Perspectives: Hamilton'S Legacy: Mechanisms of Kin Recognition in Humans

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Perspectives: Hamilton'S Legacy: Mechanisms of Kin Recognition in Humans ethologyinternational journal of behavioural biology Ethology CURRENT ISSUES – PERSPECTIVES AND REVIEWS Perspectives: Hamilton’s Legacy: Mechanisms of Kin Recognition in Humans Jill M. Mateo Department of Comparative Human Development, Institute for Mind and Biology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL, USA (Invited Review) Correspondence Abstract Jill M. Mateo, Department of Comparative Human Development & Institute for Mind and The behavior literature is replete with examples of individuals exhibiting Biology, The University of Chicago, 940 East costly acts that benefit someone else. These examples troubled Darwin so 57th Street, Chicago, IL 60637, USA. much so that he thought they would be fatal to his theory of natural selec- E-mail: [email protected] tion. A century later, W. D. Hamilton refined that theory by showing, quantitatively, that such acts could be favored if the individuals involved Received: October 18, 2014 were relatives. His theory of inclusive fitness is generally considered one Initial acceptance: November 18, 2014 of the greatest theoretical advances in evolution since Darwin’s time. Less Final acceptance: December 26, 2014 appreciated from Hamilton’s 1964 paper is the hypothesis that mecha- (M. Hauber) nisms favoring accurate kin recognition will also be selected. Here, I doi: 10.1111/eth.12358 review those recognition mechanisms and survey the literature on human kin recognition. Although not often considered, humans both produce Keywords: kin recognition, humans, cues to kinship that vary with genetic relatedness and have perceptual Hamilton, inclusive fitness, discrimination abilities to detect these cues in others and assess that relatedness. The potential functions of these abilities are discussed. Importantly, gaps in our understanding of the development and use of recognition mecha- nisms are noted. perception of like genes in other individuals, at the Introduction minimum, genes should affect ‘(1) some perceptible Fifty years ago, W. D. Hamilton not only made the feature of the organism, (2) the perception of that fea- most significant refinement to Darwin’s theory of nat- ture, and (3) the social response consequent upon ural selection with his concept of inclusive fitness what was perceived’ (Hamilton 1964b; p. 25). Today, (Hamilton 1964a), but also predicted the evolution of an understanding of kin recognition involves three mechanisms to facilitate the accurate recognition of components: the production of unique phenotypic kin (Hamilton 1964b). He posited that if one ‘could cues, or ‘labels’, the perception of these labels and their learn to recognize those of his neighbors who were degree of correspondence with a ‘recognition template’ really close relatives and could devote his beneficial (these components are the mechanism of recognition), actions to them alone an advantage to inclusive fitness and the action taken by an animal as a function of the would at once appear’ (Hamilton 1964b, p. 21). Kin similarity between its template and an encountered recognition is an unobservable internal process of phenotype (Beecher 1982; Sherman & Holmes 1985; assessing genetic relatedness that is inferred by kin dis- Reeve 1989; Gamboa et al. 1991; Mateo 2003, 2004). crimination, the observable differential treatment of In this essay, honoring the 50th anniversary of Hamil- conspecifics based on cues that vary with relatedness. ton’s theory of inclusive fitness and mechanisms of kin Hamilton posited that if relationships persist, weak recognition, I discuss these processes in humans, selection could favor the evolution of discrimination, briefly summarize behaviors that influence inclusive even among distant kin. He added that although it fitness and then survey the literature on mechanisms may seem improbable that genes could cause the leading to kin-differentiated behaviors. Ethology 121 (2015) 419–427 © 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH 419 Kin-Recognition Mechanisms in Humans J. M. Mateo Recognition can be based on prior association, as households than are genetic kin (Daly & Wilson 1980, organisms learn the labels of related individuals 1982a), demonstrating clear fitness differentials as a during early development (e.g., siblings) and later dis- function of relatedness. Adoption occurs more fre- criminate these familiar individuals from unfamiliar quently among close kin (r = 0.125–0.25) than more ones, or, organisms can learn their own phenotypes distant kin (r ≤ 0.0625); such acts may constrain and/or those of their familiar kin and later compare or future reproduction by the adopting parent (Silk match the phenotypes of unknown individuals to this 1980). People leave a larger proportion of their estate learned template (phenotype matching). Both mecha- to their close kin than to distant kin or non-kin nisms involve comparisons between phenotypes and (Smith et al. 1987; Judge & Hrdy 1992; see also Kal- templates, but prior association leads to recognition of barczyk-Steclik & Nicinska 2012 for data on financial familiar individuals only, whereas phenotype match- transfers to parents as a function of relatedness). ing, through generalization from templates, permits Genetic fathers also invest more in their children than ‘recognition’ of unfamiliar kin (Holmes & Sherman do stepfathers (e.g., Anderson et al. 1999). Differen- 1982; Sherman et al. 1997; Tang-Martinez 2001; tial financial investments may not be costly to the Mateo 2004). This distinction is important when con- parents, certainly not after death, but can have long- sidering kin-directed behaviors such as nepotism or lasting effects on the offspring, particularly when inbreeding avoidance, because phenotype matching young. Although helping behaviors (e.g., with hous- allows more refined discrimination among kin classes ing, finances, health) are more likely to be recipro- than does prior association. Note that some species cated among friends, close kin help each other more can utilize both recognition mechanisms (and others) often than distant kin, and larger acts of helping are depending on the age of kin and the context (Mateo more likely to come from kin (e.g., Essock-Vitale & 2008). Finally, phenotype matching would be difficult McGuire 1985; see also Korchmaros 2006). These if production cues do not vary predictably with kin- helping behaviors are not costly in fitness terms and ship, but such cues could be used reliably for recogni- may only have short-term benefits, but demonstrate tion when prior association correlates with that even when the stakes are low, individuals pay relatedness, such as for parent–offspring recognition. attention to kinship and behave differentially. Kin-recognition abilities have been studied most How do humans assess genetic relatedness? Con- broadly in rodents (reviewed in Mateo 2003), ventionally, we know who our kin are because we although several species in other taxa have been ele- grow up with them or our parents inform us (e.g., Lie- gantly studied (birds: Beecher 1988; insects: Getz berman et al. 2007), and the established kinship ter- 1991; Gamboa 1996; Waldman 1991; anurans: Blau- minology of each culture can provide additional stein & Waldman 1992; Pfennig et al. 1999; primates: indicators of relatedness (e.g., Fox 1967). Barring Kazem & Widdig 2013; Pfefferle et al. 2014). Most errors or intentional withholding of information (e.g., empirical studies of kin-recognition mechanisms have in cases of adoption), this is a reliable mechanism. focused on visual and olfactory discrimination of con- Indeed, sharing a parent and growing up together are specifics (see below), but relatedness potentially could excellent cues to relatedness, at least for close kin be assessed through other modalities, such as vibra- (Lieberman et al. 2007). However, humans are capa- tional signaling (e.g., Randall 1993; O’Connell-Ro- ble of using several perceptual modalities and physio- dwell et al. 2007), behavioral cues (e.g., Michener logical cues to discriminate among familiar kin and to 1973; Boncoraglio et al. 2009), or vocalizations (e.g., discriminate among unfamiliar kin and non-kin. Rendall et al. 1996; Insley 2001; Briefer et al. 2012; Akcay et al. 2013). It is important to note that there Visual Modality of Kin Recognition may be multiple, overlapping modalities indicating kinship (e.g., Coleman 2009). Given the reliance of humans on visual cues (indeed, According to Hamilton’s theory of inclusive fitness, there are neural areas that specifically respond to costly behaviors can evolve when the benefit (b)to faces; see below), facial resemblance might be the recipient, devalued by the coefficient of relation- expected to serve as indicators of kinship. Recognition ship (r), is greater than the cost (c) to the actor (rb>c). of offspring would be especially important for appro- Here, benefits and costs are measured in terms of fit- priate allocation of parental investment. Mothers can ness effects (Hamilton 1964a). Among humans, there recognize photographs of their infants within 33 h of are many examples of kin-biased behaviors, although birth, and strangers can match photographs of moth- not all involve costly behaviors. For example, spouses ers to their infants, suggesting a physical resemblance and step children are more likely to be killed in among kin (Porter et al. 1984). Because the mothers 420 Ethology 121 (2015) 419–427 © 2015 Blackwell Verlag GmbH J. M. Mateo Kin-Recognition Mechanisms in Humans had direct contact with their infants (X = 4.7 h), their among other methodological differences. For exam- recognition could
Recommended publications
  • Kin Recognition in Vertebrates: What Do We Really Know About Adaptive Value?
    WellBeing International WBI Studies Repository 6-1991 Kin Recognition in Vertebrates: What Do We Really Know About Adaptive Value? Andrew R. Blaustein Oregon State University Marc Bekoff University of Colorado John A. Byers University of Idaho Thomas J. Daniels New York Medical College Follow this and additional works at: https://www.wellbeingintlstudiesrepository.org/acwp_asie Part of the Animal Studies Commons, Comparative Psychology Commons, and the Other Animal Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Blaustein, A. R., Bekoff, M., Byers, J. A., & Daniel, T. J. (1991). Kin recognition in vertebrates: what do we really know about adaptive value?. Animal Behaviour, 41(6), 1079-1083. This material is brought to you for free and open access by WellBeing International. It has been accepted for inclusion by an authorized administrator of the WBI Studies Repository. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Kin Recognition in Vertebrates: What Do We Really Know About Adaptive Value? Andrew R. Blaustein1, Marc Bekoff2, John A. Byers3, and Thomas J. Daniels4 1 Oregon State University 2 University of Colorado 3 University of Idaho 4 New York Medical College ABSTRACT The ability of an animal to discriminate between kin and non-kin (kin recognition) has been the subject of numerous recent investigations. Grafen (Anim. Behav., 1990, 39, 42-54) recently reported that the evidence in support of kin recognition is weak and the data illustrating a preference for kin to associate in the laboratory may be more consistently explained as species recognition. It is suggested here, however, that in many cases it may be impossible to distinguish between species recognition and kin recognition, but in some cases, kin recognition seems apparent.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin Recognition in Social Insects and Other Animals-A Review of Recent Findings and a Consideration of Their Relevance for the Theory of Kin Selection
    Proc. Indian Acad. Sci. (Anim. Sci.), Vol. 94, No. 6, December 1985, pp. 587-621. © Printed in India. Kin recognition in social insects and other animals-A review of recent findings and a consideration of their relevance for the theory of kin selection RAGHAVENDRA GADAGKAR Centre for Ecological Sciences, Indian Institute of Science, Bangalore 560012, India MS received 23 May 1985; revised 26 November 1985 Abstract. Kin selection is a widely invoked mechanism to explain the origin and evolution of social behaviour in animals. Proponents of the theory of kin selection place great emphasis on the correlation between asymmetries in genetic relatedness created by haplodiploidy and the multiple origins ofeusociality in the order Hymenoptera. The fact that a female is more closely related genetically to her full sister than to her daughters makes it more profitable for a Hymenopteran female, in terms of inclusive fitness,to raise full sisters rather than daughters or full siblings with a female biased sex ratio rather than offspring. This is sometimes referred to as the haplodiploidy hypothesis. In reality however, genetic relatedness between workers in social insect colonies and the reproductive brood they rear is far below ()75, the value expected for full sisters, often below (}5 the value expected between mother and daughter and, not uncommonly, approaching zero. Such values are on account of queen turnover, multiple mating by queens or polygyny. This situation raises doubts regarding the haplodiploidy hypothesis unless workers can discriminate between full and half sisters and preferentially direct their altruism towards their full sisters only. This would still mean an effective coefficient of genetic relatedness of(}75 between altruist and recipient.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin Recognition and the Evolution of Altruism Aneil F
    doi 10.1098/rspb.2001.1611 Kin recognition and the evolution of altruism Aneil F. Agrawal Department of Biology and Center for the Integrative Study of Animal Behavior, Indiana University, 1001 East 3rd Street, Bloomington, IN 47405-3700, USA ([email protected]) In 1964, Hamilton formalized the idea of kin selection to explain the evolution of altruistic behaviours. Since then, numerous examples from a diverse array of taxa have shown that seemingly altruistic actions towards close relatives are a common phenomenon. Although many species use kin recognition to direct altruistic behaviours preferentially towards relatives, this important aspect of social biology is less well understood theoretically. I extend Hamilton’s classic work by de¢ning the conditions for the evolution of kin-directed altruism when recognizers are permitted to make acceptance (type I) and rejection (type II) errors in the identi¢cation of social partners with respect to kinship. The e¡ect of errors in recognition on the evolution of kin-directed altruism depends on whether the population initially consists of uncondi- tional altruists or non-altruists (i.e. alternative forms of non-recognizers). Factors a¡ecting the level of these error rates themselves, their evolution and their long-term stability are discussed. Keywords: altruism; kin recognition; Hamilton’s rule; recognition errors the null hypothesis is wrongly accepted. In kin recogni- 1. INTRODUCTION tion, an acceptance error (type I error) occurs when an Although various exceptions to Hamilton’s (1964a,b) rule individual identi¢es a social partner as kin when it is non- have been noted when the simplifying assumptions of the kin, whereas a rejection error (type II error) occurs when theory are violated (e.g.
    [Show full text]
  • Apis Mellifera L.)
    The development of tools for network analysis in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) Antoine Lecocq Department of Ecology Uppsala, 2011 Antoine Lecocq Title: The development of tools for network analysis in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) Supervisor: Olle Terenius, Assistant Professor Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences Department of Ecology Co Supervisors: Barbara Locke, PhD candidate SLU Department of Ecology Annette Bruun Jensen MSc, Associate Professor University of Copenhagen, Faculty of Life Sciences External mentor: Cris Luengo SLU and Uppsala University Examiner: Ingemar Fries, Professor SLU Department of Ecology Credits: 30 ECTS Level: E Course title: Independent Project in Biology - Master´s thesis Course code: EX0565 Programme/education: EnvEuro Environmental Science in Europe Place of publication: SLU, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Faculty of Natural Resources and Agricultural Sciences, Department of Ecology, Uppsala Year of publication: 2011 Project number: 2011:15 Online publication: http://stud.epsilon.slu.se Key Words: Honey bee, transmission, network, disease, light 2 ABSTRACT The honey bee (Apis mellifera) has accompanied Man for thousands of years, and yet somehow, some aspects of this most studied of insects remain uncertain. To this day, details of the physiology, disease transmission, social organisation and behaviour of this animal are still unclear. The development of technology and computing and the use of tagging and automatic monitoring have already contributed in shedding light on some of the intricacies of sociality amongst insects. In this project, we hoped to develop further tools for the study of disease transmission though social networks in the honeybee, and shed some light on factors which might affect the behaviour of the bees in an experimental setting.
    [Show full text]
  • Kin-Biased Social Behaviour in Wild Adult Female White-Faced Capuchins, Cebus Capucinus
    ANIMAL BEHAVIOUR, 2008, 76, 187e199 doi:10.1016/j.anbehav.2008.01.020 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com Kin-biased social behaviour in wild adult female white-faced capuchins, Cebus capucinus SUSAN PERRY*†,JOSEPHH.MANSON*†,LAURAMUNIZ†, JULIE GROS-LOUIS‡ &LINDAVIGILANT† *Department of Anthropology and Center for Behavior, Evolution and Culture, University of California, Los Angeles yMax Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology zDepartment of Psychology, Indiana University (Received 16 October 2007; initial acceptance 30 October 2007; final acceptance 4 January 2008; published online 27 May 2008; MS. number: A10889R) Studies of kin bias in the distribution of social behaviour in group-living matrifocal species generally underline the importance of bonds among female kin. However, few studies examine either how kin bias may be affected by variation in the availability of kin or the relevance of paternal kin. In this study, we used genetic and behavioural data to analyse correlates of coalition formation, proximity, grooming and dominance relations among female white-faced capuchins over a 10-year period during which the number of adult females in the group varied from 6 to 10. Females sided with the most closely related of two opponents when joining coalitions. Both dominance rank and kinship influenced proximity and grooming patterns. In particular, when group size was small, mean relatedness high and interdyadic var- iation in relatedness low, rank distance was a better predictor of proximity and grooming than was kinship distance. However, when group size was large, mean relatedness lower and interdyadic variation in relat- edness higher, females significantly biased their grooming and spatial proximity towards kin.
    [Show full text]
  • The Development of Tools for Network Analysis in the Honey Bee (Apis Mellifera L.)
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Epsilon Archive for Student Projects The development of tools for network analysis in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) Antoine Lecocq Department of Ecology Uppsala, 2011 Antoine Lecocq Title: The development of tools for network analysis in the honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) Supervisor: Olle Terenius, Assistant Professor Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences Department of Ecology Co Supervisors: Barbara Locke, PhD candidate SLU Department of Ecology Annette Bruun Jensen MSc, Associate Professor University of Copenhagen, Faculty of Life Sciences External mentor: Cris Luengo SLU and Uppsala University Examiner: Ingemar Fries, Professor SLU Department of Ecology Credits: 30 ECTS Level: E Course title: Independent Project in Biology - Master´s thesis Course code: EX0565 Programme/education: EnvEuro Environmental Science in Europe Place of publication: SLU, Swedish University of Agricultural Sciences, Faculty of Natural Resources and Agricultural Sciences, Department of Ecology, Uppsala Year of publication: 2011 Project number: 2011:15 Online publication: http://stud.epsilon.slu.se Key Words: Honey bee, transmission, network, disease, light 2 ABSTRACT The honey bee (Apis mellifera) has accompanied Man for thousands of years, and yet somehow, some aspects of this most studied of insects remain uncertain. To this day, details of the physiology, disease transmission, social organisation and behaviour of this animal are still unclear. The development of technology and computing and the use of tagging and automatic monitoring have already contributed in shedding light on some of the intricacies of sociality amongst insects. In this project, we hoped to develop further tools for the study of disease transmission though social networks in the honeybee, and shed some light on factors which might affect the behaviour of the bees in an experimental setting.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 16 Kin Selection Challenges to Natural Selection
    Lecture 16 Kin selection Challenges to Natural selection: Sexual Selection Kin Selection Types of social interactions Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Recipient benefits Recipient harmed Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Recipient Cooperative benefits Recipient harmed Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Recipient Cooperative Altruistic benefits Recipient harmed Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Recipient Cooperative Altruistic benefits Recipient Selfish harmed Types of social interactions “Actor” → “Recipient” Actor benefits Actor harmed Recipient Cooperative Altruistic benefits Recipient Selfish Spiteful harmed The evolution of altruism The evolution of altruism • an altruistic act benefits a recipient at a cost to the actor The evolution of altruism • an altruistic act benefits a recipient at a cost to the actor • how can altruistic behaviors evolve? The evolution of altruism • an altruistic act benefits a recipient at a cost to the actor • how can altruistic behaviors evolve? let B = benefit to recipient (surviving offspring) The evolution of altruism • an altruistic act benefits a recipient at a cost to the actor • how can altruistic behaviors evolve? let B = benefit to recipient let C = cost to actor The evolution of altruism • an altruistic act benefits a recipient at a cost
    [Show full text]
  • The Scent of the Waggle Dance
    PLoS BIOLOGY The Scent of the Waggle Dance Corinna Thom1*¤, David C. Gilley2¤, Judith Hooper1, Harald E. Esch3 1 Arizona Research Laboratories, Division of Neurobiology, University of Arizona, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America, 2 Carl-Hayden Bee Laboratory, United States Department of Agriculture, Tucson, Arizona, United States of America, 3 Department of Biology, University of Notre Dame, Notre Dame, Indiana, United States of America The waggle dance of honey bee (Apis mellifera L.) foragers communicates to nest mates the location of a profitable food source. We used solid-phase microextraction and gas chromatography coupled with mass spectrometry to show that waggle-dancing bees produce and release two alkanes, tricosane and pentacosane, and two alkenes, Z-(9)- tricosene and Z-(9)-pentacosene, onto their abdomens and into the air. Nondancing foragers returning from the same food source produce these substances in only minute quantities. Injection of the scent significantly affects worker behavior by increasing the number of bees that exit the hive. The results of this study suggest that these compounds are semiochemicals involved in worker recruitment. By showing that honey bee waggle dancers produce and release behaviorally active chemicals, this study reveals a new dimension in the organization of honey bee foraging. Citation: Thom C, Gilley DC, Hooper J, Esch HE (2007) The scent of the waggle dance. PLoS Biol 5(9): e228. doi:10.1371/journal.pbio.0050228 Introduction coordinate the activities of colony members [10,11] and hence may be expected to help organize the vital task of More than fifty years ago, Karl von Frisch demonstrated foraging.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of Kin Recognition
    Western University Scholarship@Western Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository 12-8-2015 12:00 AM The evolution of kin recognition Timothy JA Hain The University of Western Ontario Supervisor Bryan Neff The University of Western Ontario Graduate Program in Biology A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the equirr ements for the degree in Doctor of Philosophy © Timothy JA Hain 2015 Follow this and additional works at: https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/etd Part of the Ecology and Evolutionary Biology Commons Recommended Citation Hain, Timothy JA, "The evolution of kin recognition" (2015). Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository. 3431. https://ir.lib.uwo.ca/etd/3431 This Dissertation/Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Scholarship@Western. It has been accepted for inclusion in Electronic Thesis and Dissertation Repository by an authorized administrator of Scholarship@Western. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE EVOLUTION OF KIN RECOGNITION (Thesis format: Integrated Article) by Timothy John Alexander Hain Graduate Program in Biology A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy The School of Graduate and Postdoctoral Studies The University of Western Ontario London, Ontario, Canada © Timothy Hain 2015 ii Abstract The discovery that many animals are promiscuous has challenged the importance of Hamilton’s Rule because it reduces the net benefits of helping nestmates. To resolve this challenge, biologists have investigated animals’ abilities to determine degrees of relatedness among individuals using kin recognition mechanisms. I conducted a literature review and found that most animals use one of two mechanisms: “familiarity” whereby kin are remembered from interactions early in life, such as in a nest, or “phenotype matching” whereby putative kin are compared to a template of what kin should look, smell, or sound like based on relatives encountered during early life or on one’s own phenotype (called “self-referent phenotype matching”).
    [Show full text]
  • Behaviours Indicating Cannibalistic Necrophagy in Ants Are Modulated
    www.nature.com/scientificreports OPEN Behaviours indicating cannibalistic necrophagy in ants are modulated by the perception of pathogen infection level István Maák1,6*, Eszter Tóth2,3, Magdalena Lenda4,5, Gábor Lőrinczi6, Anett Kiss6, Orsolya Juhász6,7, Wojciech Czechowski1 & Attila Torma6,8 Cannibalistic necrophagy is rarely observed in social hymenopterans, although a lack of food could easily favour such behaviour. One of the main supposed reasons for the rarity of necrophagy is that eating of nestmate corpses carries the risk of rapid spread of pathogens or parasites. Here we present an experimental laboratory study on behaviour indicating consumption of nestmate corpses in the ant Formica polyctena. We examined whether starvation and the fungal infection level of the corpses afects the occurrence of cannibalistic necrophagy. Our results showed that the ants distinguished between corpses of diferent types and with diferent levels of infection risk, adjusting their behaviour accordingly. The frequency of behaviours indicating cannibalistic necrophagy increased during starvation, although these behaviours seem to be fairly common in F. polyctena even in the presence of other food sources. The occurrence and signifcance of cannibalistic necrophagy deserve further research because, in addition to providing additional food, it may be part of the hygienic behaviour repertoire. The ability to detect infections and handle pathogens are important behavioural adaptations for social insects, crucial for the ftness of both individual workers and the entire colony. Cannibalism is a taxonomically widespread phenomenon in the animal kingdom but is uncommon in most species1–4. It can be divided into two distinct behaviours: killing and eating individuals of the same species or genera (cannibalistic predation), and eating already-dead taxonomically (and even genetically) related individu- als (cannibalistic necrophagy); both behaviours have been described in humans 5.
    [Show full text]
  • Nestmate Recognition in Ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): a Review
    Myrmecological News 16 101-110 Vienna, January 2012 Nestmate recognition in ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae): a review Shelby J. STURGIS & Deborah M. GORDON Abstract Nestmate recognition is the process by which individuals discriminate between nestmates and con- and hetero-specifics. Nestmate recognition is based on recognition cues, which include cuticular hydrocarbons (CHCs). Models of nestmate recognition predict that recognition decisions are based on the overlap of recognition cues. Colony recipients assess cue differences by comparing an individual's CHC profile to an internal template, which is based on the colony-specific cues. The behavioral response to this assessment depends on cue similarities or differences with the template. Ants show graded responses to cue differences. More recent models of nestmate recognition include adjustable thresholds that account for graded responses and intra-colony individual variation in behavioral responses towards non-nestmates. Ants display differing levels of aggression towards conspecifics under different contexts, which suggests that nestmate recognition is context-dependent. Here, we review models of decision rules and the role of CHCs in nestmate recognition. We discuss the role of ecological and social context in nestmate recognition, and explore future directions of research for the field. Key words: Context-dependent, Formicidae, hydrocarbons, nestmate recognition, review. Myrmecol. News 16: 101-110 (online 23 November 2011) ISSN 1994-4136 (print), ISSN 1997-3500 (online) Received 18 June 2011; revision received 12 October 2011; accepted 13 October 2011 Subject Editor: Helge Schlüns Shelby J. Sturgis (contact author) & Deborah M. Gordon, Stanford University, Department of Biology, 371 Serra Mall, Gilbert Building, Stanford, CA 94305, USA. E-mail: [email protected] Introduction Nestmate recognition allows workers in social insect col- SILVERMAN 2005, BOS & al.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ecology and Evolution of Eusociality in Sponge-Dwelling Shrimp
    J. E. Duffy: Eusociality in sponge-dwelling shrimp In: Kikuchi, T. (editor). 2002. Genes, Behavior, and Evolution in Social Insects. University of Hokkaido Press, Sapporo, Japan. The ecology and evolution of eusociality in sponge-dwelling shrimp J. EMMETT DUFFY School of Marine Science & Virginia Institute of Marine Science, The College of William and Mary Gloucester Point, VA 23062-1346, USA. e-mail: [email protected] INTRODUCTION Recent evidence suggests that the Crustacea is the sister taxon to the Insects (Friedrich and Tautz 1995, Shultz and Regier, 2000). The two taxa share many similarities in morphology, physiology, and ecology, and just as insects dominate many terrestrial ecosystems, crustaceans often dominate those of the sea. The extraordinary ecological success of insects is due in part to the unparalleled efficiency and competitive ability resulting from highly organized cooperation found in the social taxa (Oster and Wilson 1978, Wilson 1990). Although the gigantic, superorganismal colonies of some ants and termites have no marine analog, there are nevertheless strong parallels between colonies of certain sponge-associated marine shrimp and those of both primitively social insects and cooperatively breeding vertebrates. For example, social Synalpheus are strikingly similar to termites in being diploid, generally monogamous animals with gradual development that live in cloistered environments that they must defend from competitors. One could substitute “Synalpheus” for “termite” or “Isoptera” throughout the abstract of Thorne’s (1997) review of termite eusociality and it would remain quite accurate. What can shrimp tell us about the ecology and evolution of social organization? At the broadest level, social shrimp offer a new data point in a small sample of higher taxa in which large, cooperative colonies with strong reproductive skew (eusociality in the traditional sense, Wilson, 1971) have evolved.
    [Show full text]