How Do Brief Social Interactions Change Over Time in Pair-Bonded Zebra

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

How Do Brief Social Interactions Change Over Time in Pair-Bonded Zebra bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.18.160051; this version posted June 20, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 1 Monogamy in a moment: how do brief social interactions change over time in pair-bonded zebra 2 finches (Taeniopygia guttata)? 3 Nora H. Prior1, *, Edward Smith1, Robert J. Dooling1, Gregory F. Ball1 4 5 1 Department of Psychology, University of Maryland, College Park, USA 6 7 8 9 *Corresponding author 10 Biology-Psychology Bldg. 11 University of Maryland 12 4094 Campus Dr. 13 College Park, MD 20742 14 Email: [email protected] 15 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.18.160051; this version posted June 20, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 16 Abstract 17 Research on monogamy has largely focused on marked behaviors that are unique to pair bonded 18 partners. However, these marked behaviors represent only a subset of the pair-directed behaviors that 19 partners engage in; the influence of pair bonding on mundane or subtle social interactions among 20 partners remains largely unknown. In the current study, we describe the changes that occur during brief 21 social reunions (or greets) over the course of pair bonding in zebra finches. We quantified pair-directed 22 behavior during five-minute reunions from three stages of pair bonding: initial pairing (between 4-72 23 hrs), early pairing (1-2 weeks) and late pairing (>1 month). These social interactions were 24 operationalized in multiples ways. First, we quantified the overall activity levels (call and movement 25 rates) for both the male and female. Overall, females were more active than males, but for both males 26 and females calling activity was highest during the initial timepoint (between 4-72 hrs post-pairing). We 27 quantified behavioral coordination between partners in two ways, 1) similarity in call and movement 28 rates between partners, and 2) temporal synchrony between calls and movements (via sliding 29 correlation coefficients of time-stamped calls and movements). Overall there were no effects of pairing 30 on behavioral coordination. Finally, we used principal component analyses to disentangle behavioral 31 coordination from the activity levels of the male and female. These results contribute to a growing line 32 of evidence that male and female zebra finches differentially contribute to social dynamics and highlight 33 the influence of pair bonding on the development of social dynamics. Behavioral coordination is clearly 34 important for marked interactions (e.g. duetting, courtship displays and biparental care). Our results 35 raise the question of what the roles of such mundane social interactions are in monogamous 36 partnerships. 37 38 Keywords: Behavioral synchrony, interactional synchrony, sex differences, stack call 2 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.18.160051; this version posted June 20, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 39 Introduction 40 In monogamous species, the formation and maintenance of a pair bond is necessary for the 41 successful rearing of offspring (Lack, 1968; Reichard & Boesch, 2003). The majority of research on the 42 mechanisms underlying pair bonding has focused on marked, pair-specific behaviors and interactions of 43 partners (Aragona et al., 2006; Soma & Garamszegi, 2015; Wachtmeister, 2001; Young et al., 2011; 44 Young & Wang, 2004). This approach has revealed remarkable conservation in the mechanisms of pair 45 bonding across taxa (Bales et al., 2007; Donaldson & Young, 2016; O’Connell & Hofmann, 2012; Walum 46 & Young, 2018). 47 Highly-marked behaviors and interactions are particularly evident during courtship and initial 48 pair-bond formation. For example, in prairie voles (Microtus ochrogaster), the establishment of a pair 49 bond is clearly marked by the development of selective preference/affiliation for a mate (Resendez et 50 al., 2016; Williams et al., 1992; Young et al., 2011). This marked selective attachment has been used to 51 identify the mechanisms underlying pair formation. However, these marked behaviors represent only a 52 subset of those that partners engage in, and more detailed understandings of partner interactions are 53 needed in order to further elucidate mechanisms of pair bonding. During partner preferences tests, the 54 pattern of approach and proximity time between partners continues even after initial establishment of 55 the pair bond, with the focal individual spending more time with their partners at later stages of pair 56 bonding (Scribner et al., 2020). Furthermore, important long-term impacts of pair bonding on the brain 57 and behavior of voles are revealed by differences in approach of an individual to its mate versus a 58 stranger (Scribner et al., 2020). Even in the most commonly studied model systems, remarkably little is 59 known about the consequences of pair bonding on mundane or subtle features of partner interactions. 60 Whereas monogamy is rare in mammals (<4% of species), the vast majority of birds form some 61 type of monogamous partnership (~90% of species) (Reichard & Boesch, 2003). Across bird species there 62 is considerable variation in the phenotype of monogamous bonds. Monogamous bonds vary in how long 3 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.18.160051; this version posted June 20, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 63 they are maintained; they may be transient, lasting only a season, or life-long (Black & Hulme, 1996). As 64 with prairie voles, the majority of studies focus on marked features of pair bonding (e.g. partner 65 preference, proximity time, allopreening and clumping) (Kenny et al., 2017; Prior et al., 2013; Smiley et 66 al., 2012; Tomaszycki & Adkins-Regan, 2005). However, the importance of brief social interactions has 67 also been described in numerous species: at the nest, brief social interactions appear to be essential for 68 the active coordination of parental duties between partners (I. C. A. Boucaud et al., 2016; Mariette & 69 Griffith, 2012, 2015; van Rooij & Griffith, 2013). This raises the question more broadly of how subtle 70 features of social interactions are shaped by pair bonding. 71 Here we describe the effect of pair bonding on brief social interactions in monogamous zebra 72 finch pairs. Zebra finches maintain sexually monogamous life-long pair bonds, are non-territorial, and 73 are highly gregarious. Interestingly, traditional partner preference paradigms may fail to demonstrate 74 selective preference for the partner (Prior et al., 2013), although other behavioral assays clearly show 75 that many aspects of pair-directed behavior are reserved for or more common between partners than 76 familiar conspecifics (Fernandez et al., 2017; Gill et al., 2015). Additionally, intra-pair calling dynamics, 77 across multiple contexts, appear to be important behavioral components of the zebra finch pair bond (I. 78 Boucaud et al., 2016; Pietro Bruno D’Amelio et al., 2017a; Elie et al., 2010; Fernandez et al., 2017; Gill et 79 al., 2015). 80 In the current study, we operationalize brief social reunions (both calls and movements) of pairs 81 over the course of pair bonding: initial pairing (between 4-72 hrs), early pairing (1-2 weeks) and late 82 pairing (>1 month). First, we quantified the overall activity levels (call and movement rates) for both 83 males and females. Second, we used two approaches to estimate the coordination of the activity 84 between partners, including quantifying the similarity in call and movement rates between partners as 85 well as quantifying the sliding correlation coefficients for time-stamped calls and movements (a measure 4 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.06.18.160051; this version posted June 20, 2020. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC 4.0 International license. 86 of temporal synchrony). Finally, we used principal component analyses to disentangle behavioral 87 coordination from the activity levels of the male and female. 88 89 Materials and Methods 90 Subjects and establishment of pairs 91 Twenty adult zebra finches (5-6 months old) were used in this study (10 females and 10 males). 92 Throughout the study, zebra finches were housed with ad libitum seed, water and grit on a 12L:12D light 93 cycle. This same cohort of zebra finches was also used for a subsequent experiment (Prior et al., 2019). 94 Prior to pairing, zebra finches were housed in same-sex flocks. In order to provide the 95 opportunity for pairs to freely form, birds were moved to mixed-sex flocks for 72 hours. Providing 96 individuals with the opportunity to freely form monogamous bonds is important as forced pairing can be 97 associated with lower pair fecundity (Griffith et al., 2017). Thus, birds were housed in mixed-sex flocks 98 with either a male- or female-biased sex ratio (2 females with 3 males or 3 females with 2 males).
Recommended publications
  • Origins of Language in Relation to Sexual Selection: the Effect of M
    h [ { { h [ { { 9 b a { 9 [ / Ç" Ü $ 9 " ò t '(( h [ { { L " Ç" Ü $ 9 " t * " " + $ + + , - + " L " + $ -- . / !.{Çw!/Ç Language can be viewed as sexual displays from an evolutionary perspective. As both sexes in humans contribute significantly to parental care, we can expect that both males and females use language to display their favourable qualities in order to attract potential mates. In this study, 50 participants (22 male, 28 female) rated 30 vignettes, in which a male or female protagonist attempted to impress an opposite sex friend in a conversation. In both male-female and female-male flirtations, conversations reveal- ing the speaker’s positive character traits were the most highly rated while those ex- plicitly showing the speaker’s sexual interest in the potential partner were the most poorly rated. Despite the similarities, there were significant sex differences in the rat- ings of some individual vignettes and item-groups. Such differences would be chiefly discussed from an evolutionary perspective. L The origins of language surely deserve and require a great deal of discussion among researchers from a wide range of disciplines. Based on my previous essay about the sexually attractive features of language (Szeto 2009), I will look into the relationship between sexual selection and language origins in greater depth, with a special focus on the effect of mutual mate choice on sex differences in speech content. ' h [ { { Despite the prevalence of elaborate animal communication systems, it is essen- tially unequivocal that humans are the only species which possess language (Barrett, Dunbar and Lycett 2002; Hurford 2006; Workman and Reader 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • Development of a Partner Preference Test That Differentiates Between Established Pair Bonds and Other Relationships in Socially
    American Journal of Primatology RESEARCH ARTICLE Development of a Partner Preference Test that Differentiates Between Established Pair Bonds and Other Relationships in Socially Monogamous Titi Monkeys (Callicebus cupreus) SARAH B. CARP1#, EMILY S. ROTHWELL1,2*,#, ALEXIS BOURDON3, SARA M. FREEMAN1, EMILIO FERRER4, 1,2,4 AND KAREN L. BALES 1California National Primate Research Center, University of California—Davis, Davis, California 2Animal Behavior Graduate Group, University of California—Davis, Davis, California 3AgroParisTech, Paris, France 4Department of Psychology, University of California—Davis, Davis, California Partner preference, or the selective social preference for a pair mate, is a key behavioral indicator of social monogamy. Standardized partner preference testing has been used extensively in rodents but a single test has not been standardized for primates. The goal of this study was to develop a partner preference test with socially monogamous titi monkeys (Callicebus cupreus) adapted from the widely used rodent test. In Experiment 1, we evaluated the test with pairs of titi monkeys (N ¼ 12) in a three-chambered apparatus for 3 hr. The subject was placed in the middle chamber, with grated windows separating it from its partner on one side and an opposite sex stranger on the other side. Subjects spent a greater proportion of time in proximity to their partners’ windows than the strangers’, indicating a consistent preference for the partner over the stranger. Touching either window did not differ between partners and strangers, suggesting it was not a reliable measure of partner preference. Subjects chose their partner more than the stranger during catch and release sessions at the end of the test.
    [Show full text]
  • Broad-Tailed Hummingbird Coloration and Sun Orientation 1
    1 Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 1 Two ways to display: male hummingbirds show different 2 color-display tactics based on sun orientation 3 Running header: Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 4 5 Richard K. Simpson1* and Kevin J. McGraw1 6 1School of Life Sciences, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287-4501 7 *Corresponding Author. Email: [email protected]; Phone: (480) 965-2593 8 9 ABSTRACT 10 Animals exhibit a diversity of ornaments and courtship behaviors, which often co- 11 occur and are used for communication. The sensory drive hypothesis states that these 12 traits evolved and vary due to interactions with each other, the environment, and signal 13 receiver. However, interactions between colorful ornaments and courtship behaviors, 14 specifically in relation to environmental variation, remain poorly understood. We studied 15 male iridescent plumage (gorgets), display behavior, and sun orientation during courtship 16 flights (shuttle displays) in broad-tailed hummingbirds (Selasphorus platycercus), to 17 understand how these traits interact in both space and time to produce the perceived 18 coloration of males. We also tested how gorget coloration varies among males based on 19 their plumage, behavioral, and morphological characteristics. In contrast with previous 20 work on other animals, we found that displaying males did not directionally face the sun, 21 but instead displayed on a continuum of solar orientation angles. The gorgets of males 22 who tended to face the sun during their displays appeared flashier (i.e. exhibited greater 23 color/brightness changes), brighter, and more colorful, whereas the gorgets of males who 2 Broad-tailed hummingbird coloration and sun orientation 24 tended to not face the sun were more consistently reflective (i.e.
    [Show full text]
  • RAM Dissertation
    A QUALITATIVE EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP ATTACHMENT MODEL (RAM) WITH MARRIED INDIVIDUALS A Dissertation Presented to The Graduate Faculty of The University of Akron In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Morgan C. Van Epp Cutlip August, 2013 A QUALITATIVE EXAMINATION OF THE RELATIONSHIP ATTACHMENT MODEL (RAM) WITH MARRIED INDIVIDUALS Morgan C. Van Epp Cutlip Dissertation Approved: Accepted: Advisor Department Chair Dr. John Queener Dr. Karin Jordan Committee Member Associate Dean of the College Dr. Susan I. Hardin Dr. Susan J. Olson Committee Member Dean of the Graduate School Dr. David Tokar Dr. George R. Newkome Committee Member Date Dr. Ingrid Weigold Committee Member Dr. Francis Broadway ii ABSTRACT The current study explored the theoretical underpinnings of the Relationship Attachment Model, an alternative model to understanding closeness in relationships, using deductive qualitative analysis (DQA; Gilgun, 2010). Qualitative data from married couples was used to explore whether the five bonding dynamics (i.e. know, trust, rely, commit, and sex), proposed by the RAM, existed in their marital relationships. Additionally, this study examined whether the RAM could explain fluctuations in closeness and distance in the couple’s marriage and how married couples described and talked about love in their relationship. The findings of this research indicated that the five bonding dynamics put forth by the RAM did exist in marital relationships of these couples and that the complicated dynamics that occur in marital relationships could be captured on the RAM. This research supported findings from past research on close relationships and added to the literature by proposing another model to understanding and conceptualizing close relationship dynamics.
    [Show full text]
  • Pair-Bonding, Romantic Love and Evolution: the Curious Case Of
    PPSXXX10.1177/1745691614561683Fletcher et al.Pair-Bonding, Romantic Love, and Evolution 561683research-article2014 Perspectives on Psychological Science 2015, Vol. 10(1) 20 –36 Pair-Bonding, Romantic Love, and © The Author(s) 2014 Reprints and permissions: sagepub.com/journalsPermissions.nav Evolution: The Curious Case of DOI: 10.1177/1745691614561683 Homo sapiens pps.sagepub.com Garth J. O. Fletcher1, Jeffry A. Simpson2, Lorne Campbell3, and Nickola C. Overall4 1Victoria University Wellington, New Zealand; 2University of Minnesota; 3University of Western Ontario, Canada; and 4University of Auckland, New Zealand Abstract This article evaluates a thesis containing three interconnected propositions. First, romantic love is a “commitment device” for motivating pair-bonding in humans. Second, pair-bonding facilitated the idiosyncratic life history of hominins, helping to provide the massive investment required to rear children. Third, managing long-term pair bonds (along with family relationships) facilitated the evolution of social intelligence and cooperative skills. We evaluate this thesis by integrating evidence from a broad range of scientific disciplines. First, consistent with the claim that romantic love is an evolved commitment device, our review suggests that it is universal; suppresses mate-search mechanisms; has specific behavioral, hormonal, and neuropsychological signatures; and is linked to better health and survival. Second, we consider challenges to this thesis posed by the existence of arranged marriage, polygyny, divorce, and infidelity. Third, we show how the intimate relationship mind seems to be built to regulate and monitor relationships. Fourth, we review comparative evidence concerning links among mating systems, reproductive biology, and brain size. Finally, we discuss evidence regarding the evolutionary timing of shifts to pair-bonding in hominins.
    [Show full text]
  • THE DYNAMICS of ATTACHMENT and SEX 1 Evolved to Be Connected
    Running head: THE DYNAMICS OF ATTACHMENT AND SEX 1 Evolved to Be Connected: The Dynamics of Attachment and Sex over the Course of Romantic Relationships Gurit E. Birnbaum Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya Harry T. Reis University of Rochester Word count: 2,029 (50 references) January 31, 2018 Send Correspondence: Gurit E. Birnbaum, Ph.D. Baruch Ivcher School of Psychology Interdisciplinary Center (IDC) Herzliya P.O. Box 167 Herzliya, 46150, Israel Email address: [email protected] THE DYNAMICS OF ATTACHMENT AND SEX 2 Highlights The sexual system operates as an attachment-facilitating device. Attachment processes link sexuality with relationship quality. Sexual desire functions as a visceral gauge of romantic compatibility. Desire becomes sensitive to different partner traits as relationships develop. Desire is important for relationship persistence when relationships are fragile. THE DYNAMICS OF ATTACHMENT AND SEX 3 Abstract Sexual urges and emotional attachments are not always connected. Still, joint operation of the sexual and the attachment systems is typical of romantic relationships. Hence, within this context, the two systems mutually influence each other and operate together to affect relationship well-being. In this article, we review evidence indicating that sex promotes enduring bonds between partners and provide an overview of the contribution of attachment processes to understanding the sex-relationship linkage. We then present a model delineating the functional significance of sex in relationship development. We conclude by suggesting future directions for studying the dual potential of sex for either deepening attachment to a current valued partner or promoting a new relationship when the existing relationship has become less rewarding. (120 words) Key words: attachment; sex; relationship development; romantic relationships THE DYNAMICS OF ATTACHMENT AND SEX 4 Evolved to Be Connected: The Dynamics of Attachment and Sex over the Course of Romantic Relationships Sexual urges and emotional attachments are not always connected [1].
    [Show full text]
  • Avian Monogamy
    (ISBN: 0-943610-45-1) AVIAN MONOGAMY EDITED BY PATRICIA ADAIR GOWATY AND DOUGLAS W. MOCK Department of Zoology University of Oklahoma Norman, Oklahoma 73019 ORNITHOLOGICAL MONOGRAPHS NO. 37 PUBLISHED BY THE AMERICAN ORNITHOLOGISTS' UNION WASHINGTON, D.C. 1985 AVIAN MONOGAMY ORNITHOLOGICAL MONOGRAPHS This series, published by the American Ornithologists' Union, has been estab- lished for major papers too long for inclusion in the Union's journal, The Auk. Publication has been made possiblethrough the generosityof the late Mrs. Carll Tucker and the Marcia Brady Tucker Foundation, Inc. Correspondenceconcerning manuscripts for publication in the seriesshould be addressedto the Editor, Dr. David W. Johnston,Department of Biology, George Mason University, Fairfax, VA 22030. Copies of Ornithological Monographs may be ordered from the Assistant to the Treasurer of the AOU, Frank R. Moore, Department of Biology, University of Southern Mississippi, Southern Station Box 5018, Hattiesburg, Mississippi 39406. (See price list on back and inside back covers.) OrnithologicalMonographs,No. 37, vi + 121 pp. Editors of Ornithological Monographs, Mercedes S. Foster and David W. Johnston Special Reviewers for this issue, Walter D. Koenig, Hastings Reservation, Star Route Box 80, Carmel Valley, CA 93924; Lewis W. Oring, De- partment of Biology,Box 8238, University Station, Grand Forks, ND 58202 Authors, Patricia Adair Gowaty, Department of BiologicalSciences, Clem- son University, Clemson, SC 29631; Douglas W. Mock, Department of Zoology, University of Oklahoma, Norman, OK 73019 First received, 23 August 1983; accepted29 February 1984; final revision completed 8 October 1984 Issued October 17, 1985 Price $11.00 prepaid ($9.00 to AOU members). Library of CongressCatalogue Card Number 85-647080 Printed by the Allen Press,Inc., Lawrence, Kansas 66044 Copyright ¸ by the American Ornithologists'Union, 1985 ISBN: 0-943610-45-1 ii AVIAN MONOGAMY EDITED BY PATRICIA ADAIR GOWATY AND DOUGLAS W.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Human Mating: Trade-Offs and Strategic Pluralism
    BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2000) 23, 573–644 Printed in the United States of America The evolution of human mating: Trade-offs and strategic pluralism Steven W. Gangestad Department of Psychology, University of New Mexico, Albuquerque, NM 87131 [email protected] Jeffry A. Simpson Department of Psychology, Texas A&M University, College Station, TX 77843 [email protected]. Abstract: During human evolutionary history, there were “trade-offs” between expending time and energy on child-rearing and mating, so both men and women evolved conditional mating strategies guided by cues signaling the circumstances. Many short-term matings might be successful for some men; others might try to find and keep a single mate, investing their effort in rearing her offspring. Recent evidence suggests that men with features signaling genetic benefits to offspring should be preferred by women as short-term mates, but there are trade-offs between a mate’s genetic fitness and his willingness to help in child-rearing. It is these circumstances and the cues that signal them that underlie the variation in short- and long-term mating strategies between and within the sexes. Keywords: conditional strategies; evolutionary psychology; fluctuating asymmetry; mating; reproductive strategies; sexual selection Research on interpersonal relationships, especially roman- attributes (e.g., physical attractiveness) tend to assume tic ones, has increased markedly in the last three decades greater importance in mating relationships than in other (see Berscheid & Reis 1998) across a variety of fields, in- types of relationships (Buss 1989; Gangestad & Buss 1993 cluding social psychology, anthropology, ethology, sociol- [see also Kenrick & Keefe: “Age Preferences in Mates Re- ogy, developmental psychology, and personology (Ber- flect Sex Differences in Human Reproductive Strategies” scheid 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins of the Institutions of Marriage
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Research Papers in Economics Q ED Queen’s Economics Department Working Paper No. 1180 The Origins of the Institutions of Marriage Marina E. Adshade Brooks A. Kaiser Dalhousie University Department of Economics Queen’s University 94 University Avenue Kingston, Ontario, Canada K7L 3N6 8-2008 THE ORIGIN OF THE INSTITUTIONS OF MARRIAGE Marina E. Adshade Brooks A. Kaiser August 13, 2008 Abstract Standard economic theories of household formation predict the rise of institutionalized polyg- yny in response to increased resource inequality among men. We propose a theory, within the framework of a matching model of marriage, in which, in some cases, institutionalized monogamy prevails, even when resources are unequally distributed, as a result of agricultural externalities that increase the presence of pair-bonding hormones. Within marriage, hormone levels contribute to the formation of the marital pair bond, the strength of which determines a man’swillingness to invest in his wife’schildren. These pair bonds are reinforced through phys- ical contact between the man and his wife and can be ampli…ed by externalities produced by certain production technologies. Both the presence of additional wives and the absence of these externalities reduce the strength of the marital bond and, where the …tness of a child is increas- ing in paternal investment, reduce a woman’sexpected lifetime fertility. Multiple equilibria in terms of the dominant form of marriage (for example, polygyny or monogamy) are possible, if the surplus to a match is a function of reproductive success as well as material income.
    [Show full text]
  • FEMALE BONDING in CECELIA AHERN's LOVE, ROSIE Fahriana
    FEMALE BONDING IN CECELIA AHERN’S LOVE, ROSIE Fahriana Arviyanti/1611403131 Program Studi Sastra Inggris FS Universitas 17 Agustus 1945 Jln. Semolowaru No.45, Menur Pumpungan, Sukolilo, Kota Surabaya, Jawa Timur 60118 Email: [email protected] ABSTRACT: This study is about bonding that happens among the female characters: Rosie, Stephanie, Mom, Ruby and Katie. This bonding is called female bonding. Female bonding is commonly exposed when they usually share activities and emotions each other. To prove the existence of bonding among female characters, the writer decides to do a study on the novel entitled Love, Rosie by Cecelia Ahern. Applying feminist literary criticism and qualitative research method, the writer analyzed three characteristics of female bonding: 1)Friendship 2)Attachment 3)Cooperation. From the analysis, the writer concludes that female bonding happening among the females characters on novel that shows understanding, sharing activities, worries, trust and appreciation. The female bonding can make people more positive and give good impact for each other. Keywords: bonding, female bonding, friendship, attachment, cooperation INTRODUCTION According to Hazar and Campa normative developmental transition (2013:2) early bonding experiences from parental to peer to partner from infancy through adolescence. attachment. Abel (1981:3) female The chapters in this part address the bonding exemplifies a mode of basics of ethological attachment relational self-definition whose theory, the coevolution of infant– increasing prominence is evident in caregiver behavior systems, and the the revival of psychoanalytic interest in object-relations theory and in the eventhough she is new to Rosie’s life. dynamics of transference and And her daughter, Katie who always countertransference, as well as in the supports and undertands her.
    [Show full text]
  • Short Communications Courtship Display and Mating of the Superb
    Short Communications Courtship Display and Mating of the Superb Bird of Paradise Lophorina superba D.W. & C.B. FRITH "Prionodura':Paluma via Townsville, Queensland 4816 Emu 88, 183-188 Received 17 May 1987, accepted 9 October 1987 The Superb Bird of Paradise Lophorina superba inhabits December 1977 and November 1985 and we compared rainforest and forest edge between approximately 1000 these with those of the wild birds and descriptions by other and 2250 m above mean sea level (amsl) throughout authors. mainland New Guinea. The adult male is velvet black with metallic-like oil-green iridescent plumage on the crown, an Initial dkplay activily (IDA) erectile iridescent breast shield, and with a very long erectile and spreadable cape of modified velvet black nape This was observed seven times and consists of a sleeked feathers. Peculiar horn-like naral tufts of erectile rather stiff pose, held for about five seconds, before movement of black feathers adorn the base of the upper mandible, above cape, breast and naral tuft plumage. The male slightly the nostrils, as does a similar tuft of feathers beneath the crouches with breast shield sleeked tightly back against lower mandible. The female is cryptically coloured in himself, cape held back and down against the back, wings browns and greys with distinct blackish ventral bamng, and tail held normally, head and bill pointed upwards with like many other female paradisaeids (see Gilliard 1969; eyes fixed on the female, and naral tufts projecting conspic- Cooper & Forshaw 1977). First year male plumage is like uously forward and bifurcate (Fig. 1). This pose is followed that of the female, the black and iridescent plumage of by a repeated, sudden, upward and outward extension of adult males being acquired gradually over several years the breast shield, with head and bill still pointing at the (Gilliard 1969).
    [Show full text]
  • Pair-Bonded Relationships and Romantic Alternatives 1
    Pair-bonded Relationships and Romantic Alternatives 1 Running Head: PAIRBONDED RELATIONSHIPS AND ROMANTIC ALTERNATIVES Pair-bonded Relationships and Romantic Alternatives: Toward an Integration of Evolutionary and Relationship Science Perspectives Kristina M. Durante Rutgers University Paul W. Eastwick University of Texas at Austin Eli J. Finkel Northwestern University Steven W. Gangestad University of New Mexico Jeffry A. Simpson University of Minnesota, Twin Cities Campus Note: All authors contributed equally and are listed in alphabetical order. In Press, Advances in Experimental Social Psychology Pair-bonded Relationships and Romantic Alternatives 2 Abstract Relationship researchers and evolutionary psychologists have been studying human mating for decades, but research inspired by these two perspectives often yields fundamentally different images of how people mate. Research in the relationship science tradition frequently emphasizes ways in which committed relationship partners are motivated to maintain their relationships (e.g., by cognitively derogating attractive alternatives), whereas research in the evolutionary tradition frequently emphasizes ways in which individuals are motivated to seek out their own reproductive interests at the expense of partners’ (e.g., by surreptitiously having sex with attractive alternatives). Rather than being incompatible, the frameworks that guide each perspective have different assumptions that can generate contrasting predictions and can lead researchers to study the same behavior in different ways. This paper, which represents the first major attempt to bring the two perspectives together in a cross-fertilization of ideas, provides a framework to understand contrasting effects and guide future research. This framework—the conflict-confluence model— characterizes evolutionary and relationship science perspectives as being arranged along a continuum reflecting the extent to which mating partners’ interests are misaligned versus aligned.
    [Show full text]