Principles of Image Representation in Visual Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Principles of Image Representation in Visual Cortex In: The Visual Neurosciences, L.M. Chalupa, J.S. Werner, Eds. MIT Press, 2003: 1603-1615. Principles of Image Representation in Visual Cortex Bruno A. Olshausen Center for Neuroscience, UC Davis [email protected] 1 Introduction data. Neuroscientists are interested in understand- ing how the cortex extracts certain properties of the The visual cortex is responsible for most of our con- visual environment—surfaces, objects, textures, mo- scious perception of the visual world, yet we remain tion, etc.—from the data stream coming from the largely ignorant of the principles underlying its func- retina. Similarly, engineers are interested in design- tion despite progress on many fronts of neuroscience. ing algorithms capable of extracting structure con- The principal reason for this is not a lack of data, tained in images or sound—for example, to identify but rather the absence of a solid theoretical frame- and label parts within the body from medical imag- work for motivating experiments and interpreting ing data. These problems at their core are one in the findings. The situation may be likened to trying same, and progress in one domain will likely lead to to understand how birds fly without knowledge of new insights in the other. the principles of aerodynamics: no amount of ex- The key difficulty faced by both sides is that the perimentation or measurements made on the bird core principles of pattern analysis are not well un- itself will reveal the secret. The key experiment— derstood. No amount of experimentation or techno- measuring air pressure above and below the wing as logical tinkering alone can overcome this obstacle. air is passed over it—would not seem obvious were it Rather, it demands that we devote our efforts to not for a theory suggesting why the pressures might advancing new theories of pattern analysis, and in be different. directing experimental efforts toward testing these But neuroscience can not simply turn to mathe- theories. matics or engineering for a set of principles that will In recent years, a theoretical framework for how elucidate the function of the cortex. Indeed, engi- pattern analysis is done by the visual cortex has be- neers and mathematicians themselves have had lit- gun to emerge. The theory has its roots in ideas pro- tle success in emulating even the most elementary posed more than 40 years ago by Attneave and Bar- aspects of intelligence or perceptual capabilities, de- low, and it has been made more concrete in recent spite much effort devoted to the problem over the years through a combination of efforts in engineer- past 40 years. The lack of progress here is espe- ing, mathematics, and computational neuroscience. cially striking considering the fact that the past two The essential idea is that the visual cortex contains decades alone have seen a 1000-fold increase in com- a probabilistic model of images, and that the activ- puter power (in terms of computer speed and mem- ities of neurons are representing images in terms of ory capacity), while the actual “intelligence” of com- this model. Rather than focussing on what features puters has improved only moderately by comparison. of “the stimulus” are represented by neurons, the emphasis of this approach is on discovering a good The problem: pattern analysis featural description of images of the natural environ- ment, using probabilistic models, and then relating Although seldom recognized by either side, both this description to the response properties of visual neuroscientists and engineers are faced with a com- neurons. mon problem—it is the problem of pattern analysis, In this chapter, I will focus on recent work that or how to extract structure contained in complex has attempted to understand image representation in area V1 in terms of a probabilistic model of natu- others—and is characterized by the distribution ral scenes. Section 2 will first provide an overview of P (E). the probabilistic approach and its relation to theo- From these two quantities, one can make inferences ries of redundancy reduction and sparse coding. Sec- about the environment by computing the posterior tion 3 will then describe how this framework has distribution, P (E|D), which specifies the relative been used to model the structure of natural images, probabilities of different states of the environment and section 4 will discuss the relation between these given the data. It is computed by combining P (E) models and the response properties of V1 neurons. together with P (D|E) according to Bayes’ rule: Finally, in section 5 I will discuss some of the exper- imental implications of this framework, alternative P (E|D) ∝ P (D|E)P (E)(1) theories, and the prospects for extending this ap- This simple equation provides a mathematical for- proach to higher cortical areas. mulation of the essential problem faced by the cor- tex. By itself, it does not provide all the answers to 2 Probabilistic models how the cortex works. But it does provide a guiding principle from which we can begin to start filling in The job of visual perception is to infer properties of details. the environment from data coming from the sensory receptors. What makes this such a difficult problem Redundancy reduction is that we are trying to recover information about The general idea of “perception as probabilistic in- the three-dimensional world from a two-dimensional ference” is by no means new, and in fact it goes back sensor array. This process is loaded with uncertainty at least to Helmholtz (1867/1962). Attneave (1954) due to the fact that the light intensity arriving at later pointed out that there could be a formal rela- any point on the retina arises from a combination tionship between the statistical properties of images of lighting properties, surface reflectance properties, and certain aspects of visual perception. The no- and surface shape (Adelson & Pentland 1996). There tion was then put into concrete mathematical and is no unique solution for determining these proper- neurobiological terms by Barlow (1961, 1989), who ties of the environment from photoreceptor activi- proposed a self-organizing strategy for sensory ner- ties; rather, some environments provide a more prob- vous systems based on the principle of redundancy able explanation of the data than others based on reduction—i.e., the idea that neurons should encode our knowledge of how the world is structured and information in such a way as to minimize statistical how images are formed. Visual perception is thus dependencies among them. Barlow reasoned that a essentially a problem of probabilistic inference. statistically independent representation of the envi- In order to do probabilistic inference on images, ronment would make it possible to store information two things are needed: about prior probabilities, since the joint probability x 1. A model for how a given state of the environ- distribution of a particular state could be easily ment (E) gives rise to a particular state of ac- calculated from the product of probabilities of each x P x P x tivity on the receptors (the observable data, component i: ( )=Πi ( i). It also has the ad- D). This model essentially describes the pro- vantage of making efficient use of neural resources in cess of image formation and can be character- transmitting information, since it does not duplicate ized probabilistically (to account for uncertain- information in different neurons. ties such as noise) using the conditional distri- The first strides in quantitatively testing the the- bution P (D|E). ory of redundancy reduction came from the work of Simon Laughlin and M.V. Srinivasan. They mea- 2. A model for the prior probability of the state of sured both the histograms and spatial correlations the environment. This expresses our knowledge of image pixels in the natural visual environment of of how the world is structured—which proper- flies, and then used this knowledge to make quan- ties of the environment are more probable than titative predictions about the response properties of 2 neurons in early stages of the visual system (Laugh- inputs (approximately 25:1 in cat area 17—inferred lin 1981; Srinivasan et al., 1982). They showed that from Beaulieu & Colonnier (1983) and Peters & Yil- the constrast response function of bipolar cells in the maz (1993)). If the bandwidth per axon is about fly’s eye performs histogram equalization (so that all the same, then the unavoidable conclusion is that output values are equally likely), and that lateral in- redundancy is being increased in the cortex, since hibition among these neurons serves to decorrelate the total amount of information can not increase their responses for natural scenes, confirming two (Field, 1994). The expansion here is especially strik- predictions of the redundancy reduction hypothesis. ing given the evidence for wiring length being min- Another advance was made ten years later by At- imized in many parts of the nervous system (Cher- ick (1992) and van Hateren (1992; 1993), who for- niak, 1995; Koulakov & Chklovskii 2001). So what mulated a theory of coding in the retina based on is being gained by spending extra neural resources whitening the power spectrum of natural images in in this way? space and time. Since it had been shown by Field First, it must be recognized that the real goal (1987) that natural scenes posses a characteristic of sensory representation is to model the redun- 1/f 2 spatial power spectrum, they reasoned that dancy in images, not necessarily to reduce it (Bar- the optimal decorrelating filter should attempt to low, 2001). What we really want is a meaningful whiten the power spectrum up to the point where representation—something that captures the causes noise power is low relative to the signal (since a of images, or what’s “out there” in the environment.
Recommended publications
  • Visual Cortex in Humans 251
    Author's personal copy Visual Cortex in Humans 251 Visual Cortex in Humans B A Wandell, S O Dumoulin, and A A Brewer, using fMRI, and we discuss the main features of the Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA V1 map. We then summarize the positions and proper- ties of ten additional visual field maps. This represents ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. our current understanding of human visual field maps, although this remains an active field of investigation, with more maps likely to be discovered. Finally, we Human visua l cortex comprises 4–6 billion neurons that are organ ized into more than a dozen distinct describe theories about the functional purpose and functional areas. These areas include the gray matter organizing principles of these maps. in the occi pital lobe and extend into the temporal and parietal lobes . The locations of these areas in the The Size and Location of Human Visual intact human cortex can be identified by measuring Cortex visual field maps. The neurons within these areas have a variety of different stimulus response proper- The entirety of human cortex occupies a surface area 2 ties. We descr ibe how to measure these visual field on the order of 1000 cm and ranges between 2 and maps, their locations, and their overall organization. 4 mm in thickness. Each cubic millimeter of cortex contains approximately 50 000 neurons so that neo- We then consider how information about patterns, objects, color s, and motion is analyzed and repre- cortex in the two hemispheres contain on the order of sented in these maps.
    [Show full text]
  • The Davida Teller Award Lecture, 2016 Visual Brain Development: A
    Journal of Vision (2017) 17(3):26, 1–24 1 The Davida Teller Award Lecture, 2016 Visual Brain Development: A review of ‘‘Dorsal Stream Vulnerability’’—motion, mathematics, amblyopia, actions, and attention # Janette Atkinson University College London, London, UK $ Research in the Visual Development Unit on ‘‘dorsal stream vulnerability’ (DSV) arose from research in two Introduction somewhat different areas. In the first, using cortical milestones for local and global processing from our neurobiological model, we identified cerebral visual Emerging cortical function and infant cortical impairment in infants in the first year of life. In the impairment second, using photo/videorefraction in population refractive screening programs, we showed that infant The work discussed at the beginning of this review spectacle wear could reduce the incidence of strabismus arose out of the first twenty years of my research with and amblyopia, but many preschool children, who had Oliver Braddick and our team in the Visual Develop- been significantly hyperopic earlier, showed visuo-motor ment Unit in Cambridge, particularly John Wattam- and attentional deficits. This led us to compare Bell and Shirley Anker (Atkinson, 2000). We began by developing dorsal and ventral streams, using sensitivity devising new methods, both behavioral (automated to global motion and form as signatures, finding deficits forced-choice preferential looking) and electrophysio- in motion sensitivity relative to form in children with logical (steady-state VEP/VERP—Visual Evoked Po- Williams syndrome, or perinatal brain injury in tential/Visual Event Related Potential) to measure the hemiplegia or preterm birth. Later research showed that normal visual capacities of infants such as acuity and this DSV was common across many disorders, both ‘‘ ’’ contrast sensitivity, over the first years of life (Atkin- genetic and acquired, from autism to amblyopia.
    [Show full text]
  • VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2019
    PSYC/BIBB/VLST 217 VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2019 Spring 2019 Lecture: MWF 9-10a, Leidy Labs 10 Prerequisites: PSCY 1, BIBB 109, VLST 101, or COGS 001 Synopsis: An introduction to the scientific study of vision, with an emphasis on the biological substrate and its relation to behavior. Topics will typically include physiological optics, transduction of light, visual thresholds, color vision, anatomy and physiology of the visual pathways, and the cognitive neuroscience of vision. Instructor: Alan A. Stocker, Ph.D. Office: Goddard Labs Rm 421, 3710 Hamilton Walk Phone: (215) 573 9341 Email: [email protected] Office hour: W 12-1p Teaching Assistant: Lingqi Zhang Email: [email protected] Office hour: T 5-6p, TR 5-6p Office hour location: Goddard Labs Rm 420, 3710 Hamilton Walk Course Website (CANVAS): The course has a dedicated CANVAS site. Lecture slides, homework assignments, and reading assignments/material will be posted there. Also, check the site frequently for posted announcements and Q & As on the discussion board. In general, this is the place to go first if you have any question or are in need of any information regarding the course. Requirements: Homework1-3 posted on canvas Midterm Exam 1 February 15 (in class) Midterm Exam 2 March 29 (in class) Midterm Exam 3 April 26 (in class) Final Exam (cumulative) May 13, 12-2p Policy on homework assignments: Homework assignments are essential for a proper understanding of the material. There will be three homework assignments, meant in part as preparation for each midterm exam. Assignments and due dates will be posted on CANVAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception, As You Make It
    Perception, As You Make It Behavioral & Brain Sciences commentary on Chaz Firestone & Brian Scholl, “Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects" David W. Vinson, University of California, Merced, Drew H. Abney, University of California, Merced Dima Amso, Brown University Anthony Chemero, University of Cincinnati James E. Cutting, Cornell University Rick Dale, University of California, Merced Jonathan B. Freeman, New York University Laurie B. Feldman, The University at Albany, SUNY Karl J. Friston, University College London Shaun Gallagher, University of Memphis J. Scott Jordan, Illinois State University Liad Mudrik, Tel Aviv University Sasha Ondobaka, University College London Daniel C. Richardson, University College London Ladan Shams, University of California, Los Angeles Maggie Shiffrar, California State University, Northridge Michael J. Spivey, University of California, Merced Abstract: The main question F&S pose is whether “what and how we see is functionally independent from what and how we think, know, desire, act, etc.” We synthesize a collection of concerns from an interdisciplinary set of co-authors regarding F&S’s assumptions and appeals to intuition, resulting in their treatment of visual perception as context-free. No perceptual task takes place in a contextual vacuum. How do we know that an effect is one of perception qua perception that does not involve other cognitive contributions? Experimental instructions alone involve various cognitive factors that guide task performance (Roepstorff & Frith, 2004). Even a request to detect simple stimulus features requires participants to understand the instructions (“language, memory”), keep track of them (“working memory”), become sensitive to them (“attention”), and pick up the necessary information to become appropriately sensitive (“perception”).
    [Show full text]
  • Do We Know What the Early Visual System Does?
    The Journal of Neuroscience, November 16, 2005 • 25(46):10577–10597 • 10577 Mini-Symposium Do We Know What the Early Visual System Does? Matteo Carandini,1 Jonathan B. Demb,2 Valerio Mante,1 David J. Tolhurst,3 Yang Dan,4 Bruno A. Olshausen,6 Jack L. Gallant,5,6 and Nicole C. Rust7 1Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115, 2Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, and Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48105, 3Department of Physiology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1TN, United Kingdom, Departments of 4Molecular and Cellular Biology and 5Psychology and 6Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute and School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720, and 7Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003 We can claim that we know what the visual system does once we can predict neural responses to arbitrary stimuli, including those seen in nature. In the early visual system, models based on one or more linear receptive fields hold promise to achieve this goal as long as the models include nonlinear mechanisms that control responsiveness, based on stimulus context and history, and take into account the nonlinearity of spike generation. These linear and nonlinear mechanisms might be the only essential determinants of the response, or alternatively, there may be additional fundamental determinants yet to be identified. Research is progressing with the goals of defining a single “standard model” for each stage of the visual pathway and testing the predictive power of these models on the responses to movies of natural scenes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Visual Analysis of Emotional Actions
    SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2006, 1(1) 63Á/74 The visual analysis of emotional actions Arieta Chouchourelou Rutgers University Á/ Newark Campus, Newark, NJ, USA Toshihiko Matsuka Stevens Institute of Technology, W.J. Howe School of Technology Management, Hoboken, NJ, USA Kent Harber and Maggie Shiffrar Rutgers University Á/ Newark Campus, Newark, NJ, USA Is the visual analysis of human actions modulated by the emotional content of those actions? This question is motivated by a consideration of the neuroanatomical connections between visual and emotional areas. Specifically, the superior temporal sulcus (STS), known to play a critical role in the visual detection of action, is extensively interconnected with the amygdala, a center for emotion processing. To the extent that amygdala activity influences STS activity, one would expect to find systematic differences in the visual detection of emotional actions. A series of psychophysical studies tested this prediction. Experiment 1 identified point-light walker movies that convincingly depicted five different emotional states: happiness, sadness, neutral, anger, and fear. In Experiment 2, participants performed a walker detection task with these movies. Detection performance was systematically modulated by the emotional content of the gaits. Participants demonstrated the greatest visual sensitivity to angry walkers. The results of Experiment 3 suggest that local velocity cues to anger may account for high false alarm rates to the presence of angry gaits. These results support the hypothesis that the visual analysis of human action depends upon emotion processes. Human observers demonstrate impressive visual 1988), emotional state (Atkinson, Dittrich, Gem- sensitivity to the actions of other people. This was mell, & Young, 2004) and gender (Troje, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 What Do Models of Visual Perception Tell Us About Visual Phenomenology?
    What do models of visual perception tell us about visual phenomenology? Rachel N. Denison1, Ned Block2, Jason Samaha3 1 Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University 2 Department of Philosophy, New York University 3 Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz 1 Abstract Computational models of visual processing aim to provide a compact, explanatory account of the complex neural processes that underlie visual perception and behavior. But what, if anything, do current modeling approaches say about how conscious visual experience arises from neural processing? Here, we introduce the reader to four commonly used models for understanding visual computations, neural activity, and behavior: signal detection theory, drift diffusion, probabilistic population codes, and sampling. In an attempt to bridge these modeling approaches with experimental and philosophical work on the neural basis of conscious visual perception, we lay out possible relationships between the components of the models and the contents of phenomenal visual experience. We find no unique relation between model components and phenomenal experience in any model; rather, there are multiple logically possible mappings from models to experience. Going forward, we suggest that there are scientific opportunities to develop models that predict and explain a variety of subjective reports and philosophical opportunities to consider what aspects of phenomenal experience are promising scientific targets. Keywords: vision, perception, consciousness, subjective report, computational model 2 1 Introduction The science of visual perception has a long history of developing quantitative, computational models to explain and predict visual performance on a variety of tasks. These models have typically been developed to account for objective visual performance—like observer’s accuracy, reaction time, or perceptual sensitivity in discriminating different visual stimuli.
    [Show full text]
  • Unspecific Hypervigilance and Spatial Attention in Early Visual Perception
    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published May 24, 2013 doi:10.1093/scan/nst044 SCAN (2013) 1 of 7 Timing the fearful brain: unspecific hypervigilance and spatial attention in early visual perception Mathias Weymar,1,2 Andreas Keil,2 and Alfons O. Hamm1 1Department of Biological and Clinical Psychology, University of Greifswald, 17487 Greifswald, Germany and 2Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (CSEA), University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA Numerous studies suggest that anxious individuals are more hypervigilant to threat in their environment than nonanxious individuals. In the present event-related potential (ERP) study, we sought to investigate the extent to which afferent cortical processes, as indexed by the earliest visual component C1, are biased in observers high in fear of specific objects. In a visual search paradigm, ERPs were measured while spider-fearful participants and controls searched for discrepant objects (e.g. spiders, butterflies, flowers) in visual arrays. Results showed enhanced C1 amplitudes in response to spatially directed target stimuli in spider-fearful participants only. Furthermore, enhanced C1 amplitudes were observed in response to all discrepant targets and distractors in spider-fearful compared with non-anxious participants, irrespective of fearful and non-fearful target contents. This pattern of results is in line with theoretical notions of heightened sensory sensitivity (hypervigilance) to external stimuli in high-fearful individuals. Specifically, the findings suggest that fear facilitates afferent cortical processing in the human visual cortex in a non-specific and temporally sustained fashion, when Downloaded from observers search for potential threat cues. Keywords: emotion; hypervigilance; spatial attention; C1; event-related potentials (ERPs) http://scan.oxfordjournals.org/ INTRODUCTION in specific object fear should show sensory facilitation when expecting Voluntary attention to a specific location in the environment results in to confront the feared object.
    [Show full text]
  • VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2020
    PSYC/BIBB/VLST 217 VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2020 Spring 2020 Lecture: MWF 9-10a, Leidy Labs 10 Prerequisites: PSCY 1, BIBB 109, VLST 101, COGS 001, PSYC 149/BIBB 249 Synopsis: An introduction to the scientific study of vision, with an emphasis on the biological substrate and its relation to behavior. Topics will typically include physiological optics, transduction of light, visual thresholds, color vision, anatomy and physiology of the visual pathways, and the cognitive neuroscience of vision. Instructor: Alan A. Stocker, Ph.D. Office: Goddard Labs Rm 421, 3710 Hamilton Walk Phone: (215) 573 9341 Email: [email protected] Office hour: W 12-1p Teaching Assistant: Jiang Mao Email: [email protected] Office hour: T 4-5p, TR 4-5p Office hour location: Goddard Labs Rm 420, 3710 Hamilton Walk Course Website (CANVAS): The course has a dedicated CANVAS site. Lecture slides, homework assignments, and reading assignments/material will be posted there. Also, check the site frequently for posted announcements and Q & As on the discussion board. In general, this is the place to go first if you have any question or are in need of any information regarding the course. Requirements: Homework1-3 posted on canvas Midterm Exam 1 February 21 (in class) Midterm Exam 2 March 27 (in class) Midterm Exam 3 April 24 (in class) Final Exam (cumulative) May 7, 12-2p Policy on homework assignments: Homework assignments are essential for a proper understanding of the material. There will be three homework assignments, meant in part as preparation for each midterm exam. Assignments and due dates will be posted on CANVAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perception & Visual Neuroscience
    Short tutorial: Visual Perception & Visual Neuroscience Organization of visual system Organization: lateral geniculate nucleus Ipsilateral: 2,3,5 Contralateral: 1,4,6 • Parvocellular: small cells, form • Magnocellular: large cells, motion Visual cortex • Primary visual cortex (striate cortex, V1) • Extrastriate visual areas • Retinotopic mapping in humans with fMRI Multiple visual areas: reality Macaque: about 50% of cortex is related to vision Physiology Example: direction selectivity in V1 V1 physiology Simple cells: Complex cells: • orientation selective • orientation selective • some are direction selective • some are direction selective • some are disparity selective • some are disparity selective • monocular or binocular • nearly all are binocular • separate ON and OFF • no separate ON and OFF subregions subregions • length summation • length summation (best response to long bar) Hypercomplex cells: end-stopping (best response to short bar) Brightness perception • Simultaneous brightness contrast – Effect of center- surround RF in the retina? Adelson’s checker shadow illusion See http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_illusion.html Color perception • Trichromacy • Opponent processes – R-G, B-Y, B-W – Opponent cells (center- surround opponency) Cortical basis of color perception • Evidence of a dedicated ‘color module’ in the human brain – Cerebral achromatopsia Mr. I. could hardly bear the changed appearances of people ("like animated gray statues") any more than he could bear his own changed appearance in the mirror: he shunned social intercourse and found sexual intercourse impossible. He saw people's flesh, his wife's flesh, his own flesh, as an abhorrent gray; "flesh-colored" now appeared "rat-colored" to him. This was so even when he closed his eyes, for his preternaturally vivid ("eidetic") visual imagery was preserved but now without color, and forced on him images, forced him to "see" but see internally with the wrongness of his achromatopsia.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuropsychiatry Review Series: Disorders of Visual Perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani
    Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani To cite this version: Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani. Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception.. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, BMJ Publishing Group, 2010, 81 (11), pp.1280. 10.1136/jnnp.2008.171348. hal-00587980 HAL Id: hal-00587980 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00587980 Submitted on 22 Apr 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Disorders of visual perception Dr Dominic H ffytche1,4* Dr JD Blom2,3 4 Dr M Catani 1 Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK 2 Parnassia Bavo Group, The Hague, the Netherlands 3 Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands 4 Natbrainlab, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK *Address for Correspondence Dr D H ffytche Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry PO70, King’s College
    [Show full text]
  • Achieving Precise Display Timing in Visual Neuroscience Experiments
    Journal of Neuroscience Methods 191 (2010) 171–179 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Neuroscience Methods journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jneumeth Achieving precise display timing in visual neuroscience experiments Tobias Elze ∗ Max Planck Institute for Mathematics in the Sciences, Inselstr. 22, 04103 Leipzig, Germany article info abstract Article history: In experimental visual neuroscience brief presentations of visual stimuli are often required. Accurate Received 4 May 2007 knowledge of the durations of visual stimuli and their signal shapes is important in psychophysical Received in revised form 17 June 2010 experiments with humans and in neuronal recordings with animals. In this study we measure and analyze Accepted 19 June 2010 the changes in luminance of visual stimuli on standard computer monitors. Signal properties of the two most frequently used monitor technologies, cathode ray tube (CRT) and liquid crystal display (LCD) Keywords: monitors, are compared, and the effects of the signal shapes on the stated durations of visual stimuli Computer monitors are analyzed. The fundamental differences between CRT and LCD signals require different methods for Timing precision Timing errors the specification of durations, especially for brief stimulus presentations. In addition, stimulus durations CRT on LCD monitors vary over different monitor models and are not even homogeneous with respect to LCD different luminance levels on a single monitor. The use of LCD technology for brief stimulus presentation requires extensive display measurements prior to the experiment. © 2010 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved. 1. Introduction covered with a substance called phosphor. In aperture grille CRTs the same technology is applied except that instead of small holes, 1.1.
    [Show full text]