Towhee Talk Dana A

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Towhee Talk Dana A Towhee Talk Dana A. Glei Rehab Volunteer, Bird Rescue Center California Passerine Rehabilitators Gathering Napa Valley Community College February 18, 2017 Presented and Coordinated by: Native Songbird Care & Conservation Co-Hosted with Napa Wildlife Rescue www.BirdRescueCenter.org Towhees are “Special” Towhees vex all rehabbers www.BirdRescueCenter.org Trauma X Captivity STRESS! • Do not fare well in captivity • Easily stressed • Most were “catted” (poor prognosis for any bird) • As they enter the pre-fledgling stage, “they discover they are Towhees” (Francoise at Wildcare) – That is, they become reluctant to gape. – If they don’t eat, they will die. www.BirdRescueCenter.org 68% of Young Towhees were “Catted” Sticky Traps www.BirdRescueCenter.org What Stresses Them? • Pain: Being catted is very painful. A bird in pain may be reluctant to eat. [We administer painkillers along with antibiotics to all catted birds.] • Excessive noise from humans or predatory species (e.g., jays) • Inadequate housing: overcrowded; dirty; lacking enrichment (i.e., natural greenery, comfortable perches, dirt tray, leaf litter, & water bath that encourages normal development) • Incorrect, inadequate diet: irregular/infrequent feeding schedule • Dirty feathers/feet/face www.BirdRescueCenter.org Returning Them to the Wild Early Does Not Solve the Problem Unless we can reunite them with their parents, returning them to the wild too early would be a death sentence. • CALTs mature slowly and need to learn many survival skills before they can thrive on their own: generally, 6-8 weeks from hatchling to release. • In comparison, some bird species can be released at a younger age. www.BirdRescueCenter.org What is their natural environment like in the wild? www.BirdRescueCenter.org Where do they live? • Among the most wary & furtive of all birds • Live close to the ground, often hidden in dense bushes – Chaparral scrub in coastal slopes & foothills – Along streams & canyon bottoms among oaks, manzanita, madrone, etc. – Shrubby backyards & city parks • If you hear rustling in the leaf litter, you can bet it is a Towhee • LOVES bathing www.BirdRescueCenter.org Key Elements of Towhee Habitat 1) Dense shrubs to provide shelter (e.g., Ceanothus/Blue Blossom, Coyote Bush); www.BirdRescueCenter.org 2) Seed Food Sources • Native bunch grasses (e.g., CA Fescue, Deergrass) • Note: CALTs in my yard seem to prefer native CA Fescue seed to the “bird seed” we use at BRC. www.BirdRescueCenter.org 3) Lots of leaf litter www.BirdRescueCenter.org …and 4) Taller trees for escape If you are lucky enough to have a mature native oak, treasure it—our feathered friends certainly do! www.BirdRescueCenter.org What do they eat & feed their young? • <10 days old: Entirely insects (mostly grasshoppers & caterpillars) • 10 Days old: >90% insects (including beetles & wasps); <10% vegetable (largely fiber, little fruit) • 2+ weeks old: >90% insects(largely beetles, wasps & ants) • Adult Diet: mostly seeds/grains (~80%) with ~15% insects (esp. beetles & grasshoppers) and ~5% fruit • Got Poison Oak? Often build their nests in poison oak and feast on the pale white berries www.BirdRescueCenter.org How do they behave? Nestling Stage • At 2-3 days old: will raise heads & gape in response to nest vibrations • At 8 days: will cower rather than beg when approached; will jump from nest if disturbed • Fecal sacs removed by adults, nest kept clean until fledging www.BirdRescueCenter.org How do they behave? Fledgling Stage • Stay with parents up to 6 weeks (but if parents renest, they may drive away fledges at 4 weeks) • Cannot fly at fledging, but capable runners • Spend several weeks hiding in vegetation near nest site while being fed by parents • At 3-4 weeks, young begin foraging for themselves www.BirdRescueCenter.org How do they communicate? • Many variations of the basic “tsip” note: – Territorial display call (metallic “tsip”) – Contentment/contact call (sparrow-like “tssip”) – Distress call (long peep sounds, squeaking) – Alarm call (loud warning note) – Feeding calls (loud peep crescendos at regular intervals into a short staccato call) • Hunger call: “puhlee” for as long as 30s at a time peaking at 20s. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Feed me… Feed me NOW! www.BirdRescueCenter.org Lesson #1 Setting the Mood www.BirdRescueCenter.org The Right Environment Helps a Lot • Try to emulate their natural environment as much as possible in a rehab hospital setting. • Provide a calm, quiet space (keep them separate from the “loud mouths”, particularly jays). • Group single baby birds with a buddy of the same or compatible species (SPTO, AMRO, NOMO, BHCO). • If solitary confinement is necessary, add a mirror. • Use brown (rather than white) paper liners. White stresses towhees. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Age Appropriate Housing • As soon as it is developmentally appropriate, move them to a: 1) Reptarium (simulate a more natural environment: greenery, dirt tray, leaf litter) 2) Aviary (allow more flight practice, encourage foraging and learning other skills they will need to survive) www.BirdRescueCenter.org Needs a Place to Hide www.BirdRescueCenter.org We have special “Towhee” dishes • Shallow with low edges: able to get in/out easily • Broad trays with mix of seed and worms where they can practice the “towhee two-step” www.BirdRescueCenter.org Reptarium (once they can thermoregulate) www.BirdRescueCenter.org Reptarium (“Model” before they cover it with poop) www.BirdRescueCenter.org Aviary www.BirdRescueCenter.org Aviary www.BirdRescueCenter.org Lesson #2 Keep 'em Clean! www.BirdRescueCenter.org Keep 'em Clean: Easier Said than Done • Look at all that poop! (Not to mention the empty food bowl) • Reality: They poop again before you even leave. • It is an endless job. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Lesson #3 Patience, Grasshopper! www.BirdRescueCenter.org You MUST get them to eat ...or they will starve, but force -feeding them causes stress and thus, may exacerbate the problem. • Stress can kill them: – Directly (they can truly be “scared to death”) OR – Indirectly (because it leads to starvation). Only experienced volunteers who are willing to take the time (and have the proper training) to get them to eat. www.BirdRescueCenter.org FIRST, make every effort to get them to gape • OCD Tendencies: Let them go thru their ritual (i.e., hop around, act distracted, and ignore you) until they are finally ready to gape. • Be very, very patient: WE CANNOT RUSH THEM! • Bobbing motion with the worm-filled hemostat can help switch them into eating mode. • It will seem like it takes FOREVER, and you might think they will NEVER gape...and they then do. • Once they start gaping, they will usually keep gaping easily...and they will eat a LOT! www.BirdRescueCenter.org Mimic the “Time to Eat” Vocalization • “Chipping song” imitated by the foster parent (that’s you!) has a calming effect on nervous birds. • How do you do it? – Air is pressed through the teeth while moving the lips and modulating a faint hissing-whistling sound. – They are forgiving if your call is not perfect and can get used to a wide variety of feeding calls. • Can help trigger gaping behavior when applied consistently (at the start of each feeding). • Do not overuse (or you may stress the bird). www.BirdRescueCenter.org Difficult Eaters? Add a Good Gaper • Buddy good gapers with similar-aged difficult eaters • A gaping nestling might encourage a pre-fledge to gape • A different, but compatible species is OK (e.g., cowbirds make good nursemaids). • Use colored bands to keep track of the good versus difficult eaters (and make sure they don’t revert). www.BirdRescueCenter.org LAST RESORT: Assisted Feeding • I like to “burrito” the towhee in a small washcloth (easier to hold them without squeezing to tightly). • Towhee may start gaping once you are holding it. • If not, gently open its mouth (can be challenging to hold the bird AND a food-filled hemostat while opening its mouth and inserting food). • Let it swallow before trying to give more food. • They eat a surprising amount of food; gauge the amount for an “assisted feeding” based on the consumption of similar aged towhees. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Bird Rescue Center’s Towhee Protocol www.BirdRescueCenter.org Naked/Pin-Feather Stage • Days 1-5, 10-14g, no tail growth • Placed in an incubator (at 90-95F with 50- 60% humidity) • Fed every 30 minutes • Jiggling the nest may stimulate gaping • Their eyes open on Day 5 www.BirdRescueCenter.org Nestling Stage • Days 5-10, 20-30g – Days 6-8: Feathers begin to emerge – Days 8-10: Will start hopping around • Still fed every 30 minutes • When mobile, moved out of the incubator into the Brooder Area. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Towhee Nestlings: 7 Days Old Source: The Birds of North America On-Line. www.BirdRescueCenter.org During feeding, this towhee decided to jump out of the basket! It is time to get him more space, perches, and greenery to explore. www.BirdRescueCenter.org Pre-Fledgling Stage • Days 10-13, 25-35g, “tail starter” (2cm long) • Needs space, greenery, places to perch, leaf litter, dirt tray, etc. • Often become reluctant gapers at this stage • Feeding intervals progress from 30 to 45 min • We leave sticky notes (i.e., about the idiosyncrasies of each individual) for the next feeder; their preferences change quickly! www.BirdRescueCenter.org “Tail Starter” Phase (Pre-Fledge) www.BirdRescueCenter.org True Fledgling Stage • Age 2 weeks to 2 months, 35-45g, tail growing 5mm/day • Hand feeding intervals progress from 1 to 2h • Weaning can begin when tail reaches
Recommended publications
  • Wildlife of the North Hills: Birds, Animals, Butterflies
    Wildlife of the North Hills: Birds, Animals, Butterflies Oakland, California 2005 About this Booklet The idea for this booklet grew out of a suggestion from Anne Seasons, President of the North Hills Phoenix Association, that I compile pictures of local birds in a form that could be made available to residents of the north hills. I expanded on that idea to include other local wildlife. For purposes of this booklet, the “North Hills” is defined as that area on the Berkeley/Oakland border bounded by Claremont Avenue on the north, Tunnel Road on the south, Grizzly Peak Blvd. on the east, and Domingo Avenue on the west. The species shown here are observed, heard or tracked with some regularity in this area. The lists are not a complete record of species found: more than 50 additional bird species have been observed here, smaller rodents were included without visual verification, and the compiler lacks the training to identify reptiles, bats or additional butterflies. We would like to include additional species: advice from local experts is welcome and will speed the process. A few of the species listed fall into the category of pests; but most - whether resident or visitor - are desirable additions to the neighborhood. We hope you will enjoy using this booklet to identify the wildlife you see around you. Kay Loughman November 2005 2 Contents Birds Turkey Vulture Bewick’s Wren Red-tailed Hawk Wrentit American Kestrel Ruby-crowned Kinglet California Quail American Robin Mourning Dove Hermit thrush Rock Pigeon Northern Mockingbird Band-tailed
    [Show full text]
  • California Towhee Responses to Chick Distress Calls
    The Condor 109:79–87 # The Cooper Ornithological Society 2007 OFFSPRING DISCRIMINATION WITHOUT RECOGNITION: CALIFORNIA TOWHEE RESPONSES TO CHICK DISTRESS CALLS LAURYN BENEDICT1 Museum of Vertebrate Zoology, 3101 Valley Life Sciences Building, University of California, Berkeley, CA 94720 Abstract. Accurate offspring discrimination improves parental fitness by ensuring appropriate parental investment. In colonial avian species, offspring discrimination is often mediated by recognition of individual offspring vocalizations, but spatially segregated species do not necessarily need sophisticated recognition abilities if parents can use alternative information to distinguish offspring from nonoffspring. I experimen- tally tested the hypothesis that territorial California Towhee (Pipilo crissalis) parents use a location-based decision rule, instead of true vocal recognition of offspring, when deciding whether to respond to chick distress calls. Accurate responses to offspring distress calls should be favored by natural selection because they can have large fitness benefits if parents succeed in chasing away potential nest predators. Responses to nonoffspring, in contrast, may be costly and should not be favored by natural selection. Towhee parents were presented with a series of three playback experiments in which I manipulated the identity of the vocalizing chick, the age of resident chicks, and the location of the distress call broadcast. Parents showed no evidence of individual vocal recognition and no pattern of differential response to distress calls when offspring age differed from that of the calling chick. Parents did, however, exhibit a significant tendency to approach distress calls originating near their offspring more often than distress calls originating elsewhere on their territory. These results provide support for the evolution of an offspring discrimination strategy based on a simple location-based decision rule instead of true vocal recognition.
    [Show full text]
  • A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS of SONG and RESPONSES to SONG PLAYBACK University of Colorado at Denver
    FINAL REPORT A COMPARATIVE ANALYSIS OF SONG AND RESPONSES TO SONG PLAYBACK IN THE AVIAN GENUS PIPILO Peter S. Kaplan Department of Psychology University of Colorado at Denver Abstract An experiment was undertaken to characterize the responses of Green-tailed Towhees (Pipilo chlorura) and Rufous-sided Towhees (P. erythophthalmus) to each others' songs and to the songs of five other towhee species, plus one hybrid form. A total of 12 Green-Tailed Towhees and 10 Rufous-sided Towhees from Boulder and Gilpin counties were studied at three field sites: the Doudy Draw Trail, the National Center for Atmospheric Research (NCAR), and on private land at the mouth of Coal Creek Canyon. In May, each bird was mist-netted and banded to facilitate individual identification. During the subsequent playback phase in June and July, each individual received one three-part playback trial on each of 7 consecutive or near-consecutive days. A 9-min playback trial consisted of a 3-min "pre-play" period, during which the bird was observed in the absence of song playback, a 3-min "play" period, in which tape recorded song was played to the subject from a central point in his territory, and a 3-min "post-play" period when the bird • was again observed in the absence of song playback. Order of presentation of song exemplars from different towhee species were randomized across birds. The main dependent measure was the change in the number of songs produced by the subject bird during song playback, relative to the pre-play period. Results showed that Green-tailed Towhees responded by significantly increasing their rate of singing, but only in response to Green-tailed Towhee songs.
    [Show full text]
  • 21 Sep 2018 Lists of Victims and Hosts of the Parasitic
    version: 21 Sep 2018 Lists of victims and hosts of the parasitic cowbirds (Molothrus). Peter E. Lowther, Field Museum Brood parasitism is an awkward term to describe an interaction between two species in which, as in predator-prey relationships, one species gains at the expense of the other. Brood parasites "prey" upon parental care. Victimized species usually have reduced breeding success, partly because of the additional cost of caring for alien eggs and young, and partly because of the behavior of brood parasites (both adults and young) which may directly and adversely affect the survival of the victim's own eggs or young. About 1% of all bird species, among 7 families, are brood parasites. The 5 species of brood parasitic “cowbirds” are currently all treated as members of the genus Molothrus. Host selection is an active process. Not all species co-occurring with brood parasites are equally likely to be selected nor are they of equal quality as hosts. Rather, to varying degrees, brood parasites are specialized for certain categories of hosts. Brood parasites may rely on a single host species to rear their young or may distribute their eggs among many species, seemingly without regard to any characteristics of potential hosts. Lists of species are not the best means to describe interactions between a brood parasitic species and its hosts. Such lists do not necessarily reflect the taxonomy used by the brood parasites themselves nor do they accurately reflect the complex interactions within bird communities (see Ortega 1998: 183-184). Host lists do, however, offer some insight into the process of host selection and do emphasize the wide variety of features than can impact on host selection.
    [Show full text]
  • Life History Account for California Towhee
    California Wildlife Habitat Relationships System California Department of Fish and Wildlife California Interagency Wildlife Task Group CALIFORNIA TOWHEE Melozone crissalis Family: EMBERIZIDAE Order: PASSERIFORMES Class: AVES B484 Written by: D. Dobkin, S. Granholm Reviewed by: L. Mewaldt Edited by: R. Duke Updated by: CWHR Program Staff, November 2014 DISTRIBUTION, ABUNDANCE, AND SEASONALITY The former brown towhee recently has been split into the California towhee and the canyon towhee, M. fusca (American Ornithologists' Union 1989). The California towhee is a common, characteristic resident of foothills and lowlands in most of cismontane California. Frequents open chaparral and coastal scrub, as well as brush-land patches in open riparian, hardwood hardwood-conifer, cropland, and urban habitats. Commonly uses edges of dense chaparral and brushy edges of densely wooded habitats. Also occurs in lowest montane habitats of similar structure in southern California, and locally in Siskiyou and western Modoc cos. Local on coastal slope north of southern Humboldt Co., and apparently absent from western San Joaquin Valley (Grinnell and Miller 1944, McCaskie et al. 1979, Garrett and Dunn 1981). The Inyo California towhee, M. c. eremophilus, occurs only in the Argus Mountains of southwestern Inyo Co. SPECIFlC HABITAT REQUIREMENTS Feeding: Feeds on seeds, insects, and some fruits. Gleans and scratches in litter, picks seeds and fruits from plants, and rarely flycatches (Davis 1957). Prefers to forage on open ground adjacent to brushy cover. Insects are important in breeding season, often constituting a third of the diet (Martin et al. 1961). Cover: Shrubs in broken chaparral, margins of dense chaparral, willow thickets, and brushy understory of open wooded habitats provide cover.
    [Show full text]
  • Common Birds of the Estero Bay Area
    Common Birds of the Estero Bay Area Jeremy Beaulieu Lisa Andreano Michael Walgren Introduction The following is a guide to the common birds of the Estero Bay Area. Brief descriptions are provided as well as active months and status listings. Photos are primarily courtesy of Greg Smith. Species are arranged by family according to the Sibley Guide to Birds (2000). Gaviidae Red-throated Loon Gavia stellata Occurrence: Common Active Months: November-April Federal Status: None State/Audubon Status: None Description: A small loon seldom seen far from salt water. In the non-breeding season they have a grey face and red throat. They have a long slender dark bill and white speckling on their dark back. Information: These birds are winter residents to the Central Coast. Wintering Red- throated Loons can gather in large numbers in Morro Bay if food is abundant. They are common on salt water of all depths but frequently forage in shallow bays and estuaries rather than far out at sea. Because their legs are located so far back, loons have difficulty walking on land and are rarely found far from water. Most loons must paddle furiously across the surface of the water before becoming airborne, but these small loons can practically spring directly into the air from land, a useful ability on its artic tundra breeding grounds. Pacific Loon Gavia pacifica Occurrence: Common Active Months: November-April Federal Status: None State/Audubon Status: None Description: The Pacific Loon has a shorter neck than the Red-throated Loon. The bill is very straight and the head is very smoothly rounded.
    [Show full text]
  • California Rare & Endagered Birds
    California brown pelican Pelecanus occidentalis californicus State Endangered 1971 Fully Protected Federal Endangered 1970 General Habitat: The California brown pelican uses a variety of natural and human-created sites, including offshore islands and rocks, sand spits, sand bars, jetties, and piers, for daytime loafing and nocturnal roosting. Preferred nesting sites provide protection from mammalian predators and sufficient elevation to prevent flooding of nests. The pelican builds a nest of sticks on the ground, typically on islands or offshore rocks. Their nesting range extends from West Anacapa Island and Santa Barbara Island in Channel Islands National Park to Islas Los Coronados, immediately south of and offshore from San Diego, and Isla San Martín in Baja California Norte, Mexico. Description: The brown pelican is one of two species of pelican in North America; the other is the white pelican. The California brown pelican is a large, grayish-brown bird with a long, pouched bill. The adult has a white head and dark body, but immature birds are dark with a white belly. The brown pelican weighs up to eight pounds and may have a wingspan of seven feet. Brown pelicans dive from flight to capture surface-schooling marine fishes. Status: The California brown pelican currently nests on West Anacapa Island and Santa Barbara Island in Channel Islands National Park. West Anacapa Island is the largest breeding population of California. In Mexico, the pelicans nest on Islas Los Coronados and Isla San Martín. Historically, the brown pelican colony on Islas Los Coronados was as large as, or larger than, that of recent years on Anacapa Island.
    [Show full text]
  • Voice in Communication and Relationships Among Brown Towhees
    THE CONDOR VOLUME 66 SEPTEMBER-OCTOBER, 1964 NUMBER 5 VOICE IN COMMUNICATION AND RELATIONSHIPS AMONG BROWN TOWHEES By JOET.MARSHALL,JR. This paper seeks to answer two questions: (1) What is the function of each song and call in brown towhees; that is, what information does a bird communicate to its fellows vocally, o’r how does it regulate their behavior by its voice? (2) What evi- dence does voice offer for understanding relationship by descent within the closely-knit group of brown towhee species? For the first, I would extend the analysis of Quain- tance (1938,194l) to all members of the group. As to the second question, an ingenious evolutionary reconstruction, based on museum and habitat studies, has been developed by Davis (1951). Do vocal attributes agree with his scheme? The three speciesof brown towhees, genus Pipdo, are the same size and general color and are more similar to each other than any one of them is to other ground-inhabiting finches in the same genus and in the genus Melozorte. Indeed, so close is their relation- ship that the same calls can easily be discerned in each species; although differing in timbre, similarity in form and usage proclaims them to be homologous. The Abert Towhee (Pipdo abed) occupies dense riparian woodland and mesquite thickets of the Colorado River and Gila River drainages, mostly in Arizona. The Brown Towhee proper (Pip20 fuscus) lives in brushy margins of openings in the southwestern United States and Mexico. The White-throated Towhee (Pipilo aZbicoZZis)inhabits brushy slopes, often with tree yuccas, in Puebla and Oaxaca, MCxico.
    [Show full text]
  • Biology 2 Lab Packet for Practical 4 Birds
    Bio 2 – Lab Practicum 4 1 Biology 2 Lab Packet For Practical 4 Birds Bio 2 – Lab Practicum 4 2 CLASSIFICATION: Domain: Eukarya Kingdom: Animalia Phylum: Chordata – Chordates Class: Aves – Birds Order: Struthioniformes - Ostriches Order: Galliformes - Quail Order: Rheiformes – Rheas Order: Gruiformes – Coots Order: Casuariiformes – Cassowaries Order: Charadriiformes – Gulls and Allies Order: Apterygiformes – Kiwis Order: Columbiformes – Pigeons Order: Sphenisciformes - Penguins Order: Psittaciformes – Parrots Order: Gaviiformes - Loons Order: Cuculiformes – Roadrunners Order: Podicipediformes – Grebes Order: Strigiformes - Owls Order: Procellariiformes – Tube noses Order: Caprimulgiformes – Nighthawks Order: Pelicaniformes – Pelicans Order: Apodiformes – Hummingbirds Order: Ciconiiformes – Herons/Egrets Order: Trogonifomes – Trogons Order: Phoenicopteriformes - Flamingos Order: Coraciformes – Kingfishers Order: Anseriformes – Ducks Order: Piciformes – Woodpeckers Order: Falconiformes – Raptors Order: Passeriformes - Songbirds Introduction – Birds Although chordates vary widely in appearance, they are distinguished as a phylum by the presence of four anatomical features that appear sometime during their life time. They exhibit deuterostome development and bilateral symmetry. Chordates only comprise 5% of the animal species but may be the most commonly known phylum. Birds are endothermic homeotherms which have adapted to many different ecosystems in the world. Station 1 – Class: Aves 1. What three adaptations do birds have for flight? 2. What do all species of birds have? 3. What dinosaurs did birds emerge within? When did they show up? 4. Where are birds found? Bio 2 – Lab Practicum 4 3 Station 2 – Evolutionary History - Archaeopteryx 1. What characteristics are seen in Archaeopteryx that are bird-like? 2. What characteristics are seen in Archaeopteryx that are reptile-like? Station 3 – General Characteristics - Feathers 1. What are feathers made of? 2.
    [Show full text]
  • Passerellidae Species Tree
    Passerellidae I: Spizellini, Ammodramini & Chlorospingini Lark Sparrow, Chondestes grammacus Lark Bunting, Calamospiza melanocorys Black-throated Sparrow, Amphispiza bilineata Five-striped Sparrow, Amphispiza quinquestriata SPIZELLINI Chipping Sparrow, Spizella passerina Clay-colored Sparrow, Spizella pallida Black-chinned Sparrow, Spizella atrogularis Field Sparrow, Spizella pusilla Brewer’s Sparrow, Spizella breweri Worthen’s Sparrow, Spizella wortheni Tumbes Sparrow, Rhynchospiza stolzmanni Stripe-capped Sparrow, Rhynchospiza strigiceps Grasshopper Sparrow, Ammodramus savannarum Grassland Sparrow, Ammodramus humeralis Yellow-browed Sparrow, Ammodramus aurifrons AMMODRAMINI Olive Sparrow, Arremonops rufivirgatus Green-backed Sparrow, Arremonops chloronotus Black-striped Sparrow, Arremonops conirostris Tocuyo Sparrow, Arremonops tocuyensis Rufous-winged Sparrow, Peucaea carpalis Cinnamon-tailed Sparrow, Peucaea sumichrasti Botteri’s Sparrow, Peucaea botterii Cassin’s Sparrow, Peucaea cassinii Bachman’s Sparrow, Peucaea aestivalis Stripe-headed Sparrow, Peucaea ruficauda Black-chested Sparrow, Peucaea humeralis Bridled Sparrow, Peucaea mystacalis Tanager Finch, Oreothraupis arremonops Short-billed (Yellow-whiskered) Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus parvirostris CHLOROSPINGINI Yellow-throated Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus flavigularis Ashy-throated Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus canigularis Sooty-capped Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus pileatus Wetmore’s Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus wetmorei White-fronted Chlorospingus, Chlorospingus albifrons Brown-headed
    [Show full text]
  • Behavioural Processes Can a Solitary Avian
    Behavioural Processes 82 (2009) 67–74 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Behavioural Processes journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/behavproc Can a solitary avian species use collective detection? An assay in semi-natural conditions Esteban Fernández-Juricic a,∗, Juan Antonio Delgado b, Carolina Remacha b, María Dolores Jiménez b, Vanessa Garcia c, Keiko Hori c a Department of Biological Sciences, Purdue University, 915 W. State Street, West Lafayette, IN 47907, USA b Departamento de Ecología, Facultad de Biología, Universidad Complutense de Madrid, C/ José Antonio Novais, 2, 28040 Madrid, Spain c Department of Biological Sciences, California State University Long Beach, 1250 Bellflower Blvd. (Mailstop 3702), Long Beach, CA 90840-3702, USA article info abstract Article history: Collective detection (e.g., enhanced predator detection through the vigilance of conspecifics) is expected Received 15 January 2009 to have evolved particularly in social species. However, we assessed the degree to which an avian territorial Received in revised form 13 May 2009 species (California towhee Pipilo crissalis) would use social cues about predation in a semi-natural assay. Accepted 17 May 2009 We also exposed a social species (house finch Carpodacus mexicanus) to similar conditions. California towhees increased scanning rates when foraging with conspecifics, whereas house finches increased Keywords: scanning rates when foraging solitarily, suggesting that vigilance in these species is regulated mostly Anti-predator behavior through interference competition and through predation risk, respectively. California towhees did not Collective detection Early detection show early detection, and actually the last detector in the group delayed detection in relation to solitary Predator detection individuals. House finches benefited from early detection, but the second and last detectors maintained Territorial species detection at the level of solitary individuals.
    [Show full text]
  • Avian Survey Report
    Spring/Summer 2010 Avian Survey Report Stony Creek Wind Farm Wyoming County, New York January 24, 2011 PREPARED FOR: Stony Creek Energy LLC 51 Monroe St. Suite 1604 Rockville, MD 20850 PREPARED BY: Lackawanna Executive Park 239 Main Street, Suite 301 Dickson City, PA 18519 www.shoenerenvironmental.com Stony Creek Wind Farm Avian Survey January 24, 2011 Table of Contents I. Summary and Background .................................................................................................1 Summary .......................................................................................................................1 Project Description ........................................................................................................1 Project Review Background ..........................................................................................2 II. Bald Eagle Survey .............................................................................................................3 Bald Eagle Breeding Status in New York ......................................................................3 Daily Movements of Bald Eagle in New York ...............................................................4 Bald Eagle Conservation Status in New York ................................................................4 Bald Eagle Survey Method ............................................................................................5 Analysis of Bald Eagle Survey Data ..............................................................................6
    [Show full text]