The Lamprey Pallium Provides a Blueprint of the Mammalian Motor Projections from Cortex

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Lamprey Pallium Provides a Blueprint of the Mammalian Motor Projections from Cortex Article The Lamprey Pallium Provides a Blueprint of the Mammalian Motor Projections from Cortex Graphical Abstract Authors Francisco M. Ocan˜ a, Shreyas M. Suryanarayana, ..., Brita Robertson, Sten Grillner Correspondence [email protected] In Brief Ocan˜ a et al. show that the mammalian organization with a motor cortex from which different classes of movements can be elicited was present already at the dawn of vertebrate evolution, 560 million years ago, when the lamprey line of evolution diverged from that leading to mammals. Detailed connectivity and synaptic properties are also conserved. Highlights d Pallial/cortical motor projections evolved at the dawn of vertebrate evolution d Stimulation of pallial areas in lamprey elicits eye, trunk, or oral movements d Pallial projection neurons target midbrain, reticulospinal areas, and basal ganglia d Pallial stimulation evokes monosynaptic EPSPs in tectal and reticulospinal neurons Ocan˜ a et al., 2015, Current Biology 25, 413–423 February 16, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.013 Current Biology 25, 413–423, February 16, 2015 ª2015 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.cub.2014.12.013 Article The Lamprey Pallium Provides a Blueprint of the Mammalian Motor Projections from Cortex Francisco M. Ocan˜ a,1,2,3 Shreyas M. Suryanarayana,1,3 and habenulae, have recently been shown to be conserved Kazuya Saitoh,1 Andreas A. Kardamakis,1 in great detail [9–12]. Lorenza Capantini,1 Brita Robertson,1,4 and Sten Grillner1,4,* In addition to mammals, birds (corvids) are also considered 1The Nobel Institute for Neurophysiology, Department of to have an advanced cognitive ability [13]. Nevertheless, the Neuroscience, Karolinska Institutet, 171 77 Stockholm, bird pallium consists mainly of nuclear masses except for a Sweden small laminar structure, the wulst [14, 15]. Part of the avian 2Laboratory of Psychobiology, University of Sevilla, Campus forebrain can be regarded as comparable to the mammalian Santiago Ramo´ n y Cajal, 41018 Sevilla, Spain prefrontal cortex (see [16, 17]). Furthermore, layer-specific molecular markers for layer 4 and 5 cell types in mammals also stain cells in the pallium of birds and reptiles, suggesting Summary a common reptilian origin [18]. There is limited information concerning the pallium of fish, but efferent projections to the Background: The frontal lobe control of movement in mam- optic tectum and the mesencephalic tegmentum have been re- mals has been thought to be a specific function primarily ported [19, 20]. related to the layered neocortex with its efferent connections. In the present study, we examine whether the pallium in lam- In contrast, we now show that the same basic organization is prey is the evolutionary ‘‘ancestor’’ of the mammalian cortex present even in one of the phylogenetically oldest vertebrates, that directly controls the motor output repertoire. Stimulation the lamprey. of defined pallial sites elicits specific motor patterns including Results: Stimulation of specific sites in the pallium/cortex eye and orientation of the body mediated by glutamatergic evokes eye, trunk, locomotor, or oral movements. The pallial projections to downstream motor centers. Another core projection neurons target brainstem motor centers and basal aspect is the cortical/pallial control of the basal ganglia, via ganglia subnuclei and have prominent dendrites extending projections to the striatum and subthalamic nucleus. This in- into the outer molecular layer. They exhibit the characteristic cludes the two types of input from cortex/pallium—the collat- features of pyramidal neurons and elicit monosynaptic gluta- erals of the brainstem projecting neurons and the intratelence- matergic excitatory postsynaptic potentials in output neurons phalic projections (e.g., [21]). Thus, in contrast to what has of the optic tectum, reticulospinal neurons, and, as shown been assumed, the basic projection pattern of the vertebrate earlier, basal ganglia neurons. motor pallium/cortex had already evolved in the most ancient Conclusions: Our results demonstrate marked similarities in group of vertebrates, the lamprey, 560 million years ago [6], the efferent functional connectivity and control of motor and has been maintained throughout vertebrate evolution. behavior between the lamprey pallium and mammalian neocortex. Thus, the lamprey motor pallium/cortex represents Results an evolutionary blueprint of the corresponding mammalian system. Identification of a Pallial Motor Area from which Eye, Orienting, Locomotor, and Mouth Movements Can Be Elicited Introduction The pallium is the forebrain structure in lamprey, which corre- sponds to the cerebral cortex in mammals, in particular its The mammalian frontal lobe contains areas from which move- lateral part (LPal; see Figure 1A). We have explored the possi- ments of the eyes and different parts of the body can be eli- bility of whether movements could be elicited from this struc- cited [1–3]. These motor areas project to basal ganglia subnu- ture by microstimulation of its different parts at the dorsal sur- clei as well as to different centers in the midbrain, brainstem, face and at different depths (n = 22; Figure 1; Figure S1). We and spinal cord that coordinate the movements (e.g., [4, 5]). were able to show that stimulation of partially overlapping The layered neocortex of the frontal lobes has been thought areas in preferentially the middle to caudal part of the LPal to convey unique properties regarding the control of move- could elicit eye, orienting, swimming, or mouth movements ment and to be specific to mammals and different from that (Figure 1). The areas from which movements were elicited of other classes of vertebrates. Mammals and cyclostomes are indicated in Figures 1B–1E, with the lowest threshold in (lamprey) represent two extreme groups phylogenetically. By red (10–40 mA). Either one type of movement in isolation or a exploring the pallial (cortical) organization in one of the oldest combination of movements was produced, for instance eye groups of vertebrates [6], we show that the lamprey pallium/ and orienting movements and sometimes swimming or mouth cortex had already evolved a similar organization with respect movements. The depth distribution of low threshold activation to movement control early in vertebrate evolution. The role of is represented in Figures S1F–S1H, and the movements were the lamprey pallium has been practically unknown, except evoked mainly in the deeper parts of LPal often close to the for the projections to the striatum and their synaptic properties ventricle, where many of the projection neurons to the brain- [7–10]. Other forebrain structures, such as the basal ganglia stem are located (see Figure 5 below). Recordings of eye movements were performed with coils 3Co-first author mounted around the eye to detect the movements electromag- 4Co-senior author netically (Figures 2A and 2B; also see the Supplemental Exper- *Correspondence: [email protected] imental Procedures). Figure 2C shows that the eye movement 414 Figure 1. Threshold Maps for Different Types of ABC Movements (A) Dorsal schematic view of a lamprey brain showing the reconstructed distribution of electri- cally evoked motor responses. Data from 22 ani- mals were superimposed. (B–E) Color-coded contour plots representing the threshold current needed to evoke different motor patterns. Note that the excitability of the LPal decreases from the caudal to the rostral pole. DE The scale bars represent 500 mm. Hb, habenula; LPal, lateral pallium; MPal, medial pallium; OB, ol- factory bulb; OT, optic tectum; Pi, pineal gland; Rh, rhombencephalon. See also Figure S1. labeled fibers/terminals were present in pretectum, tectum, midbrain teg- mentum, and brainstem regions that in mammals receive direct descending amplitude increases with graded stimulation between 20 and projections from the motor cortex. These projections are 60 mA (pulse train of 30 Hz). With increasing stimulus intensity described in detail below. the latency decreases whereas the amplitude increases (Fig- Palliotectal Projections Are Glutamatergic and ures 2D–2F). Similarly, an increased stimulation frequency Monosynaptic from 10 to 60 Hz (Figures 2G–2J) will lead to a larger amplitude In the pretectum, a large bundle of efferent fibers (Neurobiotin movement (from 10 to 30 Hz) and a decrease of the latency. in red in Figure 3B) was observed on the dorsal aspect forming Finally, 95% of the evoked movements were contraversive to the palliopretectal/tectal pathway, with a few of these fibers the stimulated LPal (ipsilateral eye forward and contralateral crossing to the contralateral side. Arborizations were formed eye backward). This movement can be defined as an orienting around many of the cells in the dorsal pretectum (Figure 3B). movement, and changing stimulation parameters did not alter Further caudally in the optic tectum, efferent pallial fibers the direction of eye movements. entered dorsomedially to form terminal arborizations in the The orienting movements of the body were recorded with a deep motor output layer (Figure 3C), where tectal cells that video camera, and the angle between the fixed head and the project to the different brainstem regions are located (A.A. Kar- neck area was measured (Figures 2K and 2L; Figure S2). The damakis et al., 2013, Soc. Neurosci., abstract). The direct pro- amplitude of the movements increased with an increase in jection from the lamprey pallial region to tectum is comparable stimulation intensity from 25 up to 50 mA in a linear fashion (Fig- to the projections from the mammalian frontal eye field and the ure 2L). Such a movement would normally result in an orienting avian pallial gaze center to the collicular and tectal deep layers, movement of the head. If it instead would occur during respectively [22, 23]. swimming, it would lead to a turning movement in the same di- To examine the synaptic input from the motor pallium to the rection. Swimming movements were elicited less often (Fig- deep layer of tectum, whole-cell patch-clamp recordings were ure 1D), as were movements of the mouth (Figure 1E).
Recommended publications
  • Fish Do Not Feel Pain and Its Implications for Understanding Phenomenal Consciousness
    Biol Philos (2015) 30:149–165 DOI 10.1007/s10539-014-9469-4 Fish do not feel pain and its implications for understanding phenomenal consciousness Brian Key Received: 14 April 2014 / Accepted: 6 December 2014 / Published online: 16 December 2014 Ó The Author(s) 2014. This article is published with open access at Springerlink.com Abstract Phenomenal consciousness or the subjective experience of feeling sen- sory stimuli is fundamental to human existence. Because of the ubiquity of their subjective experiences, humans seem to readily accept the anthropomorphic extension of these mental states to other animals. Humans will typically extrapolate feelings of pain to animals if they respond physiologically and behaviourally to noxious stimuli. The alternative view that fish instead respond to noxious stimuli reflexly and with a limited behavioural repertoire is defended within the context of our current understanding of the neuroanatomy and neurophysiology of mental states. Consequently, a set of fundamental properties of neural tissue necessary for feeling pain or experiencing affective states in vertebrates is proposed. While mammals and birds possess the prerequisite neural architecture for phenomenal consciousness, it is concluded that fish lack these essential characteristics and hence do not feel pain. Keywords Fish Á Pain Á Phenomenal consciousness Á Affective states Á Avoidance learning Á Neocortex Á Pallium Introduction There is a belief in some scientific and lay communities that because fish respond behaviourally to noxious stimuli, then ipso facto, fish feel pain. Sneddon (2011) clearly articulates the logic by stating: ‘‘to explore the possibility of pain perception in nonhumans we use indirect measures similar to those used for human infants who cannot convey whether they are in pain.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotional and Spatial Learning in Goldfish Is Dependent on Different
    European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 21, pp. 2800–2806, 2005 ª Federation of European Neuroscience Societies Emotional and spatial learning in goldfish is dependent on different telencephalic pallial systems Manuel Portavella1,* and Juan P. Vargas2 1Departamento de Psicologı´a Experimental. Universidad de Sevilla. C ⁄ Camilo Jose´ Cela s ⁄ n, E-41018, Seville, Spain 2SISSA. Cognitive Neuroscience Sector. Via Beirut, 2 ⁄ 4, 34014 Trieste, Italy Keywords: amygdala, avoidance learning, brain evolution, hippocampus, memory systems Abstract In mammals, the amygdala and the hippocampus are involved in different aspects of learning. Whereas the amygdala complex is involved in emotional learning, the hippocampus plays a critical role in spatial and contextual learning. In fish, it has been suggested that the medial and lateral region of the telencephalic pallia might be the homologous neural structure to the mammalian amygdala and hippocampus, respectively. Although there is evidence of the implication of medial and lateral pallium in several learning processes, it remains unclear whether both pallial areas are involved distinctively in different learning processes. To address this issue, we examined the effect of selective ablation of the medial and lateral pallium on both two-way avoidance and reversal spatial learning in goldfish. The results showed that medial pallium lesions selectively impaired the two-way avoidance task. In contrast, lateral pallium ablations impaired the spatial task without affecting the avoidance performance. These results indicate that the medial and lateral pallia in fish are functionally different and necessary for emotional and spatial learning, respectively. Present data could support the hypothesis that a sketch of these regions of the limbic system, and their associated functions, were present in the common ancestor of fish and terrestrial vertebrates 400 million years ago.
    [Show full text]
  • The Structural Model: a Theory Linking Connections, Plasticity, Pathology, Development and Evolution of the Cerebral Cortex
    Brain Structure and Function https://doi.org/10.1007/s00429-019-01841-9 REVIEW The Structural Model: a theory linking connections, plasticity, pathology, development and evolution of the cerebral cortex Miguel Ángel García‑Cabezas1 · Basilis Zikopoulos2,3 · Helen Barbas1,3 Received: 11 October 2018 / Accepted: 29 January 2019 © Springer-Verlag GmbH Germany, part of Springer Nature 2019 Abstract The classical theory of cortical systematic variation has been independently described in reptiles, monotremes, marsupials and placental mammals, including primates, suggesting a common bauplan in the evolution of the cortex. The Structural Model is based on the systematic variation of the cortex and is a platform for advancing testable hypotheses about cortical organization and function across species, including humans. The Structural Model captures the overall laminar structure of areas by dividing the cortical architectonic continuum into discrete categories (cortical types), which can be used to test hypotheses about cortical organization. By type, the phylogenetically ancient limbic cortices—which form a ring at the base of the cerebral hemisphere—are agranular if they lack layer IV, or dysgranular if they have an incipient granular layer IV. Beyond the dysgranular areas, eulaminate type cortices have six layers. The number and laminar elaboration of eulaminate areas differ depending on species or cortical system within a species. The construct of cortical type retains the topology of the systematic variation of the cortex and forms the basis for a predictive Structural Model, which has successfully linked cortical variation to the laminar pattern and strength of cortical connections, the continuum of plasticity and stability of areas, the regularities in the distribution of classical and novel markers, and the preferential vulnerability of limbic areas to neurodegenerative and psychiatric diseases.
    [Show full text]
  • Perspectives
    PERSPECTIVES reptiles, to birds and mammals, to primates OPINION and, finally, to humans — ascending from ‘lower’ to ‘higher’ intelligence in a chrono- logical series. They believed that the brains Avian brains and a new understanding of extant vertebrates retained ancestral structures, and, therefore, that the origin of of vertebrate brain evolution specific human brain subdivisions could be traced back in time by examining the brains of extant non-human vertebrates. In The Avian Brain Nomenclature Consortium* making such comparisons, they noted that the main divisions of the human CNS — Abstract | We believe that names have a pallium is nuclear, and the mammalian the spinal cord, hindbrain, midbrain, thala- powerful influence on the experiments we cortex is laminar in organization, the avian mus, cerebellum and cerebrum or telen- do and the way in which we think. For this pallium supports cognitive abilities similar cephalon — were present in all vertebrates reason, and in the light of new evidence to, and for some species more advanced than, (FIG. 1a). Edinger, however, noted that the about the function and evolution of the those of many mammals. To eliminate these internal organization of the telencephala vertebrate brain, an international consortium misconceptions, an international forum of showed the most pronounced differences of neuroscientists has reconsidered the neuroscientists (BOX 1) has, for the first time between species. In mammals, the outer traditional, 100-year-old terminology that is in 100 years, developed new terminology that part of the telencephalon was found to have used to describe the avian cerebrum. Our more accurately reflects our current under- prominently layered grey matter (FIG.
    [Show full text]
  • Foxg1confines Cajal–Retzius Neuronogenesis and Hippocampal
    The Journal of Neuroscience, April 27, 2005 • 25(17):4435–4441 • 4435 Development/Plasticity/Repair Foxg1 Confines Cajal–Retzius Neuronogenesis and Hippocampal Morphogenesis to the Dorsomedial Pallium Luca Muzio and Antonello Mallamaci Department of Biological and Technological Research, San Raffaele Scientific Institute, 20132 Milan, Italy It has been suggested that cerebral cortex arealization relies on positional values imparted to early cortical neuroblasts by transcription factor genes expressed within the pallial field in graded ways. Foxg1, encoding for one of these factors, previously was reported to be necessary for basal ganglia morphogenesis, proper tuning of cortical neuronal differentiation rates, and the switching of cortical neuro- blasts from early generation of primordial plexiform layer to late production of cortical plate. Being expressed along a rostral/lateral high- to-caudal/medial low gradient, Foxg1, moreover, could contribute to shaping the cortical areal profile as a repressor of caudomedial fates. Wetestedthispredictionbyavarietyofapproachesandfoundthatitwascorrect.WefoundthatoverproductionofCajal–Retziusneurons characterizing Foxg1Ϫ/Ϫ mutants does not arise specifically from blockage of laminar histogenetic progression of neocortical neuro- blasts, as reported previously, but rather reflects lateral-to-medial repatterning of their cortical primordium. Even if lacking a neocortical plate, Foxg1Ϫ/Ϫ embryos give rise to structures, which, for molecular properties and birthdating profile, are highly reminiscent of hippocampal plate and dentate blade. Remarkably, in the absence of Foxg1, additional inactivation of the medial fates promoter Emx2, although not suppressing cortical specification, conversely rescues overproduction of Reelin on neurons. Key words: Foxg1; Emx2; Wnt types; hippocampus; neocortex; Cajal–Retzius cells Introduction neuronogenesis (Xuan et al., 1995; Dou et al., 1999; Seoane et al., Areal specification of cortical neurons is an extremely complex 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • Broom Fish Brains Pain
    Pre-publication copy Broom, D.M. 2016. Fish brains and behaviour indicate capacity for feeling pain. Animal Sentience, 2016.010 (5 pages). Fish brains, as well as fish behaviour, indicate capacity for awareness and feeling pain Donald M. Broom Centre for Anthrozoology and Animal Welfare Department of Veterinary Medicine University of Cambridge Madingley Road Cambridge CB3 0ES U.K. [email protected] http://www.neuroscience.cam.ac.uk/directory/profile.php?dmb16 Keywords pain sentience welfare fish feelings emotions brain behaviour Abstract Studies of behaviour are of major importance in understanding human pain and pain in other animals such as fish. Almost all of the characteristics of the mammalian pain system are also described for fish. Emotions, feelings and learning from these are controlled in the fish brain in areas anatomically different but functionally very similar to those in mammals. The evidence of pain and fear system function in fish is so similar to that in humans and other mammals that it is logical to conclude that fish feel fear and pain. Fish are sentient beings. Key (2015) is scornful about evidence from studies of fish behaviour indicating that fish are aware and feel pain but presents a thorough explanation of the pain system in the human brain and concludes that fish could not feel pain, or have any other feelings, as they do not have the brain structures that allow pain and other feelings in humans. Section 2 of his paper emphasises “the cortical origins of human pain” and states that “structure determines function”, eXplaining the functions of the five layers of the human cortex.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolutionary Development of the Brain As It Pertains to Neurosurgery
    Open Access Original Article DOI: 10.7759/cureus.6748 The Evolutionary Development of the Brain As It Pertains to Neurosurgery Jaafar Basma 1 , Natalie Guley 2 , L. Madison Michael II 3 , Kenan Arnautovic 3 , Frederick Boop 3 , Jeff Sorenson 3 1. Neurological Surgery, University of Tennessee Health Science Center, Memphis, USA 2. Neurological Surgery, University of Arkansas for Medical Sciences, Little Rock, USA 3. Neurological Surgery, Semmes-Murphey Clinic, Memphis, USA Corresponding author: Jaafar Basma, [email protected] Abstract Background Neuroanatomists have long been fascinated by the complex topographic organization of the cerebrum. We examined historical and modern phylogenetic theories pertaining to microneurosurgical anatomy and intrinsic brain tumor development. Methods Literature and history related to the study of anatomy, evolution, and tumor predilection of the limbic and paralimbic regions were reviewed. We used vertebrate histological cross-sections, photographs from Albert Rhoton Jr.’s dissections, and original drawings to demonstrate the utility of evolutionary temporal causality in understanding anatomy. Results Phylogenetic neuroanatomy progressed from the substantial works of Alcmaeon, Herophilus, Galen, Vesalius, von Baer, Darwin, Felsenstein, Klingler, MacLean, and many others. We identified two major modern evolutionary theories: “triune brain” and topological phylogenetics. While the concept of “triune brain” is speculative and highly debated, it remains the most popular in the current neurosurgical literature. Phylogenetics inspired by mathematical topology utilizes computational, statistical, and embryological data to analyze the temporal transformations leading to three-dimensional topographic anatomy. These transformations have shaped well-defined surgical planes, which can be exploited by the neurosurgeon to access deep cerebral targets. The microsurgical anatomy of the cerebrum and the limbic system is redescribed by incorporating the dimension of temporal causality.
    [Show full text]
  • Sequential Pattern of Sublayer Formation in the Paleocortex and Neocortex
    Medical Molecular Morphology (2020) 53:168–176 https://doi.org/10.1007/s00795-020-00245-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Sequential pattern of sublayer formation in the paleocortex and neocortex Makoto Nasu1 · Kenji Shimamura2 · Shigeyuki Esumi1 · Nobuaki Tamamaki1 Received: 17 December 2019 / Accepted: 13 January 2020 / Published online: 30 January 2020 © The Japanese Society for Clinical Molecular Morphology 2020 Abstract The piriform cortex (paleocortex) is the olfactory cortex or the primary cortex for the sense of smell. It receives the olfac- tory input from the mitral and tufted cells of the olfactory bulb and is involved in the processing of information pertaining to odors. The piriform cortex and the adjoining neocortex have diferent cytoarchitectures; while the former has a three-layered structure, the latter has a six-layered structure. The regulatory mechanisms underlying the building of the six-layered neo- cortex are well established; in contrast, less is known about of the regulatory mechanisms responsible for structure formation of the piriform cortex. The diferences as well as similarities in the regulatory mechanisms between the neocortex and the piriform cortex remain unclear. Here, the expression of neocortical layer-specifc genes in the piriform cortex was examined. Two sublayers were found to be distinguished in layer II of the piriform cortex using Ctip2/Bcl11b and Brn1/Pou3f3. The sequential expression pattern of Ctip2 and Brn1 in the piriform cortex was similar to that detected in the neocortex, although the laminar arrangement in the piriform cortex exhibited an outside-in arrangement, unlike that observed in the neocortex. Keywords Piriform cortex (paleocortex) · Sublayer · Sequential expression · Ctip2/Bcl11b · Brn1/Pou3f3 Introduction six-layered cortex, which is a mammal-specifc feature, are well established; sequential expression of transcription fac- The piriform cortex (paleocortex) is the olfactory cortex or tors is involved in determining cell identities, and late-born the primary cortex for the sense of smell.
    [Show full text]
  • Fish and Pain Perception
    An HSUS Report: Fish and Pain Perception Stephanie Yue, Ph.D. * Abstract In several arenas—legislative, academic, corporate, advocacy, and scientific—the welfare of fish has increasingly attracted attention due in part to the expansion of the aquaculture industry, as well as the growing understanding that many handling methods, management systems, and slaughter practices can induce pain and therefore reduce animal welfare. Unlike other animals raised for human consumption, however, general consensus has not always afforded fish the presupposition that they are, in fact, capable of feeling pain. The typical arguments in support of or against attributing pain capacity to fish revolve around their neuroanatomical development, behavioral and cognitive complexity, physiology, and anatomy. After reviewing the current scientific evidence and exploring the many arguments, it is irrefutably substantiated that fish are capable of experiencing pain. Introduction Aquaculture, as defined by the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration (NOAA) of the U.S. Department of Commerce, is “the propagation and rearing of aquatic organisms in controlled or selected environments for any commercial, recreational or public purpose.”1 Described as the fastest-growing food production sector in the world, aquaculture’s growth is expected to continue.2 Indeed, simply to satisfy current worldwide fish consumption, the Food and Agriculture Organization of the United Nations predicted in 2006 that worldwide aquaculture production must nearly double in the next 25 years. 3 In the last two decades, the aquaculture industry † has expanded approximately 8% per year, and it is expected that the number of farmed fish will continue to rise, 4 perhaps surpassing the number of wild-caught animals from the world’s fisheries.
    [Show full text]
  • Satb2 Is Required for the Regionalization of Retrosplenial Cortex
    Cell Death & Differentiation (2020) 27:1604–1617 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41418-019-0443-1 ARTICLE Satb2 is required for the regionalization of retrosplenial cortex 1 2 1,2 1 1 3,4 1 Lei Zhang ● Ning-Ning Song ● Qiong Zhang ● Wan-Ying Mei ● Chun-Hui He ● Pengcheng Ma ● Ying Huang ● 1 3,4 1,5 1,2,6 Jia-Yin Chen ● Bingyu Mao ● Bing Lang ● Yu-Qiang Ding Received: 23 March 2019 / Revised: 10 October 2019 / Accepted: 11 October 2019 / Published online: 30 October 2019 © The Author(s) 2019. This article is published with open access Abstract The retrosplenial cortex (Rsp) is a transitional cortex located between the neocortex and archicortex, but the molecular mechanism specifying Rsp from the archicortex remains elusive. We here report that the transcription factor Satb2 is required for specifying Rsp identity during its morphogenesis. In Satb2 CKO mice, the boundary between the Rsp and archicortex [i.e., subiculum (SubC)] disappears as early as E17.5, and Rsp efferent projection is aberrant. Rsp-specific genes are lost, whereas SubC-specific genes are ectopically expressed in Rsp of Satb2 CKO mice. Furthermore, cell-autonomous role of Satb2 in maintaining Rsp neuron identity is revealed by inactivation of Satb2 in Rsp neurons. Finally, Satb2 represses the transcription of Nr4a2. The misexpression of Nr4a2 together with Ctip2 induces expression of SubC-specific genes in wild-type Rsp, and simultaneous knockdown of these two genes in Rsp Satb2-mutant cells prevents their fate transition to SubC identity. Thus, 1234567890();,: 1234567890();,: Satb2 serves as a determinant gene in the Rsp regionalization by repressing Nr4a2 and Ctip2 during cortical development.
    [Show full text]
  • Can Fish Really Feel Pain?
    F I S H and F I S H E R I E S , 2014, 15, 97–133 Can fish really feel pain? J D Rose1, R Arlinghaus2,3, S J Cooke4*, B K Diggles5, W Sawynok6, E D Stevens7 & C D L Wynne8 1Department of Zoology and Physiology and Neuroscience Program, University of Wyoming, Department 3166, 1000 East University Avenue, Laramie, WY 80521, USA; 2Department of Biology and Ecology of Fishes, Leibniz-Institute of Freshwater Ecology and Inland Fisheries, Mu¨ggelseedamm 310, 12587, Berlin, Germany; 3Inland Fisheries Management Laboratory, Department for Crop and Animal Sciences, Faculty of Agriculture and Horticulture, Humboldt-Universitat€ zu Berlin, Berlin, Germany; 4Fish Ecology and Conservation Physiology Laboratory, Department of Biology and Institute of Environmental Science, Carleton University, 1125 Colonel By Drive, Ottawa, ON, Canada K1S 5B6; 5DigsFish Services, 32 Bowsprit Cres, Banksia Beach, QLD 4507, Australia; 6Infofish Australia, PO Box 9793, Frenchville, Qld 4701, Australia; 7Biomedical Sciences – Atlantic Veterinary College, University of Prince Edward Island, Charlottetown, PE, Canada, C1A 4P3; 8Department of Psychology, University of Florida, Box 112250, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA Abstract Correspondence: We review studies claiming that fish feel pain and find deficiencies in the methods Steven J Cooke, Fish Ecology and Conser- used for pain identification, particularly for distinguishing unconscious detection of vation Physiology injurious stimuli (nociception) from conscious pain. Results were also frequently mis- Laboratory, Depart- interpreted and not replicable, so claims that fish feel pain remain unsubstantiated. ment of Biology and Comparable problems exist in studies of invertebrates. In contrast, an extensive litera- Institute of Environ- ture involving surgeries with fishes shows normal feeding and activity immediately mental Science, Carleton University, or soon after surgery.
    [Show full text]
  • Olfactory Bulb Projections in the Bullfrog Rana Catesbeiana
    Olfactory Bulb Projections in the Bullfrog Rana catesbeiana R GLENN NORTHCUTT AND G. JAMES ROYCE Department of Zoology, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 481 04, and Department of Anatomy, Albany Medical College, Albany, New York 12208 ABSTRACT The projections of the accessory and main olfactory bulbs of the bullfrog are described as part of a long term analysis of the morphological differences in amphibian and reptilian telencephalons. Unilateral aspiration of the accessory olfactory bulb results in an ipsilateral projection to the pars lateralis of the amygdala via the accessory olfactory tract. Degenerating fibers from the accessory olfactory bulb are tracable into the cell-free zone between the dorsal striatum and the lateral pallium, and projections to these neural populations may also exist. Unilateral lesions of the main olfactory bulb reveal two major secondary pathways: an ipsilateral medial olfactory tract that pro- jects to the rostral ventromedial portion of the medial pallium, the postolfactory eminence and the rostral, lateral and medial septal nuclei; and an ipsilateral lateral olfactory tract that projects to the dorsal striatum, the lateral pallium and the ventral half of the dorsal pallium. Two crossed secondary olfactory path- ways to the contralateral telencephalon decussate via the habenular commis- sure after entering the ipsilateral stria medullaris. A crossed lateral pathway terminates in the dorsal striatum, the caudal, lateral pallium and the ventral portion of the dorsal pallium. A crossed medial pathway terminates in the internal granule layer of the main olfactory bulb. The transition from amphibians to rep- structures and their interconnections. As tiles marks the beginning of one of the one step in a new analysis we here report major adaptive radiations in vertebrate certain new observations on the normal phylogeny.
    [Show full text]