Frey & Detterman 2004

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Frey & Detterman 2004 PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Scholastic Assessment or g? The Relationship Between the Scholastic Assessment Test and General Cognitive Ability Meredith C. Frey and Douglas K. Detterman Case Western Reserve University ABSTRACT—There is little evidence showing the relationship be- Testing Service (ETS), the group that administers the SAT along with tween the Scholastic Assessment Test (SAT) and g (general in- countless other standardized tests for the College Board, began a telligence). This research established the relationship between program of validity research with the SAT, known as the Validity Study SAT and g, as well as the appropriateness of the SAT as a Service (VSS). This service, offered free to interested colleges, com- measure of g, and examined the SAT as a premorbid measure of putes the average multiple correlation between three predictor vari- intelligence. In Study 1, we used the National Longitudinal ables (SAT math score, SAT verbal score, and high school record) and Survey of Youth 1979. Measures of g were extracted from the grade point average (GPA) among college freshmen. Between the years Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery and correlated with 1964 and 1985, the average correlation between SAT score and SAT scores of 917 participants. The resulting correlation was freshman GPA was in the range from .38 to .46 (Morgan, 1990). De- .82 (.86 corrected for nonlinearity). Study 2 investigated the spite this history of analysis, and the knowledge that g is one of the correlation between revised and recentered SAT scores and most reliable predictors of academic achievement, ETS refuses to cite scores on the Raven’s Advanced Progressive Matrices among any SAT-IQ correlations (Seligman, 1994). The College Board presi- 104 undergraduates. The resulting correlation was .483 (.72 dent, Gaston Caperton, was recently quoted in an on-line publication corrected for restricted range). These studies indicate that the of U.S. News & World Report as admitting that ‘‘in its original form [the SAT is mainly a test of g. We provide equations for converting SAT] was an IQ test’’ (Barnes, 2002). However, the people responsible SAT scores to estimated IQs; such conversion could be useful for for developing and administering the SAT insist that, once logic items estimating premorbid IQ or conducting individual difference were replaced by reading comprehension in 1946, the SAT ceased to research with college students. measure intelligence (Barnes, 2002). Rather, the College Board cur- rently asserts that the SAT measures reasoning ability, which they hold as different from intelligence. The relationship between academic achievement and general intelli- The unusual distinction made by ETS and the College Board not- gence, or g, is well established in the field of intelligence (Jensen, withstanding, some researchers have voiced support for the SAT as a 1998). One particular test of academic achievement, however, is de facto intelligence test. In his 1994 book, A Question of Intelligence, conspicuously absent from these studies. The SAT (once called the Seligman noted, ‘‘Henry Chauncey, a former president of ETS has Scholastic Aptitude Test, and now the Scholastic Assessment Test), an been quoted (in Klitgaard, Choosing Elites, 1985, p. 92) as stating that exam that generations of high school juniors and seniors have ago- the SAT is essentially an intelligence test’’ (pp. 206–207). In fact, the nized over and that has spawned innumerable preparatory books and author of the original SAT, Carl Campbell Brigham, developed his test classes, has not, surprisingly enough, been frequently analyzed in from Army intelligence tests to more accurately differentiate among conjunction with measures of general cognitive ability. individuals at the upper end of the scale (Lemann, 1999). Although That is not to say that there is a lack of research in general on the the College Board asserts that the test is substantially different from SAT. In fact, to the contrary, the SAT’s relation to college admissions its original form, Lemann asserted that, other than having its language and achievement has been the focus of much research. A study of updated, the SAT has changed little since its original version. 25,000 applicants at nine private colleges yielded unsurprising re- Furthermore, empirical evidence suggests a substantial relationship sults: Two thirds of the variance in predicting admissions decisions between the SAT and g. In a study of 339 undergraduates, Brodnick could be accounted for by academic factors, split equally between and Ree (1995) used covariance structure modeling to examine the high school record and admissions test scores, including SAT scores relationship between psychometric g, socioeconomic variables, and (Willingham & Breland, 1982). Furthermore, in 1964, the Educational achievement-test scores. They found substantial general-factor load- ings on both the math (.698) and the verbal (.804) SAT subtests. Address correspondence to D.K. Detterman, Department of Psy- Because they used the SAT to develop their measure of g, it is not chology, Case Western Reserve University, Cleveland, OH 44106; clear if this general factor is the same as that obtained from standard e-mail: [email protected]. intelligence tests. If the general factors are indeed the same, then the Volume 15—Number 6 Copyright r 2004 American Psychological Society 373 Scholastic Assessment Test and g SAT may have been overlooked as a potentially useful measure of TABLE 1 general cognitive functioning. Factor Loadings From the Factor Analysis of the Armed It would be of considerable benefit for researchers and clinicians to Services Vocational Aptitude Battery have an equation for estimating an individual’s IQ from an objective measure such as the SAT. In cases of suspected cognitive decline due Subtest Factor 1 Factor 2 to injury or illness, clinicians currently use a series of demographic General Science .881 À.116 variables to predict their clients’ premorbid IQ. For many of these Arithmetic Reasoning .858 .045 individuals, the SAT may be the only objective measure of premorbid Word Knowledge .885 .071 functioning. The method of predicting IQ with demographic variables Paragraph Comprehension .825 .175 carries with it a standard error of prediction (SEp) of 11.4 (Karzmark, Numerical Operations .724 .426 Heaton, Grant, & Matthews, 1985). It appears that there is ample room Coding Speed .657 .417 for improving the accuracy with which clinicians currently estimate Auto and Shop Information .727 À.425 premorbid IQ. Mathematics Knowledge .800 .129 À In addition to the obvious clinical application, there is a substantial Mechanical Comprehension .794 .320 À benefit to researchers’ use of a regression equation to estimate IQ. Electronics Information .829 .320 Much research in the field of psychology is carried out on under- graduate volunteers. Although there has been a recent push by the the ASVAB are shown in Table 1. They indicate a substantial loading University of California system to discontinue using SAT scores as an of all subtests of the ASVAB on a first factor, g. admission criterion, many colleges and universities continue to re- ASVAB first-factor scores were transformed to an IQ scale using the quire the SAT for admission (Barnes, 2002). Consequently, there is a following equation: IQ 5 (z à 15)1100. Finally, the IQ scores derived vast pool of potential subjects who already have a good measure of from the ASVAB were correlated with SAT scores for the 917 re- their general cognitive functioning in their transcripts. If a researcher spondents who had scores on both measures. Simple correlations could estimate IQ with sufficient accuracy from the scores on record, it between both SAT scores and ASVAB IQ scores and scores on the would eliminate hours of additional testing in cases in which measures small-N intelligence tests were also analyzed. of general intelligence are required. To these ends, we examined the relationship between the SAT and general cognitive ability in two studies. We also developed regression equations to estimate IQ from Results the SAT. Of the 11,878 subjects who had scores on all ASVAB subtests, 917 also took the SAT and were the primary focus in this study. There was a significant correlation between SAT and ASVAB IQ (r 5 .820, STUDY 1 p < .001). However, when a scatter plot of the relationship was examined (see Fig. 1a), a nonlinear relationship was evident. To correct for Method nonlinearity, we added the squared SAT score (R5.857, p < .001) to Participants the regression. A cubic component of SAT also added significantly to 5 Data were extracted from the National Longitudinal Survey of Youth the prediction of IQ (p .011), but the added variance was so small 1979 (NLSY79) data set. The NLSY79 contained a national proba- that we decided to not include it in the regression equation. Figure 1b bility sample of civilian and military subjects aged 14 to 21, with an presents a scatter plot of IQ predicted from a regression equation with overrepresentation of certain minority groups. A total of 12,686 men the squared and cubic components of SAT versus IQ obtained from the and women participated in the NLSY79. first factor of the ASVAB. Entering math and verbal subtest scores into the regression separately, as opposed to using a composite score, did not improve the prediction of IQ. Procedure The simple correlations between SAT and IQ (or whatever measure From the NLSY79 data set, we extracted measures of performance on was reported for the IQ test) for small-N intelligence tests ranged from the Armed Services Vocational Aptitude Battery (ASVAB) and the .53 to .82 for participants who had taken the SAT and also had another SAT (known as the Scholastic Aptitude Test at that time), as well as a IQ test score reported in their school records. All simple correlations number of intelligence tests that did not have scores available for as between tests are summarized in Table 2.
Recommended publications
  • Psychometric G: Definition and Substantiation
    Psychometric g: Definition and Substantiation Arthur R. Jensen University of Culifornia, Berkeley The construct known as psychometric g is arguably the most important construct in all of psychology largelybecause of its ubiquitous presence in all tests of mental ability and its wide-ranging predictive validity for a great many socially significant variables, including scholastic performance and intellectual attainments, occupational status, job performance, in- come, law abidingness, and welfare dependency. Even such nonintellec- tual variables as myopia, general health, and longevity, as well as many other physical traits, are positively related to g. Of course, the causal con- nections in the whole nexus of the many diverse phenomena involving the g factor is highly complex. Indeed, g and its ramifications cut across the behavioral sciences-brainphysiology, psychology, sociology-perhaps more than any other scientific construct. THE DOMAIN OF g THEORY It is important to keep in mind the distinction between intelligence and g, as these terms are used here. The psychology of intelligence could, at least in theory, be based on the study of one person,just as Ebbinghaus discov- ered some of the laws of learning and memory in experimentswith N = 1, using himself as his experimental subject. Intelligence is an open-ended category for all those mental processes we view as cognitive, such as stimu- lus apprehension, perception, attention, discrimination, generalization, 39 40 JENSEN learning and learning-set acquisition, short-term and long-term memory, inference, thinking, relation eduction, inductive and deductive reasoning, insight, problem solving, and language. The g factor is something else. It could never have been discovered with N = 1, because it reflects individual di,fferences in performance on tests or tasks that involve anyone or moreof the kinds of processes just referred to as intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • Does a Fitness Factor Contribute to the Association Between Intelligence
    ARTICLE IN PRESS INTELL-00516; No of Pages 11 Intelligence xxx (2009) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Intelligence journal homepage: Does a fitness factor contribute to the association between intelligence and health outcomes? Evidence from medical abnormality counts among 3654 US Veterans Rosalind Arden a,⁎, Linda S. Gottfredson b, Geoffrey Miller c a Social, Genetic, Developmental and Psychiatry Centre, Institute of Psychiatry, King's College London, London SE5 8AF, United Kingdom b School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA c Psychology Department, Logan Hall 160, University of New Mexico, MSC03 2220 Albuquerque, NM 87131-1161, USA article info abstract Available online xxxx We suggest that an over-arching ‘fitness factor’ (an index of general genetic quality that predicts survival and reproductive success) partially explains the observed associations between health Keywords: outcomes and intelligence. As a proof of concept, we tested this idea in a sample of 3654 US Fitness Vietnam veterans aged 31–49 who completed five cognitive tests (from which we extracted a g Intelligence factor), a detailed medical examination, and self-reports concerning lifestyle health risks (such Cognitive epidemiology as smoking and drinking). As indices of physical health, we aggregated ‘abnormality counts’ of Health physician-assessed neurological, morphological, and physiological abnormalities in eight Mutation load categories: cranial nerves, motor nerves, peripheral sensory nerves, reflexes, head, body, skin condition, and urine tests. Since each abnormality was rare, the abnormality counts showed highly skewed, Poisson-like distributions. The correlation matrix amongst these eight abnormality counts formed only a weak positive manifold and thus yielded only a weak common factor.
    [Show full text]
  • UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society
    UC Merced Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society Title Dynamical cognitive models and the study of individual differences. Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4sn2v2j0 Journal Proceedings of the Annual Meeting of the Cognitive Science Society, 31(31) ISSN 1069-7977 Authors Kan, Kees Jan Van Der Maas, Han L.J. Publication Date 2009 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Dynamical cognitive models and the study of individual differences Han van der Maas & Kees Jan Kan A large part of psychology concerns the study of individual differences. Why do people differ in personality? What is the structure of individual differences in intelligence? What are the roles of nurture and nature? Researchers in these fields collect data of many subjects and apply statistical methods, most notably latent structure modeling, to uncover the structure and to infer the underlying sources of the individual differences. Cognitive science usually does not concern individual differences. In cognitive models we focus on the general mechanisms of cognitive processes and not the individual properties. We believe that these two traditions of modeling cannot remain separated. Models of mechanisms necessarily precede models of individual differences. We argue against the use of latent structure models of individual differences in psychological processes that do not explicate the underlying mechanisms. Our main example is general intelligence, a concept based on the analysis of group data. Scores on cognitive tasks used in intelligence tests correlate positively with each other, i.e., they display a positive manifold of correlations. The positive manifold is arguably both the best established, and the most striking phenomenon in the psychological study of intelligence.
    [Show full text]
  • The Ravages of Reification: Considering the Iceberg and Cultural Intelligence, Towards De-Reifying Intercultural Competence
    The Ravages of Reification: Considering the Iceberg and Cultural Intelligence, Towards De-reifying Intercultural Competence Keynote Presentation for FILE IV, Colle Val d’Elsa, Sept. 28, 2013. Milton J. Bennett, Ph.D. Intercultural Development Research Institute What I'd like to do with you today is to extend a conversation begun with a couple of blogs I have published recently, adding a specific application to the topic of assessing intercultural competence. One of blogs that I wrote in June held that was time to retire the iceberg as a metaphor for culture. There were dozens of responses to that blog, from far more people than I thought would be reading the blog. This led to a very interesting conversation that I think is related to intercultural competence. The other blog is one I wrote comparing intercultural competence to the idea of intelligence, particularly its measurement as IQ. The issue around both blogs is that of "reification" (I'll give you the definition in a moment) and it is also the issue I believe we are dealing with in trying to talk about the assessment of intercultural competence. Basically, we need to "de- reify" the ideas of culture and intercultural competence back to some original root definition that would allow us to reestablish a more coherent approach to assessment. To put this talk in cultural terms, I'll be taking a more European than American approach. Americans tend to start out very optimistic about everything and in the end they do a little criticism. Europeans tend to do the reverse; they start with being very critical and sometimes end up with constructive suggestions.
    [Show full text]
  • Psychometric G As a Focus of Concerted Research Effort
    INTELLIGENCE 11, 193-198 (1987) EDITORIAL: Psychometric g as a Focus of Concerted Research Effort Arthur R. Jensen University of California, Berkeley "Intelligence" has been tried and apparently it has failed. Anyone who reads the recent book What is Intelligence? Contemporary View- points on its Nature and Definition, edited by Sternberg and Detterman (1986), in which 25 experts responded to the question posed in the title, could easily conclude that there are about as many different conceptions of "intelligence" as the number of experts. This was also true back in 1921 when the same question was asked of an earlier group of experts. Comparing the two symposia, separated by 65 years, we find scarcely more consensus among the experts today than in 1921. Shouldn't this be cause for dismay to us who work in this field? Is there something wrong with our science, that there is so little agreement, even about what are the key questions, much less the answers? One of the intellectually appealing features of progress in the more advanced empirical sciences is the increasing consensus among scientists regarding the definition and nature of the main phenomena within their purview. Investigation of some coherent body of phenomena leads to more agreement about the crucial questions and problems. True, there is always a frontier of unsettled issues and controversies in science; but with consolidation of the gains of research, there is also a growing consensus. The history of the physical and biological sciences indicates that generally, as a topic develops, an increasing majority of re- searchers acknowledge the key phenomena that call for more intensive investiga- tion.
    [Show full text]
  • The Evolution of General Intelligence
    To be published in Behavioral and Brain Sciences (in press) © Cambridge University Press 2016 Below is an unedited, uncorrected BBS Target Article recently accepted for publication. This preprint has been prepared specifically for potential commentators who wish to nominate themselves for formal commentary invitation via Editorial Manager: http://bbs.edmgr.com/. The Commentary Proposal Instructions can be accessed here: http://journals.cambridge.org/action/displaySpecialPage?pageId=5544 Please DO NOT write a commentary unless you receive a formal email invitation from the Editors. If you are invited to submit a commentary, a copyedited, corrected version of this paper will be made available. The evolution of general intelligence Judith M. Burkart, Michèle N. Schubiger, Carel P. van Schaik Anthropological Institute and Museum, Winterthurerstrasse 190, CH-8057 Zurich Corresponding author: [email protected] Abstract: The presence of general intelligence poses a major evolutionary puzzle, which has led to increased interest in its presence in nonhuman animals. The aim of this review is to critically evaluate this puzzle, and to explore the implications for current theories about the evolution of cognition. We first review domain-general and domain-specific accounts of human cognition in order to situate attempts to identify general intelligence in nonhuman animals. Recent studies are consistent with the presence of general intelligence in mammals (rodents and primates). However, the interpretation of a psychometric g-factor as general intelligence needs to be validated, in particular in primates, and we propose a range of such tests. We then evaluate the implications of general intelligence in nonhuman animals for current theories about its evolution and find support for the cultural intelligence approach, which stresses the critical importance of social inputs during the ontogenetic construction of survival-relevant skills.
    [Show full text]
  • Hans Eysenck's Theory of Intelligence, and What It Reveals About Him
    Personality and Individual Differences 103 (2016) 116–127 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Hans Eysenck's theory of intelligence, and what it reveals about him Linda S. Gottfredson School of Education, University of Delaware, Newark, DE 19716, USA article info abstract Article history: Hans Eysenck was a highly analytical, objective, independent-minded experimentalist. He personified the biolog- Received 18 March 2016 ical perspective of the Galton–Spearman ‘London School of Psychology’, which he led for many decades. His first Accepted 6 April 2016 (1939) and last publications (1998) were on intelligence. Returning to the topic in the 1960s, he formulated, tested, and promulgated the theory that general intelligence (g) is a biological phenomenon with broad social Keywords: consequences. I examine the status of Eysenck's theory, advances in the field, and social reactions to them during Hans J. Eysenck – – London School of Psychology the 1960s 1970s, 1980s 1990s, and since 2000. My perspective is that of a sociologist who, in testing alternative Intelligence theories of social inequality, was drawn inexorably into the intelligence literature, policy debates over fairness in g factor employee selection, and first-hand observation of the sort of controversies he experienced. Eysenck's 1979 and Intelligence theory 1998 textbooks on intelligence mark developments in his theory and supporting evidence during the first two Biological psychology periods. They exhibit considerable knowledge about the philosophy and history of science, and the nature of sci- Scientificcontroversy entific controversy. Advances in intelligence since 2000, in particular, from neuroimaging and molecular genetics, Social inequality vindicate his biological perspective.
    [Show full text]
  • MILL Iy Section
    impacts Genetics and general cognitive ability Robert Plomin General cognitive ability (g), often referred to as ‘general intelligence’, predicts social outcomes such as educational and occupational levels far better than any other behavioural trait. g is one of the most heritable behavioural traits, and genes that contribute to the heritability of g will certainly be identified. What are the scientific and social implications of finding genes associated with g? uring the past three decades, the behavioural sciences have emerged Dfrom an era of strict environmental explanations for differences in behaviour to a more balanced view that recognizes the importance of nature (genetics) as well as nurture (environment). This shift occurred first for behavioural disorders, including rare disorders such as autism (which has an incidence of 1 per 1,000 population), more common disorders such as schizophrenia (1 in 100), and very common disorders such as reading disability (1 in 50). More recently, it has become increasingly accepted that genetic variation makes an important con- tribution to differences among individuals in the normal range of behaviour as well as for abnormal behaviour. Moreover, many behavioural disorders, especially common ones, may represent variation at the extremes of the same genetic and environ- mental factors that are responsible for varia- Figure 1 Quantitative trait locus (QTL) perspective on complex traits. Differences among individuals tion within the normal range. For example, for most quantitative or complex traits such as reading ability are distributed as a normal bell-shaped disorders such as reading disability may not curve. Multiple genes influence complex traits as probabilistic propensities rather than be due to genetic variants that specifically predetermined programmes.
    [Show full text]
  • Volume 22 Spring 2021 the Journal of Undergraduate Research In
    The Journal of Undergraduate Research Volume 22 in Natural Sciences and Mathematics Spring 2021 The Journal of Undergraduate Research in Natural Sciences and Mathematics Volume 22 | Spring 2021 Marks of a CSUF graduate from the College of Natural Sciences and Mathematics GRADUATES FROM THE COLLEGE OF NATURAL SCIENCES AND MATHEMATICS: • Understand the basic concepts and principles of science and mathematics. • Are experienced in working collectively and collaborating to solve problems. • Communicate both orally and in writing with clarity, precision, and confidence. • Are adept at using computers to do word processing, prepare spreadsheets and graphs, and use presentation software. • Possess skills in information retrieval using library resources and the internet. • Have extensive laboratory, workshop, and field experience where they utilize the scientific method to ask questions, formulate hypotheses, design and conduct experiments, and analyze data. • Appreciate diverse cultures as a result of working side by side with many people in collaborative efforts in the classroom, laboratory, and on research projects. • Have had the opportunity to work individually with faculty members in conducting research and independent projects, often leading to the generation of original data and contributing to the research knowledge base. • Are capable of working with modern equipment, instrumentation, and techniques. 4 DIMENSIONS DIMENSIONS: The Journal of Undergraduate Research in Natural Sciences and Mathematics is an official publication of California State University, Fullerton. DIMENSIONS is published annually by CSUF, 800 N. State College Blvd., Fullerton, CA 92834. Copyright ©2021 CSUF. Except as otherwise provided, DIMENSIONS grants permission for material in this publication to be copied for use by non-profit educational institutions for scholarly or instructional purposes only, provided that 1) copies are distributed at or below cost, 2) the author and DIMENSIONS are identified, and 3) proper notice of copyright appears on each copy.
    [Show full text]
  • AP Psychology Ch. 8 Study Guide – Cognition: Thinking, Intelligence and Language
    AP Psychology Ch. 8 Study Guide – Cognition: Thinking, Intelligence and Language 1. Not being able to notice that pliers could be used as a weight when tied to a string and used to swing the string toward you like a pendulum is an example of: _____________ 2. Define functional fixedness. 3. The tendency for people to persist in using problem-solving patterns that have worked for them in the past is known as ________________________. 4. Define intelligence. 5. The "g" in Spearman's g factor of intelligence stands for ______________________. 6. Know Gardner’s 9 multiple intelligences 7. Sternberg has found that _______________________ intelligence is a good predictor of success in life but has a low relationship to _______________________ intelligence. 8. What three types of intelligence constitute Sternberg's triarchic theory of intelligence? 9. If a test consistently produces the same score when administered to the same person under identical conditions, that test can be said to be high in ______________________. 10. Which aspects are included in the definition of intellectual disability? 11. Brush up on the nature vs. nurture debate. 12. Define language. 13. The basic units of sound are called ______________________. 14. What is syntax? 15. If someone's IQ score is in the 97th percentile, what is their IQ likely to be? 16. The tendency of young children learning language to overuse the rules of syntax is referred to as 17. Tristan, a seven-year-old male, has an IQ of 128. Tristan's mental age is ______? 18. Alfred Binet was primarily concerned with (what did his work consist of)? 19.
    [Show full text]
  • The G-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: the Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations
    European Journal of Personality Eur. J. Pers. 21: 667–706 (2007) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.634 The g-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: The Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations HEINER RINDERMANN* Institute of Psychology, Otto-von-Guericke-University Magdeburg, Germany Abstract International cognitive ability and achievement comparisons stem from different research traditions. But analyses at the interindividual data level show that they share a common positive manifold. Correlations of national ability means are even higher to very high (within student assessment studies, r ¼ .60–.98; between different student assessment studies [PISA-sum with TIMSS-sum] r ¼ .82–.83; student assessment sum with intelligence tests, r ¼ .85–.86). Results of factor analyses indicate a strong g-factor of differences between nations (variance explained by the first unrotated factor: 94–95%). Causes of the high correlations are seen in the similarities of tests within studies, in the similarities of the cognitive demands for tasks from different tests, and in the common developmental factors at the individual and national levels including known environmental and unknown genetic influences. Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Key words: intelligence; knowledge; education; student assessment; educational achievement; OECD; IEA INTRODUCTION The past century of empirical research on cognitive abilities has convincingly demonstrated their relevance for both individuals and societies across a wide variety of criteria. For individuals, both IQ (Schmidt & Hunter, 2004) and literacy (OECD, 2000) have been linked with job performance, longevity (Gottfredson & Deary, 2004; OECD, 2000), and low criminality (Herrnstein & Murray, 1994).
    [Show full text]
  • The G-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: the Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations’ by Heiner Rindermann
    European Journal of Personality Eur. J. Pers. 21: 707–765 (2007) Published online in Wiley InterScience (www.interscience.wiley.com) DOI: 10.1002/per.648 Discussion on ’The g-Factor of International Cognitive Ability Comparisons: The Homogeneity of Results in PISA, TIMSS, PIRLS and IQ-Tests Across Nations’ by Heiner Rindermann OPEN PEER COMMENTARY Geographical Distribution of Mental Abilities and Its Moral Consequences JU¨ RI ALLIK Department of Psychology, University of Tartu, Estonia [email protected] Abstract Rindermann’s study provides the most comprehensive evidence so far that national scores of school assessment have systematic differences and the geographical distribution of these differences almost perfectly repeat the distribution of the mean national scores of intelligence. It is argued that without comparison with the random effects of statistical aggregation it is impossible to decide whether additional factors are needed to explain the strong association between national scores of school assessment and intelligence tests. The ignorance about real differences in mental abilities may become a source of social injustice because this does not allow natural inequalities to be arranged such that they are to the greatest benefit of the least advantageous. Copyright # 2007 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. Rindermann (this issue) provides the most comprehensive evidence so far that national scores of school assessment have systematic differences and the geographical distribution of these differences almost perfectly repeat the distribution of the mean national scores of intelligence. By analogy with many previous controversial discoveries, it is predictable that the first most typical reaction would be denial. Indeed, many critics are not able to tolerate the idea that the mean level of intelligence could systematically vary across countries and world regions.
    [Show full text]