The VLPFC Vs. the DLPFC in Down-Regulating Social Pain Using Reappraisal and Distraction Strategies

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The VLPFC Vs. the DLPFC in Down-Regulating Social Pain Using Reappraisal and Distraction Strategies Research Articles: Behavioral/Cognitive The VLPFC vs. the DLPFC in down-regulating social pain using reappraisal and distraction strategies https://doi.org/10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1906-20.2020 Cite as: J. Neurosci 2020; 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1906-20.2020 Received: 22 July 2020 Revised: 21 October 2020 Accepted: 23 October 2020 This Early Release article has been peer-reviewed and accepted, but has not been through the composition and copyediting processes. The final version may differ slightly in style or formatting and will contain links to any extended data. Alerts: Sign up at www.jneurosci.org/alerts to receive customized email alerts when the fully formatted version of this article is published. Copyright © 2020 Zhao et al. This is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution and reproduction in any medium provided that the original work is properly attributed. 1 The VLPFC vs. the DLPFC in 2 down-regulating social pain using 3 reappraisal and distraction strategies 4 5 Jun Zhao1,2,#, Licheng Mo1,#, Rong Bi1, Zhenhong He1, Yuming Chen1, Feng Xu3, Hui Xie1, 6 Dandan Zhang1,2,4,5,* 7 8 1 School of Psychology, Shenzhen University, Shenzhen, 518060, China 9 2 Institute of Brain and Psychological Sciences, Sichuan Normal University, Chengdu 610066, 10 China. 11 3 Shenzhen Yingchi Technology Co., Ltd., Shenzhen, 518057, China 12 4 Shenzhen-Hong Kong Institute of Brain Science, Shenzhen, 518060, China 13 5 Shenzhen Institute of Neuroscience, Shenzhen 518060, China 14 15 Running head: the role of LPFC in down-regulation of social pain 16 17 The authors declare that there are no conflicts of interest in relation to the subject of this study. 18 19 # The two authors contributed equally to the study. 20 21 * Corresponding author. E-mail: [email protected] 22 23 Number of pages: 29 24 Number of figures: 4; number of tables: 3 25 Words: abstract (248), introduction (688), and discussion (1652) 1 / 30 26 Abstract 27 The dorsolateral (DLPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices (VLPFC) are both 28 crucial structures involved in voluntary emotional regulation. However, it remains 29 unclear whether the functions of these two cortical regions that are involved in 30 emotional regulation—which are usually active in non-social situations—could be 31 generalized to the regulation of social pain as well. This study employed transcranial 32 magnetic stimulation (TMS) to examine the causal relationship between the 33 DLPFC/VLPFC and the emotional regulation of social pain via distraction and 34 reappraisal. Ninety human participants (45 males and 45 females) initially underwent 35 either active (DLPFC/VLPFC, n = 30/30) or sham (vertex, n = 30) TMS sessions. 36 Participants were then instructed to use both distraction and reappraisal strategies to 37 down-regulate any negative emotions evoked by social exclusion pictures. 38 Convergent results of the subjective emotional rating and electrophysiological indices 39 demonstrated that: 1) both the DLPFC and VLPFC highly facilitate the 40 down-regulation of affective responses caused by social exclusion, revealing a causal 41 role of these lateral prefrontal cortices in voluntary emotional regulation of both 42 non-social and social pain; and 2) these two cortical regions showed relative 43 functional specificity for distraction (DLPFC) and reappraisal (VLPFC) strategies, 44 which helps to refine the cortical targeting of therapeutic protocols. In addition, the 45 TMS effect was sustainable for at least one hour, showcasing the potential feasibility 46 of using this method in clinical practice. Together, these findings provide cognitive 47 and neural evidence for the targeting of the VLPFC and/or the DLPFC to improve 2 / 30 48 emotional regulation abilities, especially in social contexts. 49 50 Key words: emotional regulation; social exclusion; social pain; ventrolateral 51 prefrontal cortex; dorsolateral prefrontal cortex 52 53 54 Significance Statement 55 This study aimed to examine the role of the dorsolateral and ventrolateral prefrontal 56 cortices in emotional regulation, particularly in response to social pain through the use 57 of distraction and reappraisal strategies, as this is a relatively underexplored area of 58 inquiry. This study makes a significant contribution to the literature because our 59 results provide novel empirical information on the role of these cortical structures in 60 the processing of negative emotions elicited within certain social contexts. As such, 61 our findings have potential clinical implications, paving the way for future clinicians 62 to be able to accurately target specific brain regions among patients struggling with 63 impaired social cognition abilities, including those diagnosed with post-traumatic 64 stress disorder, autism spectrum disorder, social anxiety disorder, and depression. 65 66 67 68 69 3 / 30 70 Introduction 71 While the key brain regions for emotional regulation are primarily located in the 72 prefrontal cortex (Ochsner et al. 2012), some studies have demonstrated that explicit 73 emotional regulation using varying strategies do not involve the exact overlapping 74 neural substrates (Morawetz et al. 2017; Vrtička et al. 2011). In particular, although 75 the dorsolateral (DLPFC) and ventrolateral prefrontal cortices (VLPFC) are both 76 important in distraction and reappraisal (Buhle et al. 2014; Kohn et al. 2014), e.g., the 77 two regions often activate together during reappraisal (Morawetz et al. 2017; Ochsner 78 et al. 2012), neuroimaging studies have revealed that while the VLPFC is consistently 79 involved during reappraisal, the DLPFC is more associated with distraction (Dörfel et 80 al. 2014; Moodie et al. 2020). However, it remains unclear to what extent these two 81 brain regions are essential and specific for both of these strategies. This study 82 employed transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS) to examine the roles of the 83 VLPFC and the DLPFC in emotional regulation via distraction and reappraisal 84 strategies. 85 Meanwhile, a more urgent question is whether the brain regions involved in 86 emotional regulation observed in general, especially non-social, situations can be 87 generalized to functioning similarly in social contexts. Most previous studies have not 88 disentangled the specific brain regions involved in emotional regulation for social vs. 89 non-social emotion-eliciting events (but see Vrtička et al. 2011). Uncovering the 90 neural substrates critical for emotional regulation in social contexts is necessary not 91 only for understanding the neural mechanisms behind emotional regulation, but also 4 / 30 92 in the development of effective therapeutic protocols in clinics. For one thing, 93 negative social experiences, including dysfunctional family interactions and traumatic 94 or stressful interpersonal events are all considered major risk factors in the 95 development of psychiatric disorders such as post-traumatic stress disorder, social 96 anxiety, depression, and autism spectrum disorder (Nolan et al. 2003; Durodié and 97 Wainwright 2019). For another, growing evidence indicates that these aforementioned 98 psychiatric disorders are all associated with the maladaptive regulation of social pain 99 (Davey et al. 2011; Masten et al. 2011). Therefore, it is implied that improving one’s 100 emotional regulation abilities in response to negative social events is highly beneficial 101 for the remission of symptoms and recovery from psychiatric disorders (Laceulle et al. 102 2017). This study was designed to examine the causal relationship between the 103 VLPFC/DLPFC and the emotional regulation of social pain in order to provide neural 104 targets for clinical interventions. 105 Previous studies have demonstrated that both the VLPFC and the DLPFC are 106 associated closely with the reduction of social pain (Koban et al. 2017a; 107 Koenigsberg et al. 2010; Vijayakumar et al. 2017; Wang et al. 2017). Typically, 108 researchers have observed that the right or bilateral VLPFC is activated significantly 109 when individuals experienced social exclusion (Eisenberger et al. 2003; Hooker et al. 110 2010; Hsu et al. 2015; Masten et al. 2009; Onoda et al. 2010; Riva et al. 2012, 111 2015). Also, fMRI studies have found that enhanced activation in the DLPFC was 112 associated with a decline in subjective social distress (Koban et al. 2017b; Nishiyama 113 et al. 2015) and aggressive behaviors (Achterberg et al. 2016, 2020) following the 5 / 30 114 experience of social rejection. However, while these studies have demonstrated an 115 association between the DLPFC/VLPFC and reduced social pain, very limited studies 116 have examined the role of these regions on explicit emotional regulation within social 117 contexts (but see Koenigsberg et al. 2010). Recently, we used tDCS/TMS in an 118 explicit emotional regulation task and provided direct evidence for the role of the 119 right VLPFC on emotional regulation following the experience of social exclusion 120 (He et al. 2018, 2020a, 2020b). However, until now, no study has explored the causal 121 role of the DLPFC in the explicit regulation of social pain. This study was thus 122 inspired by this literature gap. 123 Our hypothesis is two-fold. First, the critical role of the VLPFC and the DLPFC in 124 emotional regulation during non-social situations is generalizable to social ones, 125 because previous studies have revealed a close association between the 126 VLPFC/DLPFC and
Recommended publications
  • Deception, Disinformation, and Strategic Communications: How One Interagency Group Made a Major Difference by Fletcher Schoen and Christopher J
    STRATEGIC PERSPECTIVES 11 Deception, Disinformation, and Strategic Communications: How One Interagency Group Made a Major Difference by Fletcher Schoen and Christopher J. Lamb Center for Strategic Research Institute for National Strategic Studies National Defense University Institute for National Strategic Studies National Defense University The Institute for National Strategic Studies (INSS) is National Defense University’s (NDU’s) dedicated research arm. INSS includes the Center for Strategic Research, Center for Complex Operations, Center for the Study of Chinese Military Affairs, Center for Technology and National Security Policy, Center for Transatlantic Security Studies, and Conflict Records Research Center. The military and civilian analysts and staff who comprise INSS and its subcomponents execute their mission by conducting research and analysis, publishing, and participating in conferences, policy support, and outreach. The mission of INSS is to conduct strategic studies for the Secretary of Defense, Chairman of the Joint Chiefs of Staff, and the Unified Combatant Commands in support of the academic programs at NDU and to perform outreach to other U.S. Government agencies and the broader national security community. Cover: Kathleen Bailey presents evidence of forgeries to the press corps. Credit: The Washington Times Deception, Disinformation, and Strategic Communications: How One Interagency Group Made a Major Difference Deception, Disinformation, and Strategic Communications: How One Interagency Group Made a Major Difference By Fletcher Schoen and Christopher J. Lamb Institute for National Strategic Studies Strategic Perspectives, No. 11 Series Editor: Nicholas Rostow National Defense University Press Washington, D.C. June 2012 Opinions, conclusions, and recommendations expressed or implied within are solely those of the contributors and do not necessarily represent the views of the Defense Department or any other agency of the Federal Government.
    [Show full text]
  • Correlates of Imaginative Suggestibility and Hypnotizability in Children
    University of Massachusetts Amherst ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014 1-1-2000 Correlates of imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability in children. Bruce C. Poulsen University of Massachusetts Amherst Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_1 Recommended Citation Poulsen, Bruce C., "Correlates of imaginative suggestibility and hypnotizability in children." (2000). Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014. 1271. https://scholarworks.umass.edu/dissertations_1/1271 This Open Access Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. It has been accepted for inclusion in Doctoral Dissertations 1896 - February 2014 by an authorized administrator of ScholarWorks@UMass Amherst. For more information, please contact [email protected]. CORRELATES OF IMAGINATIVE SUGGESTIBILITY AND HYPNOTIZABILITY IN CHILDREN A Dissertation Presented by BRUCE C. POULSEN Submitted to the Graduate School of the University of Massachusetts Amherst in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY February 2000 Education © Copyright by Bruce Craig Poulsen 2000 All Rights Reserved CORRELATES OF IMAGINATIVE SUGGESTIBILITY AND HYPNOTIZABILITY IN CHILDREN A Dissertation Presented by BRUCE C. POULSEN B^\ty W. Jackson, Dean S^hqol of Education ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would like to gratefully acknowledge the assistance and support of several individuals, without whom this project would not have been possible. First, I am indebted to William Matthews, Jr., Ph.D. and Irving Kirsch, Ph.D. for mitial suggestions for both the research design and statistical analysis. Karen Olness, M.D. and Steven Jay Lynn, Ph.D. both provided helpful suggestions for selecting the measurement instruments. Several individuals at Primary Children's Medical Center provided invaluable support during the data collection procedures.
    [Show full text]
  • Social Engineering in Cybersecurity: a Domain Ontology and Knowledge Graph Application Examples Zuoguang Wang1,2* , Hongsong Zhu1,2* ,Peipeiliu1,2 and Limin Sun1,2
    Wang et al. Cybersecurity (2021) 4:31 Cybersecurity https://doi.org/10.1186/s42400-021-00094-6 RESEARCH Open Access Social engineering in cybersecurity: a domain ontology and knowledge graph application examples Zuoguang Wang1,2* , Hongsong Zhu1,2* ,PeipeiLiu1,2 and Limin Sun1,2 Abstract Social engineering has posed a serious threat to cyberspace security. To protect against social engineering attacks, a fundamental work is to know what constitutes social engineering. This paper first develops a domain ontology of social engineering in cybersecurity and conducts ontology evaluation by its knowledge graph application. The domain ontology defines 11 concepts of core entities that significantly constitute or affect social engineering domain, together with 22 kinds of relations describing how these entities related to each other. It provides a formal and explicit knowledge schema to understand, analyze, reuse and share domain knowledge of social engineering. Furthermore, this paper builds a knowledge graph based on 15 social engineering attack incidents and scenarios. 7 knowledge graph application examples (in 6 analysis patterns) demonstrate that the ontology together with knowledge graph is useful to 1) understand and analyze social engineering attack scenario and incident, 2) find the top ranked social engineering threat elements (e.g. the most exploited human vulnerabilities and most used attack mediums), 3) find potential social engineering threats to victims, 4) find potential targets for social engineering attackers, 5) find potential attack paths from specific attacker to specific target, and 6) analyze the same origin attacks. Keywords: Social engineering attack, Cyber security, Ontology, Knowledge graph, Attack scenarios, Threat analysis, Attack path, Attack model, Taxonomy, Composition and structure Introduction (Wang et al.
    [Show full text]
  • Driving Distraction Away
    Driving Distraction Away www.safestart.com 1 Driving DISTRACTION Away That quote, from the National Safety Council’s president and CEO Deborah Hersman, says it all. Driving is such a big risk factor that it’s the only injury Driving a car is one of the category to receive a full section (it’s 36 pages!) in the National Safety riskiest activities any of Council’s Injury Facts 2015. us undertake in spite of The frequency and potential severity of vehicle collisions places driving in the worst corner of any risk matrix. Add distraction to the mix and driving decades of vehicle design becomes the riskiest thing we do every day. improvements and traffic The standard picture of distracted driving is a teen with the steering wheel in safety advancements. one hand and a cellphone in the other. But the issue is much bigger—and you won’t solve it unless you shift your focus from driving to distraction. DISTRACTION, NOT DRIVING, IS THE KEY The dangers of distraction aren’t confined to the car. There’s almost no difference between distracted driving, distracted working, or doing anything else while distracted. Solve distraction in all its forms and you’ll make people a lot safer. EHS Today, “Motor Vehicle Deaths in 2015 Show Large Increase Year-Over-Year”, February 17, 2016. www.safestart.com 2 Why You Should Care About DISTRACTED DRIVING Everyone drives or rides in a car. And that means distracted driving affects every single business in the world. Because no matter where a crash occurs—at work or off the job—it can affect drivers, passengers and pedestrians for weeks and months Employee Crash Injury/FatalityReducedLost Morale ProductivityEquipmentCompany Damage Liability afterwards.
    [Show full text]
  • Module 1: Understanding Distress Intolerance Page 1 • Psychotherapy • Research • Training Facing Your Feelings Introduction
    Facing Your Feelings Facing Your Feelings Facing Your Feelings Module 1 Understanding Distress Intolerance Introduction 2 What Is Distress Intolerance? 2 The Paradox… 3 Am I Distress Intolerant? 4 Healthy Distress Tolerance 5 How Does Distress Intolerance Develop? 5 Distress Intolerant Beliefs 6 Distress Escape Methods 7 Distress Intolerance Model 8 My Distress Intolerance Model 9 The Good News… 10 Module Summary 11 About the Modules 12 The information provided in the document is for information purposes only. Please refer to the full disclaimer and copyright statements available at www.cci.health.gov.au regarding the information on this website before making use of such information. entre for C linical C I nterventions Module 1: Understanding Distress Intolerance Page 1 • Psychotherapy • Research • Training Facing Your Feelings Introduction We all experience emotions. Emotions are an important part of being human, and are essential to our survival. As humans we are designed to feel a whole range of emotions, some of which may be comfortable to us, and others may be uncomfortable. Most people dislike feeling uncomfortable. There are many different ways that humans can feel uncomfortable…we can be hot, cold, tired, in pain, hungry, unwell, and the list could go on. The type of discomfort we will be talking about in these modules is emotional discomfort, or what is often called distress. We may not like it, but experiencing uncomfortable emotions is a natural part of life. However, there is a difference between disliking unpleasant emotions, but nevertheless accepting that they are an inevitable part of life and hence riding through them, versus experiencing unpleasant emotions as unbearable and needing to get rid of them.
    [Show full text]
  • The Anatomy of a Moral Panic: Western Mainstream Media's
    Changing Societies & Personalities, 2019 Vol. 3, No. 3, pp. 189–206 http://dx.doi.org/10.15826/csp.2019.3.3.071 ARTICLE The Anatomy of a Moral Panic: Western Mainstream Media’s Russia Scapegoat Greg Simons Uppsala University, Sweden Turiba University, Latvia Ural Federal University, Yekaterinburg, Russia ABSTRACT Since 2014, there has been a very concerted campaign launched by the neo-liberal Western mainstream mass media against Russia. The format and content suggest that this is an attempt to induce a moral panic among the Western publics. It seems to be intended to create a sense of fear and to switch the logic to a series of emotionally- based reactions to assertion propaganda. Russia has been variously blamed for many different events and trends around the world, such as the “destroying” of Western “democracy”, and democratic values. In many regards, Russia is projected as being an existential threat in both the physical and intangible realms. This paper traces the strategic messages and narratives of the “Russia threat” as it is presented in Western mainstream media. Russia is connoted as a scapegoat for the failings of the neo-liberal democratic political order to maintain its global hegemony; therefore, Russia is viewed as the “menacing” other and a desperate measure to halt this gradual decline and loss of power and influence. This ultimately means that this type of journalism fails in its supposed fourth estate role, by directly aiding the hegemonic political power. KEYWORDS moral panic, Western mainstream media, Russia, propaganda, scapegoating, fourth estate Received 28 July 2019 © 2019 Gregory Simons Accepted 13 September 2019 [email protected] Published online 5 October 2019 190 Greg Simons Introduction Moral panic has been developed as a concept within sociology and came to greater attention after Stanley Cohen’s 1972 book on Folk Devils and Moral Panic.
    [Show full text]
  • Examination of Psychopathic Traits and Attention Using the Image Based Parity Task Veronica Claudia Llamas
    Loma Linda University TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works Loma Linda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects 9-2014 Examination of Psychopathic Traits and Attention Using the Image Based Parity Task Veronica Claudia Llamas Follow this and additional works at: http://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd Part of the Clinical Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Llamas, Veronica Claudia, "Examination of Psychopathic Traits and Attention Using the Image Based Parity Task" (2014). Loma Linda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects. 172. http://scholarsrepository.llu.edu/etd/172 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loma Linda University Electronic Theses, Dissertations & Projects by an authorized administrator of TheScholarsRepository@LLU: Digital Archive of Research, Scholarship & Creative Works. For more information, please contact [email protected]. LOMA LINDA UNIVERSITY School of Behavioral Health in conjunction with the Faculty of Graduate Studies ____________________ Examination of Psychopathic Traits and Attention Using the Image Based Parity Task by Veronica Claudia Llamas ____________________ A Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree Doctor of Philosophy in Clinical Psychology ____________________ September 2014 © 2014 Veronica Claudia Llamas All Rights Reserved
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding and Treating Shame: the Role of the Clinician
    St. Catherine University SOPHIA Master of Social Work Clinical Research Papers School of Social Work 8-2016 Understanding and Treating Shame: The Role of the Clinician Jennifer Perez St. Catherine University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://sophia.stkate.edu/msw_papers Part of the Social Work Commons Recommended Citation Perez, Jennifer. (2016). Understanding and Treating Shame: The Role of the Clinician. Retrieved from Sophia, the St. Catherine University repository website: https://sophia.stkate.edu/msw_papers/695 This Clinical research paper is brought to you for free and open access by the School of Social Work at SOPHIA. It has been accepted for inclusion in Master of Social Work Clinical Research Papers by an authorized administrator of SOPHIA. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Understanding and Treating Shame: The Role of the Clinician by Jennifer Perez, L.S.W. MSW Clinical Research Paper Presented to the Faculty of the School of Social Work St. Catherine University and the University of St. Thomas St. Paul, Minnesota In Partial fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Master of Social Work Committee Members Dr. David Roseborough (chair) Carli Kody, PhD Shelly Richardson, PhD The Clinical Research Project is a graduation requirement for MSW students at St. Catherine University/University of St. Thomas School of Social Work in St. Paul, Minnesota and is conducted within a nine-month time frame to demonstrate facility with basic social research methods. Students must independently conceptualize a research problem, formulate a research design that is approved by a research committee and the university Institutional Review Board, implement the project, and publicly present the findings of the study.
    [Show full text]
  • Esteem Moderate the Impact of Daily Social Rejection on Restrictive Eating Behaviours Among College Women
    Psychology & Health ISSN: 0887-0446 (Print) 1476-8321 (Online) Journal homepage: http://www.tandfonline.com/loi/gpsh20 Stomaching rejection: Self-compassion and self- esteem moderate the impact of daily social rejection on restrictive eating behaviours among college women Janine B. Beekman, Michelle L. Stock & George W. Howe To cite this article: Janine B. Beekman, Michelle L. Stock & George W. Howe (2017): Stomaching rejection: Self-compassion and self-esteem moderate the impact of daily social rejection on restrictive eating behaviours among college women, Psychology & Health, DOI: 10.1080/08870446.2017.1324972 To link to this article: http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2017.1324972 Published online: 05 May 2017. Submit your article to this journal Article views: 30 View related articles View Crossmark data Full Terms & Conditions of access and use can be found at http://www.tandfonline.com/action/journalInformation?journalCode=gpsh20 Download by: [204.118.216.39] Date: 14 June 2017, At: 08:04 Psychology & Health, 2017 https://doi.org/10.1080/08870446.2017.1324972 Stomaching rejection: Self-compassion and self-esteem moderate the impact of daily social rejection on restrictive eating behaviours among college women Janine B. Beekman*,†, Michelle L. Stock and George W. Howe Department of Psychology, The George Washington University, Washington, DC, USA (Received 6 September 2016; accepted 18 April 2017) Objective: The present study examined whether having high self-esteem or a self-compassionate perspective help mitigate the impact of daily social rejection on negative affect and restrictive eating behaviours. Design: Following a baseline survey assessing self-esteem and self-compassion, 121 college women completed online daily diaries for one week.
    [Show full text]
  • The Lie of Distraction
    Turn away mine eyes from beholding vanity; and quicken thou me in thy way. psalm 119:37 I have stuck unto thy testimonies: O Lord, put me not to shame. THE LIE OF psalm 119:31 DISTRACTION 3. DEFEND YOUR PLAN. (VS. 11-12) TRUTH BE TOLD! | SUNDAY MORNING | JULY 5, 2020 DEFINITION: Have I hid, tsaphan—to store or treasure up, to conceal secretly and covertly, to strategically place in anticipation for battle. BETH. (The 2nd letter in the Hebrew alphabet the house) KEY THOUGHT: “If we are not strategic about living the truth, 9 Wherewithal shall a young man cleanse his way? By taking heed thereto we will be defeated by the deception of distraction.” according to thy word. 10 With my whole heart have I sought thee: O let me not wander from thy commandments. 11 Thy word have I hid LIE OF DISTRACTION: If I don't set priorities I will have less stress. in mine heart, that I might not sin against thee. 12 Blessed art thou, O Lord: teach me thy statutes. psalm 119:9–12 QUOTE: “We save what we treasure, and we treasure what we save.” Introduction: QUOTE: “The parallel is clear that sin bankrupts what wisdom attempts to save.” I will never forget thy precepts: for with them thou hast quickened me. psalm 119:93 QUOTE: “The truth about focus and passion is the key to DEFINITION: Sin, chata; (from chach=hook)—to miss the mark, to overcoming the deception of distraction.” err from what is right, to forfeit health and wellbeing.
    [Show full text]
  • Child Welfare Practices for Cases with Child Sexual Abuse This Is the First Edition of Child Welfare Practices for Cases with Child Sexual Abuse
    The Oregon Department of Human Services Child Welfare Practices for Cases with Child Sexual Abuse This is the first edition of Child Welfare Practices for Cases with Child Sexual Abuse. One of the goals of the Department of Human Services (DHS) is to help protect children and promote children’s safety in their homes. Assisting victims of child sexual abuse and their families helps us achieve this goal. Acknowledgements The collaborative group effort resulting in the development of these guidelines was funded by a federal grant under the Child Abuse Prevention and Treatment Act (CAPTA). The initial work group was made up of community experts who have an active role in the sexual abuse field, including victim treatment providers, sex offender treatment providers (adult and juvenile), child welfare staff, law enforcement agency (LEA) staff, child abuse intervention center (CAIC) staff (interviewers and medical providers), district attorneys, providers for exploited youth, and probation officers. Using a multidisciplinary approach, they created these guidelines for DHS staff to provide training assistance for effective intervention in child sexual abuse. These guidelines are also based on the work and ideas from DHS child welfare staff, specifically the Child Protective Services (CPS) Program. These guidelines were developed by Karen Gibbs, DHS Child Protective Services Consultant, and Skylar Kendoll, DHS CPS caseworker, with considerable assistance from the following individuals: Kim Goldstien – CARES NW Interviewer Katie Gotch – Multnomah
    [Show full text]
  • Priming and Warnings Are Not Effective to Prevent Social Engineering Attacks
    Computers in Human Behavior 66 (2017) 75e87 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Computers in Human Behavior journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/comphumbeh Full length article Priming and warnings are not effective to prevent social engineering attacks * M. Junger a, , L. Montoya b, F.-J. Overink a, 1 a University of Twente, Faculty of Behavioral, Management and Social Sciences (BMS), The Netherlands b University of Twente, Faculty of Electrical Engineering, Mathematics and Computer Science, The Netherlands article info abstract Article history: Humans tend to trust each other and to easily disclose personal information. This makes them vulnerable Received 6 June 2016 to social engineering attacks. The present study investigated the effectiveness of two interventions that Received in revised form aim to protect users against social engineering attacks, namely priming through cues to raise awareness 26 August 2016 about the dangers of social engineering cyber-attacks and warnings against the disclosure of personal Accepted 7 September 2016 information. A sample of visitors of the shopping district of a medium-sized town in the Netherlands was Available online 24 September 2016 studied. Disclosure was measured by asking subjects for their email address, 9 digits from their 18 digit bank account number, and for those who previously shopped online, what they had purchased and in Keywords: fi Priming which web shop. Relatively high disclosure rates were found: 79.1% of the subjects lled in their email Warning address, and 43.5% provided bank account information. Among the online shoppers, 89.8% of the subjects prevention filled in the type of product(s) they purchased and 91.4% filled in the name of the online shop where they Social engineering did these purchases.
    [Show full text]