal problems as a function of age and sex. J. Genet. Psychol 104, 207–214. Addiego, F. , Belzer, E. , Comolli, J. , Moger, W. , Perry, J. , and Whipple, B. (1981). Female ejaculation: A case study. J. Sex. Res. 17, 13–21. Adler, Ch . (1977). Mechanismen der Gruppenbindung. Aggression und Aggressionskontrolle der Eskimos im Thuledistrikt. Diss. München. Ahrens, R. (1954). Beiträge zur Entwicklung des Physiognomie-und Mimikerkennens. Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 2, 412–454. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1963). The development of infant-mother interaction among the Ganda. In: Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behavior, London: Methuen, pp. 67–104. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1967). Infancy in Uganda: Infant Care and the Growth of Love. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins University Press. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1969). Object relations, dependency and attachment: A theoretical review of the infantmother relationship. Child Develop. 40, 969–1025. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1973). The development of infant-mother attachment. In: Caldwell, B. M. and Ricciuti, H. N. (eds.), Child Development Research. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–95. Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1977). and its utility in cross-cultural research. In: Leiderman, P. H. , Tulkin, St. R. , and Rosenfeld, A. (eds.), Culture and Infancy, Variations in the Human Experience. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 49–67. Aldis, O. (1975). Play Fighting. New York: Academic Press. Alexander, R. D. (1974). The evolution of social behavior. Ann. Rev. Ecol. Syst. 5, 325–383. Alexander, R. D. (1977). Natural selection and the analysis of human sociality. In: Goulden, C. E. (ed.), The Changing Scenes in the Natural Sciences, 1776-1976. Philadelphia: Academy of Nat. Sciences (Sp. Publ. 12), pp. 283–337. Alexander, R. D. (1978). Natural selection and social laws. In: Engelhardt, T. and Callahan, D. (eds.), Morals, Science, and Society, Vol. 3. Hastings-on-Hudson, NY: Hastings Ctr. Inst. Soc. Alexander, R. D. (1979). Evolution and culture. In: Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, W. (eds.), Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press, pp. 59–79. (a) Alexander, R. D. (1979). Evolution, social behavior, and ethics. In: Engelhardt, T. and Callahan, D. (eds.), The Foundations of Ethics and Its Relationship to Science, Vol. 4. Hastings-on-Hudson, NY: The Hastings Center, pp. 124–155. (b) Alexander, R. D. (1979). Darwinism and Human Affairs. Seattle: University of Washington Press, (c) Alexander, R. D. (1987). The Biology of Moral Systems. New York: Aldine de Gruyter. Alland, A. (1972). Cultural evolution: The Darwinian model. Social Biol. 19, 227–239. Alland, A. and McCay, B. (1973). The Concept of Adaptation in Biological and Cultural Evolution. In: Honigman, J. (ed.), Handbook of Social and . Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 142–178. Allemann-Tschopp, G. (1978). Geschlechtsrollen. Bern: Hans Huber. Allemann-Tschopp, G. (1979). Geschlechtsrollen. Versuch einer interdisziplinären Synthese. Bern/Stuttgart/Wien: Hans Huber. Allen, D. E. and Guy, R. F. (1974). Conversion Analysis. The Sociology of Talk. The Hague: Mouton Press. von Allesch, G. J. (1931). Die nicht-euklidische Struktur des phänomenalen Raumes. Jena. Almagor, U. (1977). Raiders and Elders: A Confrontation of Generations among the Dassanetch. In: Fukui, K. and Turton, D. (eds.), Warfare among East African Herders. Senri Ethnological Studies, No. 3, Museum of Ethnology. Japan: Osaka Press, pp. 119–145. Als, H. (1977). The newborn communicates. J. Commun. 27, 66–73. 722 Als, H. , Tronick, E. and Brazelton, T. B. (1979). Analysis of face-to-face interaction in infant-adult dyads. In: Lamb, M. E. , Suomi, S. J. , and Stephenson, G. R. (eds.), Social Interaction Analysis: Methodological Issues. Madison: Univ. of Wisconsin Press, pp. 33–76. Alt, F. (1983). Frieden ist moglich. Die Politik der Bergpredigt. Munchen: Piper. Altman, I. (1975). The Environmental and Social Behavior. Monterey, CA: Brooks/Cole Publ. Comp. Altmann, J. (1974). Observational study of behavior: Sampling methods. Behaviour 49, 227–265. Amato, P. R. (1983). The effects of urbanization on interpersonal behavior. Field studies in Papua New Guinea. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 14, 353–367. Ambrose, J. A. (1960). The Smiling and Related Responses in Early Human Infancy. An Experimental and Theoretical Study of their Course and Significance. University of London, Ph. D. Dissert. Vol. 2. Ambrose, J. A. (1961). The development of the smiling response in early infancy. In: Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behaviour, Vol. I. London: Methuen, pp. 179–196. Ambrose, J. A. (1969). Stimulation in Early Infancy. London: Academic Press. Amoore, J. E. , Pelosi, P. , and Forrester, L. J. (1977). Specific anosmias to 5α-Androst-16-en-3-one and w- pentadecalactone: The urinous and musky primary odours. Chem. Senses Flavour 2, 401–425. Amoore, J. E. , Popplewell, J. R. , and Whissell-Buechy, D. (1975). Sensitivity of women to musk odor: No menstrual variation. J. Chem. Ecol. 1, 291–297. Amthauer, R. (1966). Psychologische Grundfragen der Berufswahl. VDI-Nachrichten, 20 (48). Anastasia, A. (1958). Differential Psychology. New York: Anastasi. Anderson, G. C. (1978). Der Ursprung der Intelligenz und die sensomotorische Entwicklung des Kindes. In: Steiner, G. (ed.), Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts. 7. Piaget and die Folgen. Zurich: Kindler, pp. 94–120. Anderson, P. (1983). The reproductive role of the human breast. Current Anthropol. 24, 25–45. Anderson, S. W. and Jaffe, J. (1972). The Definition, Detection and Timing of Vocalic Syllables in Speech Signals. Scientific Report No. 12 of Communication Sciences, New York State, Psychiatric Institute. Andersson, Y. , Lagercrantz, H. , Winberg, J. , and Öfverholm, U. (1978). Konjunctivit, mimik och synoeteende hos nyfödda barn före och efter Cre-de-profylax. Läkartid-ningen 75, 302–304. Angrick, M. (1983). Endorphine. Pharmazie Unserer Zeit 12, 129–134. Antonelli, A. (1979). Small Beginnings: The Effects of Social Behavior on Cognitive Development in the First Three Months. Paper presented at the British Psychological Society Developmental Section Annual Conference, University of Southampton. Archavsky, I. A. (1952). Immediate breastfeeding of newborn infant in the prophylaxis of the so-called physiological loss of weight. Vopr. Pediat. 20, 45–53. Ardrey, R. (1966). The Territorial Imperative. New York: Atheneum. Argyle, M. (1969). Social Interaction. London: Methuen. Argyle, M. and Cook, M. (1976). Gaze and Mutual Gaze. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Argyle, M. , Furnham, A. , and Graham, J. A. (1981). Social Situations. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Aries, Ph . (1978). Geschichte der Kindheit. München: dtv Wissenschaft. Asch, S. E. (1951). Effects of group pressure upon the modification and distortion of judgments. In: Guetzkow, H. (ed.), Groups, Leadership, and Men. Pittsburgh, PA: Carnegie Press. Asch, S. E. (1955). On the use of metaphor on the description of persons. In: Werner, H. (ed.), On Expressive Language. Worcester, MA: Clark University Press. Asch, S. E. (1958). The Metaphor: A Psychological Inquiry. In: Taquri, R. and Petrullo, L. (eds.), Person, Perception, and Interpersonal Behavior. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Aschoff, J. (1981). A survey on biological rhythms. In: Aschoff, J. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 4. London/New York: Plenum, pp. 3–10. (a) Aschoff, J. (1981). Freerunning and Entrained Circadian Rhythms. In: Aschoff, J. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 4. London/New York: Plenum, pp. 81–93. (b) 723 Aschoff, J. (1981). Annual rhythms in man. In: Aschoff, J. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 4. London/New York: Plenum, pp. 475–487. (c) Aschoff, J. and Wever, R. (1980). Über Reproduzierbarkeit circadianer Rhythmen beim Menschen. Klin. Wochenschr. 58, 323–335. Aschoff, J. and Wever, R. (1981). The circadian system of man. In: Aschoff, J. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 4. London/New York: Plenum, pp. 311–331. Aspey, W. P. and Blankenship, J. E. (1977). Spiders and snails and statistic tales: Application of multivariate analyses to diverse ethological data. In: Hazlett, B. A. (ed.), Quantitative Methods in the Study of Behavior. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 75–120. Aspey, W. P. and Blankenship, J. E. (1978). Comparative Ethometrics: Congruency of different multivariate analyses applied to the same ethological data. Behav. Proc. 3, 173–195. Attili, G. and Benigni, L. (1979). Interazione sociale—ruolo sessuale e comportamento verbale: lo stile retorico naturale del linguaggio femminile nell’ interazione faccia a faccia. In: Societa di Linguistica Italiana (ed.), Retorica e Scienze. Rom: Bulzoni, pp. 261–280. Attili, G. , Hold, B. , and Schleidt, M. (1982). Relationship among Peers in Kindergarten: A Cross-Cultural Study. Paper presented at the IXth Congress of the International Primatological Society, Atlanta, Georgia, USA, 8-13 August, 1982. Austen, S. (1979). Mutter-Kind-Interaktionen unmittelbar nach der Geburt. Paper presented at 6. Meeting der Int. Studiengemeinschaft für pränatale Psychologie (ISPP), Basel, 4. 9. 1979. Ausubel, D. P. and Sullivan, E. V. (1980). Das Kindesalter. Fakten—Probleme—Theorie. München: Juventa. Avedon, E. M. and Sutton-Smith, B. (1971). The Study of Games. New York: Wiley. Babad, Y. E. , Alexander, I. E. , and Babad, E. Y. (1983). Returning the smile of the stranger: Developmental patterns and socialization factors. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop. 48, [203], 5. Bachofen, J. J. (1861). Das Mutterrecht. Stuttgart. Badinter, E. (1981). Die Mutterliebe. München: Piper. Baerends G. P. (1956). Aufbau tierischen Verhaltens. In: Kükenthal, W. (ed.), Handbuch der Zoologie, Vol. 8 (10), pp. 1–32. Baerends, G. P. and Drent, R. H. (1970). The Herring Gull’s egg. Behaviour Suppl. 17. Bahuchet, S. (1983). Territoriality and Values among the Aka Pygmies in the Central African Republic. Paper presented at the 3rd Intern. Conf. on Hunters and Gatherers, Werner-Reimers-Stiftung, Bad Homburg 13-16 June, 1983. Bailey, K. G. (1987). Human Paleopsychology—Applications to Aggression and Pathological Processes. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Bailey, W. T. (1982). Affinity: An ethological perspective of the infant-father relationship in humans. Infant Behav. Develop. 5 (special ICIS issue), 12. Bakeman, R. and Brownlee, J. R. (1982). Social rules governing object conflicts in toddlers and preschoolers. In: Rubin, K. H. and Ross, H. S. (eds.), Peer Relations and Social Skills in Childhood. New York: Springer, pp. 99–111. Baker, J. W. and Schaie, K. W. (1969). Effects of aggression “alone” or with “another” on physiological and psychological arousal. J. Personality Social Psychol. 12, 80–96. Ball, W. and Tronick, F. (1971). Infant responses to impending collision, optical and real. Science 171, 818–820. Bally, G. (1945). Vom Ursprung und von den Grenzen der Freiheit, eine Deutung des Spieles bei Tier und Mensch. Basel: Birkhäuser. Bandura, A. (1973). Aggression: A Social Learning Analysis. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Bandura, A. and Walters, R. H. (1963). Social Learning and Personality Development. New York: Ronald Press. Bandura, A. , Underwood, B. , and Fromson, M. E. (1975). Disinhibition of aggression through diffusion of responsibility and dehumanization of victims. J. Res. Personality , 9, 253–269. Barash, D. P. (1977). Sociobiology and Behavior. New York/Amsterdam: Elsevier. Barkow, J. H. (1979). Human ethology: Empirical wealth, theoretical death. Behav. Sci. 2, 27. 724 Barlow, G. W. (1972). A paternal role for bulls of the Galapagos Islands sea lions. Evolution 26, 307–308. Barlow, G. W. and Rowell, T. E. (1984). The contribution of game theory to animal behavior. (Open Peer Commentary for J. Maynard-Smith.) Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 101–103. Barnard, A. (1978). The kin terminology system of the Nharo Bushmen. Cah. Etud. Afr. 72, 607–629. Barnard, C. J. (1980). Flock feeding and time budgets in the House Sparrow (Passer domesticus L.). Anim. Behav. 28, 295–309. Barner-Bárry, C. (1983). Zum Verhältnis zwischen Ethologie and Politik: Macht, Dominanz, Autorität und Aufmerksamkeitsstruktur. In: Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologic Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 101–110. Barnes, D. M. (1988). Meeting on the mind. Science 239, 142–144. (Report on the meeting of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology with reference to the discoveries of Saul Schanberg, Tiffany Field, and Gary Evonuik.) Baron, R. A. (1977). Human Aggression. New York: Plenum Press. Baron, R. A. and Ball, R. L. (1974). The aggression inhibiting influence of non-hostile humor. J. Exp. Social Psychol. 10, 23–33. Barrett, J. E. (1972). Schedules of electric shock presentation in the behavioral control of imprinted ducklings. J. Exp. Analysis Behav. 18, 305–321. Barry, H. , Bacon, M. K. , and Child, I. L. (1957). A cross-cultural survey of some sex differences in socialization. J. Abnormal Social Psychol. 55, 327–332. Barth, F. (1959/1960). The Land Use Patterns of Migratory Tribes of South Persia. Norsk. Geogr. Tidsk. 17, 1–11. Barton, S. , Birns, B. , and Ronch, J. (1971). Individual differences in the visual pursuit behavior of neonates. Child Develop. 42, 313–319. Basedow, H. (1906). Anthropological notes on the western coastal tribes of the northern territory of South Australia. Trans. Roy. Soc. S. Austr. 31, 1–62. Bateson, G. and Mead, M. (1942). Balinese Character: A Photographic Analysis. Special Publication of the New York Science, II. New York. Beaglehole, E. (1932). Property: A Study in Social Psychology. New York: Macmillan. de Beauvoir, S. (1968). Das andere Geschlecht—Sitte und Sexus der Frau. Hamburg: Rowohlt. Becker-Carus, Ch ., Buchholtz, Ch. , Etienne, A. , Franck, D. , Medioni, J. , Schöne, H. Sevenster, P. , Stamm, R. A. , and Tschanz, B. (1972). Motivation, Handlungsbereitschaft, Trieb. Z. Tierpsychol. 30, 321–326. Beit-Hallahmi, B. (1981). The Kibbutz family. Revival or survival. J. Family Issues 2, 259–274. Beit-Hallahmi, B. and Rabin, A. I. (1977). The Kibbutz as a social experiment and as a childrearing laboratory. Amer. Psychol. 32, 532–541. Bell, A. P. and Weinberg, M. S. (1978). Homosexuality: A Study of Diversity among Men and Women. New York: Simon & Schuster. Bell, A. P. , Weinberg, M. S. , and Hammersmith, S. K. (1981). Der Kinsey Institut Report über sexuelle Orientierung und Partnerwahl. München: Bertelsmann. Bell, F. L. (1934). Warfare among the Tanga. Oceania 5, 253–279. Bell-Krannhals, I. (1984). Was sich liebt, das versteckt sich—Interaktionsmuster zwischen Liebenden auf Kaileuna Island. Paper presented at meeting of the DFG-priority program “Verbale Interaktion,” 27 January, 1984, Seewiesen. Belsky, J. (1980). Child Maltreatment. Amer. Psychol. 35, 320–335. Belzer, E. (1981). Organic Expulsion of Women: A Review and Heuristic Inquiry. J. Sex. Res. 17, 1–12. Benard, Ch . and Schlaffer, E. (1980). Der Mann auf der Strasse. Hamburg: Rowohlt. Benbow, C. P. and Stanley, J. C. (1980). Sex differences in mathematical ability: Fact or artifact? Science 210, 1262–1264. Benbow, C. P. and Stanley, J. C. (1983). Sex differences in mathematical reasoning ability: More facts. Science 222, 1029–1031. Benedeck, T. (1952). Psychosexual Functions in Woman. New York: Ronald Press. Benedict, R. (1935). Patterns of Culture. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. Bennett, J. H. , Rhodes, F. A. , and Robson, H. N. (1959). A possible genetic base for Kuru. Amer. J. Hum. Genet. II, 169-187. Bennett, J. W. (1969). Northern Plainsmen: Adaptive Strategies and Agrarian Life. Chicago: Aldine. 725 Bennett, J. W. (1976). The Ecological Transition: Cultural Anthropology and Human Adaptation. New York: Pergamon. Bentley, D. R. (1971). Genetic control of an insect neuronal network. Science 174, 1139–1141. Bentley, D. R. and Hoy, R. R. (1972). Genetic control of the neuronal network generating cricket song patterns. Animal Behav. 20, 478–492. Benton, D. (1982). The influence of androstenol—a putative human pheromone—on mood throughout the menstrual cycle. Biol. Psychol. 15, 249–256. Berger, P. L. and Luckmann, T. (1970). Die gesellschaftliche Konstruktion der Wirklichkeit. Eine Theorie der Wissenssoziologie. Frankfurt/M: S. Fischer. van den Berghe, P. L. (1979). Human Family Systems. New York: Elsevier. Berghe, P. L. (1981). The Ethnic Phenomenon. New York: Elsevier. Bergman, B. A. (1971). The Effects of Groups Size, Personal Space and Success Failure on Physiological Arousal, Test Performance and uestionnaire Responses. Doctoral dissertation, Temple University, Philadelphia, Univ. Microfilm No. 71-31072. Berkowitz, L. (1962). Aggression: A Social Psychological Analysis. New York/London: McGraw-Hill. Berkowitz, L. (1970). Aggressive humor as a stimulus to aggressive responses. J. Personality Social Psychol. 16, 710–717. Bernstein, L. (1976). The Unanswered Question. Cambridge, MA: Harvard Univ. Press. Bertaux, P. (1963). Mutation der Menschheit. Neuauflage 1983. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp. Bertram, B. C. R. (1976). Kin selection in lions and in evolution. In: Bateson, P. P. and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Bettelheim, B. (1954). Symbolic Wounds. New York: Collier Books. Bettelheim, B. (1977). Kinder brauchen Märchen. Stuttgart: DVA. Beuchelt, E. and Ziehr, W. (1979). Schwarze Königreiche. Frankfurt/M: W. Krüger. Bever, T. G. 1970. The cognitive basis for linguistic structures. In: Hayes, J. R. (ed.), Cognition and the Development of Language. New York: Wiley, pp. 279–352. (a) Bever, T. G. (1970). The influence of speech performance on linguistic structures. In: Flores d’Arcais, G. B. , and Levelt, W. J. M. (eds.), Advances in Psycholinguistics. Amsterdam: North Holland Publ. Comp., pp. 4–30. (b) Bever, T. G. (1970). The Nature of Cerebral Dominance in Speech Behavior of the Child and Adult. Mechanisms of Language Development. New York/London: Academic Press, (c) Bicchieri, M. G. (1969). The Differential Use of Identical Features of Physical Habitat in Connection with Exploitative Settlement and Community Patterns: The Bambuti. Contributions to Anthropology: Band Societies. Proc. Conf. Band Organ. Ottawa, Nat. Museum. Can. Bull. 230, 65–72. Bicchieri, M. G. (1973). Hunters and Gatherers Today. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Bigelow, A. (1977). Infants’ Recognition of Mother. Paper presented to the Society for Research in Child Development, New Orleans. Bigelow, R. S. (1970). The Dawn Warriors: Man’s Evolution Toward Peace. London: Hutchinson. Billow, R. M. (1977). Metaphor: A review of the psychological literature. Psychol. Bull. 84, 81–92. Bilz, R. (1944). Zur Grundlegung einer Paläopsychologie. I. Paläophysiologie; II. Paläopsychologie. Schweizerische Z. Psychol. 3, 202–212, 272-280. Bilz, R. (1965). Der Subjektzentrismus im Erleben der Angst. In: Ditfurth, H. V. (ed.), Aspekte der Angst. Stuttgart. Binford, L. R. (1981). Bones, Ancient Man, and Modern Myths. New York/London/Toronto: Academic Press. Biocca, E. (1970). Yanoama: The Narrative of a White Girl Kidnapped by Amazonian Indians. New York: E. P. Dutton. Birdsell, J. B. (1968). Some predictions for the Pleistocene based on equilibrium systems among recent hunter- gatherers. In: Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 229–240. Birdwhistell, R. L. (1960). Kinesics and communication. In: Carpenter, E. and McLuhan, M. (eds.), Explorations in Communication. Boston: Beacon Press, pp. 54–64. Birdwhistell, R. L. (1963). The kinesic level in the investigation of the emotions. In: Knapp, P. H. (ed.), Expression of the Emotions in Man. New York: International Universities Press, pp. 123–139. 726 Birdwhistell, R. L. (1968). Communication without words. In: Alexandre, P. (ed.), L’ A venture Humaine. Encycl. Sci. de l’Homme, Vol. 5. Paris: Kister, S. A., pp. 157–166. Birdwhistell, R. L. (1970). Kinesics and Context. Philadelphia: University of Pennsylvania Press. Birdwhistell, R. L. (1973). Kinesics and Context. Essays on Body-Motion Communication. Harmonds-worth, Middlesex: Penguin Books Ltd. Birren, F. (1950). Color Psychology and Therapy. New York: McGraw-Hill. Bischof, N. (1972). The biological foundations of the incest taboo. Social Sci. Inform. 11, 7–36. (a) Bischof, N. (1972). Inzuchtbarrieren in Säugetiersozietäten. Homo 23, 330–351. (b) Bischof, N. (1973). Die biologischen Grundlagen des Inzesttabus. In: Reinert, G. (ed.), Bericht über den 27. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologic Kiel 1970. Göttingen: C. J. Hogrefe, pp. 115–142. Bischof, N. (1975). A system’s approach toward the functional connections of attachment and fear. Child Develop. 46, 801–817. Bischof, N. (1981). Aristoteles, Galilei, Kurt Lewin—und die Folgen. In: Michaelis, W. (ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Zürich 1980, Bd. 1. Göttingen/Toronto/Zürich: C. J. Hogrefe, pp. 17–39. Bixler, R. H. (1981). The incest controversy. Psychol. Rep. 49, 267–283. Bixler, R. H. (1983). Homosexual twin incest avoidance. J. Sex Res. 19, 296–302. Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1974). Male-male competition and infanticide among the langurs (Presbytis entellus) of Abu, Rajasthan. Folia Primatol. 22, 19–58. Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1976). Care and exploitation of non-human primate infants by conspecifics other than the mother. In: Rosenblatt, J. S. , Hinde, R. A. , Shaw, E. , and Beers, C. (eds.), Advances in the Study of Behavior. New York: Academic Press, pp. 101–158. Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1977). Infanticide as a primate reproductive strategy. Amer. Sci. 65, 40–49. (a) Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1977). The langurs of Abu. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, (b) Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1979). Infanticide among animals: A review, classification, and examination of the implications for the reproductive strategies of females. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 13–40. Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1981). The Woman That Never Evolved. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Blafifer-Hrdy, S. (1982). Positivist thinking encounters field primatology, resulting in agonistic behavior. Soc. Sci. Inform. 21, 245–250. Bleek, D. F. (1930). Rock-Paintings in South Africa. London: Methuen. Blehar, M. C. , Lieberman, A. F. , and Ainsworth, M. D. S. (1977). Early face-to-face interaction in its relation to later infant-mother attachment. Child Develop. 48, 181–194. Block, J. H. (1976). Debatable conclusions about sex differences. Contemp. Psychol. 21, 517–522. (a) Block, J. H. (1976). Assessing sex differences: issues, problems and pitfalls. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 22, 283–308. (b) Block, N. (1979). A Confusion among innateness. (Commentary). Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 27–29. Blurton-Jones, N. G. (1972). Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Blurton-Jones, N. G. (1976). Growing points in human ethology: Another link between ethology and the social sciences? In: Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. London: Cambridge Press, pp. 427–450. Blurton-Jones, N. G. (1984). A selfish origin for human sharing: Tolerated theft. Ethol. Sociobiol. 5, 1–3. Blurton-Jones, N. G. and Konner, M. J. (1973). Sex differences in behaviour of London and bushman children. In: Michael, R. P. and Crook, J. (eds.), Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London: Academic Press, pp. 689–750. Boal, F. (1969). Territoriality on the Shankill-Falls Divide, Belfast. Irish Geogr. 6, 30–35. Boas, F. (1895, 1970). The Social Organization and the Secret Societies of the Kwakiutl Indians. Report of the U.S. National Museum. New York: Johnson Reprint Corporation. Boas, F. (1911, rev. 1938). The Mind of Primitive Man. New York: Macmillan. Boas, F. (1928). Anthropology and Modern Life. New York: Norton. Boas, F. (1938). General Anthropology. New York: Heath. Boehn, M. V. (1976, rev. 1982). Die Mode. Eine Kulturgeschichte vom Barock bis zum Jugendstil. Adapted by I. Loschek . München: Bruckmann. Boesch, Ch . and Boesch, H. (1981). Sex differences in the use of natural hammers by wild chimpanzees. A preliminary report. J. Hum. Evol. 10, 585–593. 727 Boesiger, W. and Girsberger, H. (1967). Le Corbusier 1910-1965. Zürich: Verlag für Architektur—Artemis. Bohannan, P. (ed.) (1967). Law and Warfare. Studies in the Anthropology of Conflict. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press. Boinski-McConnell, H. and Burgess, R. L. (1978). The effect of Physical Attractiveness on Family Interaction Patterns in Problem and Non-Problem Families. The Pennsylvania State University. Bolinger, D. (1978). Intonation across . In: Greenberg, J. H. , Ferguson, Ch. A. , and Maravcsik, E. A. (eds.), Universals of Human Language. Vol. 2: Phonology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, pp. 471–524. Bolinger, D. (1980). Intonation and “nature.” In: Foster, M. L. and Brandes, St. H. (eds.), Symbol as Sense. New York: Academic Press, pp. 9–24. Bolk, L. (1926). Das Problem der Menschwerdung. Jena. Bolles, R. C. (1979). The functional significance of behavior. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 29–30. Bolles, R. C. and Faneslow, M. S. (1982). Endorphins and behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 33, 87–101. Bolton, R. (1984). The hypoglycaemia-aggression hypothesis: Debate versus research. Current Anthropol. 25, 1–53. Booth, A. and Edwards, J. N. (1980). Fathers: The invisible parent. Sex Roles 6, 445–456. Booth, D. A. and Kirk-Smith, M. D. (1980). Effect of Androstenone on Choice of Location in Other’s Presence. Joint Congress on Chemoreception ECRO IV and ISOT VII, Noordwijkerhout, Holland, S. 175. Borgia, G. (1980). Human aggression as a biological adaptation. In: Lockard, J. L. (ed.), The Evolution of Social Behavior. New York: Elsevier, pp. 165–191. Bornstein, M. H. (1975). Qualities of colour vision in infancy. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 19, 401–419. Bornstein, M. H. (1979). The pace of life: Revisited. Intern. J. Psychol. 14, 83–90. Bornstein, M. H. and Bornstein, H. G. (1976). The pace of life. Nature 259, 557–558. Boulding, K. E. (1978). Sociobiology or Biosociology? Society , Sept./Oct., pp. 28–34. Bower, T. G. R. (1966). Slant perception and shape constancy of infants. Science , 151, 832 ff. Bower, T. G. R. (1971). The object in the world of the infant. Sci. American , 225, 30–38. Bower, T. G. R. (1977). A Primer of Infant Development. San Francisco: Freeman. Bower, T. G. R. , Broughton, J. M. , and Moore, M. K. (1970). The coordination of vision and touch in infancy. Perception Psychophys. 8, 51–53. (a) Bower, T. G. R. , (1970). Infant responses to approaching objects: An indicator of response to distal variables. Perception Psychophys. 9, 193–196. (b) Bowlby, J. (1958). The nature of the child’s tie to his mother. Intern. J. Psycho-Analysis 39, 350–373. Bowlby, J. (1969). Attachment and loss. In: Masud, M. and Khan, R. (eds.), Attachment 1. London: Hogarth Press, The Int. Psycho-Analytical Library, No. 79. (a) Bowlby, J. (1969). Mütterliche Zuwendung und geistige Gesundheit. München: Kindler Taschenbücher “Geist und Psyche.” (b) Bowlby, J. (1973). Attachment and loss. In: Masud, M. and Khan, R. (eds.), Separation and Anger 2. London: Hogarth Press, The Int. Psycho-Analytical Library, No. 95. Boyd, R. and Richerson, P. J. (1982). Cultural transmission and the evolution of cooperative behavior. Hum. Ecol. 10, 325–351. Bradley, C. F. , Ross, S. E. , and Warnyca, J. (1983). A prospective study of mothers’ attitudes and feelings following cesarean and vaginal births. Birth 10, 79–83. Brain, P. F. and Benton, D. (1981). The Biology of Aggression. Alphenaan den Rijn, Netherlands: Sijtoff and Noordhoff. Bramel, D. , Taub, B. and Blum, B. (1968). An observer’s reaction to the suffering of his enemy. J. Personality Social Psychol. 8, 384–392. Brannigan, C. R. and Humphries, D. A. (1972). Human Non-Verbal Behaviour, a Means of Communication. In: Burton-Jones, N. G. (ed.), Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 37–64. Brauer, E. (1983). Kunst—Restkunst—Unkunst. Miinchen: R. P. Hartmann. Braun, H. and Elze, C. (1954). Anatomie des Menschen, Bd. 1. Heidelberg: Springer. Breland, K. and Breland, M. (1966). Animal Behavior. New York: Macmillan. Brim, O. G. and Kagan, J. (1980). Constancy and Change in Human Development. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 728 Brislin, R. W. (1980). Cross-cultural research methods. In: Altman, I. , Rapoport, A. and Wohlwill, J. F. (eds.), Human Behavior and Environment, Vol. 4. London/New York: Plenum Press, pp. 47–82. Broad, F. (1976). The Effect of Breast Freeding on Speech Development. J. La Leche League of New Zealand. Brocher, T. (1971). Psychosexuelle Grundlagen der Entwicklung. In: Hassenstein, B. (ed.): Verhaltensbiologie des Kindes. Opladen: Leske Verlag. Bronner, S. J. (1982). The haptic experience of culture. Anthropos 77, 351–361. Bronowski, J. and Bellugi, U. (1980). Language, name and concept. In: Sebeok, T. A. and Umiker-Sebeok, J. (eds.), Speaking of Apes. New York: Plenum Press, pp. 103–113. Brookfield, H. C. and Brown, P. (1963). Struggle for Land. London: Oxford University Press. Brooksbank, B. W. L. , Brown, R. , and Gustafsson, J.A. (1974). The Detection of 5α-androst-16-en-3α-ol in human male axillary sweat. Experientia 30, 864–865. Broude, G. J. (1976). Cross-cultural patterning of some sexual attitudes and practices. Behav. Sci. Res. 11, 227–262. Brown, G. L. , Goodwin, F. K. , and Bunney, W. E., Jr. (1982). Human aggression and suicide: Their relationship to neuropsychiatric diagnoses and serotonin metabolism. In: Ho, B. T. (ed.), Serotonin in Biological Psychiatry. New York: Raven Press, pp. 287–307. Brown, L. R. (1981). World population growth, soil erosion, and food security. Science 214, 995–1002. Brown, P. and Levinson, St. (1978). Universals in language usage: Politeness phenomena. In: Goody, E. N. (ed.), Questions and Politeness Strategies in Social Interaction. Cambridge/New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 56–290. Brown, R. (1965). Social Psychology. New York: Free Press. Brown, R. (1968). Words and Things. New York: Free Press. Brown, R. (1974). Die ersten Sätze von Kind und Schimpanse. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie, Vol. 2. Frankfurt/M.: Fischer Athenaeum), pp. 30–52. Brownlee, F. (1943). The social organization of the !Kung Bushmen of the Northwestern Kalahari. Africa , 14, 124–129. Browntree, L. R. and Conkey, M. W. (1980). Symbolism and the cultural landscape. Ann. Assoc. Amer. Geographers 70, 459–474. Bruner, J. S. (1974). Nature and uses of immaturity. In: Connolly, K. and Bruner, J. S. (eds.), The Growth of Competence. London: Academic Press, pp. 11–48. Bruner, J. S. (1975). The ontogenesis of speech-acts. J. Child Language 2, 1–19. (a) Bruner, J. S. (1975). From communication to language: A psychological perspective. Cognition 3, 255–287. (b) Bruner, J. S. (1981). The social context of language acquisition. Language Commun. 1, 155–178. Bruner, J. S. and Koslowski, B. (1972). Visually preadapted constituents of manipulatory action. Perception 1, 3–14. Buchwald, K. (1978). Umwelt, Mensch, Gesellschaft, Die Entstehung der Umweltproblematik. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt, 1. Die Umwelt des Menschen. München: BLV, pp. 1–46. Buchwald, K. (1980). Umwelt und Gesellschaft zwischen Wachstum und Gleichgewicht. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. 4. Umweltpolitik. München/Wien/Zürich: BLV, pp. 1–32. Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.) (1978). Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. 1. Die Umwelt des Menschen. München: BLV. Buckhalt, J. A. , Rutherford, R. B. , and Goldberg, K. E. (1978). Verbal and nonverbal interaction of mothers with their Down’s Syndrome and non-retarded infants. Amer. J. Mental Deficiency 82, 337–343. Budack, K. F. R. (1983). A harvesting people on the South Atlantic coast. S. Afr. J. Ethnol. 6, 1–7. Bullock, T. H. (1961). The origins of patterned nervous discharge. Behaviour 17, 48–59. Bullock, T. H. and Horridge, G. A. (1965). Structure and Function in the Nervous System of Invertebrates. 2 Vols. San Francisco: Freeman. Bunge, M. (1979). Some topical problems in biophilosophy. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 155–172. 729 Burckhardt, L. (1981). Das Menschenbild des Architekten. In: Michaelis, W. (ed.), Bericht über den 32. Kongress der Deutschen Gesellschaft für Psychologie in Zürich 1980, Bd. 1. Göttingen/Toronto/Zürich: C. J. Hogrefe, pp. 73–80. Burd, A. P. and Milewski, A. E. (1981). Matching of Facial Gestures by Young Infants: Imitation or Releasers? Vortrag anläβlich der Tagung der Society for Research in Child Development, Boston, April, 1981. Burgess, R. L. and Conger, R. D. (1978). Family Interaction in Abusive Neglectful and Normal Families. The Pennsylvania State University. Burghardt, G. M. (1975). Chemical prey preferences polymorphism in newborn garter snakes, Thamnophis sirtalis. Behaviour 52, 202-225. Busch, F. and McKnight, J. (1973). Parental attitudes and the development of the primary transitional object. Child Psychiat. Hum. Develop. 4, 12–20. Buss, A. H. (1961). The Psychology of Aggression. New York: Wiley. Bygott, J. D. (1972). Cannibalism among wild chimpanzees. Nature 238, 410–411. Calhoun, J. B. (1962). Population density and social pathology. Sci. Amer. 206 (2), 139–148. Calvinoux, P. (1980). The Fates of Nations. A Biological Theory of History. New York: Simon & Schuster. Campbell, D. T. (1974). Evolutionary epistemology. In: Schillp, P. (ed.), The Library of Living Philosophers, Vol. 14, 1 and 2: The Philosophy of Karl Popper, Vol. 1, pp. 413-463. Lasalle, Open Court. Campbell, D. T. (1975). On the conflicts between biological and social evolution and between psychology and moral tradition. Amer. Psychol. 30, 1103–1126. Campell, B. and Petersen, W. E. (1953). Milk let-down and orgasm in human female. Hum. Biol. 25, 165–168. Campos, J. J. Hiatt, S., Ramsay, D., Henderson, C., and Svejda, M. (1977). The emergence of fear on the visual cliff. In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The Origins of Affect. New York: Wiley. Camras, L. A. (1977). Facial expressions used by children in a conflict situation. Child Develop. 48, 1431–1435. Candland, D. K. and Mason, W. A. (1968). Infant monkey heartrate: Habituation and effects of social substitutes. Develop. Psychobiol. 1, 254–256. Caplan, A. L. (1979). Sociobiology, human nature and psychological egoism. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 27–38. Caporael, L. R. (1981). The paralanguage of caregiving: Baby talk to the institutionalized aged. J. Personality Social Psychol. 40, 876–884. Caputi, Jane E. (1984). Beauty secrets: Tabooing the ugly woman. In: R. B. Browne (ed.), Forbidden Fruits: Taboos and Tabooism in Culture. Bowling Green, Ohio: Bowling Green University Popular Press, pp. 36–56. Carlson, J. S. (1978). Kulturenvergleichende Untersuchungen im Rahmen von Piaget’s Theorie. In: Steiner, G. (ed.), Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts. 7. Piaget und die Folgen. Zürich: Kindler, pp. 709–728. Carneiro, R. L. (1970). A theory of the origin of the state. Science 169, 733–738. Carneiro, R. L. (1978). Political expansion as an expression of the principle of competitive exclusion. In: Cohen, R. and Service, E. (eds.), Origins of the State. The Anthropology of Political Evolution. Philadelphia, PA: Institute for the Study of Human Issues, pp. 205–223. Carson, R. (1962). Silent Spring. Boston: Houghton Mifflin. Cashdan, E. A. (1980). Egalitarianism among hunters and gatherers. Amer. Anthropol. 82, 116–120. Cashdan, E. A. (1983). Territoriality among human foragers: Ecological models and an application to four Bushman groups. Current Anthropol. 24, 47–55. Cassidy, J. H. (1979). Half a century on the concepts of innateness and instinct. Survey, synthesis and philosophical implications. Z. Tierpsychol. 50, 364–386. Castetter, E. F. and Beli, W. H. (1951). Yuman Indian Agriculture. Albuquerque, NM: University of New Mexico Press. Catom, H. (1981). Domesticating Nature: Thoughts on the Ethology of Modern Politics. In: White, E. (ed.), Sociobiology and Politics. Boston: (Heath), pp. 99–134. Catom, H. (1982). Biosocial Science: Knowledge for Enlightened Political Leadership. Paper presented at Annual Meeting of American Political Science Association, Denver, 11-14 Sept., 1982. 730 Cavalli-Sforza, L. L. (1981). Human evolution and nutrition. In: Kretschmer, N. and Walcher, D. N. (eds.), Food, Nutrition and Evolution. New York: Masson Publ. Inc. Chagnon, N. A. (1968). Yanomamö, The Fierce People. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Chagnon, N. A. (1971). Die soziale Organisation und die Kriege der Yanomamö-Indianer. In: Fried, M. , Harris, M. and Murphy, R. (eds.), Der Krieg. Zur Anthropologie der Aggression und des bewaffneten Konflikts. Stuttgart: Fischer, pp. 131–189. Chagnon, N. A. (1974). Studying the Yanomamö. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Chagnon, N. A. (1976). Yanomamö, the true people. National Geog. Magazine 150, 211–222. Chagnon, N. A. (1979). Mate competition, favoring close kin, and village fissioning among the Yanomamö Indians. In: Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, W. (eds.), Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press, pp. 86–132. Chagnon, N. A. (1988). Life histories, blood revenge, and warfare in a tribal population. Science 239, 985–992. Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, W. (1979). Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press. Chance, M. R. A. (1967). Attention structures as the basis of primate rank orders. Man 2 [NS], 503-518. Chance, M. R. A. and Larsen, R. R. (eds.) (1976). The Social Structure of Attention. London: Wiley. Charlesworth, W. (1978). Some models for the evolution of altruistic behavior between siblings. J. Theoret. Biol 72, 297–319. Charlesworth, W. (1981). Comments on S. L. Washburn’s review of Kenneth Bock’s “Human Nature History”: A response to sociobiology. Hum. Ethol. Newsl. 22-23 Sept., 1981. Charlesworth, W. R. and Dzur, C. (1987). Gender comparisons of preschoolers behavior and resource utilization in group problem solving. Child Develop. 58, 191–200. Chateau, J. (1969). Das Spiel des Kindes. Paderborn: Ferdinand Schöningh. Chesser, E. (1957). The Sexual, Marital, and Family Relationship of the English Woman. New York: Roy. Chevalier-Skolnikoff, S. (1973). Facial expression of emotion in nonhuman primates. In: Ekman, P. (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expression. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 11–89. Cheyne, J. A. and Efran, M. G. (1972). The effect of spatial and interpersonal variables on the invasion of group control territories. Sociometry 35, 477–489. Chisholm, G. B. (1946). The Re-establishment of Peacetime Society. The William Alanson White Memorial Lectures, 2nd series, Psychiatry IX, No. 3. Text citation from: Hayek, F. A. v. 1975. Die Irrtümer des Konstruktivismus. Tübingen: C. B. Mohr Paul Siebeck, p. 30. Chomsky, N. (1965). Aspects of the Theory of Syntax. Cambridge. MA: MIT Press. Chomsky, N. (1969). Language and the mind. Psychol. Today Magazine 13, 424–432. Chomsky, N. (1970). Sprache und Geist. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp. Cialdini, R. B. et al. (1975). Reciprocal concessions procedure for inducing compliance: The door-in-the-face technique. J. Personality Social Psychol. 31, 206–215. Cialdini, R. B. et al. (1984). Influence. How and Why People Agree to Things. New York: William Morrow. Claessen, H. J. M. and Skalnik, P. (eds.) (1978). The Early State. The Hague: Mouton. Clark, L. (1970). Is there a difference between a clitoral and a vaginal orgasm? J. Sex Res. 6, 25–28. Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1978). And daddy makes three: The father’s impact on mother and child. Child Develop. 49, 466–478. Clarke-Stewart, K. A. (1980). The father’s contribution to children’s cognitive and social development in early childhood. In: Pedersen, F. A. (ed.), The Father-Infant Relationship: Observational Studies in a Family Setting. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Clastres, P. (1974). Society Against the State. New York: Urizen Books. Claus, R. and Alsing, W. (1976). Occurrence of 5α-androst-16-en-3-one, a boar pheromone in man and its relationship to testosterone. J. Endocrinol. 68, 483–484. von Clausewitz, K. (1937, 15th ed.). Vom Kriege. Herausgegeben von K. Linnebach. Berlin. Clifton, R. , Siqueland, E. R. and Lipsitt, L. P. (1972). Conditioned headturning in human newborns as a function of conditioned response requirements and states of wakefulness. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 13, 43–57. 731 Cohen, P. S. (1980). Psychoanalysis and cultural symbolization. In: Foster, M. L. and Brandes, Sr. H. (eds.), Symbol as Sense. New York: Academic Press, pp. 45–70. Cohen, R. and Service, E. R. (eds.) (1978). Origins of the State. The Anthropology of Political Evolution. Philadelphia, PA: Inst. for the Study of Human Issues. Colby, A. , Kohlberg, L. , Gibbs, J. and Lieberman, M. (1983). A longitudinal study of moral judgment. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop. 48 [Ser. No. 200], 1-124. Colgan, P. W. (ed.) (1978). Quantitative Ethology. New York: Wiley. Colinvaux, P. A. (1980). The Feats of Nations. A Biological Theory of History. New York: Simon & Schuster. Colinvaux, P. A. (1982). Towards a theory of history: Fitness, niche and clutch of Homo sapiens. J. Ecol. 70, 393–412. Collis, G. M. and Schaffer, H. R. (1975). Synchronization of visual attention in motherinfant pairs. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 16, 315–320. Comer, R. J. and Piliavin, J. A. (1972). The effects of physical deviance upon face-to-face interaction. The other side. J. Personality Social Psychol. 23, 33–39. Comrie, B. (1983). Form and function in explaining language universals. Linguistics 21, 87–103. Condon, W. S. and Sander, L. W. (1974). Neonate movement is synchronized with adult speech: Interactional participation and language acquisition. Science 183, 99–101. Conkey, M. W. (1978). Style and information in cultural evolution: Toward a predictive model for the paleolithic. In: Redman, Ch. L. et al. (eds.), Social Archaeology Beyond Subsistence and Dating. New York: Academic Press, pp. 61–85. Cook, M. (ed.) (1981). The Bases of Human Sexual Attraction. London/New York: Academic Press, (a) Cook, M. (ed.) (1981). Social skill and human sexual attraction. In: Cook, M. (ed.), The Bases of Human Sexual Attraction. London/New York: Academic Press, pp. 145-177. (b) Corning, P. A. (1976). Toward a survival oriented policy science. In: Somit, A. (ed.), Biology and Politics. Recent Explorations, The Hague/Paris: Mouton, pp. 127–154. Corning, P. A. (1981). Rethinking categories and life. Behav. Brain Sci. 4, 286–288. (a) Corning, P. A. (1981). A Synopsis of a General Theory of Politics. Paper presented at the Annual Meeting of the American Political Science Association, Sept., New York, (b) Corning, P. A. (1983). Politik und Evolution: Kybernetik und Synergismus in der Entstehung komplexer Gesellschaften. In: Flohr, H. and Tonnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologic Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 38-60. (a) Corning, P. A. (1983). The Synergism Hypothesis. New York: McGraw-Hill, (b) Corsaro, W. A. (1979). We’re friends, right?: Childrens’ use of access rituals in nursery school. Language Soc. 8, 315–336. Coss, R. G. (1972). Eye-like Schemata: Their Effect on Behaviour. Thesis, University of Reading, PA. Coss, R. G. (1973). The cut-off hypothesis: Its relevance to the design of public places. Man-Environ. Systems 3, 417–440. Coss, R. G. (1978). Development of face aversion by the jewel fish (Hemichromis bimaculatus, Gill. 1862). Z. Tierpsychol. 48, 28–46. Cott, H. B. (1957). Adaptive Coloration in Animals. London: Methuen. Count, E. W. (1959). Eine biologische Entwicklungsgeschichte der menschlichen Sozialität. Homo , 10, 1–35. Count, E. W. (1970). Das Biogramm. Anthropologische Studien. Frankfurt/M.: S. Fischer. Craig, W. (1918). Appetites and aversions as constituents of . Biol. Bull. Woods. Hole 34, 91–107. Cranach, M. V. (1976). Methods of Inference from Animal to Human Behavior. The Hague/Paris: Mouton. Cranach, M. V. and Vine, I. (eds.) (1973). Social Communication and Movement. London: Academic Press. Cranach, M. V. , Kalbermatten, U. , Indermühle, K. , and Gugler, B. (1980). Zielgerichtetes Handeln. Bern/Stuttgart/Wien: Hans Hüber. Creutzfeldt, O. (1979). Representation der visuellen Umwelt im Gehirn (Mustererkennung und Schlüsselreize). Verhandl. Deut. Zool. Ges. 5-18. Cronin, C. L. (1980). Dominance relations and females. In: Omark, D. R. , Strayer, F. F. , and Freeman, D. G. (eds.), Dominance Relations. New York: Garland STPM Press, pp. 299–318. 732 Crusec, J. E. and Brinker, D. B. (1972). Reinforcement for imitation as a social learning determinant with implications for sex-role development. J. Personality Social Psychol. 21, 149–158. Cüceloglu, D. M. (1970). Perception of facial expressions in three cultures. Ergonomics 13, 93–100. Culbertson, G. H. and Caporeal, L. R. (1983). Baby talk speech to the elderly. Personality Soc. Psychol. Bull. 9, 305–312. Cullen, E. (1960). Experiments on the effects of social isolation on reproductive behaviour in the three-spined stickleback. Animal Behav. 8, 235. Cunningham, M. R. (1986). Measuring the physical in physical attractiveness: Quasi-ex-periments on the sociobiology of female facial beauty. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 50, 925–935. Curtiss, S. (1977). Genie: A Psycholinguistic Study of a Modern-Day “Wild Child.” New York: Academic Press. Cutler, W. B. , Preti, G. , Krieger, A. , Huggins, G. R. , Garcia, C. R. , and Lawley, H. J. (1986). Human axillary secretions influence women’s menstrual cycles: The role of donor extract from men. Hormones Behav. 20, 463–473. Cutting, J. E. and Rosner, B. (1974). Categories and boundaries in speech and music. Perception Psychophy. 16, 564–570. Daanje, A. (1950). On the locomotory movements in birds and the intention movements derived from it. Behaviour 3, 48–98. Dacy, M. , Wilson, M. , and Weghorst, S. J. (1982). Male sexual jealousy. Ethol. Sociobiol. 3, 11–27. Daldry, A. D. and Russell, P. A. (1982). Sex differences in the behavior of preschool children with novel and familiar toys. J. Genet. Psychol. 141, 3–6. Damas, D. (ed.) (1969). Contributions to Anthropology: Ecological Essays. Natural Museum of Canada. Bull. No. 230, Anthropological Series No. 86. Damon, F. H. (1982). Calendars and calendrical rites on the northern side of the Kula ring. Oceania 52, 221–239. Danielli, J. S. (1980). Altruism and the internal reward system. J. Soc. Biol. Structures 3, 87–94. Dann, H. D. (1972). Aggression und Leistung. Stuttgart: Klett. Dannhauer, H. (1973). Geschlecht und Personlichkeit. Berlin: Deutscher Verlag der Wissenschaften. Danzinger, L. and Frankl, L. (1934). Zum Problem der Funktionsreifung. Z. Kinderforsch. 43, 219–225. Dart, R. A. (1949). The bone-bludgeon hunting technique of Australopithecus. S. Afr. Sci. 2, 150–152. Dart, R. A. (1953). The predatory transition from ape to man. Intern. Anthropol. Linguistic Rev. 1, 201–218. Darwin, Ch . (1859). Origin of Species. New York: Philosophical Library. Darwin, Ch . (1872). The expression of emotion in man and animals. London: Murray. Dasen, P. R. (1977). Piagetian Psychology: Cross-cultural Contributions. New York: Gardener Press. Daucher, H. (1967). Künstlerisches und rationalisiertes Sehen. Gesetze des Wahrnehmens und Gestaltens. Schriften der pädagogischen Hochschulen Bayerns. München: Ehrenwirth Verlag. Daucher, H. (1979). Psychogenetic Aspects of aesthetics. Report INSIA-Congress in Adelaide (in press). Davis, A. and Olesen, V. (1971). Communal work and living: Notes on the dynamics of social distance and social space. Sociol. Social Res. 55, 191–202. Davis, F. C. (1981). Ontogeny of circadian rhythms. In: Aschoff, J. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, 4. London/New York: Plenum Publ. Corp., pp. 257–274. Dawkins, R. (1968). The ontogeny of a pecking preference in domestic chicks. Z. Tierpsychol. 25, 170–186. Dawkins, R. (1976). The Selfish Gene. London: Oxford University Press. Dawkins, R. (1978). Replicator selection and the extended phenotype. Z. Tierpsychol. 47, 61–76. Dawkins, R. (1979). Defining sociobiology. Nature 280, 47–428. (a) Dawkins, R. (1979). Twelve misunderstandings of kin selection. Z. Tierpsychol. 47, 184–200. (b) Day, R. H. (1972). Visual spatial illusions: A general explanation. Science 175, 1335–1340. 733 Deag, J. M. and Crook, J. H. (1971). Social behaviour and “agonistic buffering” in the wild barbary macaque Macaca sylvana L. Folia Primatol. 15, 183–200. DeCasper, A. H. and Fifer, W. P. (1980). Of human bonding: Newborns prefer their mother’s voice. Science 208, 1174–1176. DeChateau, P. and Andersson, Y. (1976). Left-side preference for holding and carrying newborn infants. II. Doll- holding and carrying from 2 to 16 years. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 18, 738–744. DeChateau, P. and Wiberg, B. (1977). Long-term effect on mother-infant behaviour of extra contact during the first hour postpartum. I. First observations at 36 hours. Acta Paediatr. Scand. 66, 137. (a) DeChateau, P. and Wiberg, B. (1977). Long-term effect on mother-infant behaviour of extra contact during the first hour postpartum. II. Follow-up at three months. Acta Paediatr. Scand. 66, 145 ff. (b) DeChateau, P. , Holmberg, H. , and Winberg, J. (1978). Left-side preference in holding and carrying. I. Mothers holding during the first week of life. Acta Paediatr. Scand. 67, 169–175. Degenhardt, A. and Trautner, H. M. (1979). Geschlechtstypisches Verhalten. Mann und Frau in psychologischer Sicht. München: C. H. Beck. Delcomyn, F. (1980). Neural Basis of Rhythmic Behavior. Science 210. 492-498. Delgado, J. M. R. (1979). Cerebral building blocks and behavioral mechanisms. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 31–32. DeLong, A. J. (1977). Yielding the floor: The kinesic signals. J. Commun. 27, 98–103. Demoll, R. (1954). Bändigt den Menschen. München: Bruckmann. Demott, B. (1980). Inzest. Der Angriff auf das letzte Tabu. Psychol. Heute 7, 14–18. Dennis, W. (1940). The effect of cradling practices upon the onset of walking in Hopi children. J. Genet. Psychol. 56, 77–86. Dennis, W. (1941). Infant development under conditions of restricted practice and of minimal social stimulation. Genet. Psychol. Monogr. 23, 143–189. Dennis, W. (1960). Causes of retardation among institutional children. Iran. J. Genet. Psychol. 96, 47–59. DePaulo, B. M. , Zuckerman, M. , and Rosenthal, R. (1980). Modality effects in the detection of deception. In: Wheeler, J. (ed.), The Review of Personality and Social Psychology. New York: Sage Publ. Deutsch, R. D. (1977). Spatial Structurings in Everyday Face-to-Face Behavior. Orangeburg, NY: The Assoc. for the Study of Man-Environment Relations, Inc. Deutsch, R. D. (1979). On the isomorphic structure of endings: An example from everyday face-to-face interaction and Balinese Legong dance. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 41–57. Devereux, G. (1983). Baubo—die personifizierte Vulva. Curare, Sonderband 1/83, Braunschweig: Vieweg, pp. 117–120. DeVore, I. (1971). The evolution of human society. In: Eisenberg, J. F. and Dillon, W. S. (eds.), Man and Beast: Comparative Social Behavior. Smithsonian Annual III. Washington, D. C: Smithsonian Inst., pp. 297–311. DeWaal, F. B. M. (1978). Exploitative and familiarity dependent support strategies in chimpanzees. Behaviour 17, 268–312. Dickey, E. C. and Knower, F. H. (1941). A note on some ethnological differences in recognition of simulated expressions of the emotions. Amer. J. Sociol. 47, 190–193. DiFranco, D. , Muir, D. W. , and Dodwell, P. C. (1978). Reaching in very young infants. Perception 7, 385–392. Dimberg, U. (1982). Facial reactions to facial expressions. Psychophysiology 19, 643–647. Dimberg, U. and Öhman, A. (1983). The effects of directional facial cues on electrodermal conditioning to facial stimuli. Psychophysiology 20, 160–167. Dingwall, W. O. (1979). The evolution of human communication systems. In: Whitaker, H. and Whitaker, H. A. (eds.), Studies in Neurolinguistics, Vol. 4. New York: Academic Press, pp. 1–95. Divale, W. T. (1971). An explanation for primitive warfare: Population control and the significance of primitive sex ratios. New Scholar 2, 173–192. Divale, W. T. (1972). System population control in the middle and upper Paleolithic: Inferences based on contemporary hunter-gatherers. World Archaeol. 4, 222–243. Dodd, J. and Jessell, T. M. (1988). Axon guidance and the patterning of neuronal projections in vertebrates. Science 242, 696–699. Dörner, D. and Reither, F. (1978). Über das Problemlösen in sehr komplexen Realitätsbereichen. Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 25, 527–551. 734 Dörner, D. and Vehrs, W. (1975). Ästhetische Befriedigung und Unbestimmtheitsreduktion. Psychol. Rev. 37, 321–334. Dörner, G. (1980). Sexual differentiation of the brain. Vitamins Hormones 38, 325–381. Dörner, G. (1981). Sex hormones and neurotransmitters as mediators for sexual differentiation of the brain. Endokrinology 78, 129–138. Dörner, G. , Rohde, W. , Stahl, F. , Krell, L. , and Masius, W. G. (1975). A neuroendocrine predisposition for homosexuality in men. Sex. Behav. 4, 1–8. Dolgin, K. G. and Sabini, J. (1982). Experimental manipulation of a human non-verbal display: The tongue show effect and observers’ willingness to interact. Anim. Behav. 30, 935–936. Dollard, J. and Miller, N. E. (1950). Personality and Psychotherapy. New York: McGraw-Hill. Dollard, J. , Doob, L. W. , Miller, N. E. , Mowrer, O. H. , and Sears, R. R. (1939). Frustration and Aggression. New Haven: Yale University Press. Doty, R. L. (1976). Reproductive endocrine influences upon human nasal chemoreception: A review. In: Doty, R. L. (ed.), Mammalian Olfaction, Reproductive Processes and Behavior. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 295–321. Draeger, D. F. and Smith, R. W. (1980). Comprehensive Asian Fighting Arts. Tokyo/New York/San Francisco: Kondansha Intern. Ltd. Draper, P. (1976). Social and economic constraints on child life among the !Kung. In: Lee, R. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Draper, P. (1978). The learning environment for aggression and anti-social behavior among the !Kung. In: Montagu, A. (ed.), Learning Non-Aggression. New York/Oxford: Oxford University Press, pp. 31–53. Duchen, M. R. and McNeilly, A. S. (1980). Hyperprolactinaemia and long-term lactational amenorrhoea. Clin. Endocrinol. 12, 621–627. Duchenne, B. (1862). Mécanisme de la Physionomie Humaine ou Analyse Electrophysiologique de l’Expression des Passions. Paris: Baillière. Dum, J. , Gramsch, Ch. , and Herz, A. (1983). Activation of hypothalamic β-endorphin pools by reward induced by highly palatable food. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behav. 18, 443–447. Duncan, S. D. (1974). On the structure of speaker-auditor interaction during speaking turns. Language Soc. 2, 161–180. Duncan, S. D. and Fiske, D. W. (1977). Face-to-Face Interaction. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Earlbaum. Dundes, A. , Leach, J. W. , and Özkök, B. (1970). The strategy of turkish boys’ verbal duelling rhymes. J. Amer. Folklore 83, 325–349. Dunkeld, J. (1978). The Function of Imitation in Infancy. Dissertation, Univ. of Edinburgh. Dunkeld, J. and Bower, T. G. R. (1976). Infant Response to Impending Optical Collision. Cited after: Bower, T. G. R. A Primer of Infant Development. San Francisco: Freeman. Dunn, K. and Dunn, R. (1977). How to Raise Independent and Professionally Successful Daughters. Englewood Cliffs, N J: Prentice Hall. Durden-Smith, J. and Desimone, D. (1983). Sex and the Brain. New York: Arbor House. Durham, W. H. (1976). Resource competition and human aggression, a review of primitive war. Quart. Rev. Biol. 51, 385–415. Durham, W. H. (1979). Toward a coevolutionary theory of human biology and culture. In: Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, W. (eds.), Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior: An Anthropological Perspective. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press, pp. 39–59. Durkheim, E. (1964). The Division of Labor in Society. New York: Free Press. Dusak Sexton, L. (1982). Wok Meri: A woman’s savings and exchange system in Highland Papua New Guinea. Oceania 52, 167–198. Dworking, E. S. and Efran, J. S. (1967). The angered: Their susceptibility to varieties of humor. J. Personality Soc. Psychol. 6, 233–236. Dyson-Hudson, N. (1966). Karimojong Politics. London: Oxford University Press. Dyson-Hudson, N. and Dyson-Hudson, R. (1969). Subsistence herding in Uganda. Sci. Amer. 220, 76–89. Dyson-Hudson, N. and Dyson-Hudson, R. (1970). The food production system of a semi-nomadic society. The Karimojong, Uganda. In: McLoughlin, P. F. M. (ed.), African Food Production Systems: Cases and Theory. Baltimore, MD: John Hopkins Press, pp. 91–124. 735 Dyson-Hudson, R. and Smith, E. A. (1978). Human Territoriality: An ecological reassessment. Amer. Anthropol. 80, 21–41. Dyson-Hudson, R. and Van Dusen, R. (1972). Foodsharing among young children. Ecol. Food Nutr. 1, 319–324. Eckermann, C. O. and Whatley, J. L. (1977). Toys and social interactions between infant peers. Child Develop. 48, 1645–1656. Eder, D. and Hallinan, M. T. (1978). Sex differences in children’s friendships. Amer. Soc. Rev. 43, 237–249. Ederer, R. (1982). Die Grenzen der Kunst. Wien/Graz/Köln: Hermann Böhlaus Nachf. Edney, J. J. (1972). Property, possession and permanence: A field study in human territoriality. J. Appl. Social Psychol. 2, 275–282. Edney, J. J. and Jordan-Edney, N. L. (1974). Territorial spacing on a beach. Sociometry , 37, 92 ff. Efron, A. (1985). The sexual body: An interdisciplinary perspective. J. Mind Behav. 6, [Special Issues 1, 2]. Efron, D. (1941). Gesture and Environment. New York: King’s Crown Press. Eggebrecht, R. (1983). Sprachmelodische und musikalische Forschungen im Kulturvergleich. Dissertation, Universität München. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1950). Über die Jugendentwicklung des Verhaltens eines männlichen Dachses (Meles meles L.) unter besonderer Berücksichtigung des Spieles. Z. Tierpsychol. 7, 327–355. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1950). Beiträge zur Biologie der Haus-und der Ährenmaus nebst einigen Beobachtungen an anderen Nagern. Z. Tierpsychol. 7, 558–587. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1951). Zur Fortpflanzungsbiologie und Jugendentwicklung des Eichhörnchens. Z. Tierpsychol. 8, 370–400. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1955). Ethologische Studien am Galápagos-Seelöwen Zalophus wollebaeki Sivertsen. Z. Tierpsychol. 12, 286–303. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1955). Über Symbiosen, Parasitismus und andere zwischenartliche Beziehungen bei tropischen Meeresfischen. Z. Tierpsychol. 12, 203–219. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1955). Der Kommentkampf der Meerechse (Amhlyrhynchus cristatus Bell) nebst einigen Notizen zur Biologie dieser Art. Z. Tierpsychol. 12, 49–62. (c) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1958). Das Verhalten der Nagetiere. In: Kükenthal, W. (ed.), Handbuch der Zoologie, Vol. 8 (10), pp. 1–88. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1959). Der Fisch Aspidontus taeniatus als Nachahmer des Putzers Lahroides dimidiatus. Z. Tierpsychol. 16, 19–25. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1962). Freiwasserbeobachtungen zur Deutung des Schwarmverhaltens verschiedener Fische. Z. Tierpsychol. 19, 165–182. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1963). Angeborenes und Erworbenes im Verhalten einiger Säuger. Z. Tierpsychol. 20, 705–754. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1967). Concepts of ethology and their significance for the study of human behavior. In: Stevenson, H. W. (ed.), Early Behavior, Comparative and Developmental Approaches. New York: Wiley, pp. 127–146. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Liebe und Haβ. Zur Naturgeschichte elementarer Verhaltensweisen. München: Piper; Serie Piper 113, 1976. (a) [English edition: (1971). Love and Hate. The Natural History of Behavior Patterns. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston.] Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1970). Mônnliche und weibliche Schutzamulette im modernen Japan. Homo 21, 175–188. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1971). Eine ethologische Interpretation des Palmfruchtfestes der Waika (Venezuela) nebst einigen Bemerkungen über die bindende Funktion von Zwiegesprächen. Anthropos 66, 767–778. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1971). Filmbeiheft: !Ko-Buschleute (Kalahari)—Schamweisen und Spotten. Homo 22, 261–266. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1972). Die !Ko-Buschmanngesellschaft: Gruppenbindung und Aggressionskontrolle. Monographien zur Humanethologie. München: Piper. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). Der vorprogrammierte Mensch. Das Ererbte als bestimmender Faktor im rnenschlichen Verhalten. Wien (Molden), Miinchen: dtv 4177, 1976. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1973). The expressive behavior of the deaf-and-blind born. In: Cranach, M. V. and Vine, I. (eds.), Social Communication and Movement. London: Academic Press, pp. 163–194. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1974). Medlpa (Mbowamb)—Neuguinea—Werberitual (Amb Kanant). Homo 25, 274–284. 736 Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975). Krieg und Frieden aus der Sicht der Verhaltensforschung. München: Piper. [English edition: 1979. The Biology of War and Peace. London: Thames and Hudson; New York: Viking Press.] (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1975 rev. ed.). Ethology, The Biology of behavior. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1976). Menschenforschung auf neuen Wegen. Wien: Molden. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1977, 3rd ed., 1984, 7th ed.). Galápagos. Die Arche Noah im Pazifik. München: Piper. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1977). Ambivalenz von Zuwendung und Abkehr im Begegnungsverhalten des Menschen. Partner-Beratung 14, 113–118. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1978). Der Mensch und seine Umwelt: Ethologische Perspektiven. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. München/Bern/Wien: BLV, pp. 102–115. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1978). Territorialität und Aggressivität der Jäger-und Sammlervölker. In: Stamm, R. A. and Zeier, H. (eds.), Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts. Band 6: Lorenz und die Folgen. Zürich: Kindler, pp. 477–494. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1978). Public places in society: Ethological perspectives. In: The Public Square, a Space for Culture. Cultures, Vol. V. The Unesco Press and la Baconnière, pp. 105-113. (c) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979). Functions of ritual. Ritual and ritualization from a biological perspective. In: Cranach, M. V. , Foppa, K. , Lepenies, W. , and Ploog, D. (eds.), Human Ethology: Claims and Limits of a New Discipline. London: Maison des Sciences de l’Homme and Cambridge University Press, pp. 3-93. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979). Human ethology: Concepts and implications for the science of man. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 1–57. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979). !Ko-Buschleute (Kalahari)—Grashüpferspiel der Männer. Homo 30, 49–54. (c) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1980). G/wi-Buschleute (Kalahari)—Krankenheilung und Trance. Homo 31, 67–78. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1981). Medlpa (Mbowamb)-Neuguinea-Ritual der Totentrauer. Homo 32, 59–70. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1981). Gesellschaftsordnung und menschliches Verhalten aus dem Blickwinkel der Evolution. In: Kaltenbrunner, K.G. (ed.), Wir sind Evolution. München: Herder Bücherei, pp. 78–93. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1982). Patterns of parent-child interaction in a cross-cultural perspective. In: Oliverio, A. and Zappella, M. (eds.), The Behaviour of Human Infants. London: Plenum Press, pp. 177–217. (a) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1982). Warfare, Man’s indoctrinability and group selection. Z. Tierpsychol. 60, 177–198. (b) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1982). Interactionism, content, and language in human ethology studies. Behav. Brain Sci. 5 [2], 273-274. (c) Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1984). Ursprung und soziale Funktion des Objektbesitzes. In: Eggers, Ch. (ed.): Bindungen und Besitzdenken beim Kleinkind. München/Wien/Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 29–50. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1987, 7th ed.). Grundriβ der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung. 7. überarb. und erweiterte Aufl., München: Piper. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. and Hass, H. (1966). Zum Projekt einer ethologisch orientierten Untersuchung menschlichen Verhaltens. Mitteil. Max-Planck-Ges. 6, 383–396. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1967). Neue Wege der Humanethologie. Homo 18, 13–23. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. and Kacher, H. (1982). Bali (Indonesien)—Legong-Tanz. Homo , 33, 46–56. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. and Sielmann, H. (1962). Beobachtungen am Spechtflnken Cactospiza pallida. J. Ornithol. 103, 92–101. Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. and Wickler, W. (1968). Die ethologische Deutung einiger Wächterfiguren auf Bali. Z. Tierpsychol. 25, 719–726. Eichler, M. (1981). The inadequacy of the monolithic model of the family. Canad. Sociol. 6, 367–388. Eickstedt, E. ν (1944, 1963). Die Forschung am Menschen. Stuttgart: F. Enke. Eimas, P. D. , Cooper, W. E. , and Corbit, J. D. (1973). Some properties of linguistic detectors. Perception Psychophys. 13, 247–252. Eimas, P. D. , Siqueland, E. R. , Jusczyk, P. , and Vigorito, J. (1971). Speech perception in infants. Science 171, 303–306. Eisenberg, J. F. (1981). The Mammalian Radiation. Chicago/London: Univ. Chicago Press. 737 Eisenberg, L. (1971). Persistent problems in the study of the biopsychology of development. In: Tobach, E. , Aronson, L. , and Shaw, E. (eds.), The Biopsychology of Development. New York: Academic Press, pp. 515–529. Ekman, P. (ed.) (1973). Darwin and Facial Expression. New York/London: Academic Press. (a) Ekman, P. (ed.) (1973). Cross-cultural studies of facial expression. In: Ekman, P. (ed.), Darwin and Facial Expression. New York/London: Academic Press, 169-222. (b) Ekman, P. (ed.) (1979). About brows: Emotional and conversational signals. In: Cranach, M. V. , Foppa, K. , Lepenies, W. , and Ploog, D. (eds.), Human Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press), pp. 169–202. Ekman, P. (ed.) (1981). Mistakes when deceiving. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 364, 269–278. Ekman, P. (ed.) (1985). Telling Lies. New York: Berkley Books. Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1975). Unmasking the Face. Englewood Cliffs, N.J.: Prentice Hall. Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1976). Measuring facial movements. Environ. Psychol. Nonverbal Behav. 1, 56–75. Ekman, P. and Friesen, W. V. (1978). Facial Action Coding System, Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press Inc. Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. V. , and Ellsworth, P. (1971). Emotions in the Human Face. New York: Pergamon Press, (a) Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. V. , and Tomkins, S. S. (1971). Facial affect scoring technique: A first validity study. Semiotica 3, 37–58. (b) Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. V. , and Scherer, K. R. (1976). Body movement and voice pitch in deceptive interaction. Semiotica 16, 23–27. Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. V. and Ancoli, S. (1980). Facial signs of emotional experience. J. Personality Social Psychol. 39, 1125–1134. Ekman, P. , Hager, J. C. , and Friesen, W. V. (1981). The symmetry of emotional and deliberate facial actions. Psychophysiology 18, 101–106. Ekman, P. , Levenson, R. W. , and Friesen, W. V. (1983). Autonomic nervous system activity distinguishes among emotions. Science 221, 1208–1210. Ekman, P. , Friesen, W. V. , and O’Sullivan, M. (1988). Smiles when lying. J. Personality Social Psychol. 54, 414–420. Elgar, M. A. and Catterall, C. P. (1981). Flocking and predator surveillance in house sparrows: Test of an hypothesis. Anim. Behav. 29, 868–872. Ellis, H. (1906). Sexual Selection in Man. Philadelphia, PA: F. A. Davis. Ellis, L. (1985). On the rudiments of possessions and property. Social Sci. Inform. 24, 113–143. Ellis, L. (1986). Evidence of neuroandrogenic etiology of sex roles from a combined analysis of human, nonhuman primate and non-primate mammalian studies. Person. Individ. Diff 7, 519–552. Ellis, L. and Ames, M. A. (1987). Neurohormonal functioning and sexual orientation: A theory of homosexuality- heterosexuality. Psychol. Bull. 101, 233–258. Eisner, N. and Hüber, F. (1973). Neurale Grundlagen artspezifischer Kommunikation bei Orthopteren. Fortsch. Zool. 22, 1–48. Elterman, H. (1983). Using popular songs to teach sociology. Teaching Sociol. 10, 519–538. Elwin, V. (1968). The kingdom of the young. London: Oxford University Press. Ember, C. R. (1978). Myths about hunter-gatherers. Ethnology 17, 439–448. Ember, M. and Ember, C. R. (1979). Male-female bonding: A cross-species study of mammals and birds. Behav. Sci. Res. 14, 37–56. Endicott, K. and Lampell-Endicott, K. (1983). The Sociology of Land Use among the Batek of Malaysia. Paper presented at the 3rd Intern. Conf. on Hunters and Gatherers, Werner-Reimers-Stiftung, Bad Homburg, 13-16 June. 1983. Engelhardt, W. (1978). Bevölkerungsentwicklung. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. 1. Die Umwelt des Menschen. München: BLV, pp. 46–55. Engelhardt, W. (1979). Verstädterung. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt 1. München: BLV, pp. 55–60. Engels, Fr . (1884). Der Ursprung der Familie, des Privateigentums und des Staates. Neuabdruck. In: Marx, K. and Engels, Fr. (eds.), Ausgewählte Schriften, Bd. 2. Berlin: Dietz Verlag, pp. 155–301. 738 Engen, T. (1982). The Perception of Odors. New York: Academic Press. Epstein, D. (1979). Beyond Orpheus. Studies in Musical Structure. Cambridge: MIT Press. Epstein, D. (1985). Tempo relations: A cross-cultural study. Music Theory Spectrum (J. Soc. Music Theory) 7, 34–71. Epstein, D. (1988). Tempo relations in music: A universal? In: Rentschler, I. Herzberger, B. , and Epstein, D. (eds.), Beauty and the Brain. Basel, Boston, Berlin: Birkhäuser, pp. 91–116. Erikson, E. H. (1950). Childhood and Society. New York: N. W. Norton. Erikson, E. H. (1966). Ontogeny of ritualization in man. Phil. Trans. Roy. Soc. London B251, 337–349. Ertel, S. (1975). Überzeugung, Dogmatismus, Wahn. IX. Int. Kolloquium der Société Int. de Psychopathologie de l’expression. Hannover. Ertel, S. (1981). Wahrnehmung und Gesellschaft. Pränztendenzen in Wahrnehmung und Bewuβmiotik 3, 107-141. Escoffier, J. , Malyon, A. , Morin, Sr. , and Raphael, S. (1980). Homophobia: Effects on scientists. Science 209, 340. Esser, A. H. (1970). Interactional Hierarchy and Power Structure on a Psychiatric Ward. In: Hutt, S.J. and Hutt, C. (eds.), Behavior Studies in Psychiatry. Oxford/New York: Pergamon, pp. 25–59. Essock-Vitale, S. M. and McGuire, M. T. (1980). Predictions derived from the theory of kin selection and reciprocation assessed by anthropological data. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 233–243. Ewer, R. F. (1968). Ethology of Mammals. London: Logos Press. Ewert, J. P. (1974). Neurobiologie und System-Theorie eines visuellen Mustererkennungsmechanismus bei Kröten. Kybernetik 14, 167–183. (a) Ewert, J. P. (1974). The neural basis of visually guided behavior. Sci. Amer. 230, 34–42. (b) Ewert, O. M. (1983). Eine historische Nachbemerkung zu Neuberger, Merz und Selg:Imitation bei Neugeborenen—eine kontroverse Befundlage (Kurzartikel). Z. f. Entwicklungspsychologie und Pädagog. Psychologie 15, 277–279. Eysenck, H. J. (1976). Sex and Personality. London: Open Books. Fagen, R. (1981). Animal Play Behaviour. Oxford: Oxford University Press. Fagot, B. I. (1978). The influence of sex of child on parental reactions to toddler children. Child Develop. 49, 459–465. Fantz, R. L. (1966). Pattern discrimination and selective attention as determinants of perceptual development from birth. In: Kidd, A. H. and Rivoire, J. L. (eds.), Perceptual Development in Children. New York: Int. Universities Press. Faublée, J. (1968). Note Sur l’Economie Ostentatoire. Rev. Tiers-Monde 9, 17–23. Fauss, R. (1988). Zur Bedeutung des Gesichts für die Partnerwahl. Homo 37, 188–201. Fehling, D. (1974). Ethologische Überlegungen auf dem Gebiet der Altertumskunde. Monographien zur klassischen Altertumskunde, 61. München: C. H. Beck. Feinman, S. (1980). Infant response to race, size, proximity, and movement of strangers. Infant Behav. Develop. 3, 187–204. Feldman, J. , Brody, N. and Miller, St. (1980). Sex differences in non-elicited neonatal behaviors. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 26, 63–73. Felipe, N. and Sommer, R. (1966). Invasions of personal space. Social Problems 14, 206–214. Fentress, J. C. (ed.) (1976). Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer. Ferguson, C. A. (1964). Baby talk in six languages. In: Gumperz, J. L. and Hymes, D. H. (eds.), Directions in Sociolinguistics. The Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 103–114. (1972). Ferguson, R. B. (1984). Warfare, Culture and Environment. Studies in Anthropology. New York/London: Academic Press. Ferguson, R. F. (1918). The Zulus and the Spartans: A comparison of their military systems. Harvard Afr. Stud. 2, 198–227. Fernald, A. (1985). Four-month-old infants prefer to listen to motherese. Infant Behav. Develop. 8, 181–195. Fernald, A. and Simon, Th. (1984). Expanded intonation contours in mothers’ speech to newborns. Develop. Psychol. 20, 104–113. Ferrari, M. (1981). An observation of the infant’s response to strangers: A test for ecological validity. J. Genet. Psychol. 139, 157–158. 739 Feshbach, S. (1961). The stimulation vs. cathartic effects of a vicarious aggressive activity. Abnormal Social Psychol. 63, 381–385. Feshbach, S. (1964). The function of aggression and the regulation of aggressive drive. Psychol. Rev. 71, 257–272. Feshbach, S. and Singer, R. (1971). Television and Aggression. San Francisco: Jossey-Bass. Field, T. M. , Woodson, R. , Greenberg, R. , and Cohen, D. (1982). Discrimination and imitation of facial expressions by neonates. Science 218, 179–181. Field, T. M. , Cohen, D. , Garcia, R. , and Greenberg, R. (1984). Mother-stranger face discrimination by the newborn. Infant Behav. Develop. 7, 19–25. Finkelhor, D. (1980). Sex among siblings: A survey of prevalence, variety, and effects Arch. Sex. Behav. 9, 171–194. Firestone, S. (1970). The Dialect of Sex. The Case for Feminist Revolution. Toronto: Bantam Books. Firth, R. (1975). Symbols, Public and Private. London: Allen & Unwin. Fisek, M. A. and Ofshe, R. (1970). The process of status evolution. Sociometry 33, 327–346. Fisher, A. E. (1955). Thesis, Pennsylvania. Cited in: In: Rajecki, D. W. , Lamb, M. E. , and Obmascher, P. (1978). Fisher, E. (1979). Woman’s Creation: Sexual Evolution and the Shaping of Society. New York: McGraw-Hill. Fisher, S. (1973). The Female Orgasm: Psychology, Physiology, Fantasy. New York: Basic Books. Flitner, A. (1975). Spielen-Lernen, München: Piper. Flitner, A. (1976). Das Kinderspiel. München: Piper. Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (1983). Selbstverständnis und Grundlagen von Biopolitics. In: Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 11–30. (a) Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (ed.) (1983). Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey. (b) Ford, C. S. and Beach, F. A. (1969). Formen der Sexualität. Hamburg: Rororo-Sexologie. Ford, M. R. and Lowery, C. R. (1986). Gender differences in moral reasoning: A comparison of the use of justice and care orientations. J. Personality Social Psychol. 50, 777–783. Fortune, R. F. (1939). Arapesh warfare. Amer. Anthropol. 41, 22–41. Fossey, D. (1977). The Behavior of the Free-Ranging Mountain Gorillas of the Virungas. Film shown at the 15th meeting of the Society of Anthropology and Human Genetic, Hamburg. Fossey, D. (1979). Development of the mountain gorilla (Gorilla gorilla beringei). The first thirty-six months. In: Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R. (eds.), The Great Apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin/Cummings, pp. 138–184. Fouts, R. S. (1975). Communication with chimpanzees. In: Kurth, G. and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (eds.), Hominisation und Verhalten. Stuttgart: Fischer, pp. 137–158. Fox, C. A. Ismail, A. A. A. , Love, D. N. , Kirkham, K. E. , and Loraine, J. A. (1972). Studies on the relationship between plasma testosterone levels and human sexual activity. J. Endocrinol. 52, 51–58. Fox, N. (1977). Attachment of kibbutz infants to mother and metapelet. Child Develop. 48, 1228–1239. Fraiberg, S. (1975). The development of human attachments in infants blind from birth. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 21, 315–334. Frank, H. (1960). Über grundlegende Sätze der Informationsästhetik. Grundlagenstudien aus Kybernetik. Geisteswissenschaft 1, 25–32. Frank, J. D. (1967). Muβ Krieg sein? Psychologische Aspekte von Krieg und Frieden. Darmstadt: Verlag Darmstädter Blätter, Schwarz & Co. Frasher, R. , Brogan, D. R. , and Nurss, J. R. (1980). Effect of model’s age and sex upon modification of children’s sex-typed toy preferences. Intern J. Women’s Stud. 3, 161–172. Freedman, D. G. (1964). Smiling in blind infants and the issue of innate vs. acquired. J. Child Psychol. Psychiat. 5, 171–184. Freedman, D. G. (1965). Hereditary control of early social behavior. In: Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behavior. London: Methuen. Freedman, D. G. (1979). Human Sociobiology. A Holistic Approach. New York: Free Press, Macmillan. 740 Freedman, J. L. and Fraser, S. C. (1966). Compliance without pressure: The foot-in-the-door technique. J. Personality Social Psychol. 4, 195–203. Freeman, C. and Jahoda, M. (1973). Zukunft aus dem Computer? Neuwied: Luchterhand. Freeman, D. (1966). Social anthropology and the scientific study of human behaviour. Man 2, 330–342. Freeman, D. (1983). Margaret Mead and Samoa. The Making and Unmaking of an Anthropological Myth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Freeman, D. (1984). Inductivism and the Test of Truth: A Rejoinder to Lowell D. Holmes and others. In: Fact and Context in Ethnography; the Samoa Controversy. Canberra Anthropology Special Volume, The Australian National University, pp. 101–192. Freisitzer, K. and Glück, H. (1979). Sozialer Wohnbau, Entstehung—Zustand—Alternatives Wien: Molden. Freud, A. (1946). The psychoanalytic study of infantile feeding disturbance. Psychoanal. Study Child! , 119-132. Freud, S. (1913). Totem und Tabu. Leipzig/Wien (Heller).—Neudruck: S. Freud Studienausgabe, Bd. IX, Frankfürt/M: S. Fischer, pp. 291–444. Freud, S. (1918). Totem and Taboo. A. A. Brill transl. New York: Moffat, Yard. Frey, S. and Pool, J. (1976). A New Approach to the Analysis of Visible Behavior. Forschungsberichte aus dem Psychologischen Institut der Universität Bern. Fried, M. H. (1961). Warfare, military organization, and the evolution of society. Anthropologica 3, 134–147. Fried, M. , Harris, M. , and Murphy, R. (eds.) (1968). War: The Anthropology of Armed Conflict and Aggression. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press. Frisch, K. V. (1941). Über einen Schreckstoff der Fischhaut und seine biologische Bedeutung. Z. Vergl. Physiol. 29, 46–145. Frobenius, L. (1903). Weltgeschichte des Krieges. Jena: Thüringer Verlagsanstalt. Frodi, A. M. and Lamb, M. E. (1978). Sex differences in responsiveness to infants: A developmental study of psychological and behavioral responses. Child Develop. 49, 1182–1188. Frodi, A. M. and Lamb, M. E. (1980). Infants at risk for child abuse. Infant Mental Health J. 1, 240–247. (a) Frodi, A. M. and Lamb, M. E. (1980). Child abusers’ responses to infant smiles and cries. Child Develop. 51, 238–241. (b) Frodi, A. M. , Lamb, M. E. Leavitt, L. A. , Donovan, W. L. , Neff, C. , and Sherry, D. (1978). Fathers’ and mothers’ responses to the faces and cries of normal and premature infants. Develop. Psychol. 14, 490–499. Fromm, E. (1974). Anatomie der menschlichen Destruktivität. Stuttgart: DVA. (a) Fromm, E. (1974). Lieber fliehen als kämpfen. Bild Wissenschaft 10, 52–58. (b) Fromm, E. (1976). Haben und Sein. Stuttgart: DVA. Fry, H. K. (1934). Kinship and descent among australian aborigines. Trans. Roy. Soc. S. Australia 58, 14–21. Fthenakis, W. E. (1983). Der Vater als sorge-und umgangsberechtigter Elternteil. In: Remschmidt, H. (ed.), Kinderpsychiatrie und Familienrecht. Stuttgart: Ferdinand Enke. Fukui, K. and Turton, D. (1979). Introduction to warfare among East African herders. Nat. Mus. Ethnol. (Osaka, Japan) 3, 1–13. Fullard, W. and Rieling, A. M. (1976). An investigation of Lorenz’ baby babyness. Child Develop. 47, 1191–1193. Furby, L. (1978). Possessions: Toward a theory of their meaning and function throughout the life cycle. Life Span Develop. Behav. 1, 297–336. Gabor, D. , Colombu, U. , and Galli, R. (1976). Das Ende der Verschwendung. Stuttgart: Deutsche Verlagsanstalt. Gallistel, C. R. (1980). The Organisation of Action. A New Synthesis. Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. Garcia, J. and Ervin, F. R. (1968). Gustatory-visceral and telereceptor-cutaneous conditioning—Adaptation in internal and external milieus. Commun. Behav. Biol. , 1, 389–415. Garcia, J. , McGowan, B. K. , Ervin, F. R. , and Koelling, R. A. (1968). Cues: Their relative effectiveness as a function of the reinforcer. Science 160, 794–795. Gardner, B. T. and Gardner, R. A. (1975). Evidence for sentence constituents in the early utterances of child and chimpanzee. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen. 104, 244–267. Fromm, E. (1980). Two comparative psychologists look at language acquisition. In: Nelson, K. E. (ed.), Children’s Language, Vol. 2. New York: Halsted Press. 741 Gardner, B. T. and Wallach, L. (1965). Shapes of figures identified as a baby’s head. Perceptual Motor Skill 20, 135–142. Gardner, R. and Heider, K. G. (1968). Gardens of War: Life and Death in the New Guinea Stone Age. London: Deutsch. Gardner, R. A. and Gardner, B. T. (1984). A vocabulary test for chimpanzees (Pan troglodytes). J. Comp. Psychol. 98, 381–404. Gareis, B. (1978). Statistische Zusammenhänge von frühkindlicher Deprivation und späterer Jugendkriminalität. In: Nitsch, K. (ed.), Was wird aus unseren Kindern. Heidelberg: Hüthig Verlag, pp. 76–80. Gaulin, St. J. C. and Schlegei, A. (1980). Paternal confidence and paternal investment: A cross-cultural test of a sociobiological hypothesis: Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 301–309. Gazzaniga, M. S. , Bogen, J. R. , and Sperry, R. W. (1963). Laterality effects in somaesthesis following cerebral commissurotomy in man. Neuropsychologia 1, 209–221. Gazzaniga, M. S. , Bogen, J. R. , and Sperry, R. W. (1965). Observations in visual perception of the cerebral hemisphere in man. Brain 88, 221–236. Gazzaniga, M. S. , LeDoux, J. E. , and Wilson, D. H. (1977). Language, praxis and the right hemisphere: Clues to some mechanisms of consciousness. Neurology 24, 1144–1147. Geber, M. (1958). The psychomotor development of African children in the first year, and the influence of maternal behavior. J. Soc. Psychol. 47, 185–195. Geber, M. (1961). Développement psychomoteur des petits Baganda de la naissance à six ans. Schweiz. Psychol. Anwendungen 20, 345–357. Geber, M. and Dean, R. F. A. (1957). The state of development of newborn African children. Lancet 272, 1216–1219. (a) Geber, M. and Dean, R. F. A. (1957). Gesell tests on african children. Pediatrics 20, 1055–1065. (b) Geber, M. and Dean, R. F. A. (1958). Psychomotor development in African children: The effects of social class and the need for improved tests. Bull. WHO 18, 471–476. Gedda, L. (1971). Body odour genetically determined. J. Amer. Med. Assoc. 217, 486. Geen, R. G. and O’Neal, E. C. (eds.) (1976). Perspectives on Aggression. New York: Academic Press. Geen, R. G. and Quanty, M. B. (1977). The catharsis of aggression: An evaluation of a hypothesis. Advan. Exp. Social Psychol. 10, 1–37. van de Geer, J. P. (1971). Introduction to Multivariate Analysis for the Social Sciences. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Gehlen, A. (1940). Der Mensch, seine Natur und seine Stellung in der Welt. Berlin/Frankfurt: Athenaeum. Geist, V. (ed.) (1978). Life Strategies, Human Evolution, Environmental Design. New York: Springer. van Gennep, A. (1960). The Rites of Passage. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press; London: Routledge & Kegan Paul Ltd. [Original: (1908). Les rites de passage.] Gerson, M. (1978). Family, Women and Socialization in the Kibbutz. Lexington, MA: Lexington Books. Geschwind, N. (1979). Specializations of the human brain. Sci. Amer. 241 (3), 158–168. Gesell, A. (1946). The ontogenesis of infant behavior. In: Carmichael, L. (ed.), Manual of Child Psychology. New York: Wiley. Gewirtz, J. L. (1961). A learning analysis of the effects of normal stimulation, privation and deprivation on the acquisition of social motivation and attachment in determinants of infant behavior. In: Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behavior, Vol. 1. London: Methuen, pp. 213–290. Ghiselin, M. T. (1974). The Economy of Nature and the Evolution of Sex. Berkeley/Los Angeles/London: University of California Press. Gibbs, F. A. (1951). Ictal and non-ictal psychiatric disorders in temporal lobe epilepsy. J. Nervous Mental Dis. 113, 522–528. Gibson, E. J. and Walk, R. D. (1960). The visual cliff. Sci. Amer. 202, 64–71. Gill, T. V. (1977). Conversations with Lana. In: Rumbaugh, D. M. (ed.), Language Learning by a Chimpanzee. The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press, pp. 225–246. Gilles, J. L. and Jamtgaard, K. (1981). Overgrazing in pastoral areas—the commons reconsidered. Socio I. Ruralis 21, 129. Ginsburg, H. J. , Fling, Sh. , Hope, M. L. , Musgrove, D. , and Andrews, Ch. (1980). Maternal holding preferences: A consequence of newborn head-turning response. Child Develop. 50, 280–281. 742 Gladue, B. A. , Green, R. , and Hellman, R. E. (1984). Neuroendocrine response to estrogen and sexual orientation. Science 225, 1496–1499. Glanzer, M. (1962). Grammatical category: A rote learning and word association analysis. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav. 1, 31–41. Godelier, M. (1978). Territory and property in primitive society. Inform. 17, 399–426. Goetze, D. (1977). Castro, Nkrumah, Sukarno. Eine vergleichende Untersuchung zur Strukturanalyse charismatischer politischer Fuhrung. Berlin: Dietrich Reimer. Goffman, E. (1959). The Presentation of Self in Everyday Life. New York: Anchor Books. Goffman, E. (1963). Behavior in Public Places: Notes on the Social Organisation of Gatherings. New York: Free Press (Macmillan). Goffman, E. (1971). Relations in Public. London: Allen Lane, Penguin Press. Goffman, E. (1976). Gender advertisements. Stud. Anthropol. Visual Commun. 3, 2. Golani, I. (1969). The Golden Jackal. Tel Aviv: The Movement Notation Society. Goldberg, D. C. Whipple, B. , Fishkin, R. E. , Waxman, H. , Fink, P. J. , and Weisberg, M. (1983). The Grafenberg spot and female ejaculation: A review of initial hypotheses. J. Sex Marital Therapy 9, 27–37. Goldberg, S. and Lewis, M. (1969). Play behavior in the year-old infant: Early sex differences. Child Develop. 40, 21–31. Goldenthal, P. , Johnston, R. E. , and Kraut, R. E. (1981). Smiling, appeasement, and the silent bared-teeth display. Ethol. Sociobiol. 2, 127–133. Goldfoot, D. A. , Westerborg-van Loon, H. , Groeneveld, W. , and Koos Slob, A. (1980). Behavioral and physiological evidence of sexual climax in the female stump-tailed macaque (Macaca arctoides). Science 208, 1477–1479. Goldin-Meadow, S. and Mylander, C. (1983). Gestural communication in deaf children: Noneffect of parental input on language development. Science 221, 372–374. Goldman, M. and Fordyce, J. (1983). Prosocial behavior as affected by eye contact, touch, and voice expression. J. Social Psychol. 121, 125–129. Goldschmidt, R. B. (1940). The Material Basis of Evolution. New Haven: Yale University Press. Goldstein, A. G. and Jeffords, J. (1981). Status and touching behavior. Bull. Psychosom. Soc. 17, 79–81. Gombrich, E. H. (1982). Ornament und Kunst. Schmucktrieb und Ordnungssinn in der Psychologie des dekorativen Schaffens. Stuttgart: Klett-Cotta. Good, K. R. (1982). Limiting Factors in Amazonian Ecology. Paper presented at the 81st Meeting Amer. Anthropol. Assoc., Washington, 4 December, 1982. Good, K. R. (1988). Ritualized Contract-Chants among the Yanomami. In: Publikationen des Humanethologischen Tonarchivs der Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, Nr. 1, Begleitpublikation zum Tondokument Nr. 1 (Andechs: Mimeo). Good, P. R. , Geary, N. , and Engen, T. (1976). The effect of estrogen on odor detection. Chem. Senses Flavour 2, 45–50. Goodall, J. (1986). The Chimpanzees of Gombe, Patterns of Behavior. Cambridge MA/London: Belknap Press of Harvard University Press. Goodall, J. , Bandora, A. , Bergmann, E. , Busse, C. , Matama, H. , Mpongo, E. , Pierce, A. , and Riss, D. (1979). Intercommunity interactions in the chimpanzee population of the Gombe National Park. In: Hamburg, D. A. and McCown, E. R. (eds.), The Great Apes. Menlo Park, CA: The Benjamin/Cummings. Goodenough Pitcher, E. and Hickey Schultz, L. (1983). Boys and Girls at Play: The Development of Sex Roles. New York: Praeger. Goodman, C. S. and Bastiani, M. J. (1984). How embryonic nerve cells recognize one another. Sci. Amer. 251 (6), 50–58. Goodman, C. S. , Bastiani, M. J. , Doe, C. Q. , Du Lac, S. , Helfand, St. L. , Kuwada, J. Y. , and Thomas, J. B. (1984). Cell recognition during neuronal development. Science 225, 1271–1279. Goodwin, G. (1971). Western Apache Raiding and Warfare. Tucson: University of Arizona Press. Gordon, R. J. (1984). The !Kung in the Kalahari exchange: An ethnohistorical perspective. In: Schrire, C. (ed.), Past and Present in Hunter-Gatherer Studies. Orlando/New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 195–224. Gottlieb, G. (1976). Early development of species-specific auditory perception in birds. In: Gottlieb, G. (ed.), Neural and Behavioral Specificity. New York: Academic Press. 743 Gottman, J. M. (1983). How children become friends. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop. 48, Nr. 3. Gould, S. J. (1980). The Panda’s Thumb. New York/London: W. W. Norton. Gower, D. B. (1972). 16-unsaturated C19 steroids. A review of their chemistry, biochemistry and possible physiological role. J. Steroid Biochem. 3, 45–103. Grafenberg, E. (1950). The role of urethra in female orgasm. Intern J. Sex. 3, 145–148. Graham, C. A. and McGrew, W. C. (1980). in female undergraduates living on a coeducational campus. Psychoneurendocrinology 5, 245–252. Graham, E. E. (1973). Yuman Warfare: An Analysis of Ecological Factors from Ethnohistorical Sources. Paper read at the 9th Intern. Congr. of Anthropological and Ethnological Sciences, Chicago. Grammer, K. (1979). Helfen und Unterstützen in Kindergruppen. Diplomarbeit des Zoologischen Instituts. Universität München. Grammer, K. (1982). Wettbewerb und Kooperation: Strategien des Eingriffs in Konflikte unter Kindern einer Kindergartengruppe. Diss. Univ. München, Fachbereich Biologie. Grammer, K. (1985). Verhaltensforschung am Menschen: Überlegungen zu den biologischen Grundlagen des “Umwegverhaltens.” In: Svilar, M. (ed.), Mensch und Tier. Bern, pp. 273–318. Grammer, K. (1986). “Social Tool Use” in Human Courtship: The Function of Laughter. Submitted for publication. Grammer, K. (1988). Biologische Grundlagen des Sozialverhaltens. Darmstadt: Wissenschaftliche Buchgesellschaft. Grammer, K. (in press). Human courtship: Biological bases and cognitive processing. In: Rasa, A. , Vogel, C. , Volland, E. (eds.), Sociobiology of Reproductive Strategies in Animals and Men. Beckenham: Croom Helm. Grammer, K. , Schropp, R. , and Shibasaka, H. (1984). Contact, conflict and appeasement— children’s interaction strategies. The use of photography. Intern. J. Visual Sociol. 2, 59–74. Grammer, K. , Schiefenhovel, W. , Schleidt, M. , Lorenz, B. , and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1988). Patterns on the face: The eyebrow flash in cross cultural comparison. Ethology 77, 270–299. Grant, E. C. (1965). The contribution of ethology to child psychiatry. In: Howell, J. G. (ed.), Modern Perspectives in Child Psychiatry. Edinburgh: Oliver and Boyd. Grant, E. C. (1968). An ethological description of non-verbal behavior during interviews. Brit. J. Med. Psychol. 41, 177–184. Grant, E. C. (1969). Human facial expression. Man 4[NS], 525-536. Gray, J. P. and Wolfe, L. D. (1983). Human female sexual cycles and the concealment of ovulation problem. J. Soc. Biol. Structures 6, 345–352. Greed, G. W. (1984). Sexual subordination: Institutionalized homosexuality and social control in Melanesia. Ethnology 23, 157–176. Green, J. A. , Jones, L. E. , and Gustafson, G. E. (1987). Perception of cries by parents and nonparents: Relation to cry acoustics. Develop. Psychol. 23, 370–382. Green, St . and Marler, P. (1979). The analysis of . In: King, F. A. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology. Vol 3: Social Behavior and Communication. New York/London: Plenum Press, pp. 73–158. Greenberg, M. and Morris, N. (1982). Engrossment—the newborn’s impact upon the father. In: Cath, S. H. , Gurwitt, A. R. , and Ross, J. M. (eds.), Father and Child, Developmental and Clinical Perspectives. Boston: Little, Brown. [Reprint from Amer. J. Orthopsychiat. 44, (1974), 520–530.] Greene, P. J. , Morgan, Ch. J. , and Barash, D. P. (1980). Hybrid vigor: Evolutionary biology and sociology. In: Lockard, J. (ed.), The Evolution of Human Social behavior. New York: Elsevier. Greif, E. B. and Gleason, J. B. (1980). Hi, thanks and goodbye: More routine information. Language Soc. 9, 159–166. Grellert, E. A. (1982). Childhood play behavior of homosexual and heterosexual men. Psychol. Rep. 51, 607–610. Grellert, E. A. , Newcomb, M. D. , and Bentler, P. M. (1982). Childhood play activities of male and female homosexuals and heterosexuals. Arch. Sex. Behav. 11, 451–478. Grieser, D. L. and Kuhl, P. K. (1988). Maternal speech to infants in a tonal language: Support for universal prosodic features in motherese. Develop. Psychol. 24 (1), 14–20. 744 Griffin, P. B. (1984). Forager resource and land use in the humid tropics: The Agta of Northeastern Luzon, the Philippines. In: Schrire, C. (ed.), Past and Present in Hunter-Gatherer Studies. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 95–121. Grimm, H. (1983). Psycholinguistische Aspekte der frühen Sprachanbahnung. Sozialpädiatr. Praxis Klinik 5, 589–593. Groos, K. (31933). Die Spiele der Tiere. Jena: Gustav Fischer. Grossmann, K. (1978). Die Wirkung des Augenöffnens von Neugeborenen auf das Verhalten ihrer Mütter. Geburtshilfe Frauenheilk. 38, 629–635. Grossmann, K. E. (1977). Frühe Einflüsse auf die soziale und intellektuelle Entwicklung. Z. Pädagogik 23, 847–880. Grossmann, K. E. and Grossman, K. (1982). Eltern-Kind-Bindung in Bielefeld. In: Immelmann, K. , Barlow, G. , Petrinovich, L. , and Maid, M. (ed.), Verhaltensentwicklung bei Mensch und Tier. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 794–799. Grüsser, O. J. (1983). Mother-child holding patterns in western art: A developmental study. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 89–94. Gruhl, H. (1975). Ein Planet wird geplündert. Frankfurt/M.: S. Fischer. Grusec, J. E. and Brinker, D. B. (1972). Reinforcement for imitation as a social learning determinant with implications for sex-role development. J. Personality Social Psychol. 21, 149–158. Gruter, M. (1979). Origins of legal behavior. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 43–51. Gruter, M. (1982). Biologically based behavioral research and the facts of law. J. Social Biol. Structures 5, 315–323. Grzimek, B. and Grzimek, M. (1959). Serengeti darf nicht sterben. Berlin: Ullstein. Guenther, M. G. (1981). Bushman and hunter-gatherer territoriality. Z. Ethnol. 106, 109–120. Gumperz, J. L. and Hymes, D. H. (eds.) (1972). Directions in Sociolinguistics. The Ethnography of Communication. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Guthrie, E. R. (1952). The Psychology of Learning. New York: Harper. Guthrie, R. D. (1976). Body Hot Spots. New York: Van Nostrand-Reinhold. Habermas, J. (1957). Können Konsumenten spielen? Frankfürter Allgem. Zeitung, No. 88, 13 April, 1957. Hailman, J. P. (1967). The ontogeny of an instinct. Behaviour Suppl. 15, 1–196. Haldane, J. B. S. (1955). Population genetics. New Biol. 18, 34–51. Hales, D. J. , Lozoff, B. , Sosa, R. , and Kennell, J. H. (1977). Defining the limits of the maternal sensitive period. Develop. Med. Child. Neurol. 19, 454–461. Hall, E. T. (1966). The Hidden Dimension. New York: Doubleday. Hall, K. R. R. (1966). Social learning in monkeys. J. Zool. 148, 15–87. Hallowell, A. I. (1940). Aggression in Saulteaux society. Psychiatry 3, 395–407. Hallpike, C. R. (1973). Functionalist interpretations of primitive warfare. Man 8, 451–470. Hamblin, R. L. (1958). Leadership and crisis. Sociometry 20, 322–335. Hamburg, B. A. (1974). The psychobiology of sex differences. An evolutionary perspective. In: Friedman, R. C. , Richart, R. M. , and Van de Wiele, R. L. (eds.), Sex Differences in Behavior. New York/London: John Wiley, pp. 373–392. Hamilton, W. D. (1964). The genetical evolution of social behavior. J. Theoret. Biol. 7, 1–52. Hamilton, W. D. (1967). Extraordinary sex ratios. Science 156, 477–488. Hamilton, W. D. (1972). Altruism and related phenomena, mainly in social insects. Ann. Rev. Syst. 3, 193–232. Hamm, M. , Russell, M. , and Koepke, J. (1979). Neonatal Imitation? Vortrag anläβlich der Tagung der Society for Research in Child Development, San Francisco, March, 1979. Hardin, G. (1968). The tragedy of the commons. Science 162, 1243–1248. Harrelson, A. L. and Goodman, C. S. (1988). Growth cone guidance in insects: Fasciclin II is a member of the immunoglobulin superfamily. Science 242, 700–708. Harris, M. (1968). The Rise of Anthropological Theory. New York: T. Y. Crowell. Harris, M. (1971). Culture, Man and Nature: An Introduction to General Anthropology. New York: Crowell. Harris, M. (1974). Cows, Pigs, Wars and Witches: The Riddles of Culture. New York: Random House. Harris, M. (1975). Culture, People and Nature: An Introduction to General Anthropology. New York: Crowell. 745 Harris, M. (1977). Cannibals and Kings: The Origins of Cultures. New York: Random House, (a) Harris, M. (1977). Why men dominate women. New York Times Magazine 46. (b) Harris, M. (1979). The Yanomamö and the causes of war in band and village societies. In: Margolis, M. and Carter, W. (eds.), Brazil: Anthropological Perspectives. Essays in Honor of Charles Wagley. New York: Columbia University Press, pp. 121–132. Harris, P. and MacFarlane, J. A. (1974). The growth of the effective visual field from birth to seven weeks. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 18, 340–348. Hartup, W. W. (1975). The origins of friendship. In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), Friendship and Peer Relations. New York: Wiley, pp. 1–26. Hass, H. (1968). Wir Menschen. Wien: Molden. Hass, H. (1970). Das Energon. Wien: Molden. Hass, H. (1981). Vorteil des Menschen: Er kann sein “Energon” verändern. In: Das neue Erfolgs und Karrierehandbuch für Selbständige und Führungskräfte. Geretsried: Verlag Beste Unternehmensführung, pp. 157–198. Hassenstein, B. (1966). Kybernetik und biologische Forschung. In: Gessner, F. (ed.), Handbuch der Biologie, Bd. 1/2: Allgemeine Biologie. Frankfurt/M.: Athenaion, pp. 629–719. Hassenstein, B. (1972). Bedingungen für Lernprozesse—teleonomisch gesehen. Nova Acta Leopoldina [N. F.] 37, 289- 320. (a) Hassenstein, B. (1972). Verhaltensbiologische Aspekte der frühkindlichen Entwicklung und ihre so-zialpolitischen Konsequenzen. Mannheimer Forum 72 (Boehringer), pp. 168-203. (b) Hassenstein, B. (1973). Verhaltensbiologie des Kindes. München: Piper, (a) Hassenstein, B. (1973). Kindliche Entwicklung aus der Sicht der Verhaltensbiologie. Kinderarzt 21, 134–136, 191- 192, 260-265, 407-410. (b) Hassenstein, B. (1977). Faktische Elternschaft. Ein neuer Begriff der Familiendynamik und seine Bedeutung. Familiendynamik 2, 104–125. Hassenstein, B. (1981). Biologisch bedeutsame Vorgänge in den ersten Lebenswochen. In: Hövels, O. , Halberstadt, E. , Laewenich, V. V. , and Eckert, I. (ed.), Geburtshilfe und Kinderheilkunde. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme, pp. 56–71. Hassenstein, B. (1982). Sexualentwicklung des Kindes in verhaltensbiologischer Sicht. In: Hellbrügge, Th. (eds.), Entwicklung der kindlichen Sexualität, München: Urban & Schwarzenberg. Hassenstein, B. (1983). Funktionsschaltbilder als Hilfsmittel zur Darstellung theoretischer Konzepte in der Verhaltensbiologie. Zool. Jb. Physiol. 87, 181–187. Hausfater, G. (1979). Comments/discussions on Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1979). Human ethology: Concepts and implications for the science of man. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 36–37. Hausfater, G. and Vogel, Ch. (1982). Infanticide in langur monkeys (genus Presbytis ): recent research and a review of hypotheses. In: Chiarelli, A. B. and Corruccini, R. S. (eds.), Advanced Views in Primate Biology. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 160–176. Havemann, R. (1980). Morgen. Die Industriegesellschaft am Scheideweg. Miinchen: Piper. Hawkes, K. (1977). Co-operation in Binumarien: Evidence for Sahlin’s model. Man 12 [NS] 459-483. Hawkes, K. (1983). Kin selection and culture. Amer. Ethnol. 10, 345–363. Hawkes, K. (in press): Helping Kin: Is it Nepotism? Hayduk, L. A. (1978). Personal space: An “evaluation” and orienting overview. Psychol. Bull. 85, 117–133. Hayduk, L. A. (1983). Personal space: Where we now stand. Psychol. Bull. 94, 293–335. Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1980). Personal space of the blind. Social Psychol. Quart. 43, 216–223. Hayek, F. A. V. (1975). Die Irrtümer des Konstruktivismus. Walter-Eucken-Institut, Vortrage und Aufsätze 51, Tubingen: J. C. B. Mohr. Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1977). Drei Vorlesungen über Demokratie, Gerechtigkeit und Sozialismus. Walter- Eucken-Institut, Vorträge und Aufsätze 63, Tübingen: J. C. B. Mohr. Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1979). Die drei Quellen der menschlichen Werte. Walter-Eucken-Institut, Vorträge und Aufsätze 70, Tübingen: J. C. B. Mohr. (a) Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1979). Liberalismus. Walter-Eucken-Institut, Vorträge und Aufsätze 72, Tübingen: J. C. B. Mohr. (b) Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1980/1981). Recht, Gesetzgebung und Freiheit. Eine neue Darstellung der liberalen Prinzipien der Gerechtigkeit und der politischen Ökonomie, 3 Bde. Landsberg a. Lech: Verlag Moderne Industrie. Hayduk, L. A. and Mainprize, St. (1983). Die überschätzte Vernunft. In: Riedl, R. and Kreuzer, F. (ed.), Evolution und Menschenbild. Hamburg: Hoffmann & Camper, pp. 164–192. 746 Hayes, L. A. and Watson, J. L. (1981). Neonatal imitation: Fact or artifact? Develop. Psychol. 17, 655–660. Haynes, H. M. , White, B. L. and Held, R. (1965). Visual accommodation in human infants. Science 148, 528–530. Healey, Ch . (1984). Trade and sociability: Balanced reciprocity as generosity in New Guinea Highlands. Amer. Ethnol. 11, 42–60. Hecaen, H. and Albert, M. L. (1978). Human Neural Psychology. New York: Wiley. Hediger, H. (1934). Zur Biologie und Psychologie der Flucht bei Tieren. Biol. Zentralbl. 54, 21–40. Heeschen, V. (1976). Überlegungen zum Begriff “Sprachliches Handeln.” Z. German. Linguistik 4, 273–301. Heeschen, V. (1982). Stil und Inhalt der Lieder und Tanztexte der Eipo. Vortrag auf Einladung der DFG im Rahmen des Symposiums “Mensch, Kultur und Umwelt im zentralen Bergland von Westneuguinea,” Berlin. Heeschen, V. (1984). Ästhetische Form und sprachliches Handeln: Sprechen auf der Suche nach einem Partner. Vortrag auf der Jahrestagung der Gesellschaft für Sprachwissenschaft, Bielefeld, (a) Heeschen, V. (1984). Durch Krieg und Brautpreis zur Freundschaft. Vergleichende Verhaltensstudien zu den Eipo und Yalenang. Vortrag gehalten auf dem Kolloquium des Schwerpunktprogramms der DFG “Mensch, Kultur und Areal im zentralen Bergland von Westneuguinea,” Seewiesen, 30, 3-1 April, 1984. (b) Heeschen, V. (1984, in press). Singen bei der Arbeit. Publ. zum Film E 2522: Heeschen, V. und Heunemann, D.: Singen bei der Arbeit. Göttingen, IWF. Heeschen, V. (in press). Humanethologische Aspekte der Sprachevolution. In: Gessinger, J. and V. Rahden (eds.), Theorien vom Ursprung der Sprache. Berlin: De Gruyter. Heeschen, V. , Schiefenhövel, W. , and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1980). Requesting, giving and taking: The relationship between verbal and nonverbal behavior in the speech community of the Eipo, Irian Jaya (West New Guinea). In: Key, R. M. (ed.), The Relationship of Verbal and Nonverbal Behavior. The Hague/Paris/New York: Mouton, pp. 139–166. Heinroth, O. (1910). Beiträge zur Biologie, insbesondere Psychologie und Ethologie der Anatiden. Verh. 5. Int. Ornith. Kongr. Berlin, pp. 589–702. Heinz, H. J. (1966). The Social Organization of the !Ko-Bushmen. Masters Thesis. Johannesburg, University of South Africa. Heinz, H. J. (1967). Conflicts, Tensions and Release of Tensions in a Bushmen Society. The Institute for the Study of Man in Africa, Isma Papers 23, 2-21. Heinz, H. J. (1972). Territoriality among the Bushmen, in general, and the !Ko, in particular. Anthropos , 67, 405–416. Heinz, H. J. (1979). The nexus complex among the !Ko-Bushmen of Botswana. Anthropos 74, 465–480. Heinz, H. J. and Maguire, B. (1974). The Ethno-Biology of the !Ko-Bushmen. Their Ethno-Botanical Knowledge and Plant Lore. Occ. Papers 1, Botswana Soc, Gaborone. Hellbrügge, Th. (1967). Chronophysiologie des Kindes. Verhandl. Deut. Ges. Innere Med. 73, 895. Heller, A. (1980). The emotional division of labor between the sexes. Soc. Prax. 7, 205–218. Helmuth, H. (1967). Zum Verhalten des Menschen: Die Aggression. Z. Ethnol. 92, 265–273. Henley, N. M. (1977). Body Politics: Power, Sex, and Nonverbal Communication. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Herdt, G. H. (1982). Rituals of Manhood. Male Initiation in Papua New Guinea. Berkeley: University of California Press. Herskovits, M. J. (1950). Man and His Works: The Science of Cultural Anthropology. New York: Knopf. Herz, A. (1984). Biochemie und Pharmakologie des Schmerzgeschehens. In: Zimmermann, C. M. and Handwerker, H. O. (eds.), Schmerz, Berlin: Springer, pp. 61–86. Heslin, R. , Nguyen, T. D. , and Nguyen, M. L. (1983). Meaning of touch from a stranger or same person. J. Nonverbal Behav. 7, 147–157. Hess, E. H. (1973). Imprinting: Early Experience and the of Attachment. New York: Van Nostrand. 747 Hess, E. H. (1975). The Tell-Tale Eye. New York: Van Nostrand Reinhold. Hess, E. H. (1977). Das sprechende Auge. Miinchen: Kindler. Hewes, G. H. (1957). The anthropology of posture. Sci. Amer. 196, 123–132. Hewes, G. W. (1973). An explicit formulation of the relationship between tool-using, tool-making and the emergence of language. Visible Language 72, 101–127. Hess, E. H. (1977). Language origin theories. In: Rumbaugh, D. M. (ed.), Language Learning by a Chimpanzee. The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press, pp. 3–53. Heymer, A. (1981). “Schnelles Brauenheben” im Kontext verschiedener sozialer Interaktionen, Homo 32 (3, 4), 1981. Hicks, D. J. (1965). Imitation and retention of film-mediated aggressive peer and adult models. J. Personality Social Psychol. 2, 97–100. Hilgard, E. R. and Bower, G. H. (1973, 3rd ed.). Theorien des Lernens, 2 Bde. Stuttgart: Klett. Engl. Edition (1975, 4th ed.). Theories of Learning. New York. Hill, J. H. (1974). Possible continuity theories of language. Language 50, 134–150. Hill, W. W. (1936). Navaho Warfare. New Haven: Yale Univ. Publications in Anthropology, 5. Hinde, R. A. (1966). Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and . New York/London: McGraw-Hill. Hess, E. H. (1974). Biological Basis of Human Social Behaviour. New York: McGraw-Hill. Hess, E. H. (1984). Why do sexes behave differently in close relationships? J. Soc. Personal Relationships 1, 471–501. Hinde, R. A. and Barden, L. A. (1985). The evolution of the teddy bear. Anim. Behav. 33, 1371–1372. Hinde, R. A. and Stevenson-Hinde, J. (1976). Towards understanding relationships: Dynamic stability. In: Bateson, P. P. G. and Hinde, R. A. (eds.), Growing Points in Ethology. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press, pp. 451–479. Hinde, R. A. , Easton, D. F. , Meller, R. E. , and Tamplin, A. (1983). Nature and determinants of preschoolers’ differential behavior to adults and peers. Brit. J. Develop. Psychol. 1, 3–19. Hines, S. M. Jr. (1983). Die Ursprünge des Staates: Traditionelle Interpretationen, aktueller Forschungsstand und der emergente Charakter des Staates. In: Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 68–79. Hirst, G. (1982). An evaluation of evidence for innate sex differences in linguistic ability. J. Psycholinguistic Res. 2, 95–113. Hite, S. (1976). The Hite Report: A Nationwide Study of Female Sexuality. New York: Dell. Hjortsjö, C.H. (1969). Man’s Face and Mimic Language. Malmö: Studentlitteratur. Hobbes, T. (1968). Leviathan. London: Penguin. Hockett, C. F. (1960). Logical considerations in the study of animal communication. Amer. Inst. Sci. Publ. 7, 392–430. Dasselbe: Z. Tierpsychol. 23, 250-254. Hodder, I. (1982). The Present Past. London: Batsford. Hoebel, B. G. (1983). Neurogene und chemische Grundlagen des Glücksgefühls. In: Gruter, M. and Rehbinder, M. (eds.), Der Beitrag der Biologie zu Fragen von Recht und Ethik. Schrift. Rechtssoziol. Rechtstatsachenforsch. 54, 87–109. Hoebel, E. A. (1967). Song duels among the Eskimo. In: Bohannan, P. (ed.), Law and Warfare, New York; Natural History Press, pp. 255–262. Hörmann, L. V. (1877). Tiroler Volkstypen. Wien. Hoffmann-Krayer, E. and Bächtold-Stäubli, H. (1930). Haberfeldtreiben. Handwörterbuch des deutschen Aberglaubens, III. Berlin: de Gruyter, p. 1291. Hoffmann-Krayer, E. and Bächtold-Stäubli, H. (1933). Katzenmusik. Handwörterbuch des deutschen Aberglaubens, IV. Berlin: de Gruyter, pp. 1125–1132. Hokanson, J. E. (1970). Psychophysiological evaluation of the catharsis hypothesis. In: Megargee, E. I. and Hokanson, J. E. (eds.), The Dynamics of Aggression. New York: Harper & Row, pp. 74–86. Hokanson, J. E. and Shetler, S. (1961). The effect of overt aggression on physiological tension level. J. Abnormal Social Psychol. 63, 446–448. Hold, B. (1974). Rangordnungsverhalten bei Vorschulkindern. Homo 25, 252–267. Hold, B. (1976). Attention structure and rank specific behaviour in pre-school children. In: Chance, M. R. A. and Larsen, R. R. (eds.), The Social Structure of Attention. London: Wiley, pp. 177–201. 748 Hold, B. (1977). Rank and behaviour: An ethological study of pre-school children. Homo 28, 158–188. Hold, B. and Schleidt, M. (1977). The importance of human odour in non-verbal communication. Z. Tierpsychol. 43, 225–239. Hold-Cavell, B. (1983). Die Entwicklung und Bedeutung des Ansehens von Kindern im Kindergarten. Universität Regensburg, Institut fur Psychologie. Hold-Cavell, B. and Stöhr, C. (1986). The significance of attention structure: when do children attend to each other? Bull. d’Ecol. Ethol. Humaines 5, 24–36. Hold-Cavell, B. C. L. and D. Borsutzky (1986). Strategies to obtain high regard: Longitudinal study of a group of preschool children. Ethol. Sociobiol. 7, 39–56. Hoist, E. V. (1935). Über den Prozeβ der zentralen Koordination. Pflügers Arch. 236, 149–158. Hoist, E. V. (1936). Versuche zur Theorie der relativen Koordination. Plügers Arch. 237, 93–121. Hoist, E. V. (1937). Baustein zu einer vergleichenden Physiologie der lokomotorischen Reflexe bei Fischen, II. Z. Vergl. Physiol. 24, 532–562. Hoist, E. V. (1939). Die relative Koordination als Phänomen und als Methode zentralnervöser Funktionsanalyse. Erg. Physiol. 42, 228–306. Hoist, E. V. (1955). Regelvorgänge der optischen Wahrnehmung. 5th Conf. Soc. Biol. Rhythm. Stockholm, pp. 26–34. Hoist, E. V. (1957). Aktive Leistungen der menschlichen Gesichtswahrnehmung. Stud. Generale 10, 231–243. Hoist, E. V. and Mittelstaedt, H. (1950). Das Reafferenz-Prinzip. Naturwissenschaften 37, 464–476. Hoist, E. V. and Saint-Paul, U. V. (1960). Vom Wirkungsgefüge der Triebe. Naturwissenschaften 18, 409–422. Holzapfel, M. (1940). Triebbedingte Ruhezustände als Ziel des Appetenzverhaltens. Naturwissenschaften 28, 273–280. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. (1969). The facial display of the catarrhine monkeys and apes. In: Morris, D. (ed.), Primate Ethology. New York: Garden City, pp. 9–98. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. (1971). Aspecten van Het Sociale Gedrag En De Communicatie Bij Humane En Hogere Niet- Humane Primaten (Aspects of the Social Behavior and Communication in Human and Higher Non-Human Primates). Rotterdam: Bonder-Offset (n. v.). van Hooff, J. A. R. A. (1973). A structural analysis of the social behavior of a semi-captive group of chimpanzees. In: Cranach, M. von and Vine, I. (eds.), Social Communication and Movement. New York: Academic Press, pp. 75–162. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. (1976). The comparison of facial expression in man and higher primates. In: Cranach, M. V. (ed.), Methods of Inference from Animal to Human Behaviour. Chicago: Aldine; The Hague/Paris: Mouton, pp. 165–196. van Hooff, J. A. R. A. (1982). Categories and sequences of behaviour: Methods of description and analysis. In: Scherer, K. and Ekman, P. (eds.), Handbook of Methods in Nonverbal Behavior Research. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 362–439. Hoon, P. W. , Bruce, K. , and Kinchloe, B. (1982). Does the menstrual cycle play a role in sexual arousal? Psychophysiology 19, 21–27. Horvath, Th . (1979). Correlates of physical beauty in men and women. Soc. Behav. Personality 7, 145–151. Horvath, Th . (1981). Physical attractiveness: The influence of selected torso parameters. Arch. Sex. Behav. 10, 21–24. Hospers, J. (1969). Introductory Readings in Aesthetics. New York: The Free Press (Collier). Hoyle, G. (1984). The Scope of Neuroethology. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 367-412. Hrdy, S. B. See Blaffer-Hrdy, S. B. , Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T. N. (1962). Receptive fields, binocular interactions and functional architecture in the cat’s visual cortex. J. Physiol. 160, 106–154. Hubel, D. H. and Wiesel, T. N. (1963). Receptive fields of cells in striate cortex of very young, visually inexperienced kittens. J. Neurophysiol. 24, 994–1002. Huber, E. (1931). Evolution facial Muscles. In: Webster, R. C. , Smith, R. C. , and Smith, K. F. (1983). Face lift, 2. Etiology of platysma cording and its relationship to treatment. Head and Neck Surg. 6, 590–595. Huber, F. (1974). Neuronal background of species-specific acoustical communication in orthopteran insects (Gryllidae). In: Broughton, W. B. (ed.), The Biology of . Symposia of the Inst, of Biol., No. 21., Chapt. 4, pp. 61-88. Oxford: Blackwell. 749 Huber, F. (1977). Lautäuβerungen und Lauterkennen bei Insekten (Grillen). Rheinisch-Westfälische Akad.Wiss., Vortrage Nr. 265, Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. Huber, F. (1983). Neural correlates of orthopteran and cicada phonotaxis. In: Huber, F. and Markl, H. (eds.), Neuroethology and Behavioral Physiology. Berlin: Springer, pp. 108–135. Hückstedt, B. (1965). Experimentelle Untersuchungen zum “Kindchenschema.” Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 12, 421–450. Hughes, A. L. (1981). Female infanticide: Sex ratio manipulation in humans. Ethol. Sociobiol. 2, 109–111. Hull, D. L. (1978). Scientific bandwagon or travelling medicine show? Society , Sept./Oct., pp. 50–59. Hulsebus, R. C. (1973). Operant conditioning of infant behavior: A review. Advan. Child Develop. Behav. 8, 111–158. Humphrey, T. (1969). Postnatal repetition of human pre-natal activity sequences with some suggestions of their neuroanatomical basis. In: Robinson, R. J. (ed.), Brain and Early Behavior. New York: Academic Press. Hunsperger, R. W. (1954). Reizversuche im periventrikulären Grau des Mittel-und Zwischenhirns (Film). Helv. Physiol. Acta 12, C4–C6. Hunt, G. T. (1940). The Wars of the Iroquois: A Study in Intertribal Trade Relations. Madison, WI: University of Wisconsin Press. Huxley, A. (1932). Brave New World. London: Chatto & Windus. Huxley, A. (1959). Brave New World Revisited. London: Chatto & Windus. Huxley, A. (21981). Schöne neue Welt, und: Dreiβig Jahre danach oder Wiedersehen mit der Schönen Neuen Welt. Miinchen; Piper. Hymes, D. H. (1970). The ethnography of speaking. In: Fishman, J. A. (ed.), Readings in the Sociology of Language. Paris/The Hague: Mouton, pp. 94–138. Ikeda, K. and Kaplan, W. O. (1970). Unilaterally patterned neural activity of a mutant gynandromorph of Drosophila melanogaster. Amer. Zool. 10, 311. Immelmann, K. (1965). Prägungserscheinungen in der Gesangsentwicklung junger Zebrafinken. Naturwissenschaften 52, 169–170. Immelmann, K. (1966). Zur Irreversibilität der Prägung. Naturwissenschaften 53, 209. Immelmann, K. (1970). Zur ökologischen Bedeutung prögungsbedingter Isolationsmechanismen. Verhandl. Deut. Zool. Ges. 64, 304–314. Immelmann, K. (1975). Ecological significance of imprinting and early learning. Annu. Rev. Ecol. Systematics 6, 15–37. Imperato-McGinley, J. , Peterson, R. E. , Gautier, T. , and Sturla, E. (1979). Androgens and the evolution of the male gender-identity among male pseudohermaphrodites with 5αreductase deficiency. New Eng. J. Med. 300, 1233–1237. Isaac, G. (1978). The foodsharing behavior of protohuman hominids. Sci. Amer. 238, 90–108. Itten, J. (1961). Kunst der Farbe. Subjektives Erleben und objektives Erkennen der Wege zur Kunst. Ravensburg: Maier. Izard, C. E. (1968). The emotions and emotion constructs in personality and culture research. In: Cattell, R. B. (ed.), Handbook of Modern Personality Theory. Chicago: Aldine. Izard, C. E. (1971). The Face of Emotion. New York: Appleton-Century-Crofts. Izard, C. E. (1981). Die Emotionen des Menschen. Weinheim: Beltz. Jackendorff, R. and Lerdahl, F. (1982). A grammatical parallel between music and language. In: Clynes, M. (ed.), Music, Mind and Brain, New York: Plenum, pp. 83–119. Jacklin, C. N. and Maccoby, E. E. (1978). Social behaviour at thirty-three months in same-sex and mixed-sex dyads. Child Develop. 49, 557–569. Jacobson, J. L. (1981). The role of inanimate objects in early peer interaction. Child Develop. 52, 618–626. Jacobson, S. W. (1979). Matching behavior in the young infant. Child Develop. 50, 425–430. Jaffe, Y. , Shapir, N. , and Yinon, Y. (1981). Aggression and its escalation. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 12, 21–36. Jahoda, G. (1954). A note on Ashanti names and their relationship to personality. Brit. J. Psychol. 45, 192–195. Jakobson, R. (1941). Kindersprache, Aphasie und allgemeine Lautgesetze. Språkvetenskapliga Sällskapets i Uppsala, Förhandlingar, pp. 1–83. 750 James, W. (1890). Principles of Psychology. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Jantsch, E. (1979). Sociobiological and sociocultural process: A non-reductionist view. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 87–92. Jarvie, I. C. (1983). The problem of the ethnographic real. Current Anthropol. 24, 313–325. Jensen, A. R. (1980). Bias in Mental Testing. New York: The Free Press, (a) Jensen, A. R. (1980). Multiple book review of bias in mental testing. Behav. Brain Sci. 3, 325–371. (b) Jensen, A. R. (1983). The definition of intelligence and factor-score indeterminancy. Behav. Brain Sci. 6, 313–315. Jessen, E. (1981). Untersuchungen zur Geschlechtererkennung—Die Zuordnung einfacher geometrischer Formen zu Mann und Frau in verschiedenen Altersstufen und Kulturen. Dissertation, Ludwig-Maximilian-Universität München. Jettmar, K. (1973). Die anthropologische Aussage der Ethnologic In: Gadamer, H.G. and Vogler, P. (eds.), Neue Anthropologic, vol. 4. Kulturanthropologie. Stuttgart: Thieme. Johnson, R. N. (1972). Aggression in Man and Animals. Philadelphia/London: Saunders. Johnson, R. S. (1982). Food, other valuables, payment and the relative scale of Ommura ceremonies (New Guinea). Anthropos 77, 509–523. Jolly, A. (1972). The Evolution of Primate Behavior. New York: Macmillan. Jouvet, M. (1972). Le discours biologiquc Rev. Med. 16-17, 1003–1063. Jürgens, J. (1971). Soziales Verhalten, Kommunikation und Hirnmechanismen. Umschau 71, 799–802. Jürgens, J. and Ploog, D. (1976). Zur Evolution der Stimmc Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkr. , 222, 117–137. Kagan, J. (1971). Change and Continuity in Infancy. New York: Wiley. Kahn, H. (1977). Vor uns die guten Jahre. Wien: Molden. Kahn-Ladas, A. , Whipple, B. , and Perry, J. D. (1982). The G-Spot and Other Recent Discoveries about Human Sexuality. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Kaiser, G. (1978). Comment/discussion. In: Nitsch, K. (eds.), Was wird aus unseren Kindern? Heidelberg: Hüthig Verlag, pp. 34–45. Kaissling, K. E. (1971). Insect olfaction. In: Beidler, L. M. (ed.), Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. 4. Chemical Senses. Berlin: Springer, pp. 351–431. Kaissling, K. E. and Priesner, E. (1970). Die Riechschwelle des Seidenspinners. Naturwissenschaften 57, 23–38. Kaitz, M. , Meschulach-Sarfaty, O. , Auerbach, J. , and Eidelman, A. (1988). A reexamination of newborns’ ability to imitate facial expressions. Develop. Psychol. 24, (1), 3-7. Kalbermatten, U. (1979). Handlung, Theorie, Methode—Ergebnisse. Dissertation, Universität Bern. Zürich: Juris Verlag. Kalbermatten, U. and Cranach, M. V. (1980). Hierarchisch aufgebaute Beobachtungssysteme zur Handlungsanalyse In: Winkler, P. (ed.), Methoden zur Analyse von Face-to-Face Situationen. Stuttgart: Metzler. Kallman, F. J. (1952). A comparative twin study on the genetic aspects of male homoseuality. J. Nerv. Mental Dis. 115, 283. Kalmus, H. (1955). The discrimination by the nose of the dog of individual human odours and in particular of the odours of twins. Brit. J. Anim. Behav. 3, 25–31. Kalnins, I. V. and Bruner, J. S. (1973). Infant sucking used to change the clarity of a visual display. In: Hulsebus, R. C: Operant Conditioning of Infant Behaviour, a Review. Advan. Child Develop. Behav. 8. Kaltenbach, K. , Weinraub, M. and Fullard, W. (1980). Infant wariness toward strangers reconsidered: Infants’ and mothers’ reactions to unfamiliar persons. Child Develop. 51, 1197–1202. Kandel, E. R. (1976). Cellular Basis of Behavior. An Introduction to Behavioral Neurobiology. San Francisco: W. H. Freeman. Kaplan, H. , Hill, K. , Hawkes, K. , and Hurtado, A. (1984). Food sharing among Ache hunter-gatherers of Eastern Paraguay. Current Anthropology. 25, 113–115. Kaplan, M. A. (1979). Is species selfishness a viable concept? J. Social Biol. Structures. 2, 1–7. Kardiner, A. (1939). The Individual and His Society. New York: Columbia University Press. Kardiner, A. (1945). The Psychological Frontiers of Society. New York: Columbia University Press. 751 Karsten, R. (1923). Blood revenge, war, and victory feasts among the Jibaro Indians of Eastern Ecuador. Bur. Amer. Ethnol. Bull. 79. Katz, R. J. and Steiner, M. (1979). Dream and motivation: a psychobiological approach. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 141–154. Kauffman-Doig, F. (1979). Sexual Behaviour in Ancient Peru. Lima-Surquillo: Kompactos. Kay, P. and Kempton, W. (1984). What is the Sapir-Whorf hypothesis? Amer. Anthropol. 86, 65–79. Keegan, J. (1976). The Face of Battle. New York: Viking Press. Keegan, J. and Darracott, J. (1981). The Nature of War. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Keesing, R. M. (1981). Cultural Anthropology. A contemporary Perspective. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Keller, H. (1979). Die Entstehung von Geschlechtsunterschieden im ersten Lebensjahr. In: Degenhardt, A. and Trautner, H. M. (eds.), Geschlechtstypisches Verhalten. München: C. H. Beck, pp. 122–144. Keller, H. (1980). Gaze and Gaze Aversion in the First Months of Life. Institut für Psychologie der Technischen Universitat Darmstadt, 80/5. (a) Keller, H. (1980). Beobachtung, Beschreibung und Interpretation von Eltern-Kind-Interaktionen im ersten Lebensjahr. Beobachtungsmanual für die ersten 4 Lebensmonate. Institut für Psychologie der Technischen Universitat Darmstadt, 80/8. (b) Keller, H. , Gauda, G. , and Miranda, D. (1980). Beobachtung, Beschreibung und Interpretation von Eltern-Kind- Interaktionen im ersten Lebensjahr. Skalen zur Beurteilung “Angemessenes Elternverhalten.” Institut für Psychologie der Technischen Universität Darmstadt. Kendon, A. (1977). Spatial organization in social encounters: The F-formation system. In: Kendon, A. (ed.), Studies in Semiotic. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press; Lisse: The Peter de Ridder Press. Kendon, A. and Ferber, A. (1973). A description of some human greetings. In: Michael, R. P. and Crook, J. H. (eds.), Comparative Ecology and Behaviour of Primates. London: Academic Press, pp. 591–668. Kennedy, J. M. (1980). Blind people recognizing and making haptic pictures. In: Hagen, M. (ed.), The Perception of Pictures. London/New York: Academic Press, pp. 263–303. Kennedy, J. M. (1982). Haptic Pictures. In: Schiff, W. and Foulke, E. (eds.), Tactual Perception. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 305–331. Kennedy, J. M. (1983). What can we learn about pictures from the blind? Amer. Sci. 71, 19–26. Kennell, J. , Jerauld, R. , Wolfe, H. , Chesler, D. , Kreger, N. , McAlpine, W. , Steffa, M. , and Klaus, M. (1974). Maternal behavior one year after early and extended postpartum contact. Develop. Med. Child Neurol. 16, 172–179. Kennell, J. , Trause, M. , and Klaus, M. (1975). Evidence for a sensitive period in the human mother. In: Brazelton, T. , Tronick, E. , Adamson, L. , Als, H. , and Wise, S. (eds.), Parent-Infant Interaction. New York: Elsevier/Excerpta Media/North Holland, pp. 87–101. Kephart, W. M. (1976). Extraordinary Groups. New York: St. Martin Press. Keupp, L. (1971). Aggressivität und Sexualität. München: Goldmann. Keverne, E. B. (1978). Olfactory cues in mammalian sexual behaviour. In: Hutchinson, J. P. (ed.), Biological Determinants of Sexual Behaviour. Chichester/New York: Wiley, pp. 727–763. Keverne, E. B. , Levy, F. , Poindron, P. , and Lindsay, D. R. (1983). Vaginal stimulation: An important determinant of maternal bonding in sheep. Science 219, 81–83. Kiener, F. (1986). Das Wort als Waffe. Zur Psychologie der verbalen Aggression. Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht. King, G. E. (1980). Alternative uses of primates and carnivores in the reconstruction of early hominid behavior. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 99–109. Kinsey, A. C. et al. (1966). Das sexuelle Verhalten des Mannes. Frankfurt/M.: Fischer Verlag.—English Edition (1948). Sexual Behavior in the Human Male. Philadelphia: Saunders. Kinsey, A. C. , Pomeroy, W. B. , and Martin, C. E. (1954). Das sexuelle Verhalten der Frau. Berlin/Frankfurt/M.: Fischer, Athenaeum. English Edition (1953). Sexual Behavior in the Human Female. Philadelphia: Saunders. 752 Kirkendall, L. A. (1961). Premarital Intercourse and Interpersonal Relationships. New York: Julian Press. Kirk-Smith, M. and Booth, D. A. (1980). Effect of androstenone on choice of location in other’s presence. In: van der Starre, H. (ed.), Olfaction and Taste, VII. London: IRL Press, pp. 397–400. Kirk-Smith, M. , Booth, D. A. , Carroll, D. , and Davies, P. (1978). Human Social Attitudes Affected by Androstenol. Research Communications in Psychology, Psychiat. Behav. 3, 379–384. Kistyakovskaya, M.Y. (1970). The Development of Movement in Children during the First Year of Life. Moskau: Pedagogika. Kitcher, P. (1985). Vaulting Ambition: Sociobiology and the Quest for Human Nature. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press. Kitcher, P. (1987). Precis of vaulting ambition. Behav. Brain Sci. 10, 61–100. Kitzinger, S. (1984). Sexualität im Leben der Frau. München: Biederstein. Klaus, M. and Kennell, J. (1976). Parent-to-infant attachment. In: Hull, D. (ed.), Recent Advances in Pediatrics. Edinburgh/London/New York: Churchill Livingstone, pp. 129–152. Klaus, M. , Kennell, J. , Plumb, N. , and Zuehlke, S. (1970). Human maternal behavior at the first contact with her young. Pediatrics 46, 187–192. Klaus, M. , Jerauld, R. , Kreger, N. , McAlpine, W. , Steffa, M. , and Kennell, J. (1972). Maternal attachment: Importance of the first post-partum days. New Engl. J. Med. 286, 460–463. Klaus, M. , Trause, M. , and Kennell, J. (1975). Does human maternal behaviour after delivery show a characteristic pattern? In: Brazelton, T. , Tronick, E. , Adamson, L. , Als, H. , and Wise, S. (eds.), Parent-Infant Interaction. New York: Elsevier/Excerpta Media/North Holland, pp. 69–95. Kleck, R. E. , Buck, P. L. , Golier, W. C. , London, R. S. , Pfeiffer, J. R. , and Vukcevic, D. P. (1968). Effect of stigmatizing conditions on the use of personal space. Psychol. Rep. 23, 111–118. Kleitman, N. (1963). Sleep and Wakefulness. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Kleitman, N. and Engelmann, T. C. (1953). Sleep characteristics of infants. J. Appl. Physiol. 6, 269–282. Klopfer, P. (1971). Mother love: What turns it on? Amer. Sci. 59, 404–407. Kluckhohn, C. (1947). Some aspects of Navaho infancy and early childhood. Psychoanal. Soc. Sci. 1, 37–86. Klüver, H. and Bucy, P. C. (1937). “Psychic blindness” and other symptoms following bilateral temporal lobectomy in rhesus monkeys. Amer. J. Physiol. 119, 352–353. Klutschak, H. W. (1881). Als Eskimo unter Eskimos. Wien: Hartleben. Kneutgen, J. (1964). Beobachtungen über die Anpassung von Verhaltensweisen an gleichförmige akustische Reize. Z. Tierpsychol. 21, 763–779. Kneutgen, J. (1970). Eine Musikform und ihre biologische Funktion. Über die Wirkungsweise der Wiegenlieder. Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 17, 245–265. Knowles, E. S. (1972). Boundaries around social space: Dyadic responses to an invader. Environ. Behav. 4, 437–447. Knowles, E. S. (1973). Boundaries around group interaction: The effect of group size and member status on boundary permeability. J. Personality Social Psychol. 26, 327–332. Knowles, N. (1940). The torture of captives by the Indians of Eastern North America. Proc. Amer. Phil. Soc. 82(2). Koch, K. F. (1970). Cannibalistic revenge in Jalé society. Nat. History 79, 40–51. Koch, K. F. (1974). War and Peace in Jalémo: The Management of Conflict in Highland New Guinea. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Koehler, O. (1954). Vorbedingungen und Vorstufen unserer Sprache bei Tieren. Zool. Anz. Suppl. 18, 327–341. Köhler, W. (1921). Intelligenzprüfungen an Menschenaffen. Berlin: Springer; Neudruck 1963. Koelega, H. S. and Koster, E. P. (1974). Some experiments on sex differences in odor perception. Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 237, 234–246. König, H. (1925). Der Rechtsbruch und sein Ausgleich bei den Eskimos. Anthropos 20, 276–315. Koenig, L. (1951). Beiträge zu einem Aktionssystem des Bienenfressers (Merops apiaster L.). Z. Tierpsychol. 8, 169–210. 753 Koenig, O. (1968). Biologie der Uniform. Naturwiss. Med. 5, 3–19, 40-50. Koenig, O. (1969). Verhaltensforschung und Kultur. In: Altner, G. (ed.), Kreatur Mensch. München: Moos-Verlag, pp. 57–84. Koenig, O. (1970). Kultur und Verhaltensforschung. München: Dtv. Koenig, O. (1975). Urmotiv Auge, München: Piper. Koepke, J. E. , Hamm, M. , Legerstee, M. , and Russell, M. (1983). Neonatal imitation: Two failures to replicate. Infant Behav. Develop. 6, 97–102. Kohlberg, L. A. (1966). A cognitive-developmental analysis of children’s sex-role concepts and attitudes. In: MacCoby, E. E. (ed.), The Development of Sex Differences. Stanford: Stanford University Press, pp. 82–173. Kohlberg, L. A. (1969). Stage and Sequence: The cognitive-developmental approach to socialization. In: Goslin, D. A. (ed.), Handbook of Socialization Theory and Research. Chicago: Rand McNally, pp. 347–480. Kohlberg, L. A. (1981). Essays on Moral Development. Vol. 1: The Philosophy of Moral Development: Moral Stages and the Idea of Justice. New York: Harper & Row. Kohl-Larsen, L. (1943). Auf den Spuren des Vormenschen. Deutsche Afrika-Expedition 1934-36 und 1937-39. Stuttgart: Strecker und Schröder. Kohlberg, L. A. (1958). Wildbeuter in Ostafrika. Die Tindiga, ein Jäger-und Sammlervolk. Berlin: Reimer. Kohr, L. (1983). Die überentwickelten Nationen. Salzburg: Alfred Winter. Kolodny, R. C. , et al. (1971). Plasma testosterone and semen analysis in male homosexuals. New Engl. J. Med. 285, 1170. Komisaruk, B. R. and Olds, J. (1968). Neuronal correlates of behavior in freely moving rats. Science 161, 810–812. Konecni, V. J. and Doob, A. N. (1972). Catharsis through displacement of aggression. J. Personality Social Psychol. 23, 379–387. Konecni, V. J. and Ebbesen, E. B. (1976). Disinhibition vs. the cathartic effect: Artifact and substance. J. Personality Social Psychol. 34, 352–365. Konishi, M. (1964). Effects of deafening on song development in two species of juncos. Condor 66, 85–102. Konishi, M. (1965). Effects of deafening on song development of American robins and black-headed grosbeaks. Z. Tierpsychol. 22, 584–599. (a) Konishi, M. (1965). The role of auditory feedback in the control of vocalization in the white-crowned sparrow. Z. Tierpsychol. 22, 770–783. (b) Konner, M. J. (1972). Aspects of the development of ethology of a foraging people. In: Blurton-Jones, N. G. (ed.), Ethological Studies of Child Behaviour. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 285–304. Konishi, M. (1975). Relations among infants and juveniles in comparative perspective. In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), Friendship and Peer Relations. New York: John Wiley, pp. 99–129. Konishi, M. (1977). Evolution of human behavior development. In: Leiderman, P. H. , Tulkin, St. R. and Rosenfeld, A. (eds.), Culture and Infancy, Variations in the Human Experience. New York: Academic Press, pp. 69–109. (a) Konishi, M. (1977). Infancy among the Kalahari Desert San. In: Leiderman, P. H. , Tulkin, St. R. and Rosenfeld, A. (eds.), Culture and Infancy, Variations in the Human Experience. New York: Academic Press, pp. 287–328. (b) Konishi, M. (1981). Evolution of human behavior development. In: Munroe, R. H. , Munroe, R. L. , and Whiting, B. B. (eds.), Handbook of Cross-Cultural Human Development. New York/London: Garland STPM Press, pp. 3–51. Konishi, M. (1982). The Tangled Wing. Biological Constraints on the Human Spirit. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Konner, M. J. and Worthman, C. (1980). Nursing frequencies, gonadal hormones, and birth spacing among !Kung hunter-gatherers. Science 207, 788–791. Korner, A. F. (1969). Neonatal startles, smiles, erections and reflex sucks as related to state, sex and individuality. Child Develop. 40, 1039–1053. Konishi, M. (1971). Individual differences at birth: Implications for early experience and later development. Amer. J. Orthopsych. 41, 608–619. Konishi, M. (1974). Methodological considerations in studying sex differences in the behavioral functioning of newborns. In: Friedman, R. D. , Richart, R. M. , and Vande Wiele, R. L. (eds.), Sex Differences in Behavior. New York/London: Wiley, 373-392. Kortlandt, A. (1972). New Perspectives on Ape and Human Evolution. Stichting voor Psychobiologie, Zoologisch Laboratorium Amsterdam. 754 Kortmulder, K. (1968). An ethological theory of the incest taboo and exogamy. Current Anthropol. 9, 437–449. Kovach, J. K. (1970). Critical period or optimal arousal? Early approach behavior as a function of stimulus, age, and breed variables in chicks. Develop. Psychol. 3, 88–97. Kovach, J. K. and Hess, E. H. (1963). Imprinting: Effects of painful stimulation on the following response. J. Comp. Physiol. Psychol. 56, 461–464. Krader, L. (1968). Formation of the State. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Krafft-Ebing, R. V. (1924, 17th ed.). Psychopathia sexualis. Stuttgart. Krauss, F. J. (1965). Das Geschlechtsleben des japanischen Volkes. Hanau: Schustek. Krauss, R. M. , Curran, N. M. , and Ferleger, N. (1983). Expressive conventions and the cross-cultural perception of emotion. Basic Appl. Soc. Psychol. 4, 295–305. Kraut, R. E. and Johnston, R. E. (1979). Social and emotional messages of smiling: An ethological approach. J. Personality Social Psychol. 37, 1539–1553. Krieger, D. T. (1983). Brain peptides: What, where, and why? Science 222, 975–985. Kroeber, A. L. (1915). Eighteen professions. Amer. Anthropol. 17, 283–288. Kroger, R. O. , Cheng, K. , and Leong, I. (1979). Are the rules of address universal? A test of Chinese usage. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 10, 395–414. Kruijt, J. (1964). Ontogeny of social behaviour in Burmese red jungle fowl (Gallus gallus spadiceus). Behaviour Suppl. 12. Kühn, H. (1958). Auf den Spuren des Eiszeitmenschen. München: Paul List. Küster, W. (1983). Ungeborgenheit, Isolierung und Verzweiflung. Spiegel. 43, 185–212. Kuguimutzakis, J. (1985). Imitation in Newborns 10-45 Minutes Old. Uppsala Psychological Reports No 376, Dept. Psychology Univ. Uppsala, Sweden ISSN 0348-3908. Kummer, H. (1973). Aggression bei Affen. In: Plack, A. (ed.), Der Mythos vom Aggressionstrieb. München: Paul List, pp. 69–91. Kummer, H. , Götz, W. , and Angst, W. (1974). Triadic differentiation: An inhibitory process protecting pair bonds in baboons. Behaviour 49, 62–87. Kuo, Z. Y. (1932). Ontogeny of embryonic behavior. J. Exp. Biol. 61, 395–430, 453-489. Kuo, Z. Y. (1967). The Dynamics of Behavior Development. New York: Random House. Kurland, J. A. (1979). Paternity, mother’s brother, and human sociality. In: Chagnon, N. A. and Irons, I. (eds.), Evolutionary Biology and Human Social Behavior. North Scituate, MA: Duxbury Press, pp. 145–180. Laban, R. (1956). Principles of Dance and Movement Notation. New York: Dance Horizons Republication. LaBarre, W. (1947). The cultural basis of emotions of gestures. J. Personality 16, 49–68. LaBarre, W. (1954). The Human Animal. Chicago/London: The University of Chicago Press. Laborit, H. (1980). L’inhibition de l’action. Paris: Masson. Labov, W. (1966). The Social Stratification of English in New York City. Washington: Center for Applied Linguistics. Labov, W. (1970). The study of language in its social context. Stud. Generale 23, 30–87. de Lacoste-Utamsing, Ch. , and Holloway, R. L. (1982). Sexual dimorphism in the human corpus callosum. Science 216, 1431–1432. Lagercrantz, H. and Slotkin, Th. A. (1986). The Stress of Being Born. Sci. Amer. 254 (4), 92–102. Lamb, M. E. (1975). Forgotten contributors to child development. Hum. Develop. 18, 245–266. Lamb, M. E. (1976). The role of the father: An overview. In: Lamb, M. E. (ed.), The Role of the Father in Child Development. New York: Wiley, pp. 1–63. (a) Lamb, M. E. (1976). Effects of stress and cohort on mother-and father-infant interaction. Develop. Psychol. 12, 435–443. (b) Lamb, M. E. (1976). Twelve-month olds and their parents: Interaction in a laboratory playroom. Develop. Psychol. 12, 237–244. (c) Lamb, M. E. (1977). The Relationships between Mothers, Fathers, Infants and Siblings in the First Two Years of Life. Paper presented at the Biennial Conference of the Int. Soc. for the Study of Behavioral Development, Pavia: Italien. (a) Lamb, M. E. (1977). Father-infant and mother-infant interaction in the first year of life. Child Develop. 48, 167–181. (b) Lamb, M. E. and Hwang, C. Ph. (1982). Maternal attachment and mother neonate bonding: A critical review. Advan. Develop. Psychol. 2, 1–39. 755 Lamb, M. E. , Frodi, A. M. , Hwang, C. Ph. , Frodi, M. , and Steinberg, J. (1982). Mother-and father-infant interaction involving play and holding in traditional and nontraditional Swedish families. Develop. Psychol. 18, 215–221. Lambert, W. W. (1981). Toward an integrative theory of children’s aggression. Ital. J. Psychol. 8, 153–164. Lambert, W. W. and Tan, A. L. (1979). Expressive styles and strategies in the aggressive actions of children of six cultures. Ethos 7, 19–36. Lancaster-Jones, F. (1963). A Demographic Survey of the Aboriginal Population of the Northern Territory with Special Reference to Buthurst Island Mission. Canberra: Australian Inst, of Aboriginal Studies. Lancy, D. F. (1983). Cross-cultural studies in cognition and mathematics. In: The Coevolution of Culture, Cognition and Schooling, Vol. 8. New York: Academic Press, pp. 185–211. Landy, D. and Mattee, D. (1969). Evaluation of an aggressor as a function of exposure to cartoon humor. J. Personality Social Psychol. 9, 237–241. Lane, C. (1979). Ritual and ceremony in contemporary Soviet society. Sociol. Rev. 27, 253–278. Langlois, J. H. , Roggman, L. A. , Casey, R. J. , Ritter, J. M. , Rieser-Danner, L. A. , and Jenkins, V. Y. (1987). Infant preferences for attractive faces: Rudiments of a stereotype? Develop. Psychol. 23 (3), 363–369. Lattimore, D. (1951). The steppes of Mongolia and the characteristics of steppe nomadism. In: American Geographic Society (ed.), Inner Asia Frontiers of China. New York: Capital Publ., pp. 53–102. van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1968). The behavior of free-living chimpanzees in the Gombe Stream Reserve. Anim. Behav. Monogr. 1, 161–311. van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1971). Wilde Schimpansen. Reinbek: Rowohlt. (a) van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1971). In the Shadow of Man. Boston/London: William Collins, (b) van Lawick-Goodall, J. (1975). The behavior of the chimpanzee. In: Kurth, G. and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (eds.), Hominisation und Verhalten. Stuttgart: G. Fischer, pp. 74–136. Lawler, L. B. (1962). Terpischore. The Story of the Dance in Ancient Greece. Dance Perspektives, 13. New York: Johnson’s Reprint Corp. Laver, J. (1975). Communicative function of phatic communion. In: Kendon, A. , Harris, R. , Key, R. M. , and Ritchie, M. (eds.), Organisation of Behavior in Face-to Face-Interaction, pp. 215-238 The Hague: Mouton. Layard, J. (1942). Stone Men of Malekula. London: Chatto & Windus. Leacock, E. B. (1978). Women’s status in egalitarian society: Implications for social evolution. Current Anthropol. 19, 247–275. Leacock, E. B. (1981). Myths of Male Dominance: Collected Articles on Women Cross-Culturally. Leacock, E. B. New York: Monthly Review Press. Lebzelter, V. (1934). Eingeborenenkulturen von Süd-und Südwestafrika. Leipzig: Hier-semann. LeDoux, J. E. , Wilson, D. H. , and Gazzaniga, M. S. (1977). A divided mind: Observations on the conscious properties of the separated hemisphere. Ann. Neurol. 2, 417–421. LeDoux, J. E. , Wilson, D. H. , and Gazzaniga, M. S. (1979). Beyond commissurotomy: Clues to consciousness. In: King, F. A. (ed.), Handbook of Behavioral Neurobiology, Vol. 2. New York/London: Plenum Press, pp. 543–554. Lee, R. B. (1968). What hunters do for a living. In: Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 30–48. Lee, R. B. (1969). Eating Christmas in the Kalahari. Nat. History 78, 14–22, 60-63. Lee, R. B. (1972). !Kung spatial organization. An ecological and historical perspective. Hum. Ecol. 1, 125–147. Lee, R. B. (1973). The !Kung Bushmen of Botswana. In: Bicchieri, M. G. (ed.), Hunters and Gatherers Today. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 327–368. Lee, R. B. (1976). Kung spatial organization: An ecological and historical perspective. In: Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Kalahari Hunter-Gatherers. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 73–97. Lee, R. B. (1979). The !Kung San. Men, Women and Work in a Foraging Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (1968). Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine. Lehner, P. (1979). Handbook of Ethological Methods. New York: Garland STPM Press. 756 Lehrman, D. S. (1953). A critique of Konrad Lorenz’s theory of instinctive behavior. Quart. Rev. Biol. 28, 337–363. Lehrman, D. S. (1970). Semantic and conceptual issues in the nature-nurture problem. In: Aronson, L. R. , Tobach, E. , Lehrman, D. S. , and Rosenblatt, J. S. (eds.), Development and Evolution of Behavior. San Francisco: Freeman, pp. 17–52. Leibman, M. (1970). The effects of sex and race norms on personal space. Environ. Behav. 2, 208–246. Leifer, A. , Leiderman, P. , Barnett, C. , and Williams, J. (1972). Effects of mother-infant separation on maternal attachment behavior. Child Develop. 43, 1203–1218. LeMagnen, J. (1952). Les phenomenes olfacto-sexuels chez l’homme. Arch. Sci. Physiol. 6, 125–160. Lenneberg, E. H. (1964). A biological perspective of language. In: Lenneberg, E. H. (ed.), New Directions in the Study of Language. Cambridge, MA: M. I. T. Press, pp. 65–88. Lenneberg, E. H. (1967). Biological Foundations of Language. New York/London: John Wiley. Lenneberg, E. H. (1974). Ein Wort unter uns. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, H. M. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie, Bd. 2. Frankfurt/M.: Fischer Athenaeum, pp. 53–70. English Edition (1969). A Word Between Us. In: Roslansky, J. D. (ed.), Communication. Amsterdam/London: Elsevier, pp. 108–131. Leonhard, K. (1966). Uber die Entstehung einer Form von Homosexualität durch ein Prägungserlebnis. Leopoldina 12, 44–152. Leontev, A. N. (1977). Activity, Consciousness, and Personality. Moskau: Gospolitizdat. Leung, E. H. L. , and Rheingold, H. L. (1981). Development of pointing as a social gesture. Develop. Psychol. 17, 215–220. Levelt, W. J. M. (1987). Hochleistung in Millisekunden-Sprechen und Sprache verstehen. Max Planck-Gesellschaft, Jahrbuch 1987, pp. 61-77. Ed. Generalverwaltung der Max-Planck-Gesellschaft München: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht, Göttingen. Levi-Montalcini, R. (1981). Views on human aggressive behavior and on wars. In: Valzelli, I. and Morgese, I. (eds.), Aggression and Violence: A Psycho/Biological and Clinical Approach. Milano: Edizioni Saint Vincent, pp. 21–30. Levi-Strauss, C. (1949). Les Structures Elémentaires de la Parenté. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France. Levi-Strauss, C. (1969). The Elementary Structures of Kinship. Boston: Beacon Press. Levy, J. (1972). Lateral specialization of the human brain: Behavioral manifestations and possible evolutionary basis. In: Kiger, J. A. (ed.), The Biology of Behavior, Corvallis, OR: Oregon State University Press. Levi-Strauss, C. (1978). Lateral differences in the human brain in cognition and behavioral control. In: Buser, P. (ed.), Cerebral Correlates of Conscious Experience. New York/Amsterdam: North Holland Publ., pp. 285–298. Lewis, H. S. (1981). Warfare and the origin of the state. In: Claessen, H. J. M. and Skalnik, P. (eds.), The Study of the State. The Hague: Mouton. Lewis, M. (1969). Infants’ responses to facial stimuli during the first year of life. Develop. Psychol. 1, 75–86. Lewis, M. and Weintraub, M. (1974). Sex of parent x sex of child—Socio-emotional development. In: Friedman, R. C. , Richart, R. M. , and Vande Wiele, R. L. (eds.), Sex Differences in Behavior. New York/London: Wiley, pp. 165–189. Lewis, M. , Young, G. , Brooks, J. , and Michaelson, L. (1975). The beginning of friendships. In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), Friendship and Peer Relations. New York: Wiley. Lewis, R. A. (1972). A developmental framework for the analysis of premarital dyadic formation. Family Process 11, 17–48. Lewontin, R. C. (1974). The Genetic Basis of Evolutionary Change. New York/London: Columbia University Press. Levi-Strauss, C. (1977). Sociobiology—A caricature of Darwinism. In: Suppe, F. and Asquath, P. (eds.), PSA 1976, Vol. 2. PSA Lansing, MI. Lex, B. W. (1979). The neurobiology of ritual trance. In: D’ Aquili, E. G. , Laughlin, C. D. , and McManus, J. (eds.), New York: Columbia Univ. Press, pp. 117–151. Ley D. , and Cybriwsky, R. (1974). Urban graffiti as territorial markers. Ann. Assoc. Amer. Geogr. 64, 491–505. 757 Ley, R. G. and Koepke, J. E. (1982). Attachment behavior outdoors: Naturalistic observations of sex and age differences in the separation behavior of young children. Infant Behav. Develop. 5, 195–201. Leyhausen, P. (1965). Über die Funktion der relativen Stimmungshierarchie (dargestellt am Beispiel der phylogenetischen und ontogenetischen Entwicklung des Beutefangs von Raubtieren). Z. Tierpsychol. 22, 412–494. Leyhausen, P. (1983). Kleidung: Schutzhülle, Selbstdarstellung, Ausdrucksmittel. In: Sitta, B. (ed.), Menschliches Verhalten, seine biologischen und kulturellen Komponenten, untersucht an den Phänomenen Arbeitsteilung und Kleidung. Freiburg/Schweiz: Universitätsverlag. Liberman, A. M. and Pisoni, D. B. (1977). Evidence for a special speech-perceiving subsystem in the human. In: Bullock, T. H. (ed.), Recognition of Complex Acoustic Signals. Life Sciences Research Report, 5, Berlin-Dahlem- Konferenzen. Berlin: Abakon, pp. 59–76. Lieberman, A. F. (1977). Preschoolers’ competence with a peer: Relations with attachment and peer experience. Child Develop. 48, 1277–1287. Lieberman, P.H. (1977). The phylogeny of language. In: Sebeok, T. A. (ed.), How Animals Communicate. Bloomington, IN: Indiana University Press, pp. 3–25. Liebowitz, M. R. (1983). The Chemistry of Love. Boston/Toronto: Little Brown. Lief, H. (1976). Introduction to sexuality. In: Sadock, B. J. , Kaplan, H. I. , and Freedman, A. M. (eds.), The Sexual Experience. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, pp. 1–6. Liegle, L. (1971). Familie und Kollektiv im Kibbuz. Weinheim: Julius Meltz Verlag. Limber, J. (1980). Language in child and chimp? In: Sebeok, T. A. and Umiker-Sebeok, J. (eds.), Speaking of Apes. New York: Plenum. Lind, J. , Vuorenkoski, V. , and Wasz-Hoeckert, O. (1973). The effect of cry stimulus on the temperature of the lactating breast of primiparae: A thermographic study. In: Morris, N. (ed.), Psychosomatic Medicine in Obstetrics and Gynaecology. Basel: S. Karger. Lipetz, V. E. and Bekoff, M. (1982). Group size and vigilance in pronghorns, Z. Tierpsychol. 58, 203–216. Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. K. (eds.) (1983). Advan. Infancy Res. 2. Lisina, M. I. (1982). The development of interaction in the first seven years of life. In: Hartup, W. W. (ed.), Review of Child Development Research, Vol. VI. Chicago: The Univ. of Chicago Press, pp. 133–174. Lisker, L. and Abramson, A. S. (1964). A cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: Accoustical measurements. Word 20, 384–422. Livi, L. (1949). Considérations théorétiques et pratiques sur le concept de “minimum de population.” Population 4, 754–756. Lockard, J. S. (1980). Evolution of Human Social Behavior. New York: Elsevier, (a) Lockard, J. S. (1980). Studies of human social signals: Theory, method and data. In: Lockard, J. S. (ed.), The Evolution of Human Social Behavior. New York: Elsevier, (b) Lockard, J. S. , Daley, P. C. , and Gunderson, V. M. (1979). Maternal and paternal differences in infant carry: US and African data. Amer. Natur. 113, 235–246. Lötsch, B. (1981). Ökologische Überlegungen für Gebiete hoher baulicher Dichte. Inform. Raumentwick. 7/8, 415-433. Lötsch, B. (1984). Auf der Suche nach dem menschlichen Maβ. Teil 1: Habitatsgestaltung für den Homo sapiens. Garten und Landschaft, H. 1, 34-40. (a) Lötsch, B. (1984). Auf der Suche nach dem menschlichen Maβ. Teil 2: Wohnbaualternativen. Menschensilo oder Schrebergarten. Garten und Landschaft, H. 6. (b) Logstrup, K. E. (1973). Asthetische Erfahrung in Dichtung und bildender Kunst. In: Gadamer, H. G. and Vogler, P. (eds.), Neue Anthropologic 4, Kulturanthropologie. Stuttgart: Georg Thieme, pp. 286–320. Lommel, A. (1962). Motiv und Variation in der Kunst des zirkumpazifischen Raumes. Publikationen des Staatl. Museums für Völkerkunde, München. Lorenz, K. (1935). Der Kumpan in der Umwelt des Vogels. J. Ornithol. 83, 137–413. Lorenz, K. (1937). Über die Bildung des Instinktbegriffes. Naturwissenschaften 25, 289–300, 307-318, 325-331. Lorenz, K. (1941). Vergleichende Bewegungsstudien an Anatiden. J. Ornithol. 89, 194–294. Lorenz, K. (1943). Psychologie und Stammesgeschichte. In: Heberer, G. (ed.), Die Evolution der Organismen. Jena: Gustav Fischer, pp. 105–127. (a) Lorenz, K. (1943). Die angeborenen Formen möglicher Erfahrung. Z. Tierpsychol. 5, 235–409. (b) 758 Lorenz, K. (1950). Ganzheit und Teil in der tierischen und menschlichen Gemeinschaft. Stud. Generate 9, 555–599. Lorenz, K. (1953). Die Entwicklung der vergleichenden Verhaltensforschung in den letzten 12 Jahren. Zool. Anz. Suppl. 16, 36–58. Lorenz, K. (1959). Die Gestaltwahrnehmung als Quelle wissenschaftlicher Erkenntnis. Z. Angew. Exp. Psychol. 6, 118–165. Lorenz, K. (1961). Phylogenetische Anpassung und adaptive Modifikation des Verhaltens. Z. Tierpsychol. 18, 139–187. Lorenz, K. (1963). Das sogenannte Böse. Wien: Borotha Schoeler. Lorenz, K. (1965). Evolution and Modification of Behavior. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press. Lorenz, K. (1966). Stammes-und kulturgeschichtliche Ritenbildung. Mitteil. Max-Planck Ges. 1, 3–30; Naturwiss. Rundschau 19, 361-370. Lorenz, K. (1971). Der Mensch biologisch gesehen. Eine Antwort an Wolfgang Schmidbauer. Stud. Generale 24, 495–515. Lorenz, K. (1973). Die Rückseite des Spiegels. Versuch einer Naturgeschichte menschlichen Erkennens. München: Piper. Lorenz, K. (1978). Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung. Wien/New York: Springer. Lorenz, K. (1983). Der Abbau des Menschlichen. München/Zürich: Piper. Lorenz, K. and Tinbergen, N. (1939). Taxis und Instinkthandlung in der Eirollbewegung der Graugans. Z. Tierpsychol. 2, 1–29. Lorenz, K. and Wuketits, F. M. (eds.) (1983). Die Evolution des Denkens. München/Zürich: Piper. Loupis, G. (1983). The Kaluli Longhouses. Oceania 53, 358–383. Luckmann, B. C. and Luckmann, T. (1977). Gespräch und Unterhaltung. In: Reden und reden lassen. Stuttgart/Hamburg/München. Luers, F. (1919). Volkskundliches aus Steinberg beim Achensee in Tirol. Bayr. Hefte Volksk. 4, 106–130. Luhmann, N. (1982). Liebe als Passion. Zur Codierung der Intimität. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp. Lumholtz, C. (1890). Among Cannibals. An Account of Four Years’ Travels in Australia and of Camp Life with the Aborigines of Queensland. London. McAllister, L. B. , Scheller, R. H. , Kandel, E. R. , and Axel, R. (1983). In situ hybridization to study the origin and fate of identified neurons. Science 222, 800–808. McBride, G. , King, M. G. , and James, J. W. (1965). Social proximity effects on galvanic skin responses in adult humans. J. Psychol. 61, 153–157. McBurney, D. H. , Levine, J. M. , and Cavanaugh, P. H. (1977). Psychophysical and social ratings of human body odour. Personality Social Psychol. Bull. 3, 135–138. McCabe, J. (1983). FDB marriage: Further support for the Westermarck hypothesis of the incest taboo? Amer. Anthropol. 85, 50–69. McCarthy, F. and McArthur, M. (1960). The food quest and the time factor in aboriginal economic life. In: Mountford, C. P. (ed.), Records of the American-Australian Scientific Expedition to Arnhem Land. Melbourne. McCauley, C. and Taylor, J. (1976). Is there overload of acquaintances in the city? Environ. Psychol. Nonverbal Behav. 1, 41–55. McClintock, M. K. (1971). Menstrual synchrony and suppression. Nature 229, 244–245. MacCluer, J. W. and Dyke, B. (1976). On the minimum size of endogamous populations. Social Biol. 23, 1–12. MacCoby, E. E. and Jacklin, C. N. (1974). The Psychology of Sex Differences. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. MacCoby, J. (1980). Sex differences in aggression: A rejoinder and reprise. Child Develop. 51, 964–980. MacDonald, K. (1983). Production, social controls, and ideology: Toward a sociobiology of the phenotype. J. Soc. Biol. Structures 6, 297–317. McDonald, D. L. (1978). Paternal behaviour at first contact with the newborn in a birth environment without intrusions. Birth Family J. 5, 123–132. McDonald, E. R. (1981). The cultural roots of ideology: Hagen’s concept of honor in the Nibelungenlied. Mankind Quart. 21, 179–204. MacFarlane, J. A. (1975). The Psychology of Childbirth. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. 759 MacFarlane, J. A. (1977). Olfaction in the development of social preferences in the human neonate. In: The Human Neonate in Parent-Infant Interaction. Ciba Found. Symp. 33, 103–117. McGraw, M. B. (1943). Neuromuscular Maturation of the Human Infant. New York: Columbia University Press. McGrew, W. C. (1972). An Ethological Study of Children’s Behavior. London: Academic Press. McGrew, W. C. (1975). Patterns of plant food sharing by wild chimpanzees. Proc. Intern. Congr. Primatol. 5, 304–309. McGuinness, D. (1981). Auditory and motor aspects of language development in males and females. In: Ansara, A. et al. (eds.), The Significance of Sex Differences in Dyslexia. Towson: The Orton Soc., Inc. McGuinness, D. and Pribram, K. H. (1979). The origins of sensory bias in the development of gender differences in perception and cognition. In: Bortner, M. , et al. (eds.), Cognitive Growth and Development: Essays in Memory of Herbert G. Birch. New York: Brunner/Mazel, pp. 3–56. McGuire, M. T. and Raleigh, M. J. (1986). Behavioral and physiological correlates of ostracism. Ecol. Sociobiol. 7, 149–156. McKenzie, B. and Over, R. (1983). Young infants fail to imitate facial and manual gestures. Infant Behav. Develop. 6, 85–95. Mackey, W. C. (1979). Parameters of the adult-male-child bond. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 59–76. McKnight, D. (1982). Conflict, healing and singing in an Australian aboriginal community. Anthropos 77, 491–508. McLean, I. G. (1983). Paternal behaviour and killing of young in arctic ground squirrels. Anim. Behav. 31, 32–44. MacLean, P. D. (1970). The Triune brain, emotion and scientific bias. In: Schmitt, F. O. , Quarton, G. C. , Melnechuk, Th. , and Adelman, G. (eds.), The Neurosciences. 2nd study program. New York: The Rockefeller University Press, pp. 336–349. McNally, R. J. (1987). Preparedness and phobias: A review. Psychol. Bull. 101 (2), 283–303. McNeill, D. (1974). Vorsymbolische Sprache. Anfänge der Grammatikentwicklung. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie, Bd. 2. Frankfurt/M.: Fischer Athenäum, pp. 110–121. McNeill, D. (1980). Sentence structure in chimpanzee communication. In: Sebeok, T. A. and Umiker-Sebeok, J. (eds.), Speaking of Apes. New York: Plenum, pp. 145–160. Madsen, M. C. and Lancy, D. F. (1981). Cooperative and competitive behavior: Experiments related to ethnic identity and urbanization in Papua New Guinea. J. Cross-Cultural Psychol. 12, 389–408. Major, B. and Heslin, R. (1982). Perceptions of cross-sex and same-sex nonreciprocal touch: It is better to give than to receive. J. Nonverbal Behav. 6, 148–162. Malinowski, B. (1920). War and weapons among the natives of the Trobriand Islands. Man 20(5). Malinowski, B. (1922). Argonauts of the Western Pacific. New York: Dutton. Malinowski, B. (1923). The problem of meaning in primitive languages. In: Ogden, C. K. and Richards, I.A. (eds.), The Meaning of Meaning. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul, pp. 296–336. Malinowski, B. (1926). Crime and Custom in Savage Society. New York: Harcourt, Brace & Co., London: Kegan Paul, Trench, Trubner. Malinowski, B. (1929). The Sexual Life of Savages in North-Western Melanesia. New York: P. R. Reynolds. Mallick, S. K. and McCandless, B. R. (1966). A study of catharsis of aggression. J. Personality Social Psychol. 4, 591–596. Malthus, Th. R. (1978). Essay on the Principle of Population. London: Johnson. Maning, F. E. (1876). Old New Zealand: A Tale of the Good Old Times; and a History of the War in the North. London. Marcuse, H. (1967). Das Ende der Utopie. Berlin (V. Maikowski, Kleine revolutionäre Bibliothek 6). (a) Marcuse, H. (1967). Der eindimensionale Mensch. Neuwied/Berlin: Luchterhand. (b) 760 Margolese, M. S. (1970). Homosexuality: A new endocrine correlate. Horm. Behav. 1, 151. Margulies, S. (1977). Principles of beauty. Psychol. Rep. 41, 3–11. Markl, H. (1974). Die Evolution des sozialen Denkens der Tiere. In: Immelmann, K. (ed.), Verhaltensforchung. München: Kindler. (Supplement of Bernhard Grzimek, Tierleben.) Markl, H. (1976). Aggression und Altruismus. Coevolution der Gegensätze im Sozialverhalten der Tiere. Konstanzer Universitätsreden. Konstanz: Universitäts-Verlag. Markl, H. (1980). Ökologische Grenzen und Evolutionsstrategie-Forschung. Festvortrag gehalten am 19. Juni 1980 in der Rheinischen Friedrich-Wilhelms-Universität zu Bonn anläβlich der Jahresversammlung der DFG. Mitteilungen der DFG 3/80. Marler, P. (1976). Sensory templates in species-specific behavior. In: Fentress, J. (ed.), Simpler Networks and Behavior. Sunderland, MA: Sinauer Assoc. Marler, P. (1978). Perception and Innate Knowledge. Proc. 13th Nobel Conf. “The Nature of Lifer” pp. 111–139. Marler, P. (1979). Development of auditory perception in relation to vocal behavior. In: Cranach, M. V. , Foppa, K. , Lepenies, W. , and Ploog, D. (eds.), Human Ethology. London/ New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 663–681. Marler, P. , and Peters, S. (1977). Selective vocal learning in a sparrow. Science 198, 519–521. Marler, P. Zoloth, St., and Dooling, R. (1981). Innate programs for perceptual development: An ethological view. In: Gollin, E. S. (ed.), Developmental Plasticity. Behavioral and Biological Aspects of Variations in Development. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 135–172. Marmor, J. (1976). Homosexuality and sexual orientation disturbances. In: Sadock, B. J. , Kaplan, H. J. , and Freedman, A. M. (eds.), The Sexual Experience. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins, pp. 374–391. Marr, D. (1982). Vision. New York: W. H. Freeman. Marshall, D. S. (1971). Sexual behavior on Mangaia. In: Marshall, D. S. and Suggs, R. C. (eds.), Human Sexual Behavior. New York: Basic Books, pp. 103–162. Marshall, L. (1959). Marriage Among the !Kung Bushmen. Africa 29, 335–365. Marshall, L. (1960). !Kung Bushmen Bands. Africa 30, 325–355. Marshall, L. (1965). The !Kung Bushmen of the Kalahari Desert. In: Gibbs, J. L. (ed.), Peoples of Africa. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 241–278. Marshall, L. (1976). The !Kung of Nyae Nyae. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Martin, P. and Caro, T. M. (1985). On the functions of play and its role in behavioral development. Advan. Study Behav. 15, 59–103. Massie, H. N. (1980). Pathological interactions in infancy. In: Field, T. M. (ed.), High Risk Infants and Children Adult and Peer Interactions. New York/London: Academic Press, pp. 79–97. Masters, R. D. (1976). The impact of ethology on political science. In: Somit, A. (ed.), Biology and Politics. The Hague/Paris: Mouton, pp. 197–233. Masters, R. D. (1981). Evolutionary Biology and the Welfare State. Vortrag anläβlich der Jahresversammlung der American Political Science Association, Sept., New York, (a) Masters, R. D. (1981). Linking ethology and political science: Photographs, political attention, and presidential elections. In: Watts, M. W. (ed.), Biopolitics: Ethological and Physiological Approaches. New Directions for Methodology of Social and Behavioral Science, No. 7. San Francisco: Jossey Bass, pp. 61–80. Masters, R. D. (1981). Empirical Analysis of Photographs in Presidential Campaigns. Vortrag auf der Jahresversammlung der American Political Science Association, Sept., New York. Masters, R. D. (1982). Is sociobiology reactionary? The political implication of inclusive-fitness theory. Quart. Rev. Biology , 57, 275–292. Masters, R. D. (1983). Ethologische Ansätze in der Politikwissenschaft. In: Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 80–101. Masters, W. H. (1960). Sexual response cycle of human female. West. J. Surg. 54, 93–120. Masters, W. H. and Johnson, V. E. (1966). Human Sexual Response. Boston: Little Brown. Masters, W. H. and Johnson, V. E. (1970). Human Sexual Inadequacy. Boston: Little Brown. Matas, L. , Arend, R. A. , and Sroufe, L. A. (1978). Continuity of adaption in the second year: The relationship between quality and later competence. Child Develop. 49, 547–556. 761 Mattingly, I. G. (1972). Speech cues and sign stimuli. Amer. Sci. 60, 327. Maurer, D. (1985). Infants’ perception of facedness. In: Field, T. and Fox, N. A. (eds.), Social Perception in Infancy. New Jersey: Ablex, pp. 73–100. Maurer, D. and Salapatek, P. (1976). Developmental changes in the scanning of faces by young infants. Child Develop. 47, 523–527. Mauss, M. (1968). Die Gabe. Form und Funktion des Austausches in archaischen Gesell-schaften. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp. Maxwell-West, M. and Konner, M. J. (1976). The role of the father: An anthropological perspective. In: Lamb, M. E. (ed.), The Role of the Father in Child Development. New York: Wiley, pp. 185–217. Maynard-Smith, J. (1964). Group selection and kin selection. Nature 201, 1145–1147. Maynard-Smith, J. (1974). The theory of games and the evolution of animal conflict. J. Theoret. Biol. 47, 209–221. Maynard-Smith, J. (1984). Game theory and the evolution of behaviour. Behav. Brain Sci. 7, 95–125. Maynard-Smith, J. and Price, G. R. (1973). The logic of animal conflicts. Nature 246, 15–18. Mayr, E. (1950). Ecological factors in speciation. Evolution 1, 263–288. Mayr, E. (1970). Evolution und Verhalten. Verhandl. Deut. Zool. Ges. , 64, 322–336. Mayr, E. (1976). Evolution and the Diversity of Life. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press, Harvard Univ. Press. Mazur, A. (1976). Effects of testosterone on status in small groups. Folia Primatol. 26, 214–226. Mazur, A. (1977). Interpersonal spacing on public benches in “contact” vs. “noncontact” cultures. J. Social Psychol. 101, 53–58. Mazur, A. and Lamb, Th. A. (1980). Testosterone, status, and mood in human males. Hormones Behav. 14, 236–246. Mazur, A. , Rosa, E. , Faupel, M. , Heller, J. , Leen, R. , and Thurman, B. (1980). Physiological aspects of communication via mutual gaze. Amer. J. Sociol. 86, 50–74. Mead, M. (1930). Growing up in New Guinea. New York: William Morrow. Mead, M. (1935). Sex and Temperament in Three Primitive Societies. New York: William Morrow. Mead, M. (1949). Male and Female., New York: William Morrow. Mead, M. (1965). Leben in der Südsee. München: Szczesny. Meadows, D. L. , Meadows, D. H. , Zahn, E. , and Milling, P. (1972). Grenzen des Wachstums. Stuttgart: Deutsche Verlagsanstalt. Meaney, M. J. , and Stewart, J. (1985). Sex differences in social play: The socialization of sex roles. Advan. Study Behav. 15, 1–58. Megargee, E. I. (1969). Influence of sex roles on the manifestations of leadership. J. Appl. Psychol. 53, 377–382. Megitt, M. J. (1962). Desert People. Sydney: Angus & Robertson. Megitt, M. J. (1965). Desert People. A Study of the Walbiri Aborigines of Central Australia. Chicago/London: University of Chicago Press, (a) Megitt, M. J. (1965). The Lineage System of the Mae Enga of New Guinea. New York: Barnes & Noble, (b) Meiselman, K. C. (1979). Incest. San Francisco: Jossey Bass. Melrose, D. R. , Reed, H. C. B. , and Patterson, R. L. S. (1971). Androgen steroids associated with boar odour as an aid to the detection of oestrus in pig artificial insemination. Brit. Vet. J. 127, 495–502. Meltzoff, A. N. (1981). Imitation, intermodal cooperation and representation in early infancy. In: Butterworth, G. (ed.), Infancy and Epistomology. Brighton: Harvester Press, pp. 88–114. Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1977). Imitation of facial expression and manual gestures by human neonates. Science 198, 75–78. Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1983). The Origins of Imitation in Infancy: Paradigm, Phenomena, and Theories. In: Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee-Collier, C. K. (eds.), Advances in Infancy Research, Vol. 2. Norwood, NJ: Ablex Publ. Corp., pp. 265–301. (a) Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1983). Methodological Issues in Studies of Imitation: Comments on McKenzie and Over and Koepke et al. Infant Behav. Develop. 6, 103–108. (b) Meltzoff, A. N. and Moore, M. K. (1983). Newborn infants imitate adult facial gestures. Child Develop. 54, 702–709. (c) Mendelson, M. J. and Haith, M. M. (1976). The relation between audition and vision in the human newborn. Monogr. Soc. Res. Child Develop. 41, 1–72. 762 Menn, L. (1976). Pattern, Control, and Contrast in Beginning Speech: A Case Study in the Development of Word Norm and Word Function. Thesis, University of Illinois. Merz, F. (1965). Aggression und Aggressionstrieb. In: Thomae, H. (ed.), Handbuch der Psychologie, Bd. 2: Motivation. Göttingen: C. J. Hogrefe, pp. 569–601. Metzger, W. (1936). Gesetze des Sehens. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp; 2nd ed., 1954. (1953). Psychologie. Darmstadt: Steinkopff. Meyer, P. (1983). Macht und Gewalt im Evolutionsprozeβ. Eine biosoziologische Perspektive. In: Flohr, H. and Tönnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 60–68. Meyer-Holzapfel, M. (1956). Das Spiel bei Säugetieren. Handb. Zool. 8 (10), 1–36. Michael, R. P. , Bonsall, R. W. , and Warner, P. (1975). Human vaginal secretions: Volatile fatty acid content. Science 186, 1217–1219. Michel, Th . (1983). Interdependenz von Wirtschaft und Umwelt in der Eipo-Kultur von Moknerkon: Bedingungen für Produktion und Reproduktion bei einer Dorfschaft im zentralen Bergland von Irian Jaya (West-Neuguinea), Indonesien. In: Helfrich, K. , Jacobshage, V. , Koch, G. , Krieger, K. , Schiefenhövel, W. , and Schultz, W. (eds.), Mensch, Kultur und Umwelt im zentralen Bergland von West-Neuguinea, Vol. 11. Berlin: Reimer. Midgley, M. (1979). Gene-Juggling. Philosophy 54, 439–458. Mikhailov, F. T. (1976). The Riddle of the Human Ego. Moskau: Molodaya gvardiya. Milgram, St . (1963). Behavioral study of obedience. J. Abnormal Social Psychol 67, 372–378. Milgram, St . (1966). Einige Bedingungen von Autoritätsgehorsam und seiner Verweigerung. Z. Exp. Angew. Psychol. 13, 433–463. Milgram, St . (1974). Obedience to Authority: An Experimental View. New York: Harper & Row. Miller, A. (1980). Am Anfang war Erziehung. Frankfurt/M: Suhrkamp. Miller, G. A. (1956). The magical number seven, plus or minus two: Some limits on our capacity for processing information. Psychol. Rev. 63, 81–97. Miller, G. A. (1974). Einige psychologische Aspekte der Grammatik. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie, Bd. 1. Frankfurt/M: Fischer Athenäum, pp. 3-31. (a) Miller, G. A. (1974). Vier philosophische Probleme der Psycholinguistik. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie, Bd. 2. Frankfurt/M: Fischer Athenäum, pp. 215-238. (b) Mills, J. N. (1974). Development of circadian rhythms in infancy. In: Davis, J. A. and Dobbing, J. (eds.), Scientific Foundations of Pediatrics. London: Heinemann. Mills, M. and Melhuish, E. (1974). Recognition of mother’s voice in early infancy. Nature , 252, 123–124. Minturn, L. and Lambert, W. W. (1969). Mothers of Six Cultures. New York/London: Wiley. Mitford, N. (1956). Noblesse Oblige. An Enquiry into the Identifiable Characteristics of the English Aristocracy. New York/London: Harper & Row. Mitscherlich, M. (1984). Die Bedeutung des Übergangsobjektes für die Entfaltung des Kindes. In: Eggers, Ch. (ed.), Bindungen und Besitzdenken beim Kleinkind. München/Wien/Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 185–203. Model, P. G. , Bornstein, M. B. , Crain, St. M. , and Pappas, G. D. (1971). An electron microscopic study of the development of synapsis in cultured fetal mouse cerebrum continuously exposed to xylocaine. J. Cell Biol 49, 362–371. Moewes, W. (1978). Stadt-Land-Verbund. Ein neues Leitbild für eine zukünftige Sied-lungsstruktur. Schriftenreihe Wissenschaft und Technik, Technische Universität Darmstadt, 175-218. Mohnot, S. M. (1971). Some aspects of social changes and infant-killing in the hanuman Langur Presbytis entellus (Primates: Cercopithecidae) in Western India. Mammalia 35, 175–198. Mohr, A. (1971). Häuflgkeit und Lokalisation von Frakturen und Verletzungen am Skelett vor-und frühgeschichtlicher Menschengruppen. Ethnogr. Archäol. Z. 12, 139-142. Mohr, H. (1979). Wissenschaft und Ethik. In: Hassenstein, B. , Mohr, H. , Osche, G. , Sander, K. , and Wülker, W. (eds.), Freiburger Vorlesungen zur Biologie des Menschen. Heidelberg: Quelle & Meyer, pp. 184–221. 763 Moir, C. (1934). Recording the contradictions of human pregnant and non-pregnant uterus. Trans. Edinburgh Obst. Soc. 54, 93–120. Monberg, T. (1979/1980). Self-abasement as part of a social process. Folk 21–22, 125–132. Money, J. (1960). Phantom orgasm in the dreams of paraplegic men and women. Arch. General Psychiatry 3, 373–382. Money, J. and Daléry, M. D. (1976). Iatrogenic homosexuality: Gender identity in seven 46, XX chromosomal females with hyperadrenocortical hermaphroditism born with a penis, three reared as boys, four reared as girls. J. Homosexuality 1 (4), 357–371. Money, J. and Ehrhardt, A. A. (1972). Man and Woman, Boy and Girl: The Differentiation and Dimorphism of Gender Identity from Conception to Maturity. Baltimore: Johns Hopkins University Press. Montagner, H. (1978). L’enfant et la communication. Paris: Pernoud-Stock. Montagu, A. (1976). The Nature of Human Aggression. New York: Oxford University Press. Montagu, A. (1971). Touching: The Human Significance of the Skin. New York/London: Columbia University Press. Montross, L. (1944). War through the Ages. New York/London: Harper & Brothers. Moore, J. (1984). The evolution of reciprocal sharing. Ethol. Sociobiol. 5, 5–14. Moore, M. M. (1985). Nonverbal courtship patterns in women: Context and consequences. Ethol. Sociobiol. 6, 237–247. Morath, M. (1977). Differences in the non-crying vocalizations of infants in the first four months of life. Neuropädiatrie 8, 543–545. Moreno, F. B. (1942). Sociometric status of children in a nursery school group. Sociometry 4, 395–411. Morey, R. V., Jr. and Marwitt, J. P. (1973). Ecology, Economy and Warfare in Lowland South America. Paper read at the 9th Intern. Cong, of Anthropological and Ethnological Sciences, Aug./Sept. 1973, Chicago. Morgan, B. J. T. , Simpson, M. J. A. , Hanby, J. P. , and Hall-Craggs, J. (1976). Visualizing interaction and sequential data in animal behavior: Theory and application of cluster analysis methods. Behaviour 56, 1–43. Morgan, Ch. J. (1979). Eskimo hunting groups, social kinship, and the possibility of kin selection in humans. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 83–86. Morgan, L. H. (1877). Ancient Society, or Researches in the Lines of Human Progress from Savagerty, Through Barbarism to Civilisation. London: Macmillan. Morgan, S. W. and Mausner, B. (1973). Behavioral and fantasied indicators of avoidance of success in men and women. J. Personality 41, 457–470. Morinaga, S. (1933). Untersuchungen über die Zöllnersche Täuschung. Jap. J. Psychol. 8, 195–242. Morishita, H. and Siegfried, W. (1983). Erkennen von Emotionen in der Mimik von Kabuki-Schauspielern. Vergleich japanischer und europäischer Einstufungen von 17 Bildern. Unveröffentlichtes Manuskript. Morris, D. (1957). “Typical intensity” and its relation to the problem of ritualization. Behaviour 11, 1–1. Morris, D. (1963). Biologie der Kunst. Düsseldorf: Rauch Verlag. Morris, D. (1968). The Naked Ape: A Zoologist’s Study of the Human Animal. New York: McGraw-Hill. Morris, D. (1977). Manwatching: A Field Guide to Human Behavior. New York: Abrams; London: Jonathan Cape; Lausanne: Elsevier. Morris, D. (1981). Das Spiel—Faszination und Ritual des Fuβballs. München/Zürich: Droemer-Knaur. Morris, D. , Collett, P. , Marsh, P. , and O’Shaughnessy, M. (1979). Gestures, Their Origins and Distribution. London: Jonathan Cape. Moss, H. A. (1974). Early sex differences and mother-infant interactions. In: Friedman, R. C. , Richart, R. M. , and Vande Wiele, R. L. (eds.), Sex Differences in Behavior. London/New York: Wiley, pp. 149–163. Moyer, K. E. (1968/1969). Internal impulses to aggression. Trans. NY Acad. Sci. (2nd Ser) 31, 104–114. Moyer, K. E. (1971). Experimental Grundlagen eines physiologischen Modells aggressiven Ver-haltens. In: Schmidt- Mummendey, A. and Schmidt, H. D. (eds.), Aggressives Verhalten. München: Juventa. (a) 764 Moyer, K. E. (1971). The Physiology of Hostility. Chicago: Markham. (b) Moyer, K. E. (1981). A Physiological model of aggression with implications for control. In: Valzelli, I. and Morgese, I. (eds.), Aggression and Violence: A Psycho/Biological and Clinical Approach. Milano: Edizione Saint Vincent, pp. 72–81. Moyer, K. E. (1987). Violence and Aggression: A Physiological Perspective. New York: Paragon House. Mühlmann, W. E. (1940). Krieg und Frieden. In: Kulturgeschichtliche Bibliothek, Rh. , Bd. 2. Heidelberg: Winter’s Universitäts-Buchhdlg. Mueller, E. (1979). (Toddlers + toys) = (An autonomous social system). In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. A. (eds.), The Child and Its Family. New York/London: Plenum, pp. 169–194. Mueller, E. and Brenner, J. (1977). The origins of social skills and interaction among playgroup toddlers. Child Develop. 48, 854–861. Müller, H. and Kühne, K. (1980). Zur Analyse interaktiver Episoden. Unveröffentlichte Lizentiatsarbeit, Universität Bern 1974. In: Cranach, M. V. , Kalbermatten, U. , In-Dermühle, K. , and Gugler, B. (eds.), Zielgerichetes Handeln. Bern/Stuttgart/Wien: Hans Huber, pp. 186 ff. Müller, K. E. (1984). Die bessere und die schlechtere Hälfte: Ethnologie des Geschlech-terkonflikts. Frankfurt/New York: Campus Verlag. Müller-Schwarze, D. (ed.) (1978). Evolution of Play Behaviour. Stroutsburg, PA: Dowden, Hutchinson & Ross. Müsch, H. (1976). Exhibitionismus, Phalluskult und Genitalpräsentieren. Sexualmedizin , 5, 358–363. Muncy, R. J. (1973). Sex and Marriage in Utopian Communities: 19th Century America. Bloomington, IN: University of Indiana Press. Murdock, G. D. (1949). Social Structure. New York: Macmillan. Murdock, G. P. and White, D. R. (1969). Standard Cross-Cultural Sample. Ethnology 8, 329–369. Murdock, P. M. (1967). Ethnographic Atlas. University of Pittsburgh Press, PA. Murozumi, M. , Chow, I. , and Patterson, C. (1969). Chemische Konzentration von Bleiteilchen, von Staub und Seesalz in den Schneeschichten von Grönland und dem Südpol. Zitiert nach: Deutsche Zeitung—Christ und Welt vom 29.1.1971. Murphy, R. F. (1957). Intergroup hostility and social cohesion. Amer. Anthropol. 59, 1018–1035. Murphy, R. F. (1960). Headhunter’s Heritage. Berkeley: University of California Press, CA. Murra, J. (1958). On Inca political structure. In: System of Political Control and Bureaucracy in Human Societies. Seattle: Washington University Press, pp. 30—41. Murra, J. (1972). El “control vertical” de un maximo de pisos ecologicos en la economia de las sociedades andinas. Visita de Pedro de Leon. Univ. Huanuco (Peru). Murray, L. (1977). Infants’ Capacities for Regulating Interactions with Their Mothers and the Function of Emotions. Thesis, University of Edinburgh. Murray, L. and Trevarthen, C. (1986). The infant’s role in mother-infant communications. J. Child. Language 13, 15–29. Mussen, P. H. (ed.) (1970). Carmichael’s Manual of Child Psychology, Vol. I and II. New York: Wiley. Mussen, P. H. and Rutherford, M. (1963). Parent-child relations and parental personality in relation to young children’s sex role preferences. Child Develop. 34, 589–607. Musterle, W. A. (1984). Linear-Kombinationen aus stimmungsreinen Computer-Faces. Vortrag am Institutsseminar des Instituts für Theoretische Chemie am 2 Sept. 1984, Universität Tübingen. Musterle, W. and Rössler, O. E. (1986). Computer faces: The human Lorenz matrix. Bio Systems 19, 61–80. Myers, B. J. (1984). Mother-infant bonding: The status of this critical-period hypothesis. Develop. Rev. 4, 240–274. (a) Myers, B. J. (1984). Mother-infant bonding: Rejoinder to Kennell and Klaus. Develop. Rev. 4, 283–288. (b) Naaktgeboren, C. and Bontekoe, E. H. M. (1976). Vergleichend-geburtskundliche Betrachtungen und experimentelle Untersuchungen über psychosomatische Störungen der Schwangerschaft und des Geburtsablaufes. Z. Tierzüch. Züchtungsbiol. 93, 264–320. 765 Nance, J. (1975). The Gentle Tasaday: A Stone Age People in the Philippine Rain Forest. New York/London: Harcout, Brace, Jovanovich. Nance, J. (1977). Tasaday, Steinzeitmenschen im philippinischen Regenwald. München: Paul List. Nansen, F. (1903). Eskimoleben. Leipzig: Meyer. Napier, J. R. (1962). The evolution of the hand. Sci. Amer. 207, 56–63. Neimark, E. D. (1978). Die Entwicklung des Denkens beim Heranwachsenden. In: Steiner, G. (ed.), Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts. 7: Piaget und die Folgen. Zürich: Kindler, pp. 155–171. Nelson, E. W. (1896/1997). The Eskimo About Bering Strait. Annual Report 18, Vol. 1, Washington, D. C.: Bureau of American Ethnology. Nesbitt, P. D. and Steven, G. (1974). Personal space and stimulus intensity at a southern California amusement park. Sociometry 37, 105–115. Netting, R. M. C. (1968). Hill farmers of Nigeria: Cultural ecology of the Kofyar of the Jos Plateau. Amer. Ethnol. Soc. Monogr. 46. Netting, R. M. C. (1971). The Ecological Approach in Cultural Study. McCaleb Modules in Anthropology. Reading, MA: Addison-Wesley. Nettleship, M. A. , Dalegivens, R. and Nettleship, A. (eds.) (1975). War, Its Causes and Correlates. The Hague: Mouton. Neuberger, H. , Merz, J. , and Selg, H. (1983). Imitation bei Neugeborenen—eine kontroverse Befundlage. Zeitschrift für Entwicklungspsychologie und padagog. Psychologie 15, 267–276. Neuhaus, H. (1981). Wie antihomosexuell sind Sexualkundebücher? Sexualpädagogik Familienplanung 9, 28–30. Neumann, G. H. (1977). Vorurteile und Negativeinstellungen Behinderten gegenüber— Entstehung und Möglichkeiten des Abbaues aus der Sicht der Verhaltensbiologie. Rehabilitation 16, 101–106. Neumann, G. H. (1981). Normatives Verhalten und aggressive Auβenseiterreaktionen bei gesellig lebenden Vögeln und Säugern. Forschungsberichte des Landes Nordrhein-Westfalen, Nr. 3014. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. Nevermann, H. (1941). Ein Besuch bei Steinzeitmenschen. Stuttgart: Franckh’sche Verlagshandlung. Newcomb, W. W. (1950). A Re-examination of the causes of Plains warfare. Amer. Anthropol. 52, 317–330. Newman, J. and McCauley, C. (1977). Eye contact with strangers in city, suburb and small town. Environ. Behav. 9, 547–558. Newton, N. (1958). Influence of let-down reflex in breast feeding on mother-child relationship. Marriage Family Living 20, 18–20. Nguyen-Clausen, A. (1987). Ausdruck und Beeinfluβbarkeit der kindlichen Bildnerei. In: Prinz von Hohenzollern, J. G. and Liedtke, M. (eds.), Vom Kritzeln zur Kunst. Bad Heilbrunn: Julius Klinkhardt. Niemeyer, C. L. and Anderson, J. R. (1983). Primate harassment of matings. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 205–220. Nitsch, K. (ed.) (1978). Was wird aus unseren Kindern? Gesellschaftspolitische Folgen frühkindlicher Vernachlässigung. Heidelberg: Hüthig. Noel, G. L. , Suh, H. K. , and Frantz, A. G. (1972). Induction of Prolactin by Breast Stimulation in Humans. Excer. Med. Internat. Cong. Ser. 256. Nottebohm, F. (1970). Ontogeny of bird song. Science 167, 950–966. Obonai, T. (1935). Contributions to the study of psychophysical induction. VI. Experiments on the Müller-Lyer illusion. Jap. J. Psychol. 10, 37–39. Obonai, T. and Asano, T. (1937). Contributions to the study of psychophysical induction. IX. The study of the retinal irradiation. Jap. J. Psychol. 12, 1–12. Obonai, T. and Hino, H. (1930). Experimentelle Untersuchungen über die Wahrnehmung der geteilten Flachenräume. Jap. J. Psychol. 5, 2–5. O’Connor, S. M. , Vietze, P. M. , Hopkins, J. B. , and Altmeier, W. A. (1977). Postpartum Extended Maternal-Infant Contact: Subsequent Mothering and Child Health. San Francisco: Soc. for Pediatric Research. Oettingen, G. V. (1985). Erziehungsstil und Sozialordnung. Beobachtungen in zwei englischen Kindergärten. Dissertation des Fachbereichs Biologie der Universität München. 766 Öhman, A. and Dimberg, U. (1978). Facial expressions as conditioned stimuli for electrodermal responses: A case of preparedness. J. Personality Social Psychol. 36, 1251–1258. Ohala, J. J. (1984). An ethological perspective on common cross-language utilisation of Fo of voice. Phonetica 41, 1–16. Olds, J. (1956). Pleasure centers in the brain. Sci. Amer. 193, 105–116. Oiler, D. K. and Eilers, R. E. (1988). The role of audition in infant babbling. Child Develop. 59, 441–449. Omark, D. R. and Edelman, M. S. (1976). The development of attention structures in young children. In: Chance, M. R. A. and Larsen, R. R. (eds.), The Social Structure of Attention. London: Wiley, pp. 119–153. Omark, D. R. , Strayer, F. F. , and Freedman, D. G. (1980). Dominance Relations: An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interactions. New York/London: Garland STPM Press. Orians, G. H. (1980). Habitat selection: General theory and applications to human behavior. In: Lockard, S. J. (ed.), The Evolution of Human Social Behavior. New York: Elsevier, pp. 49–66. Orlansky, H. (1949). Infant care and personality. Psychol. Bull. 46, 1–48. Orlick, T. D. (1981). Positive socialization via cooperative games. Develop. Psychol. 17, 426–429. Orwell, G. (1949). 1984. Harmondsworth: Penguin Books. Osborn, F. (1948)., Our Plundered Planet. New York: Little Brown. Osche, G. (1979). Kulturelle Evolution: Biologische Wurzeln, Vergleich ihrer Mechanismen mit denen des biologischen Evolutionsgeschehens. In: Hassenstein, B. , Mohr, H. , Osche, G. , Sander, K. , and Wülker, W. (eds.), Freiburger Vorlesungen zur Biologie des Menschen. Heidelberg: Quelle & Meyer, pp. 33–50. (a) Osche, G. (1979). Vom Tier zum Menschen—Schlüsselereignis der morphologischen und verhaltensbiologischen Evolution. In: Hassenstein, B. , Mohr, H. , Osche, G. , Sander, K. , and Wülker, W. (eds.), Freiburger Vorlesungen zur Biologie des Menschen. Heidelberg: Quelle & Meyer, pp. 7–32. (b) Osofsky, J. D. (1979). Handbook of Infant Development. New York: Wiley. Ostermeyer, H. (1979). Ehe, Isolation zu zweit? Frankfürt/M: Fischer Taschenbuch. Otterbein, K. F. (1970). The Evolution of War. New Haven: HRAF Press. Paciornik, M. and Paciornik, C. (1983). Birth and rooming-in: Lessons learned from the forest indians of Brazil. Birth 10, 115–130. Packard, V. (1958). Die geheimen Verführer. Düsseldorf: Econ. Packard, V. (1963). The Pyramid Climbers. New York/London: McGraw-Hill. Palluck, R. J. and Esser, A. H. (1971). Controlled experimental modification of aggressive behavior in territories of severely retarded boys. Amer. J. Mental Deficiency , 76, 23–29. (a) Palluck, R. J. (1971). Territorial behavior as an indicator of changes in clinical behavioral condition of severely retarded boys. Amer. J. Mental Deficiency 76, 284–290. (b) Panksepp, J. (1981). Brain opioids—A neurochemical substrate for narcotic and social dependence. In: Cooper, S. J. (ed.), Theory in Psychopharmacology, Vol. 1. London: Academic Press, pp. 149–175. Palluck, R. J. (1982). Toward a general psychobiological theory of emotions. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 407–467. Palluck, R. J. (1985). Mood changes. In: Frederiks, J. A. M. (ed.), Handbook of Clinical Neurology, Vol. 1 (45): Clinical Neuropsychology. Amsterdam/New York: Elsevier, pp. 271–285. Palluck, R. J. (1986). The neurochemistry of behavior. Annu. Rev. Psychol. 37, 77–107. Panksepp, J. , Herman, B. H. , Wilberg, T. , Bishop, P. , and DeEskinazi, F. G. (1978). Endogenous opioids and social behavior. Neurosci. Biobehav. Rev. 4, 473–487. Papouček, H. (1969). Individual variability on learned responses in human infants. In: Robinson, R. J. (ed.), Brain and Early Behavior. London: Academic Press. Papoušek, H. and Papoušek, M. (1977). Die ersten sozialen Beziehungen: Entwicklungschance oder pathogene Situation. Praxis Psychother. 3, 97–108. Papoušek, M. (1984). Wurzeln der kindlichen Bindung an Personen und Dinge: Die Rolle der integrativen Prozesse. In: Eggers, Ch. (eds.), Bindungen und Besitzdenken beim Kleinkind. München/Wien/Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 155–184. 767 Parke, R. D. (1980). The family in early infancy: Social interactional and attitudinal analyses. In: Pedersen, F. (ed.), The Father-Infant Relationship: Observational Studies in a Family Context. New York: Praeger, pp. 44–70. Parke, R. D. and O’Leary, S. E. (1976). Father-mother-infant interaction in the newborn period: Some findings, some observations, and some unresolved issues. In: Riegel, K. and Meacham, J. (eds.), The Developing Individual in a Changing World. Vol. II, Social and Environmental Issues. The Hague: Mouton, pp. 653–664. Parke, R. D. and Sawin, D. B. (1975). Infant characteristics and behavior as elicitors of maternal and paternal responsibility in the newborn period. Soc. Res. Child Develop. Denver. Parke, R. D. and Sawin, D. B. (1977). The family in early infancy: Social interactional and attitudinal analysis. Soc. Res. Child Develop., New Orleans. Parke, R. D. and Suomi, St. Jr. (1980). Adult male-infant relationships: Human and non-human primate evidence. In: Immelmann, K. , Barlow, G. , Main, M. , and Petrinovitch, L. (eds.), Behavioral Development: The Bielefeld Interdisciplinary Project. New York: Cambridge University Press. Parker, S. (1976). The precultural basis of incest taboo: Toward a biosocial theory. Amer. Anthropol. 78, 285–305. Parker, S. T. and Gibson, K. R. (1979). A developmental model for the evolution of language and intelligence in early hominids. Behav. Brain Sci. 2, 367–408. Parmelee, A. H. (1961). A study of one infant from birth to eight months of age. Acta Paediat. 50, 160–170. Passarge, S. (1907). Die Buschmänner der Kalahari. Berlin: D. Reimer. Pastor, D. L. (1980). The Quality of Mother-Infant Attachment and Its Relationship to Toddlers’ Initial Sociability with Peers. Paper presented at the Intern. Conf. on Infant Studies, New Haven, April, 1980. Pastore, R. E. (1976). Categorical and perception: A critical re-evaluation. In: Hirsch, S. K. et al. (eds.), Hearing and Davis: Essays Honoring Hallowell Davis. St. Louis: Washington University Press. Patterson, F. G. (1978). The gestures of a gorilla: Language acquisition in another pongid. Brain Language 5, 12–97. Patterson, M. L. , Mullens, S. , and Romano, J. (1971). Compensatory reactions to spatial intrusion. Sociometry 34, 114–121. Patterson, R. L. S. (1968). Identification of 3α-hydroxy-5α-androst-16-ene as the musk odour component of boar sub- maxillary salivary gland and its relationship to the sex odour taint in pork meat. J. Sci. Food Agri. 19, 434–438. Pearson, K. G. (1972). Central programming and reflex control of walking in the cockroach. J. Exp. Biol. 56, 173–193. Peiper, A. (1951). Instinkt und angeborenes Schema beim Säugling. Z. Tierpsychol. 8, 449–456. Peiper, A. (1953). Schreit-und Steigbewegungen beim Neugeborenen. Arch. Kinderheilk. 147, 135. Pentland, B. , Pitcairn, T. K. , Gray, J. M. , and Riddle, W. (1987). The effects of reduced expression in Parkinson’s disease on impression formation by health professionals. Clin. Rehab. 1, 307–313. Peplau, A. (1976). Fear of success in dating couples. Sex Roles 2, 249–258. Perry, J. and Whipple, B. (1981). Pelvic muscle strength of female ejaculators: Evidence in support of a new theory of orgasm. J. Sex Res. 17, 22–39. Persson-Benbow, C. and Stanley, J. (1980). Sex differences in mathematical ability: Fact or artifact? Science 210, 1262–1264. Peterson, N. (1963). Family ownership and right of disposition in Sukkertoppen District, West Greenland. Folk 5, 270–281. Peterson, N. (1972). Totemism yesterday: Sentiment and local organization among the Australian aborigines. Man 7, 12–32. Peterson, N. V. (1975). Hunter-gatherer territoriality: The perspective from Australia. Amer. Anthropol. 77, 53–68. Peterson, N. (1979). Territorial adaptations among desert hunter-gatherers: The !Kung and Australians compared. In: Burnham, P. C. and Ellen, R. F. (eds.), Social and Ecological Systems. London/New York: Academic Press, pp. 111–129. 768 Peterson, St. A. (1983). Biosoziale Korrelate politischen Verhaltens. In: Flohr, H. and Tännesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 127–132. Pfeiffer, J. E. (1969). The Emergence of Man. New York: Evanston; London: Harper & Row. Pfeiffer, J. E. (1977). The Emergence of Society: A Prehistory of the Establishment. New York: McGraw-Hill. Phillips, S. , King, S. , and Dubois, L. (1978). Spontaneous activities of female versus male newborn. Child Develop. 49, 590–597. Piaget, J. (1932). The Moral Judgment of the Child. London: Kegan Paul. Piaget, J. (1953). The Origin of Intelligence in the Child. London: Routledge & Kegan Paul. Piaget, J. (1970). Mémoire et Intelligence. In: La Memoire. Symposium de l’Association de Psychologie de langue française. Paris: Presses Universitaires de France, pp. 169–178. Piaget, J. (1975). Nachahmung, Spiel und Traum. Die Entwicklung der Symbolfunktion beim Kinde. Stuttgart: Klett. (a) Piaget, J. (1975). Sprechen und Denken des Kindes. Düsseldorf: Schwann, (b) Piaget, J. (1976). Psychologie der Intelligenz. Miinchen: Kindler, Kindler-Taschenbuch 2167. Piaget, J. (1980). The Psychogenesis of Knowledge and Its Epistemological Significance. In: Piattelli-Palmarini, M. (ed.), Language and Learning. The Debate between Jean Piaget and Noam Chomsky. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, pp. 23–34. Pilleri, G. (1960). Über das Auftreten von “Kletterbewegungen” im Endstadium eines Falles von Morbus Alzheimer. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkunde 200, 455–461. (a) Pilleri, G. (1960). Kopfpendeln (“Leerlaufendes Brutsuchen”) bei einem Fall von Pickscher Krankheit. Arch. Psychiatr. Nervenkunde 200, 603–611. (b) Pilleri, G. (1961). Orale Einstellung nach Art des Klüver-Bucy-Syndroms bei hirnatophischen Prozessen. Schweiz. Arch. Neurol. Neurochirg. Psychiatr. 87, 286–298. Pitcairn, T. K. and Schleidt, M. (1976). Dance and decision: An analysis of a courtship dance of the Medlpa, New Guinea. Behaviour 58, 298–316. Pleger, J. (1976). Das Phänomen der Aggression. Dissertation. Erziehungswissenschaft an der Pädagogischen Hochschule Ruhr, Bd. I + II. Dortmund: Heme. Ploog, D. (1964). Verhaltensforschung und Psychiatric In: Gruhle, H.W. , Jung, R. , Mayer-Gross, W. , and Müller, M. (eds.), Psychiatrie der Gegenwart, Bd. 1/1. Berlin/Göttingen/Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 291–443. Ploog, D. (1966). Experimentelle Verhaltensforschung. Nervenarzt 37, 443–447. Ploog, D. (1969). Psychobiologie des Partnerschaftsverhaltens. Nervenarzt 40, 245–255. Ploog, D. (1972). Kommunikation in Affengesellschaften und deren Bedeutung für die Verstündigungsweisen des Menschen. In: Gadamer, H. G. and Vogler, P. (eds.), Neue Anthropologie, Bd. 2. Stuttgart: Thieme/dtv- Wissenschaftl. Reihe, pp. 98–178. Ploog, D. (1980). Soziologie der Primaten. In: Kisker, K. P. , Meyer, J.E. , Müller, C. , and Strümgren, E. (eds.), Psychiatrie der Gegenwart, Bd. 1/2. Berlin: Springer, 2. Aufl., pp. 379–544. Ploog, D. , Blitz, J. , and Ploog, F. (1963). Studies on social and sexual behavior of the squirrel monkey (Saimiri sciureus). Folia Primatol. 1, 29–66. Ploog, D. , Hopf, S. , and Winter, P. (1967). Ontogenese des Verhaltens von Totenkopf-Affen (Saimiri sciureus). Psychol. Forsch. 31, 1–41. Plutchik, R. (1980). A general psychoevolutionary theory of emotion. In: Plutchik, R. and Kellerman, H. (eds.), Emotion: Theory, Research, and Experience, Vol. 1. New York: Academic Press. Pöppel, E. (1978). Time perception. In: Held, R. , Leibowitz, H. W. , and Teüber, H.L. (eds.), Handbook of Sensory Physiology, Vol. III, Heidelberg: Springer, pp. 713–729. Pöppel, E. (1982). Lust und Schmerz. Berlin: Severin und Siedler. Pöppel, E. (1983). Musikerleben und Zeitstruktur. In: Kreuzer, F. (ed.), Auge macht Bild, Ohr Macht Klang, Hirn macht Welt. Salzburger Musikgesprach, Wien: Franz Deutike, pp. 76–87. Pöppel, E. (1984). Grenzen des Bewuβtseins: Über Wirklichkeit und Welterfahrung. Stuttgart: Deutsche Verlagsanstalt. Pöppel, E. and Sütterlin, C. (1983). Wahrnehmungs-Asymmetrie und Gestaltung von Links und Rechts in Bildern. Neuropsychologische Aspekte der ästhetischen Wahrnehmung. Symposium “Biology of Esthetics” Reimers-Stiftung, Bad Homburg. Pöppel, E. , Held, R. , and Frost, D. (1973). Residual visual function after brain wounds involving the central visual pathways in man. Nature 243, 295–296. 769 Popp, J. and DeVore, I. (1979). Aggressive competition and social dominance theory. In: Hamburg, D. , and McCown, E. (eds.), The Great Apes. Menlo Park, CA: Benjamin Cummings, pp. 317–338. Popper, K. R. (1973). Objektive Erkenntnis. Ein evolutionärer Entwurf. Hamburg: Hoffmann und Campe. Porter, R. H. and Moore, J. D. (1981). Human kin recognition by olfactory cues. Physiol. Behav. 27, 493–495. Portisch, H. (1970). Friede durch Angst: Augenzeuge in den Arsenalen des Atomkrieges. Wien: Molden. Poshivalov, V. P. (1986). Ethological pharmacology as a tool for animal aggression research. In: Brain, P. F. and Ramirez, M. (eds.), Cross-Disciplinary Studies on Aggression. Sevilla: Publicaciones de la Universidad de Sevilla, pp. 17–49. Prechtl, H. F. R. (1953). Stammesgeschichtliche Reste im Verhalten des Sauglings. Umschau 21, 656–658. Prechtl, H. F. R. (1958). The directed head turning response and allied movements of the human baby. Behaviour 13, 212–242. Prechtl, H. F. R. (1981). The study of neural development as a perspective of clinical problems. In: Connolly, K. J. and Prechtl, H. F. R. (eds.), Maturation and Development. London: W. Heinemann Med. Books, pp. 198–215. Prechtl, H. F. R. and Lenard, H. G. (1968). Verhaltensphysiologie des Neugeborenen. In: Linneweh, F. (ed.), Fortschritte der Pädologie, Bd. II. Berlin: Springer, pp. 88–122. Prechtl, H. F. R. and Schleidt, W. M. (1950). Auslösende und steuernde Mechanismen des Saugaktes, I u. II. Z. Vergl. Physiol. 32, 252–262, 33, 53-62. Premack, D. (1971). Language in the chimpanzee? Science 172, 808–822. Preti, G. , Cutler, W. B. , Garcia, C. R. , Huggins, G. R. , and Lawley, H. J. (1986). Human axillary secretions influence women’s menstrual cycles: The role of donor extract of females. Hormones Behav. 20, 474–482. Pribram, K. H. (1979). Behaviourism, phenomenology and holism in psychology: A scientific analysis. J. Social Biol. Structures 2, 65–72. Prioleau, L. , Murdock, M. , and Brody, N. (1983). An analysis of psychotherapy versus placebo. Behav. Brain Sci. 6, 275–310. Proshansky, H. M. (1966). The development of intergroup attitudes. In: Hoffman, L. W. and Hoffman, M. L. (eds.), Review of Child Development Research, Vol. 2. New York: Russell Sage Found., pp. 311–371. Provine, Robert R. (1986). Yawning as a stereotyped action pattern and releasing stimulus. Ethology 72(2), 89–176, 109–122. Purifoy, F. E. (1981). Endocrine-environment interaction in human variability. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 10, 141–162. Quanty, M. (1976). Aggression catharsis: Experimental investigation and implications. In: Geen, R. G. and O’Neal, E. C. (eds.), Perspectives on Aggression. New York: Academic Press, pp. 99–132. Queyrat, Fr . (1905). Les Jeux des Enfants. Paris: Alcan. Radcliffe-Brown, A. R. (1930). The social organization of Australian tribes. Oceania 1, 34–63. Rader, N. , Bausano, M. , and Richards, J. E. (1980). On the nature of the visual-cliffavoidance response in human infants. Child Develop. 51, 61–68. Radnitzky, G. and Bartley, W. W. (eds.) (1987). Evolutionary Epistemology, Theory of Rationality, and the Sociology of Knowledge. La Salle, IL: Open Court Publ. Ragan, J. M. (1982). Gender displays in portrait photographs. Sex Roles 8, 33–43. Rainer, R. (1978). Kriterien der wohnlichen Stadt. Graz: Akademische Druck-und Verlagsanstalt. Rajecki, D. W. , Lamb, M. E. , and Obmascher, P. (1978). Toward a general theory of infantile attachment: A comparative review of aspects of the social bond. Behav. Brain Sci. 3, 417–464. (a) Rajecki, D. W. , Lamb, M. E. , and Suomi, S. J. (1978). Effects of multiple peer separation in domestic chicks. Develop. Psychol. 14, 387–397. (b) Rancourt-Laferriere, D. (1979). Some semiotic aspects of the human penis. VS (Versus) , 24, 37–82. Rancourt-Laferriere, D. (1983). Four adaptive aspects of the female orgasm. J. Soc. Biol. Structures 6, 319–333. 770 Rapoport, A. (1982). The Meaning of the Built Environment. Beverly Hills: Sage. Rappaport, R. A. (1968). Pigs for the Ancestors. New Haven/London: Yale University Press. Rasmussen, K. (1908). People of the Polar North. London: Ed. G. Herring. Rattner, J. (1970). Aggression und menschliche Natur. Olten: Walter. Rauh, H. (1984). Soziale Interaktion und Gruppenstruktur bei Krabbelkindern. In: Eggers, Ch. (ed.), Bindungen und Besitzdenken beim Kleinkind. München/Wien/Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 204–232. Rausch, G. and Scheich, H. (1982). Dendritic spine loss and enlargement during maturation of the speech control system in the mynah bird (Gracula religiosa). Neurosci. Lett. 29, 129–133. Razran, G. H. S. (1938). Conditioning away social bias by the luncheon technique. Psychol. Bull. 35, 693. Redican, W. (1975). Facial expressions in nonhuman primates. In: Rosenblum, L. A. (ed.), Primate Behavior, Vol. 4. London: Academic Press, pp. 103–194. Regan, D. T. (1971). Effects of a favor and liking on compliance. J. Exp. Social Psychol. 7, 627–639. Reich, W. (1949). Die sexuelle Revolution. Frankfurt/M.: Fischer. Reimarus, H. S. (1762). Allgemeine Betrachtungen über die Triebe der Thiere, hauptsächlich über ihre Kunsttriebe. Hamburg. Reis, D.J. (1974). Central neurotransmitters in aggression. In: Frazier, S. H. (ed.), Aggression. Proceedings (Research Publications) of the Association for Nervous and Mental Diseases. Baltimore: Williams and Wilkins, pp. 119–148. Remane, A. (1952). Die Grundlagen des natürlichen Systems der vergleichenden Anatomie und der Phylogenetik. Leipzig: Geest & Portig. Renggli, F. R. (1976). Angst und Geborgenheit. Soziokulturelle Folgen der Mutter-Kind-Beziehung im ersten Lebensjahr. Ergebnisse aus der Verhaltensforschung, Psychoanalyse und Ethnologic Hamburg: Rowohlt. Rensch, B. (1957). Asthetische Faktoren bei Farb-und Formbevorzugungen von Affen. Z. Tierpsychol. 14, 71–99. Rensch, B. (1958). Die Wirksamkeit ästhetischer Faktoren bei Wirbeltieren. Z. Tierpsychol. 15, 447–461. Rensch, B. (1963). Versuche über menschliche “Auslöser-Merkmale” beider Geschlechter. Z. Morphol. Anthropol. 53, 139–164. Reynolds, P. C. (1982). Affect and instrumentality: An alternative view on Eibl-Eibesfeldt’s human ethology. Behav. Brain Sci. 5, 267–273. Reynolds, V. (1966). Open grounds in human evolution. Man 1, 441–452. Rheingold, H. J. (1961). The effect of environmental stimulation upon social and exploratory behavior in the human infant. In: Foss, B. M. (ed.), Determinants of Infant Behavior, Vol. 1. London: Methuen, pp. 143–177. Rheingold, H. J. and Eckerman, C. (1973). Fear of the stranger: A critical examination. In: Reese, H. (ed.), Advances in Child Development and Behavior, 8. New York: Academic Press. Rheingold, H. L. and Adams, J. L. (1980). The significance of speech to newborn. Develop. Psychol. 16, 397–403. Richards, J. E. and Rader, N. (1981). Crawling-onset age predicts visual cliff avoidance in infants. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Perception Performance 7 (2), 382–387. Richerson, P. J. (1977). Ecology and human ecology: A comparison of theories in the biological and social sciences. Amer. Ethnol. 4, 1–26. Riedl, R. (1979). Biologie der Erkenntnis. Die stammesgeschichtlichen Grundlagen der Vernunft. (with cooperation of R. Kaspar.) Berlin/Hamburg: Paul Parey. Riedl, R. (1981). Die Folgen des Ursachendenkens. In: Watzlawick, P. (ed.), Die erfundene Wirklichkeit. München/Zürich: Piper, pp. 67–90. Ringler, N. M. , Kennell, J. H. , Jarvella, R. , Navojosky, B. J. , and Klaus, M. H. (1975). Mother-to-child speech at 2 years: Effects of early postnatal contact. J. Pediatr. 86, 141–144. Ringler, N. M. , Trause, M. A. , Klaus, M. H. , and Kennell, J. (1978). The effects of extra postpartum contact and maternal speech patterns on children’s IQs, speech, and language comprehension at five. Child Develop. 49, 862–865. Robbins, J. H. (1980). Breaking the taboos: Further reflections on mothering. J. Human Psychol. 20, 27–40. 771 Robinson, Ch. L. , Lockard, J. S. , and Adams, R. M. (1979). Who looks at a baby in public. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 87–91. Robson, K. S. (1967). The role of eye-to-eye contact in maternal-infant-attachment. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 8, 13–35. Roeder, K. D. (1955). Spontaneous activity and behavior. Sci. Monthly 80, 362–370. Rödholm, M. (1981). Early Mother-Infant and Father-Infant Interaction. Göteborg. Röhrig, L. (1967). Gebärde, Metapher, Parodie. Düsseldorf: Pädagogischer Verlag Schwann. Rohner, R. P. (1975). They Love Me, They Love Me Not: A World-Wide Study of the Effects of Parental Acceptance and Rejection. New Haven: HRAF Press. Rohner, R. P. (1976). Sex differences in aggression: Phylogenetic and enculturation perspective. Ethos 4, 57–72. Roper, M. K. (1969). A survey of evidence for intrahuman killing in the Pleistocene. Current Anthropol. 10, 427–459. Rose, F. G. G. (1960). Classification of Kin, Age Structure and Marriage among the Groote Eylandt Aborigines. A Study in Method and a Theory of Australian Kinship. Berlin: Akademischer Verlag. Rose, R. , Bernstein, I. , and Gordon, T. (1975). Consequences of social conflict on plasma testosterone levels in rhesus monkeys. Psychosomat. Med. 37, 50–61. Rosenblum, L. A. (1971). Infant attachment in monkeys. In: Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), The Origins of Human Social Relations. New York: Academic Press. Rosenblum, L. A. and Harlow, H. F. (1963). Approach-avoidance conflict in the mothersurrogate situation. Psychol. Rep. 12, 83–85. Rosenfeld, H. M. (1982). Measurement of body motion and orientation. In: Scherer, K. R. and Ekman, P. (eds.), Handbook of Methods in Nonverbal Behavior Research. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press, pp. 199–286. Rosenzweig, M. R. and Leiman, A. L. (1982). Physiological Psychology. Lexington, MA: C. D. Heath. Rossi, A. S. (1975). A biosocial perspective on parenting. Daedalus 106, 1–32. Rothchild, J. and Wolf, S. B. (1976). The Children of the Counterculture. New York: Doubleday. Rothmann, M. and Teüber, E. (1915). Einzelausgabe der Anthropoidenstation auf Teneriffa: I. Ziele und Aufgaben der Station sowie erste Beobachtungen an den auf ihr gehaltenen Schimpansen. Berlin. 1.-20. Abhandlungen Preuβische Akademie der Wissenschaften. Rubin, R. T. , Reinisch, J. M. , and Haskett, R. F. (1981). Postnatal gonadal steroid effects on human behavior. Science 211, 1318–1324. Rubinshtein, S. L. (1973). Fundamentals of General Psychology. Moskau: Uchpedgiz. Rudolph, W. (1968). Der kulturelle Relativismus. Forschungen zur Ethnologie und Sozialpsychologie, Bd. 6. Berlin: Dunker & Humblot. Rumbaugh, D. M. and Gill, T. V. (1977). Lana’s acquisition of language skills. In: Rumbaugh, D. M. (ed.), Language Learning by a Chimpanzee. The Lana Project. New York: Academic Press, pp. 165–192. Rump, G. Ch. (1978). Bildstruktur—Erkenntnisstruktur. Gegenseitige Bedingungen von Kunst und Verhalten. Kastellaun: A. Henn Verlag. Rump, G. Ch. (1980). Verhaltensforschung und Kunstgeschichte. In: Hahn, M. and Schuster, M. (eds.), Fortschritte der Kunstpsychologie. Frankfürt/M.: P. A. Lang. Ruse, M. (1979). Sociobiology: Sense or Nonsense? Dordrecht/Boston/London: D. Reidel. Russell, M. J. (1976). Human olfactory communication. Nature 260, 520–522. Russell, M. J. , Switz, G. M. , and Thompson, K. (1980). Olfactory Influences on the Human Menstrual Cycle. Pharmacol. Biochem. Behavior 12, 737–738. Russell, M. J. , Mendelson, T. , and Peeke, H. V. S. (1983). Mother’s identification of their infant’s odors. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 29–31. Sackett, G. P. (1966). Monkeys reared in isolation with pictures as visual input: Evidence for an innate releasing mechanism. Science 154, 1468–1473. Sadock, S. B. J. and Sadock, V. A. (1976). Techniques of coitus. In: Sadock, S. B. J. , Kaplan, H. J. , and Freedman, A. M. (eds.), The Sexual Experience. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins, pp. 206–216. Sadock, S. B. J. , Kaplan, H. J. , and Freedman, A. M. (eds.) (1976). The Sexual Experience. Baltimore, MD: Williams & Wilkins. Sager, S. F. (1981). Sprache und Beziehung. Linguistische Untersuchungen zum Zusammenhang von sprachlicher Kommunikation und zwischenmenschlicher Beziehung. Tübingen: Max Niemeyer Verlag. 772 Sager, S. F. (1983). Die Manifestation universaler Verhaltensdispositionen im Verbal verhalten. Zwischenbericht zum DFG-Projekt SA 346/1-2. Sagi, A. (1981). Mothers’ and non-mothers’ identification of infant cries. Infant Behav. Develop. 4, 37–40. Sagi, A. and Hoffmann, M. L. (1978). Emphatic distress in the newborn. Develop. Psychol. 12, 175–176. Sagi, A. , Lamb, M. E. , Shoha, R. , Dvir, R. , and Lewkowicz, K. S. (1985). Parent-infant interaction in families on Israeli kibbutzim. Intern. J. Behav. Develop. 8, 273–284. Sahlins, M. D. (1960). The origin of society. Sci. Amer. 204, 76–87. (1976, rev. 77). The Use and Abuse of Biology. An Anthropological Critique of Sociobiology. Ann Arbor: University of Michigan Press. vom Sal, F. S. and Howard, L. S. (1982). The regulation of infanticide and parental behavior: Implications for reproductive success in male mice. Science 215, 1270–1272. Salamone, F. A. (1982). Personal, identity and ethnicity. Anthropos 77, 475–490. Saling, M. M. and Cooke, W. L. (1984). Cradling and transport of infants by South African mothers: A cross-cultural study. Current Anthropol. 25, 333–335. Salk, L. (1973). The role of the heartbeat in relations between mother and infant. Sci. Amer. 228, 24–29. Salzen, E. A. (1967). Imprinting in birds and primates. Behaviour 28, 232–254. Salzman, F. (1979). Aggression and gender. In: Hubbard, R. and Lowe, M. (eds.), Genes and Gender, II: Pitfalls in Research on Sex and Gender. New York: Gordian Press. Sanday, P. R. (1980). Margaret Mead’s view of sex roles in her own and other societies. Amer. Anthropol. 82, 340–348. Sander, F. (1931). Gestaltpsychologie und Kunsttheorie. Ein Beitrag zur Psychologie der Architektur. Neue Psychol. Stud. 8, 311–334. Sano, T. (1983). A catalogue of the facial behavior patterns of Japanese preschool children. J. Anthropol. Soc. Nippon 91, 323–336. Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. and Rumbaugh, D. M. (1982). Ape-language research is alive and well: A reply. Anthropos 77, 568–573. Savin-Williams, R. C. (1979). Dominance hierarchies in groups of early adolescents. Child Develop. 50, 923–935. Savin-Williams, R. C. (1980). Social interactions of adolescent females in natural groups. In: Foot, H. , Chapman, T. , and Smith, J. (eds.), Friendship and Social Relations in Children. Sussex: Wiley. Sawin, D. B. (1981). Fathers’ interactions with infants. In: Weiss-Bourd, B. and Musick, J. S. (eds.), Infants: Their Social Environments. Washington: Nat. Assoc. Educat. of Young Children, pp. 147–167. Sbrzesny, H. (1976). Die Spiele der !Ko-Buschleute. Monographien zur Humanethologie 2. München: Piper. Scanzoni, J. (1972). Sexual bargaining: Power politics in the American marriage. Englewood Cliffs, NJ: Prentice Hall. Schaal, B. , Montagner, H. , Hertling, E. , Bolzoni, D. , Moyse, A. , and Quichon, A. (1980). Les stimulations olfactives dans les relations entre l’enfant et la mère. Reprod. Nutr. Develop. 20, 843–858. Schaefer, H. (1978). Humanökologisch-anthropologische Grundlagen der Umweltgestaltung. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. 1. Die Umwelt des Menschen. Miinchen: BLV. (a) Schaefer, H. (1978). Schluβbetrachtung. In: Nitsch, K. (eds.), Was wird aus unseren Kindern? Gesellschaftspolitische Folgen frühkindlicher Vernachlässigung. Heidelberg: Hüthig, pp. 100–119.(b) Schaefer, H. and Flaschar, W. (1978). Energiewirtschaft und Umweltbeeinflussung. In: Buchwald, K. and Engelhardt, W. (eds.), Handbuch für Planung, Gestaltung und Schutz der Umwelt. 1. Die Umwelt des Menschen. München: BLV. Schäfer, U. (1979). Herausgefordert—Bewohner und Architekten diskutieren mit Journalisten. Bauen Wohnen , 1/2, 6- 10. Schaffer, H. R. (1966). The onset of fear of strangers and the incongruity hypothesis. J. Child Psychol. Psychiatry 7, 95–106. (ed.) (1977) Studies in Mother-Infant Interaction. London/New York: Academic Press. Schaffer, H. R. and Emerson, P. E. (1964). The Development of Social Attachments in Infancy. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development 29. 773 Schaller, J. , Carlsson, S. G. , and Larsson, K. (1979). Effects of extended post-partum mother-child contact on the mother’s behavior during nursing. Infant Behav. Develop. 2, 319–324. Scharf, J.H. (1981). Os incae, Blutgruppe 0 und boreische Sprachverwandtschaft. Anat. Anz. 150, 175–211. (a) Scharf, J.H. (1981). Das erste Wort? Morph. Jahrbuch 127. (b) Schebesta, P. (1941). Die Bambuti-Pygmäen vom Ituri, II. Inst. Royal Colonial Beige. Bruxelles: Libraire Falk. Schegloff, E. (1968). Sequencing rules in conversational openings. Amer. Anthropol. 70, 1075–1095. Scheller, R. H. and Axel, R. (1984). How genes control an innate behavior. Sci. Amer. 250, 44–52. Scheller, R. H. , Jackson, J. F. , McAllister, L. B. , Rothman, B. S. , Mayeri, E. , and Axel, R. (1983). A single gene encodes multiple neuropeptides mediating a stereotyped behavior. Cell 32, 7–22. Scheller, R. H. , Jackson, J. F. , McAllister, L. B. , Schwartz, J. H. , Kandel, E. R. , and Axel, R. (1982). A family of genes that codes for ELH, a neuropeptide eliciting a stereotyped pattern of behavior in aplysia. Cell 28, 707–719. Schelsky, H. (1955). Soziologie der Sexualität. Hamburg: Rowohlt. Scheman, J. , Lockard, J. S. , and Mehler, B. L. (1977). Anatomical influence on book-carrying behavior. Bull. Psychonomic Soc. 93, 367–370. Schenkel, R. (1956). Zur Deutung der Phasianidenbalz. Ornith. Beobacht. 53, 182–201. Scherer, K. R. (1972). Judging personality from voice: A cross-cultural approach to an old issue in interpersonal perception. J. Personality 40, 191–210. Scherer, K. R. (1977). Affektlaute und vokale Embleme. In: Rosner, R. and Reinecke, H. P. (eds.), Zeichenprozesse—Semiotische Forschung in den Einzelwissenschaften. Wiesbaden: Athenaion, pp. 199–214. Scherer, K. R. (1979). Personality markers in speech. In: Scherer, K. R. and Giles, H. (eds.), Social Markers in Speech. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press. Scherer, K. R. and Ekman, P. (eds.) (1982). Handbook of Methods in Nonverbal Behavior Research. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press. Scherer, K. R. and Giles, H. (eds.) (1979). Social Markers in Speech. Cambridge/London/New York: Cambridge University Press. Scherer, K. R. and Oshinsky, J. S. (1977). Cue utilization in emotion attribution from auditory stimuli. Motivation Emotion 1, 331–346. Scherer, K. R. and Wolf, J. J. (1973). The voice of confidence: Paralinguistic cues and audience evaluation. J. Res. Personality 17, 31–44. Scherer, K. R. , Uno, H. , and Rosenthal, R. (1972). A cross-cultural analysis of vocal behavior as a determinant of experimenter expectancy effects—A Japanese case. Intern. J. Psychol. 7, 109–117. Scherer, K. R. , Walbott, G. , and Scherer, U. (1979). Methoden zur Klassiflkation von Bewegungsverhalten. Ein funktionaler Ansatz. Z. Semiotik 1, 177–192. Schetelig, H. (1979). Die Bedeutung des Stillens in der Ernährung des Säuglings. Fortschr. Med. 97, 349–352. Schiefenhövel, G. and Schiefenhövel, W. (1978). Eipo, Irian Jaya (West-Neuguinea)—Vor-gänge bei der Geburt eines Mädchens und Änderung der Infantizid-Absicht. Homo 29, 121–138. Schiefenhövel, W. (1979). Aggression and Aggression-Control among the Eipo, Highlands of West-Neuguinea. Poster Paper XVI. International Ethological Conference, Vancouver. Schiefenhövel, W. (1980). “Primitive” Childbirth—Anachronism or Challenge to “Modern” Obstetrics? In: Ballabriga, A. and Gallart, A. (eds.), Proceedings of the 7th European Congress of Perinatal Medicine, Barcelona, pp. 40–49. Schiefenhövel, W. (1982). Die natürliche Geburt—Wie Eipo-Kinder auf die Welt kommen. Neue Zürcher Zeit. 113 (19.5.82), 33. Schiefenhövel, W. (1983). Geburten bei den Eipo. In: Schiefenhovel, W. and Sich, D. (eds.), Die Geburt aus ethnomedizinischer Sicht. Wiesbaden: Vieweg, pp. 41–56. (a) Schiefenhövel, W. (1983). Der ethnomedizinische Beitrag zur Diskussion um die optimale Geburtshilfe. In: Schiefenhövel, W. and Sich, D. (eds.), Die Geburt aus ethnomedizinischer Sicht. Wiesbaden: Vieweg, pp. 241–246. (b) 774 Schiefenhövel, W. (1984). Bindung und Lösung—Sozialisationspraktiken im Hochland von Neuginea. In: Eggers, Ch. (ed.), Bindungen und Besitzdenken beim Kleinkind. München/Wien/Baltimore: Urban & Schwarzenberg, pp. 51–80. Schiefenhövel, W. and Bell-Krannhals, I. (1986). Wer teilt, hat Teil an der Macht: Systeme der Yams-Vergabe auf den Trobriand-Inseln, Papua Neuguinea. Mitteil. Anthropol. Ges. Wien , 116, 1939. Schiefenhövel, W. and Sich, D. (eds.) (1983). Die Geburt aus ethnomedizinischer Sicht. Beiträge und Nachtrage zur IV. Internationalen Fachtagung der Arbeitsgemeinschaft Ethnomedizin über traditionelle Geburtshilfe und Gynäkologie in Göttingen. Wiesbaden: Vieweg. Schindler, H. (1980). Humanethologie und Ethnologic Z. Ethnol. 105, 67–93. Schindler, H. (1982). Language, alliance and descent. Anthropos 77, 524–532. Schjelderup-Ebbe, Th . (1922). Soziale Verhältnisse bei Vögeln. Z. Psychol. 90, 106–107. (a) Schindler, H. (1922). Beiträge zur Sozialpsychologie des Haushuhns. Z. Psychol. 88, 225–252. (b) Schleidt, M. (1980). Personal odor and nonverbal communication. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 225–231. Schleidt, M. (1984). Beziehungen zwischen Riechen, Pheromonen und Abhängigkeit. In: Keup, W. (ed.), Biologie der Sucht. 5. wiss. Sympos. der Dtsch. Hauptstelle gegen die Suchtgefahren, Hamm: Arbeitsgemeinsch. der Fachverlage. Schleidt, M. (1988). A universal time constant operating in human short-term behaviour repetitions. Ethology 77, 67–75. Schleidt, M. , Neumann, P. and Morishita, H. (1988). Pleasure and disgust: memories and associations of pleasant and unpleasant odours in Germany and Japan. Chem. Senses 13, 279–293. Schleidt, M. , Hold, B. , and Attili, G. (1981). A cross-cultural study on the attitude towards personal odors. J. Chem. Ecol. 7, 19–31. Schleidt, M. , Poppel, E. , and Eibl-Eibesfeldt, I. (1987). A universal constant in temporal segmentation of human short-term behaviour. Naturwissenschaften 74, 289–290. Schleidt, W. M. (1973). Tonic communication; continual effects of discrete signs in animal communication systems. J. Theoret. Biol. 42, 359–386. Schlosser, K. (1952). Körperliche Anomalien als Ursache sozialer Ausstoβung bei Naturvölkern. Z. Morphol. Anthropol. 44, 220–236. (a) Schlosser, K. (1952). Der Signalismus in der Kunst der NaturvÖlker. (Biologisch-psychologische Gesetzlichkeiten in den Abweichungen von der Norm des Vorbildes.) Arbeiten a. d. Mus. f. Völkerkunder Univ. Kiel, I. Kiel: Mühlau. (b) Schlosser, K. (1952). Der Rangkampf biologisch und ethnologisch gesehen. Act. IV. Congr. Inern. Sci. Anthropol. Ethnol., Wien , 2, 43–50. (c) Schmidbauer, W. (1971). Jäger und Sammler. München: Selecta Verlag. Schmidbauer, W. (1973). Territorialität und Aggression bei Jagern und Sammlern. Anthropos 68, 548–558. Schmidt-Mummendey, A. and Schmidt, H. D. (eds.) (1971). Aggressives Verhalten. München: Inventa. Schneider, D. (1962). Electrophysiological investigation on the olfactory specificity of sexual attracting substances in different species of moths. J. Insect. Physiol. 8, 15–30. Schneider, H. (1975). Entwicklung und Sozialisation der Primaten. München: tuduv Verlagsges. Schneider, W. (1976). Wörter machen Leute. Magie und Macht der Sprache. München: Piper; 3rd ed. 1983. Schneirla, T. C. (1966). Behavioral Development and Comparative Psychology. Quart. Rev. Biol. 41, 283–302. Schober, H. and Rentschler, I. (1979). Das Bild als Schein der Wirklichkeit. München: Moos Verlag. Schoeck, H. (1980). Der Neid. München/Wien: Herbig. Schoetzau, A. and Papoušek, H. (1977). Mütterliches Verhalten bei der Aufnahme von Blickkontakt mit Neugeborenen. Z. Entwicklungs Padagog. Psychol. 9, 231–239. Schröder, M. (1978). Untersuchungen zur Identifikation von Klageliedern aus verschiedenen Kulturen. Analyse der rhythmischen Struktur der Testlieder. Diplomarbeit, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München. 775 Schropp, R. (1982). Das Anbieten von Objekten als Strategic im Umfeld aggressiver Interaktionen. Diplomarbeit an der Ludwig-Maximilians-Universität München. Schubert, G. (1973). Biopolitical behavior: The nature of the political animal. Polity 6, 240–275. Schubert, G. (1975). Biopolitical behavioral theory. Political Sci. Rev. 4, 402–428. Schubert, G. (1981). The sociobiology of political behavior. In: White, E. (ed.), Sociobiology and Human Politics. Lexington, MA: D. C. Heath & Co., Lexington Books, pp. 193–238. Schubert, G. (1982). Infanticide by usurper hanuman langur males: A sociobiological myth. Soc. Sci. Inform. 21, 199–244. Schubert, G. (1983). The structure of attention: A critical review. J. Soc. Biol. Struct. 6, 65–80. (a) Schubert, G. (1983). Soziobiologie und politisches Verhalten. In: Flohr, H. and Tonnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 111–126. (b) Schultze-Westrum, Th . (1974). Biologie des Friedens. München: Kindler. Schumacher, A. (1982). On the significance of stature in human society. J. Human Evol. 11, 697–701. Schuster, M. and Beisl, H. (1978). Kunst-Psychologie “Wodurch Kunstwerke wirken.” Köln: DuMont. Schuster, R. H. (1978). Ethological theories of aggression. In: Kutash, I. L. , Kutash, S. B. , Schlesinger, L. B. et al. (eds.), Violence. Perspectives on Murder and Aggression. San Francisco, CA: Jossey Bass, pp. 74–100. Schwartzman, H. B. (1978). Transformations. The Anthropology of Children’s Play. New York/London: Plenum Press. Schwarzer, A. (1976). “Das ewig Weibliche ist eine Luge.” Simone de Beauvoir über die Situation der Frauen nach dem Jahr der Frau. Der Spiegel, 15. Scott, J. P. (1960). Aggression. Chicago: Chicago University Press. Seal, H. L. (1966). Multivariate Statistical Analysis for Biologists. London: Methuen. Searle, J. R. (1983). Intentionality. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Sears, R. R. , Rau, L. , and Alpert, R. (1965). Identification and Child Rearing. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press. Seay, B. , Alexander, B. K. , and Harlow, H. F. (1964). Maternal behavior of socially deprived rhesus monkeys. J. Abnorm. Soc. Psychol. 69, 345–354. Sebeok, Th. A. and Umiker-Sebeok, J. (eds.) (1980). Speaking of Apes. New York/London: PlenumPress. Sedlaček, K. and Sychra, A. (1963). Die Melodie als Faktor des emotionellen Ausdrucks. Folia Phoniatr. 15, 89–98. Sedlaček, K. and Sychra, A. (1969). The Method of Psychoacoustic Transformation Applied to the Investigation of Expression in Speech and Music. Kybernetika Cislo 1, Rocnik 5. Segall, M. , Campbell, D. , and Herskovits, M. (1966). The Influence of Culture on Visual Perception. Indianapolis, IN: Bobbs-Merrill. Seitelberger, F. (1981). Neurobiologische Grundlagen der menschlichen Freiheit. In: Böhme, W. (ed.), Mensch und Kosmos. Herrenalber Texte, 33 Frankfurt: Lembeck, pp. 26–47. Seitz, A. (1940). Die Paarbildung bei einigen Cichliden. Z. Tierpsychol. 4, 40–84. Seligman, M. E. P. (1970). On the generality of the laws of learning. Psychol. Rev. 77, 406–418. Seligman, M. E. P. (1971). Phobias and preparedness. Behav. Therapy 2, 307–320. Senft, G. (1982). Tonband T 3A. Mündliche Mitteilung. Tape-transcription. Service, E. R. (1962). Primitive Social Organization and Evolutionary Perspective. New York: Random House. Service, E. R. (1971). Primitive Social Organization. New York: Random House. Service, E. R. (1978). Classical and modern theories of the origins of government. In: Cohen, R. and Service, E. R. (eds.), Origins of the State: The Anthropology of Political Evolution. Philadelphia, PA: Institute for the Study of Human Issues, pp. 21–34. Seywald, A. (1977). Körperliche Behinderung. Frankfürt a. M./New York: Campus. Seywald, A. (1980). Anstoβnahme an sichtbar Behinderten. Rheinstetten: Schindele. Shafton, A. (1976). Conditions of Awareness: Subjective Factors in the Social Adaptations of Man and Other Primates. Portland, OR: Riverston Press. Sharpe, E. (1968). Psycho-physical problems revealed in language: An investigation of metaphor. In: Brierlye, M. (ed.), Collected Papers on Psychoanalysis. London: Hogarth, The International Psychoanalytical Library Nr. 36. 776 Shelley, W. B. , Hurley, H. J. , and Nichols, A. C. (1953). Axillary odor. A.M.A. Arch. Dermatol. Syphilol. 68, 430–446. Shepher, J. (1971). Mate selection among second generation kibbutz adolescents and adults: Incest avoidance and negative imprinting. Arch. Sex. Behav. 1, 293–307. Shepher, J. (1983). Incest—A Biosocial View. New York/London: Academic Press. Sherif, M. and Sherif, C. W. (1966). Groups in Harmony and Tension. New York: Octagon. Sherman, J. A. (1978). Sex-Related Cognitive Differences. Springfield, IL: Charles C. Thomas. Sherzer, J. (1970). La parole chez les Abipones. L’Homme 10, 42–76. Shettleworth, J. S. (1975). Reinforcement and the organisation of behavior in golden hamsters. Hunger, environment and food reinforcement. J. Exp. Psychol. Anim. Behav. Processes 104, 56–87. Shields, W. M. and Shields, L. M. (1983). Forcible rape: An evolutionary perspective. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 115–136. Shigetomi, C. C. , Hartmann, D. P. , and Gelfand, D. M. (1981). Sex differences in children’s altruistic behavior and reputations for helpfulness. Develop. Psychol. 17, 434–437. Short, R. V. (1984). Breast feeding. Sci. Amer. 250, 23–29. Shorter, E. (1977). Die Geburt der modernen Familie. Hamburg: Rowohlt. [English, ed.: The Making of the Modern Family. New York: Basic Books Inc.] Shostak, M. (1981). Nisa, Life and Words of a !Kung Woman. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Siegel, B. (1970). Defensive structuring and environmental stress. Amer. J. Sociol. 76, 11–32. Siegfried, W. (1983). Development of Space and Time Structures in Opening Phases of Dance Groups. Symposium on Biological Aspects of Aesthetics. Organized by the Werner-von-Reimers-Stiftung, Bad Homburg, 10-13, June. Siegfried, W. (1988). Dance, the fugitive form of art—Aesthetics as behavior. In: Rentschler, I. , Herzberger, B. , Epstein, D. (eds.), Beauty and the Brain. Basel, Boston, Berlin: Birkhäuser, pp. 117–148. Silberbauer, G. B. (1972). The G/wi Bushmen. In: Bicchieri, M. G. (ed.), Hunters and Gatherers Today. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, pp. 271–326. Silberbauer, G. B. (1973). Socio-Ecology of the G/wi Bushmen. Thesis, Dept. Anthropol. and Sociol., Monash Univeristy. Simner, M. L. (1971). Newborns’ response to the cry of another infant. Develop. Psychol. 5, 136–150. Sinclair-de-Zwart, H. (1974). Psychologie der Sprachentwicklung. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie. Bd. 2, Frankfürt/M: Fischer Athenäum, pp. 73–109. Singer, D. (1968). Aggression arousal, hostile humor, catharsis. J. Personality Social Psychol. Monogr. Suppl. 8 (Vol. 1, Pt. 2). Singer, P. (1981). The Expanding Circle: Ethics and Sociobiology. Oxford: Clarendon. Sipes, R. G. (1973). War, sports and aggression: An empirical test of two rival theories. Amer. Anthropol. 75, 64–86. Siqueland, E. R. and Lipsitt, L. P. (1965). Conditioned headturning in human newborns. J. Exp. Child Psychol. 3, 356–376. Sivard, R. L. (1980). Entwicklung der Militar-und Sozialausgaben in 140 Ländern der Erde. UN-Texte, 25, WMSE- Report 1979. Skinner, B. F. (1938). The Behavior of Organisms. New York: Appleton Century Crofts. Skinner, B. F. (1957). Verbal Behavior. London: Methuen. Skinner, B. F. (1971). Beyond Freedom and Dignity. New York: Knopf. Skolnick, A. (1973). The Intimate Environment. Boston: Little Brown. Skrzipek, K. H. (1978). Menschliche “Auslösermerkmale” beider Geschlechter. I. Attrappenwahluntersuchungen der Verhaltensentwicklung. Homo 29, 75–88. Skrzipek, K. H. (1981). Menschliche “Ausläsermerkmale” beider Geschlechter. II. Attrappenwahluntersuchungen des geschlechtsspezifischen Erkennes bei Kindern und Erwachsenen. Homo 32, 105–119. Skrzipek, K. H. (1982). Menschliche “Auslösermerkmale” beider Geschlechter. III. Untersuchung der Verhaltensentwicklung mit reduzierten Attrappen. Homo 33, 1–12. 777 Skrzipek, K. H. (1983). Stammesgeschichtliche Dispositionen der geschlechtsspezifischen Serialisation des Kindes—eine experimentelle humanethologische Analyse. Homo 34, 227–238. Slaby, R. G. and Frey, K. S. (1975). Development of gender constancy and selective attention to same-sex models. Child Develop. 46, 849–856. Slobin, D. I. (1969). Questions of Language Development in Cross-Cultural Perspective. Working Paper No. 14, Language-Behavior Research Laboratory, Berkeley, CA. Slobin, D. I. (1974). Kognitive Voraussetzungen der Sprachentwicklung. In: Leuninger, H. , Miller, M. H. , and Müller, F. (eds.), Linguistik und Psychologie. Bd. 2, Frankfurt/M: Fischer Athenaum, pp. 122–165. Smith, H. W. (1981). Territorial spacing on a beach revisited: A cross-national exploration. Soc. Psychol. Quart. 44, 132. Smith, K. J. (1977). The Behavior of Communicating. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Smith, L. and Martinsen, H. (1977). The behavior of young children in a strange situation. Scand. J. Psychol. 18, 43–52. Smith, P. K. , and Daglish, L. (1977). Sex differences in parent and infant behavior in the home. Child Develop. 48, 1250–1254. Smith, P. K. , Eaton, L. , and Hindmarch, A. (1982). How one-year-olds respond to strangers: A two-persons situation. J. Genet. Psychol. 140, 147–148. Smith, P. M. (1979). Sex markers in speech. In: Scherer, K. R. and Giles, H. (eds.), Social Markers in Speech. London: Cambridge University Press, pp. 109–146. Sneath, P. H. and Sokal, R. (1973). Numerical Taxonomy. San Francisco, CA: W. H. Freeman. Snow, C. E. and Ferguson, C. A. (eds.) (1977). Talking to Children. Input and Acquisition. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. Snow, C. E. Arlman-Kupp, A. , Hassing, Y. , Jobse, J. , Joosten, J. , and Vorster, J. (1976). Mother’s speech in three social classes. J. Psycholing. Res. 5, 1–20. Snow, M. E. , Jacklin, C. N. , and Maccoby, E. E. (1983). Sex-of-child differences in father-child interaction at one year of age. Child Develop. 54, 227–232. Snyder, S. H. (1980). Biological Aspects of Mental Disorder. New York: Oxford Univ. Press. Snyder, S. H. (1984). Drug and neurotransmitter receptors in the brain. Science 224, 22–31. Snyderman, G. S. (1948). Behind the tree of peace. A sociological analysis of Iroquois warfare. Pa. Archeologist 18, 3–4. Sobotta, J. and Becher, H. (1972, 17th ed.). Atlas der Anatomie des Menschen, Bd. 1. München/Berlin/Wien: Urban & Schwarzenberg. Somit, A. (ed.) (1976). Biology and Politics. Recent Explorations. The Hague/Paris: Mouton. Somit, A. and Slagter, R. (1983). Biopolitics: Heutiger Stand und weitere Entwicklung, In: Flohr, H. and Tonnesmann, W. (eds.), Politik und Biologie. Berlin: Paul Parey, pp. 30–37. Sommer, R. and Becker, F. D. (1969). Territorial Defense and the Good Neighbor. J. Personality Social Psychol. 11, 85–92. Sorenson, E. R. (1967). A research film program in the study of changing man: Research filmed material as a foundation for continued study of non-recurring human events. Current Anthropol. 8, 443–469. Sorenson, E. R. (1976). The Edge of the Forest. Land, Childhood and Change in a New Guinea Protoagricultural Society. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Inst. Press. Sorenson, E. R. and Gadjusek, D. C. (1966). The study of child behavior and development in primitive cultures. Pediatrics Suppl. 37, 149–243. Sostek, A. M. , Scanlon, J. W. , and Abramson, D. C. (1982). Postpartum contact and maternal confidence and anxiety: A confirmation of short-term effects. Infant Behav. Develop. 5, 323–329. Soussignan, R. and Koch, P. (1985). Rhythmical stereotypes (Leg-Swinging) associated with reductions in heart- rate in normal school children. Biol. Psychol. 21, 1–7. Spalding, D. A. (1873). Instinct with original observation on young animals. MacMillan’s Mag. 27, 282–283. [Neudruck Brit. J. Anim. Behav. 2, 1-11, 1954.] Sparling, D. W. and Williams, J. D. (1978). Multivariate analyses of avian vocalizations. J. Theoret. Biol. 74, 83–107. 778 Spemann, H. (1938). Embryonic Development and Induction. New Haven: Yale University Press. Sperry, R. W. (1945). The problem of central nervous reorganization after nerve regeneration and muscle transposition. Quart. Rev. Biol. 20, 311–369. (a) Sperry, R. W. (1945). Restoration of vision after crossing of optic nerves and after contralateral transplantation of eye. J. Neurophysiol. 8, 115–128. (b) Sperry, R. W. (1964). The great cerebral commissure. Sci. Amer. 210, 42–52. Sperry, R. W. (1965). Selective communication in nerve nets: Impulse specificity vs. connection specificity. Neurosci. Res. Program Bull. 3, 37–43. Sperry, R. W. (1971). How a brain gets wired for adaptive function. In: Tobach, E. , Aronson, L. R. , and Shaw, E. (eds.), The Biopsychology of Development. London: Academic Press, pp. 27–44. Sperry, R. W. (1974). Lateral specialisation in the surgically separated hemispheres. In: Schmitt, F.O. , and Worden, F. G. (eds.), The Neurosciences-Third Study Program. Cambridge: MIT Press. Sperry, R. W. and Preilowski, B. (1972). Die beiden Gehirne des Menschen. Bild Wissenschaft , pp. 920–928. Spiel, W. (1980). Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts, Bd. 12: Konsequenzen für die Pädagogik, 2. Zürich: Kindler. Spiro, M. E. (1954). Is the family universal? Amer. Anthropol. 56, 839–846. Spiro, M. E. (1958). Children of the Kibbutz. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Spiro, M. E. (1979). Gender and Culture: Kibbutz Women Revisited. Durham, North Carolina: Duke University Press. Spitz, R. (1965). The First Year of Life. New York: International University Press. Spitz, R. (1968). Die anaklitische Depression. In: Bittner, G. and Schmid-Cords, E. (eds.), Erziehung in früher Kindheit. München: Piper. Sroufe, L. A. (1977). Wariness of stranger and the study of infant development. Child Develop. 48, 731–746. Stacey, B. (1980). Infant-mother attachment: A social psychological perspective. Soc. Behav. Personality 8, 33–40. Stahl, F. , Doerner, G. , Ahrens, L. , and Graudenz, W. (1976). Significantly decreased apparently free testosterone levels in plasma of male homosexuals. Endokrinologie 68, 115–117. Stamps, J. A. and Barlow, G. W. (1973). Variation and stereotypy in the displays of anolis geneus (Sauria: Iguanidae). Behaviour 47, 67–94. Stanjek, K. (1978). Das Überreichen von Gaben: Funktion und Entwicklung in den ersten Lebensjahren. Z. Entwicklungspsychol. Pädagog. Psychol. 10, 103–113. Stanjek, K. (1979). Die Entwicklung des menschlichen Besitzverhaltens. Dissertation der Fak. Biologie der Ludwig- Maximilians-Universität München (Materialien aus der Bildungsforschung, 16, Max-Planck-Institut für Bildungsforschung Berlin). Staub, E. and Noerenberg, H. (1981). Property rights, deservingness, reciprocity, friendship: The Transactional Character of Children’s Sharing Behavior. J. Personality Social Psychol. 40, 271–289. Stein, L. (1980). The chemistry of reward. In: Routtenberg, A. (ed.), Biology of Reinforcement: Facets of Brain- Stimulation Reward. New York: Academic Press, pp. 109–132. Steinbuch, K. (71973). Kurskorrektur. Stuttgart: Seewald. Steiner, G. (ed.) (1978). Die Psychologie des 20. Jahrhunderts. 7. Piaget und die Folgen. Zürich: Kindler. Steiner, J. E. (1973). The gustofacial response: Observation on normal and anencephalic newborn infants. In: Bosma, J. F. (ed.), Symposium on Oral Sensation and Perception— IV (Development in the Fetus and Infant). Bethesda, MD: Dhew & Fogarty Int. Center, pp. 254–278. Steiner, J. E. (1974). Innate discriminative human facial expression to taste and smell stimulation. (Discussion paper). Ann. NY Acad. Sci. 237, 229–233. Steiner, J. E. (1979). Human facial expressions in response to taste and smell stimulation. Child Develop. Behav. 13. Steiner, J. E. and Horner, R. (1972). The human gustofacial response. Israel J. Med. Sci. 8, 32. 779 Steklis, H. D. and Raleigh, M. J. (1979). Behavioral and neurobiological aspects of primate vocalization and facial expression. In: Steklis, H. D. and Raleigh, M. J. (eds.), Neurobiology of Social Communication in Primates. New York: Academic Press, pp. 257–282. Stent, G. S. , Kristan, W. B. , Friesen, W. O. , Ort, C. A. , Poon, M. , and Calabrese, R. L. Stent, G. S. , Kristan, W. B. , Friesen, W. O. , Ort, C. A. , Poon, M. , and Calabrese, R. L. (1978). Neuronal generation of the leech swimming movement. Science 200, 1348–1557. Stephens, W. N. (1962). The Oedipus Complex: Cross-Cultural Evidence. New York: Free Press of Glencoe. Stephens, W. N. (1963). The Family in Cross-Cultural Perspective. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Stern, D. N. (1971). A micro-analysis of mother-infant interaction behavior regulating social contact between a mother and her 3½ months old twins. J. Amer. Acad. Child Psychiatry 10, 501–517. Stern, D. N. (1974). Mother and infant at play: The dyadic interaction involving facial, vocal and gaze behaviours. In: Lewis, M. and Rosenblum, L. (eds.), The Effect of the Infant on Its Caregiver. London: Wiley, pp. 187–214. Stern, D. N. (1977). The First Relationship, Mother and Infant. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press. Stern, D. N. , Beebe, B. , Jaffe, J. , and Bennett, S. L. (1977). The infant’s stimulus world during social interaction: A study of caregiver behaviours with particular reference to repetition and timing. In: Schaffer, H. R. (ed.), Studies on Interactions in Infancy. London: Academic Press, pp. 177–203. Stern, D. N. , Spieker, S. , and MacKain, K. (1982). Intonation contours as signals in maternal speech to prelingusitic infants. Develop. Psychol. 18, 727–735. Steward, J. H. (1938). Basin-Plateau aboriginal sociopolitical groups. Bureau Amer. Ethnol. Bull , p. 120. Stewart, R. B. (1983). Sibling attachment relationships: Child-infant interactions in the strange stituation. Develop. Psychol. 19, 192–199. Stewart, V. M. (1973). Tests of the “Carpentered World” Hypothesis by race and environment in America and Zambia. Intern. J. Psychol. 8, 83–94. Stokoe, W. C. , Croneberg, C. G. , and Casterline, D. (1965). A Dictionary of American Sign Language. Washington, D.C.: Gallaudet College Press. Stoller, R. J. (1975). Perversion: The Erotic Form of Hatred. New York: Pantheon Books. [Perversion: Die erotische Form von Haβ. Hamburg, Rowohlt 1979.] Stopa, R. (1972). Structure of Bushman and Its Traces in Indo-European. Kraków: Polska Akademia Nauk. Stopa, R. (1975). Evolution der Sprache. Nova Acta Leopoldina 42 [N. F.] 218, 355-375. Paper presented at the annual meeting of the Deutsche Akademie der Naturforscher Leopoldina, Halle, October 1973. Strathern, A. (1971). The Rope of Moka: Big-Men and Ceremonial Exchange in Mount Hagen, New Guinea. London/New York: Cambridge University Press. Strathern, A. (1974). Melpa Amb Kenan. Courting Songs of the Melpa people. Institute of Papua New Guinea Studies. The printery Port Moresby. Strathern, A. (1979). Ongka. A Self-Account by a New Guinea Big-Man. London: Duckworth. Strathern, A. (1983). Biology and Aesthetics: Some Thoughts from Papua New Guinea. Paper presented at the 3rd International Conference on Hunters and Gatherers, Reimers-Stiftung, Bad Homburg, June 1983. Stratton, P. (ed.) (1982). Psychobiology of the Human Newborn. Chichester/New York: Wiley, (a) Stratton, P. (1982). Rhythmic function in the newborn. In: Stratton, P. (ed.), Psychobiology of the Human Newborn. Chichester/New York: Wiley, pp. 119–145. (b) Strayer, J. (1980). A Naturalistic study of empathic behaviors and their relation to affective states and perspective- taking sills in preschool children. Child Develop. 51, 815–822. Strehlow, C. (1915). Die Aranda-und Loritja-Stämme in Central-Australien. Veröffentlichungen Städtischen Völkermuseums Frankfürt/M. , 4, 1–78. Strehlow, T. G. (1970). Geography and totemic landscape in Central Australia: A functional study. In: Berndt, R. M. (ed.), Australian Aboriginal Anthropology. Nedland, W. Australia: University of W. Australia Press, pp. 92–140. Studdert-Kennedy, M. (1982). Die Anfange der Sprache. In: Immelmann, K. , Barlow, G. , 780 Petrinovich, L. , and Main, M. (eds.), Verhaltensentwicklung bei Mensch und Tier. Das Bielefeld-Projekt. Berlin/Hamburg: Paul Parey, pp. 640–667. Sugawara, K. (1984). Spatial proximity and bodily contact among the Central Kalahari San. Af. Study Monogr. Suppl. 3, 1–43. Sugiyama, Y. (1964). Group composition, population density and some sociological observations of Hanuman langurs (Presbytis entellus). Primates 5, 7–37. Sutton-Smith, B. (1979). The play of girls. In: Kopp, C. B. and Kirkpatrick, M. (eds.), Becoming Females: Perspectives and Development. New York: Plenum, pp. 229–257. Svaare, B. B. (1983). Hormones and Aggressive Behavior. New York: Plenum Press. Swadesh, M. (1948). Motivations in Nootka warfare. Southwest. J. Anthropol. 4, 76–93. Sweet, L. (1965). Camel pastoralism in North Arabia and the minimal camping unit. In: Vayda, A. P. (ed.), Environment and Cultural Behavior. Garden City, NY: Natural History Press, pp. 155–180. Sweet, W. H. , Ervin, F. , and Mark, V. H. (1969). The relationship of violent behaviour to focal cerebral disease. In: Garattini, S. and Sigg, E. B. (eds.), Aggressive Behaviour. Amsterdam: Excerpta Media Foundation, pp. 336–352. Symons, D. (1980). The evolution of human sexuality. (Multiple Book Rev.) Behav. Brain Sci. 3, 171–214. Tajfel, H. (ed.) (1978). Differentiation between Social Groups. London/New York: Academic Press. Tajfel, H. (1982). Social Identity and Intergroup Relations. Cambridge: Cambridge Univ. Press. Tanaka, J. (1980). The San Hunter-Gatherers of the Kalahari. A Study in Ecological Anthropology. Tokyo: University of Tokyo Press. Tartter, V. C. (1980). Happy talk: Perceptual and acoustic effects of smiling on speech. Perception Psychophys. 27, 24–27. Taub, E. , Ellman, St. J. and Berman, A. J. (1965). Deafferentiation in monkeys. Effects on conditioned grasp response. Science 151, 593–594. Tauber, M. A. (1979). Sex Differences in parent-child interaction styles during a free-play session. Child Develop. 50, 981–988. Taylor, S. (1981). Symbol and ritual under national socialism. Brit. J. Sociol. 32, 504–520. Teleki, G. (1973). Predatory Behavior of Wild Chimpanzees. Lewisburg: Bucknell University Press. Tembrock, G. (1975). Phonetische Eigenschaften von Primatenlauten im -Aspekt. Nova Acta Leopoldina 42 [N. F.] (with record), 218, 343-353. Terhardt, E. (1982). Die psychoakustischen Grundlagen der musikalischen Akkordgrundtöne und deren algorithmische Bestimmung. In: Dalhlhaus, C. and Krause, M. (eds.), Tiefenstruktur der Musik. Technische Universität, Berlin, pp. 23–50. Terhardt, E. (1984). The concept of musical consonance: A link between music and psychoacoustics. Music Perception 1, 276–295. Terrace, H. S. (1979). Nim. New York: Alfred A. Knopf. Terrace, H. S. , Petitto, L. A. , Sanders, R. J. , and Bever, T. G. (1979). Can an ape create a sentence? Science 206, 891–902. Thelen, E. (1980). Determinants of amounts of stereotyped behavior in normal human infants. Ethol. Sociobiol. 1, 141–150. Thomae, H. (ed.) (1954). Handbuch der Psychologie 3: Entwicklungspsychologie. Göttingen: C. J. Hogrefe. Thomae, H. (1983). Learning to walk is still an “old” problem: A reply to Zelazo (1983). J. Motor Behav. 15, 139–161. Thomae, H. (1984). Learning to walk: Ecological demands and phylogenetic constraints. In: Lipsitt, L. P. and Rovee- Collier, C. (eds.), Advances in Infancy Research, Vol. 3. Norwood, NJ: Ablex, pp. 213–260. Thompson, E. P. (1972). Rough Music. Le Charivari anglais. Ann. ESC 27, 285–312. Thornhill, R. and Wilmsen-Thornhill, N. (1983). Human rape: An evolutionary analysis. Ethol. Sociobiol. 4, 137–173. Thorpe, W. H. (1958). The learning of song patterns by birds, with special reference to the song of the chaffinch, Fringilla coelebs. Ibis 100, 535–570. Thorpe, W. H. (1961). Sensitive periods in the learning of animals and men. A study of imprinting with special reference to the introduction of cyclic behavior. In: Thorpe, W. H. and Zangwill, O. L. (eds.), Current Problems in Animal Behavior. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, pp. 194–224. 781 Tieger, T. (1980). On the biological basis of sex differences in aggression. Child Develop. 51, 943–963. Tiger, L. (1969). Men in Groups. New York: Random House. Tiger, L. (1976). Ions of emotion and political behavior: Notes on prototheory. In: Somit, A. (ed.), Biology and Politics. Paris: Mouton, pp. 263–267. Tiger, L. and Fox, R. (1966). The zoological perspective in social science. Man 1, 75–81. Tiger, L. (1971). The Imperial Animal. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston. Tiger, L. and Shepher, J. (1975). Women in the Kibbutz. New York: Harcourt, Brace Jovanovich, Inc. Tinbergen, E. A. and Tinbergen, N. (1972). Early Childhood Autism—An Ethological Approach. Fortschritte der Verhaltensforschung 10. Berlin: Parey. Tinbergen, J. (ed.) (1977). Wir haben nur eine Zukunft. Opladen: Westdeutscher Verlag. Tinbergen, N. (1940). Die Übersprungbewegung. Z. Tierpsychol. 4, 1–40. Tinbergen, N. (1948). Social releasers and the experimental method required for their study. Wiss. Bull , 60, 6–52. Tinbergen, N. (1951). The Study of Instinct. London: Oxford University Press. Tinbergen, N. (1955). Tiere untereinander. Berlin: Parey. Tinbergen, N. (1959). Einige Gedanken über “Beschwichtigungsgebarden.” Z. Tierpsychol. 16, 651–665. Tinbergen, N. (31963). The Herring Gull’s World. A Study of the Social Behavior of Birds. London: Collins. Tinbergen, N. (1981). On the history of war. In: Valzelli, I. and Morgese, I. (eds.), Aggression and Violence: A Psycho/Biological and Clinical Approach. Milano: Edizione Saint Vincent, pp. 31–38. Tinbergen, N. and Kuenen, D. J. (1939). Uber die auslosenden und richtungsgebenden Reizsituationen der Sperrbewegung von jungen Drosseln (Turdus m. merula L. and T. e. ericetorum Turton). Z. Tierpsychol. 3, 37–60. Tinbergen, N. and Tinbergen, E. A. (1983). “Autistic” Children: New Hope for a Cure. London: Allen & Unwin. Tinbergen, N. , Imperkoven, M. , and Franck, D. (1967). An experiment on spacing-out as a defense against predation. Behaviour 28, 307–321. Tinbergen, N. , Broekhuysen, G. J. , Feekes, F. , Houghton, J. C. W. Kruuk, H. , and Szulc, E. (1962). Eggshell removal by the blackheaded gull (Lazarus ribidundus L.): A behavior component of camouflage. Behaviour 19, 74–117. Tobach, E. (1976). Evolution of behavior and comparative method. Intern. J. Psychol. 11, 185–201. Tobach, E. , Gianutsos, J. , Topoff, H. R. , and Gross, C. G. (1974). The Four Horsemen: Racism, Militarism and Social Darwinism. New York: Behavioral Publications. Tobias, Ph. V. (1964). Bushman-hunter-gatherers. A study in human ecology. In: Davis, D. H. S. (ed.), Ecological Studies in Southern Africa. The Hague: W. Junk. Tomkins, S. S. and McCarter, R. (1964). What and where are the primary affects? Some evidence for a therory. Percept. Motor Skills 18, 119–158. Tomoda, Z. (1937). The perception of figures as dependent upon their form and size. Jap. J. Psychol. 12, 433–450. Tornay, S. (1979). Armed conflicts in the lower Oma Valley, 1970-1976. Senri Ethnol. Stud. 3, 97–117. Trautner, H. M. (1979). Psychologische Theorien der Geschlechtsrollen-Entwicklung. In: Degenhardt, A. and Trautner, H. M. (eds.), Geschlechtstypisches Verhalten. München: C. H. Beck, pp. 50–84. Trevarthen, C. (1975). Growth of visuomotor coordination in infants. J. Human Movement Stud. 1, 57. Trevarthen, C. (1979). Instincts for human understanding and for cultural cooperation: Their development in infancy. In: Cranach, M. V. , Foppa, K. , Lepenies, W. , and Ploog, D. (eds.), Human Ethology, Claims and Limits of a New Discipline. London/Cambridge: Cambridge University Press, Paris (Maison des Sciences de l’Homme), pp. 530–571. Trevarthen, C. (1983). Interpersonal abilities of infants as generators for transmission of language and culture. In: Oliverio, A. and Zappella, M. (eds.), The Behavior of Human Infants. New York/London: Plenum, pp. 145–176. Trevarthen, C. and Hubley, P. (1978). Secondary intersubjectivity: confidence, confiding and acts of meaning in the first year. In: Lock, A. (ed.), Action, Gestures and Symbol: The emergence of language. London: Academic Press, pp. 183–229. 782 Triandis, H. C. and Lambert, W. W. (1958). A restatement and test of Schlosberg’s theory of emotion with two kinds of subjects from Greece. J. Abnormal Social Psychol. 56, 321–328. Tripp, C. A. (1975). The Homosexual Matrix. New York: Signet. Trivers, R. L. (1971). The evolution of reciprocal altruism Quart. Rev. Biol. 46, 35–37. Trivers, R. L. (1972). Parental investment and sexual selection. In: Campbell, B. (ed.), Sexual Selection and the Descent of Man, 1871-1971. London: Heinemann, pp. 136–179. Trivers, R. L. (1974). Parent offspring conflict. Amer. Zool. 14, 249–264. Trojan, F. (1975). Biophonetik. Mannheim: Bibliograph. Institut-Wissenschaftsverlag. Tronick, E. , Als, H. , Adamson, L. , Wise, S. , and Brazelton, T. B. (1978). The infant’s response to entrapment between contradictory messages in face-to-face interaction. Amer. Acad. Child Psychiatry , pp. 1–13. Turco, R. P. , Toon, O. B. , Ackerman, T. P. , Pollack, J. B. , and Sagan, C. (1983). Nuclear winter: Global consequences of multiple nuclear explosions. Science 222, 1283–1292. Turke, P. W. (1984). Effects of ovulatory concealment and synchrony on protohominid mating systems and parental roles. Ethol. Sociobiol. 5, 33–44. Turnbull, C. M. (1961). The Forest People. London: Chatto & Windus. Turnbull, C. M. (1965). The Mbuti Pygmies. An ethnographic survey. Anthropol. Papers. Amer. Mus. Nat. Hist. 50, 282. Turner, F. and Pöppel, E. (1983). The neural lyre: Poetic meter, the brain and time. Poetry , August 1983, pp. 277–309. Turney-High, H. H. (1949). Primitive War, Its Practices and Concepts. Columbia, SC: University of South Carolina Press. Turton, D. (1979). War, peace and Mursi identity. Senri Ethnol. Stud. 3, 179–210. Tyler, D. S. (1979). Time-sampling, a matter of convention. Anim. Behav. 27, 801–810. Tyrell, H. (1978). Family as the original institution — Recent speculation regarding an old question. Kölner Z. Sozio. Sozial-Psychol. 30, 611–651. Tyson, J. E. (1977). Nursing and prolactin secretion: Principal determinants in the mediation of puerperal infertility. In: Crossignani, P. G. and Robyn C. (eds.), Prolactin and Human Reproduction. New York: Academic Press. Tyson, J. E. , Carter, J. N. , Andreasson, B. , Huth, J. , and Smith, B. (1978). Nursing mediated prolactin and luteinising hormone secretion during puerperal lactation. Fertility Sterility 30, 154–162. Ultan, R. (1978). Size-sound symbolism. In: Greenberg, J. H. , Ferguson, C. A. , and Maravcsik, E. A. (eds.), Universals of Human Language, 2 Phonology. Stanford, CA: Stanford University Press, pp. 525–568. Umiker-Sebeok, J. and Sebeok, Th. A. (1981). Clever Hans and smart simians. The self-fulfilling prophecy and kindred methodological pitfalls. Anthropos 76, 89–165. Umiker-Sebeok, J. and Sebeok, Th. A. (1982). Rejoinder to the Rumbaughs. Anthropos 77, 574–578. Uyenoyama, M. K. (1979). Evolution of altruism under group selection in large and small populations in fluctuating environments. Theoret. Popul. Biol. 15, 58–85. Uyenoyama, M. K. and Feldman, M. W. (1980). Theories of kin and group selection: A population genetics perspective. Theoret. Popul. Biol 17, 380–414. Valzelli, L. and Morgese, L. (eds.) (1981). Aggression and Violence: A Psychobiological and Clinical Approach. Proceedings of the First St. Vincent Special Conference, 14/15 Oct., 1980. Centro Culturale e congressi, Saint Vincent. Vandell, D. L. , Wilson, K. S. , and Buchanan, N. R. (1980). Peer interaction in the first year of life: An examination of its structure, content, sensitivity to toys. Child Develop. 51, 481–488. Varney, N. R. and Vilensky, J. A. (1980). Neuropsychological implications for preadaptation and language evolution. J. Human Evol. 9, 223–226. Vayda, A. P. (1960). Maori Warfare. Polynesian Society Maori Monographs, 2, Wellington. Vayda, A. P. (1961). Expansion and warfare among Swidden agriculturalists. Amer. Anthropol. 63, 346–358. Vayda, A. P. (1967). Research on the functions of primitive war. Peace Res. Soc. Intern. Pap. Bd. 7 Vayda, A. P. (1970). Maoris and muskets in New Zealand: Disruption of a war system. Political Sci. Quart. 85, 560–584. 783 Vayda, A. P. (1971). Phases in the process of war and peace among the marings of New Guinea. Oceania 42, 1–24. (a) Vayda, A. P. (1971). Hypothesen zur Funktion des Krieges. In: Fried, M. , Harris, M. , and Murphy, R. (eds.), Der Krieg. Frankfürt/M.: S. Fischer, pp. 103–110. (b) Vayda, A. P. and McCay, B. (1975). New directions in ecology and ecological anthropology. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 4, 293–306. Vayda, A. P. and Rappaport, R. A. (1968). Ecology, cultural, and non-cultural. In: Clifton, J. A. (ed.), Introduction to Cultural Anthropology. Boston: Houghton Mifflin, pp. 477–497. Vedder, H. (1937). Die Buschmänner Südwestafrikas and ihre Weltanschauung. South Afr. J. Sci. 24, 416–436. Verba, M. , Stambak, M. , and Sinclair, H. (1982). Physical knowledge and social interaction in children from 18 to 24 months of age. In: Forman, G. (ed.), Action and Thought: Sensory Motor Schemes to Symbolic Operations. London/New York: Academic Press, pp. 267–296. Verebey, K. (1982). Opioids in mental illness theories, clinical observations and treatment possibilities. Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci. , p. 398. Vester, F. (1972). Das Uberlebensprogramm. München: Kindler. Vicedom, C. F. and Tischner, H. (1943/1948). Die Mbowamb. Band I. Die Kultur der Hagenbergstämme. Monograph, zur Völkerkunde 1. Hamburg. Vierling, J. S. and Rock, J. (1967). Variations in olfactory sensitivity to exaltolide during the menstrual cycle. J. Appl. Physiol. 22, 311–315. Vinacke, W. E. (1949). The judgement of facial expressions by three national-racial groups in Hawaii: I. Caucasian faces. J. Personality 17, 407–429. Vining, D. (1982). On the possibility of reemergence of a dysgenic trend with respect to intelligence in American fertility differentials. Intelligence 6, 241–264. (a) Vining, D. (1982). Fertility differentials and the status of nations: A speculative essay on Japan and the West. Mankind Quart. 22, 311–353. (b) Vining, D. (1986). Social versus reproductive success: The central theoretical problem of human sociobiology. Behav. Brain Sci. 9, 167–216. Vinter, A. (1985). L’Imitation chez le Nouveau Né: imitation, representation et mouvement dans les premiers mois de la vie. Neuchatel/Paris: Delacheaux. Vinter, A. (1986). A developmental perspective on behavioral determinants. Acta Psychol. 63, 337–349. Vogel, Ch . (1977). Geschlechtstypisches Verhalten bei nichtmenschlichen Primaten-zugleich ein Beitrag zur Evolution geschlechtstypischen Verhaltens beim Menschen. Paper presented at the annual meeting of Anthropology and Humangenetics. Hamburg, 21 Sept. 1977. (a) Vogel, Ch . (1977). Primatenforschung — Beiträge zum Selbstverständnis des Menschen. Vortragsreihe der Niedersächsischen Landesregierung zur Forderung der wiss. Forschung in Niedersachsen. Göttingen: Vandenhoek & Ruprecht. (b) Vogel, Ch . (1979). Der Hanuman-Langur (Presbytis entellus), ein Paradeexemplar für die theoretischen Konzepte der “Soziobiologie”? Verhandl. Deut. Zool. Ges. , pp. 73–89. Vogel, C.H. and Loch, H. (1984). Reproductive parameters. Adult-male replacements, and infanticide among free- ranging langurs (Presbytis entellus) at Jodhpur (Rajasthan), India. In: Hausfater, G. and Blaffer Hrdy, S. (eds.), Infanticide: Comparative and Evolutionary Perspectives. New York: Aldine, pp. 237–255. Vogel, Ch ., Voland, E., and Winter, M. (1979). Geschlechtstypische Verhaltensentwicklung bei nichtmenschlichen Primaten. In: Degenhardt, A. and Thautner, H. M. (eds.), Geschlechtstypisches Verhalten. Miinchen: C. H. Beck, pp. 145–181. Vollmer, G. (1975). Evolutionare Erkenntnistheorie. Stuttgart: Hirzel. Vollmer, G. (1983). Mesokosmos and objektive Erkenntnis-Über Probleme, die von der evolutionären Erkenntnistheorie gelöst werden. In: Lorenz, K. and Wuketits, F. M. (eds.), Die Evolution des Denkens. München/Zürich: Piper, pp. 29–91. Vossen, A. (1971). Die Früherfassung zerebral geschädigter Kinder. Deut. Ärzteblatt 68, 3136–3144. Vrugt, A. and Kerkstra, A. (1984). Sex differences in nonverbal communication. Semiotica 50, 1–41. Vygotsky, L. S. (1960). Selected Psychological Research. Moskow: APN RSFSR. de Waal, F. (1982). Chimpanzee Politics-Power and Sex among Apes. London: Jonathan Cape. 784 de Waal, F. (1986). The integration of dominance and social bonding in primates. The Quart. Rev. Biol. 61, 459–479. Wade, M. J. (1978). A critical review of the models of group selection. Quart. Rev. Biol. 53, 101–114. Wade, M. J. (1980). Kin selection: Its components. Science 210, 665–667. Wagner, Sh . and Winner, E. (1979/1980). Metaphorical Mapping in Human Infants. Paper presented at the Eastern Psychological Association meetings, April, 1979. Wallhäusser, E. and Scheich, H. (1987). Auditory imprinting leads to differential 2-deoxy-glucose uptake and dendritic spine loss in the chick rostral forebrain. Develop. Brain Res. 31, 29–44. Walters, R. H. and Thomas, E. L. (1963). Enhancement of punitiveness by visual and audiovisual displays. Canad. J. Psychol. 17, 244–255. Wangerin, G. and Weiss, G. (1976). Heinrich Tessenow. Essen: Verlag Richard Bacht. Warburton, D. M. (1975). Brain, Behavior, and Drugs: Introduction to the Neurochemistry of Behaviour. London: Wiley. Warner, W. L. (1930). Murngin Warfare. Oceania 1, 457–494. Warnke, M. (1984). Politische Architektur in Europa. Köln: Dumont. Washburn, S. L. and Hamburg, D. A. (1965). The implications of primate research. In: DeVore, I. (ed.), Primate Behavior. New York: Holt, Reinhart & Winston, pp. 607–622. Washburn, S. L. and Hamburg, D. A. (1968). Aggressive behavior in old world monkeys and apes. In: Jay, P. C. (ed.), Primates: Studies in Adaptation and Variability. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, pp. 458–478. Wasserman, G. A. and Stern, D. N. (1978). An early manifestation of differential behavior toward children of the same and opposite sex. J. Genet. Psychol. 133, 129–137. Waters, W. , Matas, L. , and Sroufe, L. A. (1975). Infants’ reactions to an approaching stranger: Description, validation and functional significance of wariness. Child Develop. 46, 348–356. Waters, E. , Wippman, J. , and Sroufe, L. A. (1979). Attachment, positive effect and competence in the peer group. Child Develop. 50, 821–829. Watson, J. S. (1971). Cognitive-perceptual development in infancy: Setting for the seventies. Merrill-Palmer Quart. 17, 139–152. Watson, J. S. (1972). Smiling, cooing, and “The Game.” Merrill-Palmer Quart. 18, 323–329. Watson, J. S. (1977). Depression and the perception of control in early childhood. In: Schulterbrandt, J. G. and Raskin, A. (eds.), Depression in Childhood: Diagnosis, Treatment and Conceptual Models. New York: Raven. Watson, J. S. (1979). Perception of contingency as a determinant of social responsiveness. In: Thoman, E. B. (ed.), Origins of the Infant’s Social Responsiveness. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, pp. 33–64. Watson, J. S. and Ramsey, C. T. (1972). Reactions to response-contingent stimulation in early infancy. Merrill- Palmer Quart. 18, 219–227. Watzlawick, P. (ed.) (1981): Die erfundene Wirklichkeit. Beiträge zum Konstruktivismus. München/Zürich: Piper. Wawra, M. (1985). Aufschauverhalten und Gruppengröβe beim Menschen. Diplomarbeit, Zoologisches Institut der Universität Freiburg im Breisgau. Weber, M. (1981). Der Beruf zur Politik. Berlin: Duncker & Humblot. Webster, M. Jr. and Driskell, J. E. (1983). Beauty as status. Amer. J. Sociol. 89, 140–165. Wedgwood, C. H. (1930). Some aspects of warfare in Melanesia. Oceania 1, 5–33. Weinberg, S. K. (1955). Incest Behavior. New York: Citadel. Weisfeld, C. C. , Weisfeld, G. E. , and Callaghan, J. W. (1982). Female inhibition in mixed-sex competition among young adolescents. Ethol. Sociobiol. 3, 29–42. Weisfeld, C. C. , Weisfeld, G. E. , Warren, R. A. , and Freedman, D. G. (1983). The spelling bee: A naturalistic study of female inhibition in mixed-sex competition. Adolescence 18, 695–708. Weisfeld, G. E. (1980). Social dominance and human motivation. In: Omark, D. R. , Strayer, F. F. , and Freedman, D. G. (eds.), Dominance Relations. An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interactions. New York/London: Garland STPM Press, pp. 273–286. Weisfeld, G. E. and Berger, J. M. (1983). Some features of human adolescence viewed in evolutionary perspective. Human Develop. 26, 121–133. 785 Weisfeld, G. E. , Omark, D. R. , and Cronin, C. L. (1980). A longitudinal and cross-sectional study of dominance in boys. In: Omark, D. R. , Strayer, F. F. , and Freedman, D. G. (eds.), Dominance Relations: An Ethological View of Human Conflict and Social Interactions. New York/London: Garland STPM Press, pp. 205–216. Weisfeld, G. E. , Block S. A. , and Ivers, J. W. (1984). Possible determinants of social dominance among adolescent girls. J. Genet. Psychol. 144, 115–129. Weisner, Th. S. (1979). Urban-rural differences in sociable and disruptive behavior of Kenya children. Ethnology 18, 153–172. (a) Weisner, Th. S. (1979). Some cross-cultural perspectives on becoming female. In: Kopp, C. B. and Kirkpatrick, M. (eds.), Becoming Female. New York: Plenum, pp. 313–332. (b) Weisner, Th. S. and Gallimore, R. (1977). My brother’s keeper: Child and sibling caretaking. Current Anthropol. 18, 169–199. Weitzman, E. D. (1982). Chronobiology of man. Sleep, temperature and neuroendocrine rhythms. Human Neurohiol. 1, 173Weisner, Th. S.183. von Weizäcker, C. Fr. (1977). Der Garten des Menschlichen. Beitrage zur geschichtlichen Anthropologic. München: Hanser. Werner, H. (1948). Comparative Psychology of Mental Development. New York: Follett. Werner, H. and Kaplan, B. (1963). Symbol Formation. New York: Wiley. Wertheimer, M. (1927). Gestaltpsychologische Forschung. In: Saupes Einführung in die neuere Psychologie. Osterwieck/Harz. West, M. M. and Konner, M. J. (1976). The Role of the Father: An Anthropological Perspective. In: Lamb, M. E. (ed.), The Role of the Father in Child Development. New York: Wiley, pp. 185–216. West, S. G. and Brown, T. J. (1975). Physical attractiveness, the severity of the emergency and helping: A field experiment and interpersonal simulation. J. Exp. Social Psychol. 11, 531–538. West-Eberhard, M. J. (1975). The evolution of social behavior by kin selection. Quart. Rev. Biol. 50, 1–33. Westermarck, E. (1894). The History of Human Marriage. London: Macmillan. Westin, A. (1970). Privacy and Freedom. New York: Atheneum. Wever, R. (1975). The circadian multi-oscillator system of man. J. Chronobiol. 3, 19–55. Wever, R. (1978). Grundlagen der Tagesperiodik beim Menschen. In: Heimann, H. and Pflug, B. (eds.), Rhythmusprobleme in der Psychiatrie. Stuttgart/New York: Fischer, pp. 1–23. Wever, R. (1980). Die Tagesperiodik des Menschen. Grundlagen und Probleme Betriebsärztliches 1, 1–32. Wex, M. (1979). “Weibliche” und “männliche” Körpersprache als Folge patriarchalischer Machtverhältnisse. Hamburg: Verlag M. Wex. White, L. (1959). The Evolution of Culture. New York: McGraw-Hill. Whiting, B. B. and Edwards, C. P. (1973). A cross-cultural anaylsis of sex-differences in the behavior of children aged three through eleven. J. Social Psychol. 91, 171–188. Whiting, J. W. M. (1941). Becoming a Kwoma. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press. Whyte, W. (1965). Herr und Opfer der Organisation. Düsseldorf: Econ. Wickler, W. (1965). Über den taxonomischen Wert homologer Verhaltensmerkmale. Naturwissenschaften 52, 441–444. Wickler, W. (1966). Ursprung und biologische Deutung des Genitalprasentierens mannlicher Primaten. Z. Tierpsychol. 23, 422–437. Wickler, W. (1967). Socio-sexual signals and their intraspeciflc imitation among primates. In: Morris, D. (ed.), Primate Ethology. London: Weidenfeld & Nicolson, pp. 69–147. Wickler, W. (1967). Vergleichende Verhaltensforschung und Phylogenetik. In: Heberer, G. (ed.), Die Evolution der Organismen. Stuttgart: Fischer, pp. 420–508. Wickler, W. (1968). Mimikry-Signalfalschung in der Natur. Miinchen: Kindler. Wickler, W. (1969). Sind wir Sunder? Naturgesetze der Ehe. Miinchen: Droemer. Wickler, W. (1971). Die Biologie der Zehn Gebote. Miinchen: Piper, Neuausgabe 1975 (Serie Piper 72). Wickler, W. (1977). Vom Ursprung sozialer Abhangigkeiten. Vortrag der Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, November 1976, Regensburg. Max-Planck-Gesellschaft, Jahrbuch 1977, 86-102 Wickler, W. (1978). Die Evolution unsozialen Verhaltens. Berl. Munch. Tierarztl. Wochschr. 91, 486–488. Wickler, W. (1978). Abnormes Sozialverhalten als Strategic desgesunden Individuums. Festvortrag, Oktober 1978, Regensburg, 61. Fortbildungstagung für Ärzte. (b) 786 Wickler, W. (1979). Pre-Wilsonian Sociobiology. Z. Tierpsychol. 49, 433–434. Wickler, W. and Seibt, U. (1977). Das Prinzip Eigennutz. Ursachen und Konsequenzen sozialen Verhaltens. Hamburg: Hoffmann und Campe. Wickler, W. and Seibt, U. (1981). Monogamy in Crustacea and Man. Z. Tierpsychol. 57, 215–234. Wickler, W. and Seibt, U. (1982). Song Splitting in the Evolution of Dueting. Z. Tierpsychol. 59, 127–140. (a) Wickler, W. and Seibt, U. (1982). Alt-Ekuadorianische Spinnwirtel und ihre Bildmotive. Beitr. Allgem. Vergleich. Archäol. 4, 315–419. (b) Wickler, W. and Uhrig, D. (1969). Bettelrufe, Antwortszeit und Rassenunterschiede im Begruβungsduett des Schmuckbartvogels. Z. Tierpsychol. 26, 651–661. Wiepkema, P. R. (1961). An ethological analysis of the reproductive behavior of the bitterling (Rhodeus amarus Bloch). Extr. Arch. Néerland. Zool. 14, 103–199. Wiessner, P. (1977). Hxaro: A Regional System of Reciprocity for Reducing Risk among the !Kung San. Ph. D. Diss., University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, University Microfilms. Wiessner, P. (1980). History and Continuity in !Kung San Reciprocal Relationships. Paper presented at 2. Int. Conf. on Hunting and Gathering Societies, Quebec, September, 1980. Wiessner, P. (1981). Measuring the impact of social ties on nutritional status among the !Kung San. Soc. Sci. Inform. 20, 641–678. (a) Wiessner, P. (1982). Risk, reciprocity and social influences on !Kung San economics. In: Leacock, E. R. and Lee, R. B. (eds.), Politics and History in Band Societies. London: Cambridge University Press, pp. 62–84. Wiessner, P. (1983). Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. Amer. Antiquity 48, 253–276. Wiessner, P. (1984). Reconsidering the behavioral basis for style: A case study among the Kalahari San. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 3, 190–234. (a) Wiessner, P. (1984). Foodsharing among Children in four Cultures. Paper prepared for a Symposium of the Reimers Foundation on Ritual Aspects of Food. Bad Homburg, December, 1984. (b) Wiley, R. H. (1973). The strut display of the male sage grouse: A “Fixed” action pattern. Behavior 47, 129–152. Wilhelm, H. J. (1953). Die !Kung-Buschleute. Jahr. Museums Völkerkunde (Leipzig) 12, 91–189. Williams, B. J. (1974). A model of band society. Amer. Antiquity 39, 1–138. Williams, B. J. (1980). Cognitive Limitation and Hunting Band Size. Vortrag auf der 2. Int. Conf. on Hunting and Gathering Societies, Quebec, 19.-24. Sept. 1980; Proceed., pp. 147–172. Williams, B. J. (1981). A critical review of models in sociobiology. Ann. Rev. Anthropol. 10, 163–192. Williams, G. C. (1966). Adaptation and Natural Selection: A Critique of Some Current Evolutionary Thought. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Williams, J. E. and Best, D. L. (1982). Measuring Sex Stereotypes. Beverley Hills, CA: Sage. Wilson, D. S. (1975). A theory of group selection. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S. 72, 143–146. Wilson, D. S. (1977). Structured demes and the evolution of group-advantageous traits. Amer. Natur. 111, 157–185. Wilson, E. O. (1963). Pheromones. Sci. Amer. 208, 100–114. Wilson, E. O. (1975). Sociobiology: The New Synthesis. Cambridge, MA: Belknap Press — Harvard University Press. Wilson, E. O. (1976). The social instinct. Bull. Amer. Acad. Arts Sci. 30, 11–25. Wilson, E. O. (1978). Altruism. Harvard Mag. Nerv. Dev. , pp. 23–28. Winberg, J. and DeChateau, P. (1982). Early social development: Studies of infant-mother interaction and relationship. In: Hartup, W. W. (ed.), Review of Child Development Research, Vol. VI. Chicago: The University of Chicago Press, pp. 1–44. Winkelman, M. (1986). Trance states: A theoretical model and cross-cultural analysis. Ethos 14, 174–203. Winkelmayer, R. , Exline, R. V. , Gottheil, E. , and Paredes, A. (1971). Cross-cultural differences in judging emotions. Unveröffentlicht. In: Ekman, P. (ed.) (1973), Darwin and Facial Expression. New York — London: Academic Press, p. 201. Winterhoff-Spurk, P. (1983). Die Funktionen von Blicken und Lächeln beim Auffordern. Europäische Hochschulschriften, Reihe 6, Psychologie. Frankfürt/Bern/New York: Peter Lang. 787 Wirtz, P. and Wawra, M. (1986). Vigilance and group size In Homo sapiens. Ethology (Z. Tierpsychol.) , 71, 283–286. Witkin, H. A. , Goodenough, D. R. , and Karp, S. A. (1967). Stability of cognitive style from childhood to young adulthood. J. Personality Social Psychol. 7, 291–300. Wittelson, S. (1978). Sex differences in the neurology of cognition: Psychological, social, educational and political implications. In: Sullerot, E. (ed.), Le Fait Feminin. Paris: Fayard, pp. 289–303. Wittig, M. and Petersen, A. (eds.) (1979). Sex-Related Differences in Cognitive Functioning. New York/London: Academic Press. Wobst, H. M. (1976). Locational relationship in paleolithic society. J. Human Evol. 5, 49–58. Wobst, H. M. (1977). Stylistic behavior and information exchange. In: Cleland, C. E. (ed.), Papers for the Director: Research Essays in Honor of James B. Griffin. Anthropology Papers. Museum of Anthropology, University of Michigan, 61, 317-342. Wolf, A. P. (1966). Childhood association, sexual attraction and the incest taboo: A Chinese case. Amer. Anthropol. 68, 883–898. Wolf, A. P. (1970). Childhood association and sexual attraction: A fürther test of the Westermarck hypothesis. Amer. Anthropol. 72, 503–515. Wolf, A. P. (1974). More on Childhood Association and Fertility in Taiwan. Unveröffentlichtes Manuskript Wolfgang, J. and Wolfgang, A. (1968). Personal Space — An Unobtrusive Measure of Attitudes Toward the Physically Handicapped. Proceedings of the 76th Annual Convention of the American Psychological Association, pp. 653–654. Woodburn, J. (1968). Stability and flexibility in Hadza residential groupings. In: Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 103–110. Woodhouse, H. C. (1987). Inter-and intragroup aggression illustrated in the rock paintings of South Africa. S. Afr. J. Ethnol. 10 (1), 42–48. Woodwell, G. M. (1978). The carbon dioxide question. Sci. Amer. 238, 34–43. Wright, Q. (rev. 1965). A Study of War. Chicago: University of Chicago Press. Wülker, W. (1976). Weltmodelle. Biologie Unserer Zeit 6, 148–155. Wülker, W. (1976). Die groβen Kreisläufe der Natur. In: Todt, D. (ed.), Funkkolleg Biologie, Begleittext, Sendg. 23, Weinheim: Beltz. Wunderlich, D. (1970/1971). Pragmatik, Sprechsituation, Deixis. Lili, 1/2, 153–190. Wunderlich, D. (1973). Probleme einer linguistischen Pragmatik. Papiere zur Linguistik, 4, 1-19. Wunderlich, D. (1974). Grundlagen der Linguistik. Hamburg: Reinbek. (a) Wunderlich, D. (1974). Pragmatik. Einleitung und Referenzsemantik. In: Lehrgang Sprache. Einführung in die moderne Linguistik. Basel/Tübingen: Weinheim, pp. 789-811. (b) Wynne-Edwards, V. C. (1962). Animal Dispersion in Relation to Social Behaviour. London: Oliver & Boyd. Yellen, J. E. and Harpending, H. (1972). Hunter-gatherer populations and archaeological inference. World Archaeol. 4, 244–253. Yengoyan, A. A. (1968). Demographic and ecological influences on aboriginal Australian marriage sections. In: Lee, R. B. and DeVore, I. (eds.), Man the Hunter. Chicago: Aldine, pp. 185–199. Yerkes, R. M. and Yerkes, A. W. (1929). The Great Apes. New Haven: CT: Yale Univ. Press. Yogman, M. W. (1981). Games fathers and mothers play with their infants. Infant Mental Health J. 2, 241–248. Yogman, M. W. (1982). Development of the father-infant relationship. In: Fitzgerald, H. , Lester, B. , and Yogman, M. W. (eds.), Theory and Research in Behavioral Pediatrics. Vol. 1. New York: Plenum, pp. 221–279. (a) Yogman, M. W. (1982). Observations on the father-infant relationship. In: Cath, S. H. , Gurwitt, A. R. , and Ross, J. M. (eds.), Father and Child: Developmental and Clinical Perspectives. Boston: Little, Brown, pp. 101–122. (b) Yokoyama, S. and Felsenstein, J. (1978). A model of kin selection for an altruistic trait considered as a quantitative character. Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. U.S. 75, 420–422. Yonas, A. , Pettersen, L. , and Lockman, J. J. (1979). Young infants’ sensitivity to optical information for collision. Canad. J. Psychol. 33, 268–276. Young, M. V. (1971). Fighting with Food. Leadership, Values, and Social Control in a Massim Society. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press. 788 Youniss, J. (1982). Die Entwicklung und Funktion von Freundschaftsbeziehungen. In: Edelstein, W. and Keller, M. (eds.), Perspektivität und Interpretation: Beiträge zur Entwicklung sozialen Verstehens. Frankfürt/M.: Suhrkamp, pp. 78–108. Zaporozhets, A. V. (1978). The significance of early periods of childhood for formation of children’s personality. In: Antsyferova, L. I. (ed.), Principles of Development in Psychology. Moskow: Nauka. von Zastrow, B. and Vedder, H. (1930). Die Buschmänner. In: Schulz-Ewerth, E. and Adam, L. (eds.), Das Eingeborenenrecht: Togo, Kamerun, Sudwestafrika, die Südseekolonien. Stuttgart: Strecker & Schröder. Zelazo, P. R. (1976). From reflexive to instrumental behavior. In: Lipsitt, L. P. (ed.), Developmental Psychobiology: The Significance of Infancy. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum. Zelazo, P. R. (1983). The development of walking: New findings on old assumptions. J. Motor Behavior 2, 99–137. Zeskind, Ph. S. and Lester, B. M. (1978). Acoustic features and auditory perception of the cries of newborns with prenatal and perinatal complication. Child Develop. 49, 580–589. Zinn, H. (1981). Sozialisation von Kindern und Jugendlichen unter beengten Wohn-und Wohnumfeldbedingungen. Inform. Raumentwickl. 7/8, 435-444. Zinser, H. (1981). Der Mythos des Mutterrechts. Frankfurt/M.: Ullstein. Zivin, G. (1977). Facial gestures predict preschoolers’ encounter outcomes. Soc. Sci. Information 16, 715–730.(a) Zivin, G. (1977). On becoming subtle: Age and social rank changes in the use of a facial gesture. Child Develop. 48, 1314–1321. (b) Zivin, G. (1982). Watching the sands shift: Conceptualizing development of nonverbal mastery. In: Feldman, R. S. (ed.), The Development of Nonverbal Communication in Children. New York: Springer.