Functional Neuroanatomy of the Human Visual System: a Review of Functional MRI Studies

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Functional Neuroanatomy of the Human Visual System: a Review of Functional MRI Studies Chapter 8 Functional Neuroanatomy of the Human Visual 8 System: A Review of Functional MRI Studies Mark W. Greenlee, Peter U. Tse Core Messages ■ This chapter reviews work on the meth- ■ In a significant advance beyond the tra- od of functional magnetic resonance ditional localistic “one region, one type imaging (fMRI), which has been used of processing” paradigm, new methods, to describe the structural and functional such as dynamic causal modeling and anatomy of the human visual system. discriminant analysis, seek to determine ■ Exploitation of the endogenous para- temporal relationships among the fMRI magnetic contrast agent deoxyhemoglo- time series of multiple brain regions. bin has yielded functional maps of: ■ Applying these new methods, neurosci- - lateral geniculate nucleus of the thala- entists can discern how spatially distrib- mus uted brain regions interact via feedfor- - the columnar organization of primary ward and feedback signals sent within visual cortex neural circuits. - multiple representations of the visual ■ fMRI promises to contribute more to our hemifields in the ventral and dorsal understanding of the complex neural visual pathways circuits that subserve visual perception - the interface between the visual sys- and visuospatial cognition. tem and cortical networks underlying the control of oculomotor behavior, visual working memory, and higher visual cognition. 8.1 Introduction erate under the false impression that we perceive the world in a veridical way. Perceived qualities Our visual system is one of the great success sto- such as redness or brightness do not exist in the ries of evolution. Together with the other sensory world; they are creations of our brain. Of course systems, its purpose is to provide information they generally correspond to properties of energy about the world so that we can operate within in the environment, such as spectral composition a changing environment to fulfill our goals. The or luminance, but percepts are not the same as representations that our visual system creates of what they represent. This is made obvious by the events and objects within a 3D scene are so accu- fact that there is no visible light impinging on the rate and produced so quickly, that most of us op- cortex; there are only neurons communicating in 120 Functional Neuroanatomy of the Human Visual System: A Review of Functional MRI Studies total darkness. Moreover, cortical neurons do not Functional magnetic resonance imag- respond directly to light at all. They only respond ing (fMRI) offers a means by which neu- to their particular set of dendritic inputs. The roscientists can determine how the brain culmination of multiple stages of neuronal pro- constructs visual perception. Visual informa- cessing within the dark space of our skull is our tion can evoke or modify neural activity in a ma- visual experience of an external world. How the jority of cortical areas. Loss of any single function highly ambiguous pattern of light that is detected of the visual system can impair a patient’s at the retina is transformed into visual conscious- ability to efficiently interact with the environ- ness remains one of the greatest unsolved prob- ment. Thus, our motivation to achieve a scien- lems of science. tifically grounded understanding of how the In some cases, which we tend to dismiss as mere brain realizes visual perception and visual con- visual illusions, the visual system makes mistakes. sciousness is correspondingly high. This chapter It tells us that a color, shape, motion, or light is reviews our current state of knowledge about present when we know that it is not. Visual illu- the systems-level functional anatomy of the hu- sions testify to the fact that even ordinary visual man visual system. The emphasis will be placed 8 experiences are constructions of the brain. They on the results of fMRI as a noninvasive imaging occur as a consequence of the way the visual sys- technique that combines structural informa- tem processes its input. In general, these process- tion (gray/white matter, fiber tracts) with func- ing steps lead to veridical information about real- tional activity (blood-oxygen-level dependent, world objects. But in cases where they do not, we BOLD) with a spatial resolution of 1 mm (or can learn something about the steps that the vi- better) and a temporal resolution of under 1 s. sual systems invokes in creating visual representa- Sparing excessive methodological detail, the tions. For example, when you see a light jumping techniques used to make retinotopic maps of back and forth on the top of an ambulance, you the human visual cortex and the resultant nor- know that there is no real motion. You perceive malized atlases will be described. Taking ad- these sequences of lights as motion, even though vantage of the functional specificity of higher you know that there is no physical movement visual areas, topographical representations of present, a phenomenon known as apparent mo- image features such as color, form, and mo- tion. From this mistake we can infer how motion tion (speed and direction), among other as- percepts in general are constructed. What we have pects, will be discussed. Stereopsis, the ability learned from such instances is that the visual sys- to detect disparity in fused binocular images, tem has prior assumptions concerning the map- is detected by neurons with binocular receptive ping between sensory input and the structure of fields. Functional MRI has revealed neural cor- the world. Indeed, the visual system must have relates of disparity detection and clarified its role these “priors” because visual input is often am- in the perception of motion-in-depth of loom- biguous. For example, countless 3D objects could ing stimuli. Finally we consider recent work lead to any given pattern of 2D retinal images. To on the interface between the visual system and create veridical 3D representations within a frac- systems involved in the preparation of oculomo- tion of a second, the visual system must solve this tor action. In this context, the neural control of inverse mapping problem on the basis of infor- saccadic eye movements has been investigated mation processing procedures that also make use with fMRI and the functional connectivity be- of extraretinal signals. The visual system must tween the frontal and parietal eye fields and areas construct object representations that can only be in the visual cortex are now better understood. inferred on the basis of the input. How the visual The analysis of such “micro-behavior” opens system accomplishes this is partially understood, a window into the functioning of the human but much remains to be explained. Many of the brain with respect to higher cognitive functions most basic issues remain unexplained, including, such as visual working memory and conscious- for example, the neural code used by neurons to ness. communicate among themselves, and the neural basis of consciousness. 8.4 Striate and Extrastriate Visual Areas in Human Visual Cortex (V1, V2, V3) 121 8.2 Imaging the Lateral Each visual neuron has a receptive field that re- Geniculate Nucleus sponds to stimuli falling within a well-defined re- gion of this retinotopic space. Figure 8.1 depicts With the increase of magnetic field strength from a schematic illustration of human visual area 1 1.5 to 4 Tesla and greater, it has now become pos- (V1) and its corresponding retinotopic map of sible to map subcortical structures. The human visual space taken from Horton and Hoyt (1991). lateral geniculate nucleus (LGN) is estimated Neighboring neurons in the cortical sheet exhibit to have a volume that varies from 91 mm3 to receptive fields that overlap in visual space. As- 157 mm3 (Andrews et al. 1997). Using high-field suming steady fixation, the human visual cortex fMRI, Kastner et al. (2004) and Schneider et al. can be sequentially stimulated using flickering (2004) mapped the human LGN using hemifield checkerboard wedge or ring stimuli (Engel et al. checkerboard stimuli. They showed that the am- 1994; Sereno et al. 1995; Dale et al. 1999; Tootell plitude of the BOLD response in LGN depends et al. 1995; Brewer et al. 2002) thereby evoking on the attentional state of the observer, suggest- a traveling wave of activation over the cortical ing a functional role for the massive cortico-tha- surface. To take advantage of computational al- lamic feedback projections to the LGN that have gorithms such as fast Fourier transform (FFT), been observed anatomically. That is to say, cor- this traveling wave of activity can be reduced to tico-thalamic projections modify the thalamic a temporal frequency distribution with a maxi- input received by the cortex via neural mecha- mal amplitude at the stimulation (rotational) nisms that subserve selective attention. frequency (Warnking et al. 2002). The phase of Sylvester et al. (2005) acquired BOLD re- this FFT component thus provides a reliable in- sponses from the LGN and V1 while subjects dex of the spatial location of the peak of activity performed saccades in both an illuminated Gan- during the stimulation cycle, after correcting for zfeld and in the dark. Interestingly, saccades in the time lag of the hemodynamic response. Using the full-field light condition led to a suppression these methods, accurate and reproducible maps of activity in the LGN and V1, whereas saccades of the human visual cortex have been produced in the dark led to an increase in activity. Their in which the borders between V1, V2 and higher findings suggest that signals from oculomotor retinotopic areas are revealed by the so-called centers have a suppressive effect on on-going mirror sign (a reversal of the phase sequence activity in V1 and the LGN, in line with recent from vertical to horizontal meridian, or the other work from our laboratory (Vallines and Green- way round).
Recommended publications
  • Visual Cortex in Humans 251
    Author's personal copy Visual Cortex in Humans 251 Visual Cortex in Humans B A Wandell, S O Dumoulin, and A A Brewer, using fMRI, and we discuss the main features of the Stanford University, Stanford, CA, USA V1 map. We then summarize the positions and proper- ties of ten additional visual field maps. This represents ã 2009 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. our current understanding of human visual field maps, although this remains an active field of investigation, with more maps likely to be discovered. Finally, we Human visua l cortex comprises 4–6 billion neurons that are organ ized into more than a dozen distinct describe theories about the functional purpose and functional areas. These areas include the gray matter organizing principles of these maps. in the occi pital lobe and extend into the temporal and parietal lobes . The locations of these areas in the The Size and Location of Human Visual intact human cortex can be identified by measuring Cortex visual field maps. The neurons within these areas have a variety of different stimulus response proper- The entirety of human cortex occupies a surface area 2 ties. We descr ibe how to measure these visual field on the order of 1000 cm and ranges between 2 and maps, their locations, and their overall organization. 4 mm in thickness. Each cubic millimeter of cortex contains approximately 50 000 neurons so that neo- We then consider how information about patterns, objects, color s, and motion is analyzed and repre- cortex in the two hemispheres contain on the order of sented in these maps.
    [Show full text]
  • The Davida Teller Award Lecture, 2016 Visual Brain Development: A
    Journal of Vision (2017) 17(3):26, 1–24 1 The Davida Teller Award Lecture, 2016 Visual Brain Development: A review of ‘‘Dorsal Stream Vulnerability’’—motion, mathematics, amblyopia, actions, and attention # Janette Atkinson University College London, London, UK $ Research in the Visual Development Unit on ‘‘dorsal stream vulnerability’ (DSV) arose from research in two Introduction somewhat different areas. In the first, using cortical milestones for local and global processing from our neurobiological model, we identified cerebral visual Emerging cortical function and infant cortical impairment in infants in the first year of life. In the impairment second, using photo/videorefraction in population refractive screening programs, we showed that infant The work discussed at the beginning of this review spectacle wear could reduce the incidence of strabismus arose out of the first twenty years of my research with and amblyopia, but many preschool children, who had Oliver Braddick and our team in the Visual Develop- been significantly hyperopic earlier, showed visuo-motor ment Unit in Cambridge, particularly John Wattam- and attentional deficits. This led us to compare Bell and Shirley Anker (Atkinson, 2000). We began by developing dorsal and ventral streams, using sensitivity devising new methods, both behavioral (automated to global motion and form as signatures, finding deficits forced-choice preferential looking) and electrophysio- in motion sensitivity relative to form in children with logical (steady-state VEP/VERP—Visual Evoked Po- Williams syndrome, or perinatal brain injury in tential/Visual Event Related Potential) to measure the hemiplegia or preterm birth. Later research showed that normal visual capacities of infants such as acuity and this DSV was common across many disorders, both ‘‘ ’’ contrast sensitivity, over the first years of life (Atkin- genetic and acquired, from autism to amblyopia.
    [Show full text]
  • VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2019
    PSYC/BIBB/VLST 217 VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2019 Spring 2019 Lecture: MWF 9-10a, Leidy Labs 10 Prerequisites: PSCY 1, BIBB 109, VLST 101, or COGS 001 Synopsis: An introduction to the scientific study of vision, with an emphasis on the biological substrate and its relation to behavior. Topics will typically include physiological optics, transduction of light, visual thresholds, color vision, anatomy and physiology of the visual pathways, and the cognitive neuroscience of vision. Instructor: Alan A. Stocker, Ph.D. Office: Goddard Labs Rm 421, 3710 Hamilton Walk Phone: (215) 573 9341 Email: [email protected] Office hour: W 12-1p Teaching Assistant: Lingqi Zhang Email: [email protected] Office hour: T 5-6p, TR 5-6p Office hour location: Goddard Labs Rm 420, 3710 Hamilton Walk Course Website (CANVAS): The course has a dedicated CANVAS site. Lecture slides, homework assignments, and reading assignments/material will be posted there. Also, check the site frequently for posted announcements and Q & As on the discussion board. In general, this is the place to go first if you have any question or are in need of any information regarding the course. Requirements: Homework1-3 posted on canvas Midterm Exam 1 February 15 (in class) Midterm Exam 2 March 29 (in class) Midterm Exam 3 April 26 (in class) Final Exam (cumulative) May 13, 12-2p Policy on homework assignments: Homework assignments are essential for a proper understanding of the material. There will be three homework assignments, meant in part as preparation for each midterm exam. Assignments and due dates will be posted on CANVAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Perception, As You Make It
    Perception, As You Make It Behavioral & Brain Sciences commentary on Chaz Firestone & Brian Scholl, “Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects" David W. Vinson, University of California, Merced, Drew H. Abney, University of California, Merced Dima Amso, Brown University Anthony Chemero, University of Cincinnati James E. Cutting, Cornell University Rick Dale, University of California, Merced Jonathan B. Freeman, New York University Laurie B. Feldman, The University at Albany, SUNY Karl J. Friston, University College London Shaun Gallagher, University of Memphis J. Scott Jordan, Illinois State University Liad Mudrik, Tel Aviv University Sasha Ondobaka, University College London Daniel C. Richardson, University College London Ladan Shams, University of California, Los Angeles Maggie Shiffrar, California State University, Northridge Michael J. Spivey, University of California, Merced Abstract: The main question F&S pose is whether “what and how we see is functionally independent from what and how we think, know, desire, act, etc.” We synthesize a collection of concerns from an interdisciplinary set of co-authors regarding F&S’s assumptions and appeals to intuition, resulting in their treatment of visual perception as context-free. No perceptual task takes place in a contextual vacuum. How do we know that an effect is one of perception qua perception that does not involve other cognitive contributions? Experimental instructions alone involve various cognitive factors that guide task performance (Roepstorff & Frith, 2004). Even a request to detect simple stimulus features requires participants to understand the instructions (“language, memory”), keep track of them (“working memory”), become sensitive to them (“attention”), and pick up the necessary information to become appropriately sensitive (“perception”).
    [Show full text]
  • Principles of Image Representation in Visual Cortex
    In: The Visual Neurosciences, L.M. Chalupa, J.S. Werner, Eds. MIT Press, 2003: 1603-1615. Principles of Image Representation in Visual Cortex Bruno A. Olshausen Center for Neuroscience, UC Davis [email protected] 1 Introduction data. Neuroscientists are interested in understand- ing how the cortex extracts certain properties of the The visual cortex is responsible for most of our con- visual environment—surfaces, objects, textures, mo- scious perception of the visual world, yet we remain tion, etc.—from the data stream coming from the largely ignorant of the principles underlying its func- retina. Similarly, engineers are interested in design- tion despite progress on many fronts of neuroscience. ing algorithms capable of extracting structure con- The principal reason for this is not a lack of data, tained in images or sound—for example, to identify but rather the absence of a solid theoretical frame- and label parts within the body from medical imag- work for motivating experiments and interpreting ing data. These problems at their core are one in the findings. The situation may be likened to trying same, and progress in one domain will likely lead to to understand how birds fly without knowledge of new insights in the other. the principles of aerodynamics: no amount of ex- The key difficulty faced by both sides is that the perimentation or measurements made on the bird core principles of pattern analysis are not well un- itself will reveal the secret. The key experiment— derstood. No amount of experimentation or techno- measuring air pressure above and below the wing as logical tinkering alone can overcome this obstacle.
    [Show full text]
  • Do We Know What the Early Visual System Does?
    The Journal of Neuroscience, November 16, 2005 • 25(46):10577–10597 • 10577 Mini-Symposium Do We Know What the Early Visual System Does? Matteo Carandini,1 Jonathan B. Demb,2 Valerio Mante,1 David J. Tolhurst,3 Yang Dan,4 Bruno A. Olshausen,6 Jack L. Gallant,5,6 and Nicole C. Rust7 1Smith-Kettlewell Eye Research Institute, San Francisco, California 94115, 2Departments of Ophthalmology and Visual Sciences, and Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48105, 3Department of Physiology, University of Cambridge, Cambridge CB2 1TN, United Kingdom, Departments of 4Molecular and Cellular Biology and 5Psychology and 6Helen Wills Neuroscience Institute and School of Optometry, University of California, Berkeley, Berkeley, California 94720, and 7Center for Neural Science, New York University, New York, New York 10003 We can claim that we know what the visual system does once we can predict neural responses to arbitrary stimuli, including those seen in nature. In the early visual system, models based on one or more linear receptive fields hold promise to achieve this goal as long as the models include nonlinear mechanisms that control responsiveness, based on stimulus context and history, and take into account the nonlinearity of spike generation. These linear and nonlinear mechanisms might be the only essential determinants of the response, or alternatively, there may be additional fundamental determinants yet to be identified. Research is progressing with the goals of defining a single “standard model” for each stage of the visual pathway and testing the predictive power of these models on the responses to movies of natural scenes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Visual Analysis of Emotional Actions
    SOCIAL NEUROSCIENCE, 2006, 1(1) 63Á/74 The visual analysis of emotional actions Arieta Chouchourelou Rutgers University Á/ Newark Campus, Newark, NJ, USA Toshihiko Matsuka Stevens Institute of Technology, W.J. Howe School of Technology Management, Hoboken, NJ, USA Kent Harber and Maggie Shiffrar Rutgers University Á/ Newark Campus, Newark, NJ, USA Is the visual analysis of human actions modulated by the emotional content of those actions? This question is motivated by a consideration of the neuroanatomical connections between visual and emotional areas. Specifically, the superior temporal sulcus (STS), known to play a critical role in the visual detection of action, is extensively interconnected with the amygdala, a center for emotion processing. To the extent that amygdala activity influences STS activity, one would expect to find systematic differences in the visual detection of emotional actions. A series of psychophysical studies tested this prediction. Experiment 1 identified point-light walker movies that convincingly depicted five different emotional states: happiness, sadness, neutral, anger, and fear. In Experiment 2, participants performed a walker detection task with these movies. Detection performance was systematically modulated by the emotional content of the gaits. Participants demonstrated the greatest visual sensitivity to angry walkers. The results of Experiment 3 suggest that local velocity cues to anger may account for high false alarm rates to the presence of angry gaits. These results support the hypothesis that the visual analysis of human action depends upon emotion processes. Human observers demonstrate impressive visual 1988), emotional state (Atkinson, Dittrich, Gem- sensitivity to the actions of other people. This was mell, & Young, 2004) and gender (Troje, 2002).
    [Show full text]
  • 1 What Do Models of Visual Perception Tell Us About Visual Phenomenology?
    What do models of visual perception tell us about visual phenomenology? Rachel N. Denison1, Ned Block2, Jason Samaha3 1 Department of Psychology and Center for Neural Science, New York University 2 Department of Philosophy, New York University 3 Department of Psychology, University of California, Santa Cruz 1 Abstract Computational models of visual processing aim to provide a compact, explanatory account of the complex neural processes that underlie visual perception and behavior. But what, if anything, do current modeling approaches say about how conscious visual experience arises from neural processing? Here, we introduce the reader to four commonly used models for understanding visual computations, neural activity, and behavior: signal detection theory, drift diffusion, probabilistic population codes, and sampling. In an attempt to bridge these modeling approaches with experimental and philosophical work on the neural basis of conscious visual perception, we lay out possible relationships between the components of the models and the contents of phenomenal visual experience. We find no unique relation between model components and phenomenal experience in any model; rather, there are multiple logically possible mappings from models to experience. Going forward, we suggest that there are scientific opportunities to develop models that predict and explain a variety of subjective reports and philosophical opportunities to consider what aspects of phenomenal experience are promising scientific targets. Keywords: vision, perception, consciousness, subjective report, computational model 2 1 Introduction The science of visual perception has a long history of developing quantitative, computational models to explain and predict visual performance on a variety of tasks. These models have typically been developed to account for objective visual performance—like observer’s accuracy, reaction time, or perceptual sensitivity in discriminating different visual stimuli.
    [Show full text]
  • Unspecific Hypervigilance and Spatial Attention in Early Visual Perception
    Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience Advance Access published May 24, 2013 doi:10.1093/scan/nst044 SCAN (2013) 1 of 7 Timing the fearful brain: unspecific hypervigilance and spatial attention in early visual perception Mathias Weymar,1,2 Andreas Keil,2 and Alfons O. Hamm1 1Department of Biological and Clinical Psychology, University of Greifswald, 17487 Greifswald, Germany and 2Center for the Study of Emotion and Attention (CSEA), University of Florida, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA Numerous studies suggest that anxious individuals are more hypervigilant to threat in their environment than nonanxious individuals. In the present event-related potential (ERP) study, we sought to investigate the extent to which afferent cortical processes, as indexed by the earliest visual component C1, are biased in observers high in fear of specific objects. In a visual search paradigm, ERPs were measured while spider-fearful participants and controls searched for discrepant objects (e.g. spiders, butterflies, flowers) in visual arrays. Results showed enhanced C1 amplitudes in response to spatially directed target stimuli in spider-fearful participants only. Furthermore, enhanced C1 amplitudes were observed in response to all discrepant targets and distractors in spider-fearful compared with non-anxious participants, irrespective of fearful and non-fearful target contents. This pattern of results is in line with theoretical notions of heightened sensory sensitivity (hypervigilance) to external stimuli in high-fearful individuals. Specifically, the findings suggest that fear facilitates afferent cortical processing in the human visual cortex in a non-specific and temporally sustained fashion, when Downloaded from observers search for potential threat cues. Keywords: emotion; hypervigilance; spatial attention; C1; event-related potentials (ERPs) http://scan.oxfordjournals.org/ INTRODUCTION in specific object fear should show sensory facilitation when expecting Voluntary attention to a specific location in the environment results in to confront the feared object.
    [Show full text]
  • VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2020
    PSYC/BIBB/VLST 217 VISUAL NEUROSCIENCE Spring 2020 Spring 2020 Lecture: MWF 9-10a, Leidy Labs 10 Prerequisites: PSCY 1, BIBB 109, VLST 101, COGS 001, PSYC 149/BIBB 249 Synopsis: An introduction to the scientific study of vision, with an emphasis on the biological substrate and its relation to behavior. Topics will typically include physiological optics, transduction of light, visual thresholds, color vision, anatomy and physiology of the visual pathways, and the cognitive neuroscience of vision. Instructor: Alan A. Stocker, Ph.D. Office: Goddard Labs Rm 421, 3710 Hamilton Walk Phone: (215) 573 9341 Email: [email protected] Office hour: W 12-1p Teaching Assistant: Jiang Mao Email: [email protected] Office hour: T 4-5p, TR 4-5p Office hour location: Goddard Labs Rm 420, 3710 Hamilton Walk Course Website (CANVAS): The course has a dedicated CANVAS site. Lecture slides, homework assignments, and reading assignments/material will be posted there. Also, check the site frequently for posted announcements and Q & As on the discussion board. In general, this is the place to go first if you have any question or are in need of any information regarding the course. Requirements: Homework1-3 posted on canvas Midterm Exam 1 February 21 (in class) Midterm Exam 2 March 27 (in class) Midterm Exam 3 April 24 (in class) Final Exam (cumulative) May 7, 12-2p Policy on homework assignments: Homework assignments are essential for a proper understanding of the material. There will be three homework assignments, meant in part as preparation for each midterm exam. Assignments and due dates will be posted on CANVAS.
    [Show full text]
  • Visual Perception & Visual Neuroscience
    Short tutorial: Visual Perception & Visual Neuroscience Organization of visual system Organization: lateral geniculate nucleus Ipsilateral: 2,3,5 Contralateral: 1,4,6 • Parvocellular: small cells, form • Magnocellular: large cells, motion Visual cortex • Primary visual cortex (striate cortex, V1) • Extrastriate visual areas • Retinotopic mapping in humans with fMRI Multiple visual areas: reality Macaque: about 50% of cortex is related to vision Physiology Example: direction selectivity in V1 V1 physiology Simple cells: Complex cells: • orientation selective • orientation selective • some are direction selective • some are direction selective • some are disparity selective • some are disparity selective • monocular or binocular • nearly all are binocular • separate ON and OFF • no separate ON and OFF subregions subregions • length summation • length summation (best response to long bar) Hypercomplex cells: end-stopping (best response to short bar) Brightness perception • Simultaneous brightness contrast – Effect of center- surround RF in the retina? Adelson’s checker shadow illusion See http://web.mit.edu/persci/people/adelson/checkershadow_illusion.html Color perception • Trichromacy • Opponent processes – R-G, B-Y, B-W – Opponent cells (center- surround opponency) Cortical basis of color perception • Evidence of a dedicated ‘color module’ in the human brain – Cerebral achromatopsia Mr. I. could hardly bear the changed appearances of people ("like animated gray statues") any more than he could bear his own changed appearance in the mirror: he shunned social intercourse and found sexual intercourse impossible. He saw people's flesh, his wife's flesh, his own flesh, as an abhorrent gray; "flesh-colored" now appeared "rat-colored" to him. This was so even when he closed his eyes, for his preternaturally vivid ("eidetic") visual imagery was preserved but now without color, and forced on him images, forced him to "see" but see internally with the wrongness of his achromatopsia.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuropsychiatry Review Series: Disorders of Visual Perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani
    Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception. Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani To cite this version: Dominic Ffytche, Jan Dirk Blom, Marco Catani. Neuropsychiatry Review series: Disorders of Visual perception.. Journal of Neurology, Neurosurgery and Psychiatry, BMJ Publishing Group, 2010, 81 (11), pp.1280. 10.1136/jnnp.2008.171348. hal-00587980 HAL Id: hal-00587980 https://hal.archives-ouvertes.fr/hal-00587980 Submitted on 22 Apr 2011 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Disorders of visual perception Dr Dominic H ffytche1,4* Dr JD Blom2,3 4 Dr M Catani 1 Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK 2 Parnassia Bavo Group, The Hague, the Netherlands 3 Department of Psychiatry, University of Groningen, Groningen, the Netherlands 4 Natbrainlab, Department of Forensic and Neurodevelopmental Sciences, Institute of Psychiatry, King’s College London, UK *Address for Correspondence Dr D H ffytche Department of Old Age Psychiatry, Institute of Psychiatry PO70, King’s College
    [Show full text]