Quick viewing(Text Mode)

Springer International Publishing AG, Part of Springer Nature 2018 J

Springer International Publishing AG, Part of Springer Nature 2018 J

L

Lagomorpha Navigation to guide their movements in the landscape and the timing thereof. Anthony Caravaggi The order is comprised of two School of Biological Earth and Environmental families, the and ( or Sciences, University College Cork, Cork, Ireland leporids) and the (Ochotonidae). The navi- School of Biological Sciences, Medical Biology gational capabilities of lagomorphs are poorly Centre, Queen’s University Belfast, Belfast, UK understood, with the few movement studies to have been conducted focusing on dispersal, home range behavior, foraging, and habitat use. Synonyms Indeed, movement behavior has been entirely unstudied in most lagomorph species. Studying Behavior; ; Movement; ; the navigational capabilities of small-to-medium- sized can be challenging as the equip- ment required for fine-scale data collection (e.g., GPS collars) are often too heavy and/or have Definition limited battery life. For example, the maximum weight for GPS collars is defined by the size of the – Navigation the theory and practice of charting a subject. This is known as the “5% rule” (Cochran course to a remote goal (Schöne 1984). 1980); the weight of the collar should not exceed 5% of the focal ’s mass. For a very large hare weighing 5 kg, that would equate to a GPS Introduction collar weighing 250 g. The typical battery life for such a collar is around 500 days, though that is The daily movements of are governed by based on the recording of only a handful of “fixes” many factors including the need to find food, (i.e., records of the individual’s location) per day – water, conspecifics, and favorable environmental not enough for nuanced analysis of movement conditions while trying to avoid predators, domi- behavior such as navigational strategies nant ecological competitors, and unfavorable hab- (Handcock et al. 2009). The frequency of fixes itats and conditions. Animals use external cues required for such studies would exhaust the bat- such as odor (Gagliardo 2013), landmarks tery in only a few days. However, technological (Baader 1996), day length (Baoping et al. 2009), advances such as the use of micro-GPS and snap- the position of the stars (Dacke et al. 2013), and shot receivers (McMahon et al. 2017), as well as the Earth’s magnetic field (Chernetsov et al. 2017) increasingly lightweight collars and improved

# Springer International Publishing AG, part of Springer Nature 2018 J. Vonk, T. K. Shackelford (eds.), Encyclopedia of Animal Cognition and Behavior, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-3-319-47829-6_1164-1 2 Lagomorpha Navigation batteries, mean that many obstacles are slowly population dynamics. The benefits conferred by being overcome. It is likely that future technolog- individuals moving within and between ical improvements and dedicated research will populations and into previously uncolonized hab- lead to a greater understanding of movement itats are such that dispersal is considered to under- behavior and navigational strategies, particularly pin the persistence of many species (Avril et al. in larger lagomorphs. 2011). The causes of dispersal are poorly under- Given the lack of navigation-specific literature, stood, though it is likely that inbreeding avoid- here I broadly summarize research on movement ance, mate competition, resource availability, and behavior, particularly in the context of home the density of conspecifics all play a role. The ranges, habitat use, and dispersal, in each group availability of cover vegetation may be a particu- of lagomorphs. larly important factor in determining the eventual settlement locations of dispersing females (Avril Hares et al. 2011). Dispersal is ubiquitous and common Hares (genus Lepus; 32 species) are crepuscular/ among hares, though breeding dispersal occurs at nocturnal open-habitat specialists with long hind lower frequencies than natal dispersal. Moreover, legs which allow them to flee from potential pred- the proportion of young which disperse from the ators at high speeds. They are poorly adapted for a natal range, and the distances travelled, varies digging and rest during that day in above ground between and within species. However, movement depressions, known as forms. As they are not behavior during dispersal can give clues to navi- reliant on burrows for shelter and protection, gational capabilities. For example, dispersing lev- hares generally range over much wider areas erets have been observed to use direct long- than most rabbits and certainly all pikas (see distance exploratory movements over 1 km before “▶ Lagomorpha Life History”, Table 1). How- returning to the same resting location, over several ever, all hares and their offspring (“leverets”) days (Reid and Harrison 2010). This return behav- make use of emergent vegetation for concealment. ior suggests a capacity for accurate navigation on For example, the Irish hare (L. timidus a landscape scale. hibernicus), a subspecies of the mountain hare The locomotor activity of hares often varies (L. timidus), favors open-field habitats seasonally, with an increase in activity particularly (Dingerkus and Montgomery 2002) but is also around the start of the breeding season. During associated with rough grassland which offers this time, individuals will move from their home potential cover in the form of stands of rushes ranges in search of mating opportunities. It is (Juncus sp.; Caravaggi et al. 2015). Similarly, possible, perhaps even likely, that olfaction plays woolly hares (L. oiostolus) favor montane grass- a role in the location of mates, though olfactory lands for foraging but require intermediate shrub communication in hares is currently not a research habitats for cover (Lu 2011). Researchers study- priority. Locomotion may also be affected by per- ing movements of translocated European hares ceived predation risk (i.e., lunar illumination) and (L. europaeus) found that, rather than establishing local predator density and abundance (Rogowitz home ranges at release sites, the translocated indi- 1997); hares spend more time moving between viduals moved to find better habitats which were habitat patches, reducing stationary foraging more similar to those from which they had been time in each patch. removed (Ferretti et al. 2010). Hares will also recurrently use a network of paths between forag- Rabbits ing and/or resting sites (Caravaggi et al. 2016). There are ten genera of rabbits (Brachylagus, Dispersal, i.e., the movement of young out of Bunolagus, Capolagus, Nesolagus, Oryctolagus, the natal range (natal dispersal) or the movement Pentalagus, Poelagus, Pronolagus, Romerolagus, of adults to another breeding area (breeding dis- Sylvilagus), with a total of 29 species. Rabbits persal), is an important process with a key role in differ physically from hares in that they have Lagomorpha Navigation 3 comparatively shorter hind legs and shorter ears idahoensis) found that females dispersed up to and are physically smaller (▶ Lagomorpha Life three times further than males (Estes-Zumpf History, Table 1). Old World rabbits (all genera and Rachlow 2009). Dispersal distance may be except for Sylvilagus) are diggers, creating under- related to population density (e.g., European ground holes, burrows, and communal warrens in rabbit) (Richardson et al. 2002), with long- which to rest, avoid threats (e.g., predation, poor distance movements being more common in weather), and raise their young (known as kits or low-density populations. kittens). In contrast, new world rabbits (Sylvilagus sp.) will either utilize holes or burrows dug by Pikas other species or raise their young in aboveground There are 30 species of pika, all of which are forms. The movement behavior of rabbits is classified within a single genus, Ochotona. Pikas poorly understood, with substantial knowledge are the smallest of the lagomorphs gaps in all aspects. (▶ Lagomorpha Life History, Table 1), with Rabbits choose their home ranges with refer- rounded bodies and ears and no visible tail. ence to the vegetative composition of the land- Pikas are broadly split into two groups: (i) rock- scape, preferring areas with an abundance of or talus-dwelling species and (ii) species of grass- vegetation which could be used for cover. For land or steppe habitats. The two groups have example, researchers in Spain found that the war- different social behaviors, though they do not rens of European rabbits (O. cuniculus) were differ in life history or ecology. As with other strongly associated with the Retama mono- leporids, there is little information available on sperma, a dense flowering bush (Dellafiore et al. their movement behavior and navigational 2008). Rabbits make use of cover vegetation to capabilities. provide shelter and concealment, often using the Steppe and grassland pikas are social, living in same paths through ostensibly safe habitats to often high-density groups. In contrast, talus pikas reach foraging areas in their home ranges. are largely solitary and highly territorial. Never- European rabbits also live in underground war- theless, the dispersal behavior of the two groups is rens which may span a considerable area and have similar. For example, both breeding and natal multiple entrances. Within their home range, dispersal distances of plateau pika (O. curzoniae; therefore, European rabbits have a number of a steppe species) and American pika (O. princeps; options which offer safety from potential threats. a talus-dwelling species) are often extremely Flight to places of concealment is usually direct, short, rarely extending beyond two family ranges suggesting that the animal is aware of its position (Dobson et al. 1998). However, genetic analyses relative to relevant landscape features. suggested that American pikas may occasionally The proportion of dispersing juveniles is often disperse up to 2 km from their natal range and that high, particularly in low-density populations, and, intermediate movements may be common as with many species, dispersing cohorts (Peacock 1997). Juvenile males are often the pri- are often male-biased. However, levels of dis- mary dispersers in plateau pikas, a pattern not persal may be similar between the sexes when observed in American pika. populations occur at high densities (Richardson The (re)colonization potential of pikas has et al. 2002). In many species, dispersing subadults been shown to be strongly influenced by the barely venture beyond their natal home range; degree of habitat patch connectivity, as well as males are often more likely than females to make patch quality (Franken and Hik 2004). However, long-distance movements. For example, one male while juveniles are more likely colonize neighbor- marsh rabbit (S. palustris) was recorded as having ing patches in fragmented habitats, dispersal rates dispersed over 2 km from its place of birth, com- are not necessarily compromised, with a mixture pared to most females which did not leave their of short- and long-distance dispersal events natal ranges (Forys and Humphrey 1996). In con- resulting in a genetically contiguous meta- trast, a study of pygmy rabbits (Brachylagus population (Peacock and Smith 1997). Thus, 4 Lagomorpha Navigation dispersing pikas likely use similar exploratory References behaviors to those found in other lagomorphs, facilitated by emergent vegetation and landscape Avril, A., Léonard, Y., Letty, J., Péroux, R., Guitton, J.-S., features (e.g., rocks). Researchers investigating & Pontier, D. (2011). Natal dispersal of European hare in a high-density population. Mammalian Biology – the social behavior of the northern pika Zeitschrift Für Säugetierkunde, 76(2), 148–156. (O. hyperborean) have shown that the species Baader, A. P. (1996). The significance of visual landmarks exhibits spatial awareness and recall, with indi- for navigation of the giant tropical ant, Paraponera viduals recurrently returning to the same shelter clavata (Formicidae, Ponerinae). Insectes Sociaux, 43(4), 435–450. and breeding pairs using multiple collective food Baoping, R., Ming, L., Yongcheng, L., & Fuwen, W. caches (Gilwicz et al. 2005). (2009). Influence of day length, ambient temperature, and seasonality on daily travel distance in the Yunnan snub-nosed monkey at Jinsichang, Yunnan, China. American Journal of Primatology, 71(3), 233–241. Conclusion Caravaggi, A., Montgomery, W. I., & Reid, N. (2015). Range expansion and comparative habitat use of insu- lar, congeneric lagomorphs: Invasive European hares Navigational abilities are common features Lepus europaeus and endemic Irish hares Lepus among animals. All lagomorphs exhibit behaviors timidus hibernicus. Biological Invasions, 17(2), which can be interpreted in terms of navigational 687–698. Caravaggi, A., Zaccaroni, M., Riga, F., Schai-Braun, S. C., skills, including foraging behavior and the utili- Dick, J. T. A., Montgomery, W. I., & Reid, N. (2016). zation of burrows and/or vegetation for cover. An invasive-native mammalian species replacement Therefore, it is reasonable to assume that lago- process captured by camera trap survey random morphs have long-term awareness and under- encounter models. Remote Sensing in Ecology and Conservation, 2(1), 45–58. standing of the composition of the habitats and Chernetsov, N., Pakhomov, A., Kobylkov, D., Kishkinev, microenvironments found within their home D., Holland, R. A., & Mouritsen, H. (2017). Migratory ranges. These cognitive maps (i.e., representa- Eurasian reed warblers can use magnetic declination to tions of an animal’s environment which are used solve the longitude problem. Current Biology, 27, 2647–2651. in decision-making; Tolman 1948) allow them to Cochran, W. (1980). Wildlife telemetry. In S. Schemnitz effectively navigate between foraging and resting (Ed.), Wildlife management techniques manual areas within their home ranges and beyond such (pp. 507–520). Washington: The Wildlife Society. as when making exploratory forays beyond the Dacke, M., Baird, E., Byrne, M., Scholtz, C. H., & War- rant, E. J. (2013). Dung beetles use the Milky Way for natal range. However, navigation and movement orientation. Current Biology, 23(4), 298–300. behavior are understudied in lagomorphs, and, Dellafiore, C. M., Gallego Fernández, J. B., & Vallés, S. M. hence, data are scarce. There is scope to expand (2008). Habitat use for warren building by European research in spatial cognition, e.g., mental map- rabbits (Oryctolagus cuniculus) in relation to landscape structure in a sand dune system. Acta Oecologica, ping, into lagomorphs, thus giving researchers 33(3), 372–379. greater insight into population dynamics and hab- Dingerkus, S. K., & Montgomery, W. I. (2002). A review itat utilization and requirements, as well as poten- of the status and decline in abundance of the Irish Hare tially benefitting conservation and management (Lepus timidus hibernicus) in Northern Ireland. Mam- mal Review, 32(1), 1–11. processes. Dobson, F. S., Smith, A. T., & Gao, W. X. (1998). Social and ecological influences on dispersal and philopatry in the plateau pika (Ochotona curzoniae). Behavioral Ecology, 9(6), 622–635. Cross-References Estes-Zumpf, W. A., & Rachlow, J. L. (2009). Natal dis- persal by pygmy rabbits (Brachylagus idahoensis). – ▶ Home Range Journal of Mammalogy, 90(2), 363 372. ▶ Ferretti, M., Paci, G., Porrini, S., Galardi, L., & Bagliacca, Lagomorpha Life History M. (2010). Habitat use and home range traits of resident ▶ Mammalia and relocated hares (Lepus europaeus, Pallas). Italian ▶ Movement/Locomotion Journal of Animal Science, 9(3), e54. ▶ Spatial Memory Lagomorpha Navigation 5

Forys, E. A., & Humphrey, S. R. (1996). Home range and Peacock, M. M. (1997). Determining natal dispersal pat- movements of the lower keys marsh rabbit in a highly terns in a population of North American pikas fragmented habitat. Journal of Mammalogy, 77(4), (Ochotona princeps) using direct mark-resight and 1042–1048. indirect genetic methods. Behavioral Ecology, 8(3), Franken, R. J., & Hik, D. S. (2004). Influence of habitat 340–350. quality, patch size and connectivity on colonization and Peacock, M. M., & Smith, A. T. (1997). The effect of extinction dynamics of collared pikas Ochotona habitat fragmentation on dispersal patterns, mating collaris. Journal of Animal Ecology, 73(5), 889–896. behavior, and genetic variation in a pika (Ochotona Gagliardo, A. (2013). Forty years of olfactory navigation in princeps) metapopulation. Oecologia, 112(4), birds. Journal of Experimental Biology, 216(12), 524–533. 2165–2171. Reid, N., & Harrison, A. T. (2010). Post-release GPS Gilwicz, J., Witczuk, J., & Pagacz, S. (2005). Spatial tracking of hand-reared Irish hare Lepus timidus behaviour of the rock-dwelling pika (Ochotona hyper- hibernicus leverets, Slemish, Co. Antrim, Northern borea). Journal of Zoology, 267(02), 113. Ireland. Conservation Evidence, 7,32–38. Handcock, R. N., Swain, D. L., Bishop-Hurley, G. J., Richardson, B., Hayes, R., Wheeler, S., & Yardin, M. Patison, K. P., Wark, T., Valencia, P., ...O’Neill, C. J. (2002). Social structures, genetic structures and dis- (2009). Monitoring animal behaviour and environmen- persal strategies in Australian rabbit (Oryctolagus tal interactions using wireless sensor networks, GPS cuniculus) populations. Behavioral Ecology and Socio- collars and satellite remote sensing. Sensors, 9(5), biology, 51(2), 113–121. 3586–3603. Rogowitz, G. L. (1997). Locomotor and foraging activity Lu, X. (2011). Habitat use and abundance of the woolly of the white-tailed jackrabbit (Lepus townsendii). Jour- hare Lepus oiostolus in the Lhasa mountains, Tibet. nal of Mammalogy, 78(4), 1172–1181. Mammalia, 75(1), 35–40. Schöne, H. (1984). Spatial orientation. Princeton: McMahon, L. A., Rachlow, J. L., Shipley, L. A., Forbey, Princeton University Press. J. S., Johnson, T. R., & Olsoy, P. J. (2017). Evaluation Tolman, E. C. (1948). Cognitive maps in rats and men. of micro-GPS receivers for tracking small-bodied Psychological Review, 55, 189–208. mammals. PLoS One, 12(3), e0173185.