A Literature Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

A Literature Review Modern Psychological Studies Volume 12 Number 1 Article 5 2006 Effectiveness of treatment interventions for adults suffering from anger disorders: a literature review Amy Levitt Muhlenberg College Mark J. Sciutto Muhlenberg College Follow this and additional works at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Levitt, Amy and Sciutto, Mark J. (2006) "Effectiveness of treatment interventions for adults suffering from anger disorders: a literature review," Modern Psychological Studies: Vol. 12 : No. 1 , Article 5. Available at: https://scholar.utc.edu/mps/vol12/iss1/5 This articles is brought to you for free and open access by the Journals, Magazines, and Newsletters at UTC Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Modern Psychological Studies by an authorized editor of UTC Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Effectiveness of Treatment In- terventions for Adults Suffer- Amy Levitt ing from Anger Disorders: A Advisor: Professor Mark J. Sciutto, Ph.D. Literature Review Muhlenberg College Anger is a Common Problem medical problems that result from suppressed Anger plays a significant role in everyday anger are artery disease, cancer and hypertension life. Defined, it is a feeling "produced by active (Greer & Morris, 1975). processes, including remembrances of the past, expectations of the future, awareness of current Anger Should be More Frequently Addressed behavioral and physiological responses and in Treatment Outcome Literature comparison of actual to desired behaviors and Even though anger has been shown to be judgments of what others are thinking" detrimental to oneself and others, various treat- (Kassinove & Sukhodolsky, 1995, p. 177). ment outcome research expressing methods to Referred to as a harmful, phenomenological or decrease its prevalence have been extremely internal feeling state, anger encompasses percep- scarce. Therapists find it particularly odd that tual and cognitive distortions such as anger is a neglected topic in treatment literature misattributions of blame, feelings of injustice, since anger is a frequent subject in psycho- subjective labeling, negative physiological therapy. Evidence for this lack of support for alterations and tendencies to engage in socially treatment outcome research is that the primary destructive behavior. The totality of cognitive interest in the past twenty-five years based on and feeling states differentiates anger from other keywords in PsycINFO has been on anxiety and similar feelings such as sadness and anxiety. depression (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). The Sometimes anger is brief and fairly intense. neglect of anger in past literature renders little However, at other times, it is unrelenting, severe empirical help to clinical practitioners. and highly disruptive in one's life as well the As the detrimental effects of anger have been lives of loved ones, co-workers and others in known for a long time, it is imperative that a one's social network. Overt anger, also known review of treatment outcome research be per- as aggression, can lead to a negative self-con- formed to assess treatments — pharmacological cept, pessimistic judgments by others, feelings of and psychological — with the greatest efficacy. low self-worth, inability to function in the To prove efficacy, researchers should establish workplace, physical and verbal injury to self or empirically supported treatments (ESTs), i.e. others, destruction of property and family con- treatments shown to be valuable in controlled flict (Deffenbacher & Stark, 1992). Anger is research with a clearly defined population also problematic when suppressed. Examples of (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). Controlled re- 30 search allows the experimenter to infer that the dependent on his or her interpretations, thoughts effects of an experiment are due to the treatment and self-statements of real-life experiences, and not to chance or confounding factors such as desire to help patients recognize and dispute the incidence of conditions with very similar their irrational and imprecise thoughts to con- participants in terms of attitudes or physical struct more adaptive cognitions (Beck, 1976). health, or passage of time (Campbell & Stanley, This type of treatment for anger has produced an 1963). average effect size of .93, which is a large Efficacy is also best demonstrated in random- treatment effect. ized clinical trials (RCTs) — group designs in Another form of treatment that has been which individuals are randomly assigned to a proven effective in the healing of anger is Anger treatment (Chambless & Hollon, 1998). Also Management Training (Richardson & Suinn, critical to prove efficacy is replication by two or 1972). The client is asked to re-construct past more independent researchers. Replication helps experiences that have provoked angry feelings, protect from drawing incorrect assumptions of such as an argument with a significant other or the efficacy of a treatment based on one unusual co-worker. The therapist helps the patient finding. arrange these experiences in a hierarchical To identify a population for research, many fashion, with increasing levels of severity. have used the Diagnostic and Statistical Manual Concurrently, the therapist teaches the client of Mental Disorders (DSM-IV-TR) because the ways to relax him or herself. Once capable to diagnostic labels in the manual are standardized, display these relaxation techniques, the client is allowing reliable diagnoses to be made that are instructed to re-enact the least anger-provoking familiar to both clinicians and researchers. experience. Repetition ensues, with the patient Another way to identify a population for re- honing skills in associating the anger-provoking search is through cutoff scores on reliable and experience with relaxation, both in therapy and valid questionnaires or interviews identifying the at home. Relaxation is successively paired with problem of interest. However, the question arises increasing levels of anger-provoking contexts. that some internal experiences of the human The authors illustrated that Anger-management being that a researcher may desire to investigate training also has shown to be very effective, with such as anger have not been classified in the an average effect size of 1.01. DSM-IV-TR. Given the potential severity of anger-related problems, it is surprising that anger The Present Study: Comparison of Effect has received such little attention in the psycho- Sizes for Recent Treatment Outcome Re- logical literature. It is therefore imperative to search on Anger, 1995 - 2005 compare and contrast treatment intervention Despite the previous lack of treatment out- outcomes for this common physiological, behav- come studies on anger, there appears to be an ioral, cognitive and socially reinforced experi- increase in the quality of the literature and the ence, with the goal of not only finding the most number of studies within the past ten years. effective treatment programs when anger is Effect sizes for these studies and others were excessive and disruptive but possibly a separate computed to assess which measures are most categorization in the DSM-IV-TR manual. effective in the assessment of anger (self-report, physiological and behavioral), which popula- What We Know about Treatment Strategies tions were most likely to comply with treatment for Anger (voluntary versus mandated/offender), which We do have some information regarding the studies included a control/comparison group, the effectiveness of various treatments for anger. most frequently indicated treatment (Cognitive- Cognitive therapies, based on the hypothesis that Behavioral, Anger Management Training and the emotional functioning of an individual is pharmacotherapy), the range of follow-up period 31 (smallest, largest, and median duration), the sixty-one individuals participated in Anger average treatment period (smallest, largest, and Management Training, with an average sample median duration), average sample sizes and size of 140.3 per study. Other treatments used dropout, effect sizes for various measures of were Dialectical Behavior Therapy, in which the anger (anger-in, anger-out, anger control, aggres- therapist discusses recent unhealthy or danger- sion/violence) and comparison of effect sizes of ous behaviors with the client in a hierarchical treatments solely targeting anger versus anger/ manner, from its precursors to cognitive and aggression. Therefore, the purpose of this litera- behavioral barriers that prevented the client from ture review was to assess more recent research to choosing more positive behaviors, to exploring outline factors that might affect treatment out- various alternative options that the client can use come for anger, with an ultimate goal of deter- in the future. This therapy had a sample size of mining which treatments are most efficacious for eight participants. The two pharmacotherapies the treatment of anger. used were Citalopram and Topiramate. Citalopram, a selective serotonin reuptake Method inhibitor (S SRI) that is taken primarily for depression, contained forty-five participants. Materials Topiramate, an anticonvulsant, included twenty- Effect sizes, which measure the magnitude of nine participants. Both pharmacotherapies a treatment effect, were computed using a pro- significantly increased anger control. gram called POWPAL and ZUMASTAT The longest duration of a study was eleven (Gorman, Primavera, & Allison, 1995; ZumaStat months, while the shortest time period
Recommended publications
  • What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—And Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word
    What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate, Robert Pearlman Perspectives in Biology and Medicine, Volume 62, Number 1, Winter 2019, pp. 95-110 (Article) Published by Johns Hopkins University Press For additional information about this article https://muse.jhu.edu/article/722412 Access provided at 26 Apr 2019 00:52 GMT from University of Washington @ Seattle What We Mean When We Talk About Suffering—and Why Eric Cassell Should Not Have the Last Word Tyler Tate* and Robert Pearlman† ABSTRACT This paper analyzes the phenomenon of suffering and its relation- ship to medical practice by focusing on the paradigmatic work of Eric Cassell. First, it explains Cassell’s influential model of suffering. Second, it surveys various critiques of Cassell. Next it outlines the authors’ concerns with Cassell’s model: it is aggressive, obscure, and fails to capture important features of the suffering experience. Finally, the authors propose a conceptual framework to help clarify the distinctive nature of sub- jective patient suffering. This framework contains two necessary conditions: (1) a loss of a person’s sense of self, and (2) a negative affective experience. The authors suggest how this framework can be used in the medical encounter to promote clinician-patient communication and the relief of suffering. *Center for Ethics in Health Care and School of Medicine, Oregon Health and Science University, Portland. †National Center for Ethics in Health Care, Washington, DC, and School of Medicine, University of Washington, Seattle. Correspondence: Tyler Tate, Oregon Health and Science University, School of Medicine, Depart- ment of Pediatrics, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Road, Portland, OR 97239-3098.
    [Show full text]
  • Examining Affective Forecasting and Its Practical and Ethical Implications
    Examining Affective Forecasting and its Practical and Ethical Implications Emily Susan Brindley BSc Psychology 2009 Supervisor: Prof. David Clarke Contents Page Introduction 1 1. Affective Forecasting – What do we know? 1 2. Biases – Why people cannot predict their emotions accurately 3 2.1 Impact Bias 3 2.2 ‘Focalism’ 4 2.3 Immune Neglect 4 2.4 Dissimilar Context 4 3. The Self-Regulating Emotional System 5 4. Affective Forecasting Applied 6 4.1 Healthcare 6 4.2 Law 7 5. Can AFing be improved? 8 6. Ethics: Should people be taught to forecast more accurately? 10 Conclusions 12 References 13 Examining Affective Forecasting and its Practical and Ethical Implications Introduction Emotions are important in guiding thoughts and behaviour to the extent that they are used as heuristics (Slovic, Finucane, Peters & MacGregor, 2007), and are crucial in decision-making (Anderson, 2003). Affective forecasting (AFing) concerns an individual’s judgemental prediction of their or another’s future emotional reactions to events. It is suggested that “affective forecasts are among the guiding stars by which people chart their life courses and steer themselves into the future” (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg & Wheatley, 1998; p.617), as our expected reactions to emotional events can assist in avoiding or approaching certain possibilities. We can say with certainty that we will prefer good experiences over bad (ibid); however AFing research demonstrates that humans are poor predictors of their emotional states, regularly overestimating their reactions. Further investigation of these findings shows that they may have critical implications outside of psychology. If emotions are so influential on behaviour, why are people poor at AFing? Furthermore, can and should individuals be assisted in forecasting their emotions? These issues, along with the function of AFing in practical applications, are to be considered and evaluated.
    [Show full text]
  • Religious Perspectives on Human Suffering: Implications for Medicine and Bioethics
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Sydney eScholarship Postprint This is a pre-copyedited, author-produced PDF of an article accepted for publication in [Journal of Religion and Health] following peer review. The definitive publisher-authenticated version [Fitzpatrick SJ, Kerridge IH, Jordens CFC, Zoloth L, Tollefsen C, Tsomo KL, Jensen MP, Sachedina A, Sarma D. Religious perspectives on human suffering: Implications for medicine and bioethics. Journal of Religion and Health 2016; 55:159–173] is available online at http://link.springer.com/article/10.1007/s10943-015-0014-9 Please cite as: Fitzpatrick SJ, Kerridge IH, Jordens CFC, Zoloth L, Tollefsen C, Tsomo KL, Jensen MP, Sachedina A, Sarma D. Religious perspectives on human suffering: Implications for medicine and bioethics. Journal of Religion and Health 2016; 55:159–173. Religious perspectives on human suffering: Implications for medicine and bioethics Scott J FitzpatrickA,B, Ian H KerridgeB, Christopher F C JordensB , Laurie ZolothC, Christopher TollefsenD, Karma Lekshe TsomoE, Michael P JensenF, Abdulaziz SachedinaG, Deepak SarmaH (2015/16) ACentre for Rural and Remote Mental Health, University of Newcastle, Orange, Australia; BCentre for Values, Ethics and the Law in Medicine (VELiM), University of Sydney, Sydney, Australia; CCentre for Bioethics, Science and Society, Northwestern University Feinberg School of Medicine, Chicago, Illinois, USA; DDepartment of Philosophy, University of South Carolina, Colombia, South Carolina, USA USA; EDepartment of Theology and Religious Studies, University of San Diego, San Diego, California, USA; FMoore Theological College, Sydney, Australia; GAli Vural Ak Centre for Global Islamic Studies, George Mason University, Fairfax, Virginia, USA; HReligious Studies, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, Ohio, USA.
    [Show full text]
  • ANXIETY DISORDERS in ADULTS the Disorders, Their Treatment and Prevention
    Angstlidelser hos voksne, engelsk Information about ANXIETY DISORDERS IN ADULTS The disorders, their treatment and prevention Psykiatri og Social psykinfomidt.dk CONTENTS 03 What are anxiety disorders? 05 Why do some people suffer from anxiety disorders? 06 What happens in the brain when someone suffers from anxiety? 07 What types of anxiety disorders are there? 08 What are the symptoms of anxiety disorders? 10 Different degrees of anxiety disorders 11 How are they diagnosed? 12 What treatment is available for anxiety disorders? 15 What can be done to prevent anxiety disorders? 16 What can you do yourself if you are suffering from anxiety? 18 What can relatives do? 21 Where can you find more information? Anxiety disorders are the most common mental disorders in the population in the Western world. This brochure describes anxiety disorders, their specific characteristics as well as what they have in common, and their treatment. It also offers some advice to anxiety sufferers and their relatives. This brochure is for adults (age 18 and above), and it is mainly intended for people being treated for anxiety disorders by the psychiatric service in Region Midtjylland, and for their relatives. It is important for you and your relatives to learn about anxiety disorders. The more you know, the better you will be able to relate to the disorder when it occurs, and prevent relapses. Regional psychiatry in Region Midtjylland has six clinics offering outpatient treatment of anxiety disorders. We hope this brochure will help you and your relatives to become better informed about anxiety disorders. Kind regards The psychiatric service in Region Midtjylland Tingvej 15, 8800 Viborg Tel.
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Suffering in Theories of Emotion
    Suffering and Emotion 1 Running head: SUFFERING AND EMOTION The Role of Suffering in Theories of Emotion Joseph de Rivera Frances L. Hiatt School of Psychology Clark University Suffering and Emotion 2 There is a good deal more suffering in human life that there is in our theories of emotion. One reason for this appears to be that our theories tend to focus on individual emotions or moods. Thus, we focus on terms such as anger or anxiety without considering that there common root is anguish. Another reason is that we want to ground our theories in our evolutionary heritage, so we focus on emotions that we share with other animals and that are clearly observable, such as the Big Four of anger, fear, happiness and sadness. Yet while animals certainly appear to experience pain, and show the effects of loss, they do not the way anticipate pain and loss in the way we do when we consider aging, sickness, death, and the loss of those we love. Hence, in addition to examining specific emotions and relating emotion to our animal heritage, it is important to contexturalize emotion in our broader human being. When we do this we must come to grips with emotional experiences such as suffering and mysticism. When we begin with such experiences, rather than with facial expressions, we find that they are focused on our relationship with something that is radically Other than ourselves. Today, in our secular culture with its scientific parlance, we may speak as Jim Averill has, of our “system of cognitive constructs,” but these cognitive constructs are beliefs about the World in which we live.
    [Show full text]
  • IS THERE MEANING in SUFFERING? by Dimitrios G
    IS THERE MEANING IN SUFFERING? By Dimitrios G. Oreopoulos MD. Toronto, Canada This article was first published in Humane Medicine, Volume 5, Number 2, 2005. What is suffering? Before we explore its meaning we should understand how we define suffering. In 1982 the New England Journal of Medicine published the now classical paper “The Nature of Suffering and the Goals of Medicine.” In it, Eric Cassel said that suffering can be defined as a state of severe distress associated with events that threaten the intactness of the person. It can occur in relation to any aspect of the person in the realm of his social role, his group identification, his relation with self or body, or in relation to family or relation with a personal or transcendental source of meaning. Suffering never affects only one part of a person but it affects the whole being; i.e. physical, emotional, mental, spiritual and social aspects.1,2 Suffering can occur when one does not expect it, or the patient does not report it when it is expected. Also, a person can suffer enormously at the distress of another, especially a loved one Finally, as I mentioned in my introduction, suffering is a unique personal experience and can only be defined by the person who suffers.1,2 Pain and suffering Although closely identified, these two experiences are phenomenologically distinct. It is believed that the greater the pain the greater the suffering but this is not always the case. Thus childbirth can be extremely painful yet it is considered to be rewarding.
    [Show full text]
  • Towards a Conceptual Model of Affective Predictions in Palliative Care
    Accepted Manuscript Towards a conceptual model of affective predictions in palliative care Erin M. Ellis, PhD, MPH, Amber E. Barnato, MD, MPH, Gretchen B. Chapman, PhD, J. Nicholas Dionne-Odom, PhD, Jennifer S. Lerner, PhD, Ellen Peters, PhD, Wendy L. Nelson, PhD, MPH, Lynne Padgett, PhD, Jerry Suls, PhD, Rebecca A. Ferrer, PhD PII: S0885-3924(19)30059-4 DOI: https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2019.02.008 Reference: JPS 10037 To appear in: Journal of Pain and Symptom Management Received Date: 4 September 2018 Revised Date: 24 January 2019 Accepted Date: 10 February 2019 Please cite this article as: Ellis EM, Barnato AE, Chapman GB, Dionne-Odom JN, Lerner JS, Peters E, Nelson WL, Padgett L, Suls J, Ferrer RA (2019). Towards a conceptual model of affective predictions in palliative care. Journal of Pain and Symptom Management, 57(6), 1151-1165. doi: https:// doi.org/10.1016/j.jpainsymman.2019.02.008. This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain. ACCEPTED MANUSCRIPT 1 Running title: AFFECTIVE PREDICTIONS IN PALLIATIVE CARE Towards a conceptual model of affective predictions in palliative care Erin M. Ellis, PhD, MPH (National Cancer Institute, Bethesda, MD, USA; [email protected]) Amber E.
    [Show full text]
  • Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience
    This PDF is available from The National Academies Press at http://www.nap.edu/catalog.php?record_id=18548 Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience ISBN Arthur A. Stone and Christopher Mackie, Editors; Panel on Measuring 978-0-309-29446-1 Subjective Well-Being in a Policy-Relevant Framework; Committee on National Statistics; Division on Behavioral and Social Sciences and 204 pages Education; National Research Council 6 x 9 PAPERBACK (2013) Visit the National Academies Press online and register for... Instant access to free PDF downloads of titles from the NATIONAL ACADEMY OF SCIENCES NATIONAL ACADEMY OF ENGINEERING INSTITUTE OF MEDICINE NATIONAL RESEARCH COUNCIL 10% off print titles Custom notification of new releases in your field of interest Special offers and discounts Distribution, posting, or copying of this PDF is strictly prohibited without written permission of the National Academies Press. Unless otherwise indicated, all materials in this PDF are copyrighted by the National Academy of Sciences. Request reprint permission for this book Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved. Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience Subjective Well-Being Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience Panel on Measuring Subjective Well-Being in a Policy-Relevant Framework Arthur A. Stone and Christopher Mackie, Editors Committee on National Statistics Division of Behavioral and Social Sciences and Education Copyright © National Academy of Sciences. All rights reserved. Subjective Well-Being: Measuring Happiness, Suffering, and Other Dimensions of Experience THE NATIONAL ACADEMIES PRESS 500 Fifth Street, NW Washington, DC 20001 NOTICE: The project that is the subject of this report was approved by the Governing Board of the National Research Council, whose members are drawn from the councils of the National Academy of Sciences, the National Academy of Engineering, and the Institute of Medicine.
    [Show full text]
  • Emotion Suffering and Compassion
    Emotion Suffering and Compassion: The Links Among Adverse Life Experiences, Empathy, Compassion, and Prosocial Behavior Daniel Lim and David DeSteno Online First Publication, January 11, 2016. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000144 CITATION Lim, D., & DeSteno, D. (2016, January 11). Suffering and Compassion: The Links Among Adverse Life Experiences, Empathy, Compassion, and Prosocial Behavior. Emotion. Advance online publication. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000144 Emotion © 2016 American Psychological Association 2016, Vol. 15, No. 6, 000 1528-3542/16/$12.00 http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000144 Suffering and Compassion: The Links Among Adverse Life Experiences, Empathy, Compassion, and Prosocial Behavior Daniel Lim and David DeSteno Northeastern University Experiencing past adversity traditionally has been linked to negative life outcomes. However, emerging evidence suggests that heterogeneity exists with respect to links between adversity and resilience, with adversity often enhancing cooperation in the face of joint suffering. Here, the authors present 2 studies designed to examine if the severity of past adversity is associated with an enduring propensity for empathy-mediated compassion, and, if so, whether the resulting compassion directly is, in turn, linked to behavior meant to relieve the suffering of others. Using both MTurk and laboratory-based paradigms, the authors find that increasing severity of past adversity predicts increased empathy, which in turn, is linked to a stable tendency to feel compassion for others in need. In addition, they demonstrate that the resulting individual differences in compassion appear to engender behavioral responses meant to assist others (i.e., charitable giving, helping a stranger). Keywords: compassion, empathy, prosocial behavior, suffering, life adversity Supplemental materials: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/emo0000144.supp Is past suffering associated with hardened hearts or warmed nized to inhibit adaptive social functioning, including major de- ones? Answering this question is of central import for two reasons.
    [Show full text]
  • The Origins and Transformation of Emotional Pain Version 3.2 by Hans Welling & Neta Ofer
    The Origins and Transformation of Emotional Pain version 3.2 by Hans Welling & Neta Ofer (Hans Welling, Integra, Lisbon, Portugal) (Neta Ofer, The new school of psychotherapy of the IDC and Geha hospital for mental health, Herzelia, Israel) Abstract The authors present an integrative model of emotional pain derived from their practice with AEDP (Fosha, 2000). The model is centered around 3 different types of pain: core emotional pain; relational pain, and self-pain. The authors show how each pain originates from 3 different innate fears and how triggering the emotional memory of such pain is central in psychological suffering. They argue that correct conceptualization is essential to work with the right transformational process, as healing is achieved for each type of emotional pain by 3 corresponding distinct processes. 1. For transforming core emotional pain: regulation of emotion by undoing aloneness and processing emotions and action tendencies to completion. 2. For transforming relational pain: validation of needs and promoting connection through expression and reception. 3. For transforming self-pain: undoing shame by compassion and the re-assessment and integration of parts that were disowned as unworthy. The authors argue that patients generally have all 3 kinds of pain, and an ongoing conceptualization can be very helpful to identify what kind of pain is active in the session, to understand what healing process is required at each moment. 1. Introduction We consider emotional pain as one of the most important sources of human suffering, and we view psychotherapy as the fundamental method for transforming this emotional pain and opening the door to wellbeing and thriving.
    [Show full text]
  • Animal Sentience and Emotions
    Animal Sentience and Emotions: The Argument for Universal Acceptance © iStock.com/TheImaginaryDuck © iStock.com/Eriko Hume © iStock.com/Eriko © iStock.com/global_explorer © iStock.com/Delpixart Prepared by Ingrid L. Taylor, D.V.M. Research Associate, Laboratory Investigations Department 1 People for the Ethical Treatment of Animals | 2021 © iStock.com/Jérémy Stenuit than “learning and memory.”4 Thus, in observations of Introduction animal behavior, descriptive labels that did not attribute any intentionality were acceptable. Noted primatologist Frans de Though the fact of animal sentience is implicit in Waal describes how, when he observed the way chimpanzees experimentation,1 researchers have traditionally downplayed would reconcile with a kiss after a fight, he was pressured to and ignored certain aspects of it, and in nonvertebrate use the phrase “postconflict reunions with mouth-to-mouth species they have often denied it altogether. While it is contact” rather than the terms “reconciliation” and “kiss.” established that vertebrate animals feel pain and respond to He goes on to state that for three decades in primatology pain drugs in much the same ways that humans do,2 emotions research, simpler explanations had to be systematically such as joy, happiness, suffering, empathy, and fear have countered before the term “reconciliation” was accepted often been ignored, despite the fact that many psychological in situations in which primates quite obviously “monitored and behavioral experiments are predicated on the assumption and repaired social relationships.”5 De Waal notes that this that animals feel these emotions and will consistently react dependence on descriptive labels, i.e., that animals can be based on these feelings.
    [Show full text]
  • Sustained Experience of Emotion After Loss of Memory in Patients with Amnesia
    Sustained experience of emotion after loss of memory in patients with amnesia Justin S. Feinsteina,b,1, Melissa C. Duffa,c, and Daniel Tranela,b aDivision of Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Neurology, University of Iowa College of Medicine, bDepartment of Psychology, and cDepartment of Communication Sciences and Disorders, University of Iowa, Iowa City, IA 52242-1053 Edited* by Marcus E. Raichle, Washington University, St. Louis, MO, and approved March 17, 2010 (received for review December 9, 2009) Can the experience of an emotion persist once the memory for what central feature of these patients’ condition is a profound induced the emotion has been forgotten? We capitalized on a rare impairment in forming new conscious memories about events that opportunity to study this question directly using a select group of unfold in their daily lives. This severe memory deficit confers a patients with severe amnesia following circumscribed bilateral unique opportunity to investigate whether the experience of an damage to the hippocampus. The amnesic patients underwent a emotion can outlast the memory for what caused it. sadness induction procedure (using affectively-laden film clips) to The dissociation between emotion and memory in patients with ascertain whether their experience of sadness would persist beyond amnesia harkens back to 1911, when the Swiss neurologist Cla- their memory for the sadness-inducing films. The experimentshowed parède concealed a pin between his fingers while greeting one of his that the patients continued to experience elevated levels of sadness amnesic patients with a handshake (18). The sharp pin surprised the well beyond the point in time at which they had lost factual memory patient and elicited a small amount of pain that quickly dissipated.
    [Show full text]