Behavior Research Methods 2007, 39 (2), 175-191

G*Power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences

Franz Faul Christian-Albrechts-Universität Kiel, Kiel, Germany

Edgar Erdfelder Universität Mannheim, Mannheim, Germany

and

Albert-Georg Lang and Axel Buchner Heinrich-Heine-Universität Düsseldorf, Düsseldorf, Germany

G*Power (Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) was designed as a general stand-alone power analysis program for statistical tests commonly used in social and behavioral research. G*Power 3 is a major extension of, and improvement over, the previous versions. It runs on widely used computer platforms (i.e., Windows XP, Win- dows Vista, and Mac OS X 10.4) and covers many different statistical tests of the t, F, and c2 test families. In addition, it includes power analyses for z tests and some exact tests. G*Power 3 provides improved effect size calculators and graphic options, supports both distribution-based and design-based input modes, and offers all types of power analyses in which users might be interested. Like its predecessors, G*Power 3 is free.

Statistics textbooks in the social, behavioral, and biomed- reviews of which we are aware (Kornbrot, 1997; Ortseifen, ical sciences typically stress the importance of power analy- Bruckner, Burke, & Kieser, 1997; Thomas & Krebs, 1997). ses. By definition, the power of a statistical test is the prob- It has been used in several power tutorials (e.g., Buchner, ability that its null hypothesis (H0) will be rejected given that Erdfelder, & Faul, 1996, 1997; Erdfelder, Buchner, Faul, & it is in fact false. Obviously, significance tests that lack sta- Brandt, 2004; Levin, 1997; Sheppard, 1999) and in statis- tistical power are of limited use because they cannot reliably tics textbooks (e.g., Field, 2005; Keppel & Wickens, 2004; discriminate between H0 and the alternative hypothesis (H1) Myers & Well, 2003; Rasch, Friese, Hofmann, & Naumann, of interest. However, although power analyses are indispens- 2006a, 2006b). Nevertheless, the user feedback that we re- able for rational statistical decisions, it was not until the late ceived coincided with our own experience in showing some 1980s that power charts (see, e.g., Scheffé, 1959) and power limitations and weaknesses of G*Power 2 that required a tables (see, e.g., Cohen, 1988) were supplemented by more major extension and revision. efficient, precise, and easy-to-use power analysis programs In the present article, we describe G*Power 3, a program for personal computers (Goldstein, 1989). G*Power 2 (Erd- that was designed to address the problems of G*Power 2. felder, Faul, & Buchner, 1996) can be seen as a second- We begin with an outline of the major improvements in ­generation power analysis program designed as a stand- G*Power 3 and then discuss the types of power analyses cov- alone application to handle several types of statistical tests ered by this program. Next, we describe program handling commonly used in social and behavioral research. In the past and the types of statistical tests to which it can be applied. 10 years, this program has been found useful not only in the We then discuss the statistical algorithms of G*Power 3 and social and behavioral sciences but also in many other disci- their accuracy. Finally, program availability and some Inter- plines that routinely apply statistical tests, including biology net resources supporting users of G*Power 3 are described. (Baeza & Stotz, 2003), genetics (Akkad et al., 2006), ecol- ogy (Sheppard, 1999), forest and wildlife research (Mellina, Improvements in G*Power 3 in Hinch, Donaldson, & Pearson, 2005), the geosciences (Bus- Comparison With G*Power 2 bey, 1999), pharmacology (Quednow et al., 2004), and med- ical research (Gleissner, Clusmann, Sassen, Elger, & Helm- G*Power 3 is an improvement over G*Power 2 in five staedter, 2006). G*Power 2 was evaluated positively in the major respects. First, whereas G*Power 2 requires the

E. Erdfelder, [email protected]

175 Copyright 2007 Psychonomic Society, Inc. 176 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

DOS and Mac OS 7–9 operating systems that were com- research process, and the specific research question, five mon in the 1990s but are now outdated, G*Power 3 runs different types of power analysis can be reasonable (cf. on the personal computer platforms currently in widest Erdfelder et al., 2004; Erdfelder, Faul, & Buchner, 2005). use: Windows XP, Windows Vista, and Mac OS X 10.4. We describe these methods and their uses in turn. The Windows and Mac versions of the program are es- sentially equivalent. They use the same computational A Priori Power Analyses routines and share very similar user interfaces. For this In a priori power analyses (Cohen, 1988), sample reason, we will not differentiate between these versions in size N is computed as a function of the required power what follows; users simply have to make sure to download level (1 2 b), the prespecified significance level a, and the version appropriate for their operating system. the population effect size to be detected with probability Second, whereas G*Power 2 is limited to three types 1 2 b. A priori analyses provide an efficient method of of power analyses, G*Power 3 supports five different controlling statistical power before a study is actually con- ways to assess statistical power. In addition to the a pri- ducted (see, e.g., Bredenkamp, 1969; Hager, 2006) and ori, post hoc, and compromise power analyses that were can be recommended whenever resources such as the time already covered by G*Power 2, the new program offers and money required for data collection are not critical. sensitivity analyses and criterion analyses. Third, G*Power 3 provides dedicated power analysis Post Hoc Power Analyses options for a variety of frequently used t, F, z, c2, and In contrast to a priori power analyses, post hoc power exact tests in addition to the standard tests covered by analyses (Cohen, 1988) often make sense after a study G*Power 2. The tests captured by G*Power 3 and their has already been conducted. In post hoc analyses, 1 2 b effect size parameters are described in the Program Han- is computed as a function of a, the population effect size dling section. Importantly, users are not limited to these parameter, and the sample size(s) used in a study. It thus tests because G*Power 3 also offers power analyses for becomes possible to assess whether or not a published generic t, F, z, c2, and binomial tests for which the non- statistical test in fact had a fair chance of rejecting an in- centrality parameter of the distribution under H1 may correct H0. Importantly, post hoc analyses, like a priori be entered directly. In this way, users are provided with analyses, require an H1 effect size specification for the a flexible tool for computing the power of basically any underlying population. Post hoc power analyses should statistical test that uses t, F, z, c2, or binomial reference not be confused with so-called retrospective power anal- distributions. yses, in which the effect size is estimated from sample Fourth, statistical tests can be specified in G*Power 3 data and used to calculate the observed power, a sample using two different approaches: the distribution-based ap- estimate of the true power.1 Retrospective power analy- proach and the design-based approach. In the distribution- ses are based on the highly questionable assumption that based approach, users select the family of the test statistic the sample effect size is essentially identical to the effect (t, F, z, c2, or ) and the particular test within size in the population from which it was drawn (Zumbo & that family. This is how power analyses were specified in Hubley, 1998). Obviously, this assumption is likely to be G*Power 2. In addition, a separate menu in G*Power 3 false, and the more so the smaller the sample. In addition, provides access to power analyses via the design-based sample effect sizes are typically biased estimates of their approach: Users select (1) the parameter class to which population counterparts (Richardson, 1996). For these the statistical test refers (correlations, means, proportions, reasons, we agree with other critics of retrospective power regression coefficients, variances) and (2) the design of analyses (e.g., Gerard, Smith, & Weerakkody, 1998; Hoe- the study (e.g., number of groups, independent vs. depen- nig & Heisey, 2001; Kromrey & Hogarty, 2000; Lenth, dent samples). On the basis of the feedback we received 2001; Steidl, Hayes, & Schauber, 1997). Rather than use about G*Power 2, we expect that some users might find retrospective power analyses, researchers should specify the design-based input mode more intuitive and easier to population effect sizes on a priori grounds. To specify the use. effect size simply means to define the minimum degree Fifth, G*Power 3 supports users with enhanced graph- of violation of H0 a researcher would like to detect with ics features. The details of these features will be outlined a probability not less than 1 2 b. Cohen’s definitions of in the Program Handling section. small, medium, and large effects can be helpful in such effect size specifications (see, e.g., Smith & Bayen, 2005). Types of Statistical Power Analyses However, researchers should be aware of the fact that these conventions may have different meanings for differ- The power (1 2 b) of a statistical test is the complement ent tests (cf. Erdfelder et al., 2005). of b, which denotes the Type II or beta error probability of falsely retaining an incorrect H0. Statistical power de- Compromise Power Analyses pends on three classes of parameters: (1) the significance In compromise power analyses (Erdfelder, 1984; level (i.e., the Type I error probability) a of the test, (2) the Erdfelder et al., 1996; Müller, Manz, & Hoyer, 2002), size(s) of the sample(s) used for the test, and (3) an effect both a and 1 2 b are computed as functions of the ef- size parameter defining H1 and thus indexing the degree fect size, N, and the error probability ratio q 5 b/a. To of deviation from H0 in the underlying population. De- illustrate, setting q to 1 would mean that the researcher pending on the available resources, the actual phase of the prefers balanced Type I and Type II error risks (a 5 b), G*Power 3 177 whereas a q of 4 would imply that b 5 4a (cf. Cohen, analyses defined in the previous section, (3) provide the 1988). Compromise power analyses can be useful both input parameters required for the analysis, and (4) click on before and after data collection. For example, an a priori “Calculate” to obtain the results. power analysis might result in a sample size that exceeds In the first step, the statistical test is chosen using the available resources. In such a situation, a researcher the distribution-based or the design-based approach. could specify the maximum affordable sample size and, G*Power 2 users probably have adapted to the distribution- using a compromise power analysis, compute a and 1 2 b based approach: One first selects the family of the test associated with, say, q 5 b/a 5 4. Alternatively, if a study statistic (t, F, z, c2, or exact test) using the “Test fam- has already been conducted but has not yet been analyzed, ily” menu in the main window. The “Statistical test” menu a researcher could ask for a reasonable decision criterion adapts accordingly, showing a list of all tests available for that guarantees perfectly balanced error risks (i.e., a 5 b) the test family. For the two-groups t test, for example, one given the size of the sample and the critical effect size would first select the t family of distributions and then in which he or she is interested. Of course, compromise “Means: Difference between two independent means (two power analyses can easily result in unconventional sig- groups)” in the “Statistical test” menu (see Figure 1). Al- nificance levels greater than a 5 .05 (in the case of small ternatively, one might use the design-based approach of samples or effect sizes) or less than a 5 .001 (in the case test selection. With the “Tests” pull-down menu in the top of large samples or effect sizes). However, we believe that row, it is possible to select (1) the parameter class to which the benefit of balanced Type I and Type II error risks often the statistical test refers (i.e., correlation and regression, offsets the costs of violating significance level conven- means, proportions, variances, or generic) and (2) the de- tions (cf. Gigerenzer, Krauss, & Vitouch, 2004). sign of the study (e.g., number of groups, independent vs. dependent samples). For example, a researcher would Sensitivity Analyses select “Means” followed by “Two independent groups” to In sensitivity analyses, the critical population effect size specify the two-groups t test (see Figure 2). The design- is computed as a function of a, 1 2 b, and N. Sensitivity based approach has the advantage that test options refer- analyses may be particularly useful for evaluating pub- ring to the same parameter class (e.g., means) are located lished research. They provide answers to questions such as in close proximity, whereas in the distribution-based ap- “What effect size was a study able to detect with a power proach they may be scattered across different distribution of 1 2 b 5 .80 given its sample size and a as specified families. by the author? In other words, what is the minimum ef- In the second step, the “Type of power analysis” menu fect size to which the test was sufficiently sensitive?” In in the center of the main window should be used to choose addition, it may be useful to perform sensitivity analyses the appropriate analysis type. In the third step, the power before conducting a study to see whether, given a lim- analysis input parameters are specified in the lower left of ited N, the size of the effect that can be detected is at all the main window. To illustrate, an a priori power analysis realistic (or, for instance, much too large to be expected for a two-groups t test would require a decision between a realistically). one-tailed and a two-tailed test, a specification of Cohen’s (1988) effect size measure (d) under H1, the significance Criterion Analyses level a, the required power (1 2 b) of the test, and the Finally, criterion analyses compute a (and the associ- preferred group size allocation ratio n2/n1. The final step ated decision criterion) as a function of 1 2 b, the effect consists of clicking on “Calculate” to obtain the output in size, and a given sample size. Criterion analyses are alter- the lower right of the main window. natives to post hoc power analyses. They may be reason- For instance, input parameters specifying a one-tailed able whenever the control of a is less important than the t test, a medium effect size of d 5 0.5, a 5 .05, 1 2 b 5 control of b. In case of goodness-of-fit tests for statistical .95, and an allocation ratio of n2/n1 5 1 would result in models, for example, it is most important to minimize the a total sample size of N 5 176 (88 observation units in b risk of wrong decisions in favor of the model (H0). Re- each group; see Figures 1 and 2). The noncentrality pa- searchers could thus use criterion analyses to compute the rameter d defining the t distribution under H1, the decision significance level a which is compatible with b 5 .05 for criterion to be used (i.e., the critical value of the t statis- a small effect size. tic), the degrees of freedom2 of the t test, and the actual Whereas G*Power 2 was limited to the first three types power value are also displayed. Note that the actual power of power analysis, G*Power 3 covers all five types. On the will often be slightly larger than the prespecified power basis of the feedback we received from G*Power 2 users, in a priori power analyses. The reason is that noninteger we believe that any question related to statistical power sample sizes are always rounded up by G*Power to obtain that arises in research practice can be categorized under integer values consistent with a power level not lower than one of these analysis types. the prespecified one. In addition to the numerical output, G*Power 3 dis- Program Handling plays the central (H0) and the noncentral (H1) test statistic distributions along with the decision criterion and the as- Using G*Power 3 typically involves the following sociated error probabilities in the upper part of the main four steps: (1) Select the statistical test appropriate for window (see Figure 1).3 This supports understanding of the problem, (2) choose one of the five types of power the effects of the input parameters and is likely to be a 178 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Figure 1. The distribution-based approach of test specification in G*Power 3.0. useful visualization tool in the teaching of, or the learning (s) in the populations underlying the groups to calculate about, inferential . The distributions plot can be Cohen’s d 5 | m1 2 m2 |/s. Clicking on the “Calculate and printed, saved, or copied by clicking on the right mouse transfer to main window” button copies the computed ef- button inside the plot area. fect size to the appropriate field in the main window. The input and output of each power calculation in a Another useful option is the Power Plot window (see G*Power session is automatically written to a protocol Figure 3), which is opened by clicking on “X–Y plot for a that can be displayed by selecting the “Protocol of power range of values” on the lower right side of the main win- analyses” tab in the main window. It is possible to clear dow (see Figures 1 and 2). the protocol or to print, save, and copy the protocol in the By selecting the appropriate parameters for the y- and same way as the distributions plot. x‑axes, one parameter (a, 1 2 b, effect size, or sample size) Because Cohen’s (1988) book on power analysis appears can be plotted as a function of any other parameter. Of the to be well-known in the social and behavioral sciences, we remaining two parameters, one can be chosen to draw a fam- made use of his effect size measures whenever possible. ily of graphs, whereas the fourth parameter is kept constant. Researchers unfamiliar with these measures and users For instance, sample size can be drawn as a function of the who prefer to compute Cohen’s measures from more basic power 1 2 b for several different population effects sizes parameters can click on the “Determine” button to the left while a is kept at a particular value. The plot may be printed, of the “Effect size” input field (see Figures 1 and 2). A saved, or copied by clicking on the right mouse button inside drawer will open next to the main window and provide the plot area. Selecting the “Table” tab reveals the data un- access to an effect size calculator tailored to the selected derlying the plot; they may be copied to other applications. test (see Figure 2). For the two-groups t test, for example, The Power Plot window inherits all input parameters of users can specify the means (m1, m2) and the common SD the analysis that is active when the “X–Y plot for a range of G*Power 3 179

Figure 2. The design-based approach of test specification in G*Power 3.0 and the “Effect size” drawer.

Figure 3. The Power Plot window of G*Power 3.0. 180 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner values” button is clicked. Only some of these parameters esis that adding more predictors increases the value of R2 can be directly manipulated in the Power Plot window. For (Cohen, 1988, chap. 9). According to Cohen’s criteria, instance, switching from a plot of a two-tailed test to a plot effect sizes ( f 2) of 0.02, 0.15, and 0.35 are considered of a one-tailed test requires choosing the “Tail(s): One” small, medium, and large, respectively. option in the main window and then clicking on the “X–Y plot for a range of values” button. Tests for Means (Univariate Case) Table 3 summarizes the power analysis procedures for Types of Statistical Tests tests on means. G*Power 3 supports all cases of the t test for means described by Cohen (1988, chap. 2): the test for G*Power 3 provides power analyses for test statistics independent means, the test of the null hypothesis that the following t, F, c2, or standard normal distributions under population mean equals some specified value (one sample H0 (either exact or asymptotic) and noncentral distributions case), and the test on the means of two dependent samples of the same test families under H1. In addition, it includes (matched pairs). Cohen’s d and dz are used as effect size power analyses for some exact tests. In Tables 2–9, we indices. Cohen defines ds of 0.2, 0.5, and 0.8 as small, briefly describe the tests currently covered by G*Power 3. medium, and large effects, respectively. Effect size dialogs Table 1 lists the symbols used in Tables 2–9 and their are available to compute the appropriate effect size param- meanings. eter from means and SDs. For example, assume we want to compare visual search times for targets embedded in rare Tests for Correlation and Regression versus frequent local contexts in a within-subjects design Table 2 summarizes the procedures supported for test- (cf. Hoffmann & Sebald, 2005, Experiment 1). It is ex- ing hypotheses on correlation and regression. One-sample pected that the mean search time for targets in rare contexts tests are provided for the point–biserial model—that is, (e.g., 600 msec) should decrease by at least 10 msec (i.e., the model for correlations between a binary variable and to 590 msec) in frequent contexts as a consequence of local a continuous variable—and for correlations between two contextual cuing. If prior evidence suggests population normally distributed variables (Cohen, 1988, chap. 3).4 SDs of, say, s 5 25 msec in each of the conditions and a The latter test uses the exact sample correlation coefficient correlation of 5 .70 between search times in the two con- distribution (Barabesi & Greco, 2002) or, optionally, a ditions, we can use the “Effect size” drawer of G*Power 3 large-sample approximation based on Fisher’s r-to-z trans- for the matched pairs t test to calculate the effect size dz 5 formation. The two-sample test for differences between 0.516 (see the second row of Table 3 for the formula). By two correlations uses Cohen’s (1988, chap. 4) effect size q selecting a post hoc power analysis for one-tailed matched and is based on Fisher’s r-to-z transformation. Cohen de- pairs t tests, we easily see that for dz 5 0.516, a 5 .05, and fines qs of 0.10, 0.30, and 0.50 as small, medium, and large N 5 16 participants, the power (1 2 b) is only .47. Thus, effects, respectively. provided that the assumptions outlined above are appropri- The two procedures available for the multiple regres- ate, the nonsignificant statistic [t(15) 5 1.475] obtained by sion model handle the cases of (1) a test of an overall Hoffmann and Sebald (2005, Experiment 1, p. 34) might ­effect—that is, the hypothesis that the population value in fact be due to a Type II error. This interpretation would of R2 is different from zero—and (2) a test of the hypoth- be consistent with the fact that Hoffmann and Sebald ob-

Table 1 Symbols and Their Meanings As Used in the Tables Symbols Meaning

m (mi) population mean (in group i)   m (mi) vector of population means (in group i) mx2y population mean of the difference N total sample size ni sample size in group i s standard deviation in the population sm standard deviation of the effect sx2y standard deviation of the difference l noncentrality parameter of the noncentral F and χ2 distribution d noncentrality parameter of the noncentral t distribution df degrees of freedom df1, df2 numerator and denominator degrees of freedom, respectively r (ri) population correlation (in group i) 2 2 RY⋅A, RY⋅A,B squared multiple correlation coefficients, corresponding to the proportion of Y variance that can be accounted for by multiple regression on the set of predictor variables A and A∪B, respectively S population variance–covariance matrix M matrix of regression parameters (population means) C contrast matrix (contrasts between rows of M) A contrast matrix (contrasts between columns of M) p (pi) probability of success (in group i) G*Power 3 181

Table 2 Tests for Correlation and Regression Test Null Noncentrality Parameter Test Family Hypothesis Effect Size Other Parameters and Degrees of Freedom

Difference from t tests 5 0 2 r r ρ zero: point biserial δ = 2 ⋅ N model 1 − ρ df 5 N 2 2 Difference from exact r 5 c r Constant constant tests correlation c (bivariate normal) q Inequality of two z tests r1 5 r2 q 5 z1 2 z2 m = correlation 1 s 1 1 + ρi coefficients zi = ln 2 1 n1+ n 2 − 6 − ρi s = (n1− 3)( n 2 − 3)

2 2 2 Multiple F tests RY ⋅ A 5 0 R Number of l 5 f N f 2 = YA⋅ regression: 2 predictors p (#A) df1 5 p 2 1 − RYA⋅ deviation of R df2 5 N 2 p 2 1 from zero Multiple F tests R2 5 R2 2 2 Total number of l 5 f 2 N Y ⋅ A,B Y ⋅ A 2 RRYA⋅ ,BY− ⋅A regression: f = 2 predictors p df1 5 q 2 1 − RYA⋅ ,B increase of R (#A 1 #B) df2 5 N 2 p 2 1 Number of tested predictors q (#B) served significant local contextual cuing effects in each of to compensate for such adverse effects in tests of within the other four experiments they reported. effects or between–within interactions, the noncentrality The procedures provided by G*Power 3 to test effects in parameter and the degrees of freedom of the F distribu- between-subjects designs with more than two groups (i.e., tion can be multiplied by a correction factor e (Geisser & one-way ANOVA designs and general main effects and Greenhouse, 1958; Huynh & Feldt, 1970). e 5 1 if the interactions in factorial ANOVA designs of any order) are sphericity assumption is met and approaches 1/(m 2 1) identical to those in G*Power 2 (Erdfelder et al., 1996). In with increasing degrees of violation of sphericity, where all these cases, the effect size f as defined by Cohen (1988) m denotes the number of repeated measurements. is used. In a one-way ANOVA, the “Effect size” drawer G*Power provides three separate yet very similar rou- can be used to compute f from the means and group sizes tines to calculate power in the univariate approach for of k groups and an SD common to all groups. For tests of between effects, within effects, and interactions. If the to- effects in factorial designs, the “Effect size” drawer offers be-detected effect size f is known, these procedures are the possibility of computing effect size f from the vari- very easy to apply. To illustrate, Berti, Münzer, Schröger, ance explained by the tested effect and the error variance. and Pechmann (2006) compared the pitch discrimination Cohen defines fs of 0.1, 0.25, and 0.4 as small, medium, ability of 10 musicians and 10 control subjects (between- and large effects, respectively. subjects factor A) for 10 different interference conditions New in G*Power 3 are procedures for analyzing main (within-subjects factor B). Assuming that A, B, and A 3 effects and interactions for A 3 B mixed designs, where B effects of medium size ( f 5 0.25; see Cohen, 1988; A is a between-subjects factor (or an enumeration of Table 3 of the present article) should be detected given a the groups generated by cross-classification of several correlation of r 5 .50 between repeated measures and a ­between-subjects factors) and B is a within-subjects fac- significance level of a 5 .05, the power values of the F tor (or an enumeration of the repeated measures generated tests for the A main effect, the B main effect, and the A 3 by cross-classification of several within-subjects factors). B interaction are easily computed as .30, .95, and .95, re- Both the univariate and the multivariate approaches to re- spectively, by inserting f 5 0.25, a 5 .05, the total sample peated measures (O’Brien & Kaiser, 1985) are supported. size (20), the number of groups (2), the number of repeti- The multivariate approach will be discussed below. The tions (10), and r 5 .50 into the appropriate input fields of univariate approach is based on the sphericity assump- the procedures designed for these tests. tion. This assumption is correct if (in the population) all If the to-be-detected effect size f is unknown, it must be variances of the repeated measurements are equal and all computed from more basic parameters characterizing the correlations between pairs of repeated measurements are expected population scenario under H1. To demonstrate equal. If all the distributional assumptions are met, then the the general procedure, we will show how to do post hoc univariate approach is the most powerful method (Muller power analyses in the scenario illustrated in Figure 4 as- & Barton, 1989; O’Brien & Kaiser, 1985). Unfortunately, suming the variance and correlations structure defined the assumption of equal correlations is violated quite in matrix SR1. We first consider the power of the within often, which can lead to very misleading results. In order effect: We select the “F tests” family, the “Repeated mea- 182 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Table 3 Tests for Means (Univariate Case) Test Null Noncentrality Parameter Test Family Hypothesis Effect Size Other Parameters and Degrees of Freedom __ µ − c Difference from t tests m 5 c d = d 5 d√​ N ​ constant (one- σ df 5 N 2 1 sample case) __ Inequality of t tests mx2y 5 0 d 5 dz√​ N ​ µx− y two dependent dz = df 5 N 2 1 means σ x− y (matched pairs) 2 2 σx− y = σ x + σ y − 2 ρσx σ y Inequality of t tests 5 µ− µ m1 m2 d = 1 2 n n two independent σ δ = d 1 2 n+ n means 1 2 df 5 N 2 2 2 ANOVA, fixed F tests m 2 m 5 0 σ µ Number of l 5 f N i f = effects, one i 5 1, . . ., k σ groups k df1 5 k 2 1 k way: inequality 2 df2 5 N 2 k n of multiple ∑ j(µ i − µ) means σ 2 = i=1 µ N 2 ANOVA, fixed F tests m 2 m 5 0 σ µ Total number of l 5 f N i f = effects, i 5 1, . . ., k σ cells in the df1 5 q multifactor design k df2 5 N 2 k designs, and Degrees of planned freedom of the comparisons tested effect q

2 ANOVA: F tests m 2 m 5 0 σ µ Levels of l 5 f uNe i f repeated i 5 1, . . ., k = between factor k σ u = m measures, 1+()m − 1 ρ between effects Levels of df1 5 k 2 1 repeated measures df2 5 N 2 k factor m 2 ANOVA: F tests m 2 m 5 0 σ µ l 5 f uN i f = repeated i 5 1, . . ., m σ u = m measures, Population 1 − ρ within effects correlation df1 5 (m 2 1)e among repeated df2 5 (N 2 k)(m 2 1)e measures r 2 ANOVA: F tests mij 2 mi 2 . . . σ µ l 5 f uNe f = m repeated mj 1 m 5 0 σ u = measures, i 5 1, . . ., k For within and 1 − ρ between–within j 5 1, . . ., m within–between df1 5 (k 2 1)(m 2 1)e interactions interactions: df2 5 (N 2 k)(m 2 1)e Nonsphericity correction ε

sures: Within factors, ANOVA-approach” test, and “post to matrix SR1, we insert .3 in the “Corr among rep mea- hoc” as the type of power analysis. Both the “Number of sures” input field and—since sphericity obviously holds groups” and “Repetitions” fields are set to 3. Total sample in this case—set nonsphericity correction e to 1. To deter- 2 size is set to 90 and a error probability to .05. Referring mine effect size f, we first calculate sm, the variance of the

Time 1 Time 2 Time 3 mi• ni Group 1 10 15 20 15 30  10 0.. 3 0 1  9 0..3 0.3 Group 2 10 12 15 12.333 30 sr = 0.. 3 9 0 3 sr = 0.. 3 9 0 3 2   1   Group 3 10 12 12 11.333 30 0.. 1 0 3 8  0.. 3 0 3 9  m•j 10 13 15.667 m•• 5 12.889

Figure 4. Sample 3 3 3 repeated measures designs. Three groups are repeatedly measured at three dif- ferent times. The shaded portion of the table is the postulated matrix M of population means mij. The last column of the table contains the sample size of each group. The symmetric matrices SRi specify two dif- ferent covariance structures between measurements taken at different times: The main diagonal contains the SDs of the measurements at each time, and the off-diagonal elements contain the correlations between pairs of measurements taken at different times. G*Power 3 183 within effect. From the three column means m•j of matrix that the vector of population means is identical to a speci- M and the grand mean m••, we get fied constant mean vector. The “Effect size” drawer can 2 be used to calculate the effect size D from the difference   σ µ = m 2 c and the expected variance–covariance matrix under 2 2 2 H1. For example, assume that we have two variables, a (10 −12.889) + (13 −12.889) + (15.667 − 12.8899)   difference vector m 2 c 5 (1.88, 1.88) under H1, vari- 3 2 2 ances s1 5 56.79, s2 5 29.28, and a covariance of 11.98 = 5.35679. (Rencher, 1998, p. 106). To perform a post hoc power Clicking on the “Determine” button next to the “Effect analysis, choose “F tests,” then “Multivariate: Hotelling size” label opens the “Effect size” drawer. We choose the T 2, one group” and set the analysis type to “Post hoc.” “From variances” option and set “Variance explained by Enter 2 in the “Response variables” field and then click special effect” to 5.357 and “Variance within groups” to on the “Determine” button next to the “Effect size” label. 92 5 81. Clicking on the “Calculate and transfer to main In the “Effect size” drawer, at “Input method: Means and window” button calculates an effect size f 5 0.2572 and . . . ,” choose “Variance–covariance matrix” and click on transfers f to the effect size field in the main window. “Specify/edit input values.” Under the “Means” tab, insert Clicking on “Calculate” yields the results: The power is 1.88 in both input fields; under the “Cov sigma” tab, in- .997, the critical F value with df1 5 2 and df2 5 174 is sert 56.79 and 29.28 in the main diagonal and 11.98 as the 3.048, and the noncentrality parameter l is 25.52. The off-diagonal element in the lower left cell. Clicking on the procedure for tests of between–within interactions ef- “Calculate and transfer to main window” button initiates fects (“Repeated measures: Within–between interac- the calculation of the effect size (0.380) and transfers it to tions, ANOVA-approach”) is almost identical to that just the main window. For this effect size, a 5 .05, and a total described. The only difference is in how the effect size f sample size of N 5 100, the power amounts to .9282. The is computed. Here, we first calculate the variance of the procedure in the two-group case is exactly the same, with residual values mij 2 mi• 2 m•j 1 m•• of matrix M: the following exceptions. First, in the “Effect size” drawer 2 two mean vectors have to be specified. Second, the group σ µ = sizes may differ. (10−10 −15+12.889)2 + ... +(.12 −15 667−11.333+ 12.)889 2 The MANOVA tests in G*Power 3 refer to the multi- 9. 0 variate general linear model (O’Brien & Muller, 1993; O’Brien & Shieh, 1999): Y 5 XB 1 , where Y is N 3 = 1..90123 e p of rank p, X is N 3 r of rank r, and the r 3 p matrix Using the “Effect size” drawer in the same way as above, B contains fixed coefficients. The rows of e are taken to we get an effect size f 5 0.1532, which results in a power of be independent p-variate normal random vectors with .653. To test between effects, we choose “Repeated measures: mean 0 and p 3 p positive definite covariance matrix S. Between factors, ANOVA-approach” and set all parameters The multivariate general linear hypothesis is H : CBA 5 to the same values as before. Note that in this case we do not 0 Q0, where C is c 3 r with full row rank and A is p 3 a need to specify e—no correction is necessary because tests with full column rank (in G*Power 3, Q is assumed to be of between factors do not require the sphericity assumption. 0 zero). H0 has df1 5 a ⋅ c degrees of freedom. All tests of To calculate the effect size, we use “Effect size from means” the hypothesis H refer to the matrices in the “Effect size” drawer. We select three groups, set “SD 0 s within each group” to 9, and insert for each group the cor- h = −1 responding row mean mi• of M (15, 12.3333, 11.3333) and T −1 N CBUC XT wXCT  CBU an equal group size of 30. Effect size f 5 0.1719571 is cal- ( − Θ0 )  ( )  ( − Θ0 ) culated, and the resulting power is .488.   Note that G*Power 3 can easily handle pure repeated = Nh* measures designs without any between-subjects factors and (see, e.g., Frings & Wentura, 2005; Schwarz & Müller, T 2006) by choosing the “Repeated measures: Within fac- EU=ΣU(N − r), tors, ANOVA-approach” procedure and setting the num- where X¨ is a q 3 q essence model matrix, W is a q 3 q di- T ber of groups to 1. agonal matrix containing weights wj 5 nj/N, and X X 5 T * N(X¨ WX¨ ) (see O’Brien & Shieh, 1999, p. 14). Let {f1, * 21 * Tests for Mean Vectors (Multivariate Case) . . ., fs} be the s 5 min(a,c) eigenvalues of E H and 21 G*Power 3 contains several procedures for performing {f1, . . ., fs} the s eigenvalues of E H/(N 2 r)—that is, * power analyses in multivariate designs (see Table 4). All fi 5 fi N/(N 2 r). these tests belong to the F test family. G*Power 3 offers power analyses for the multivariate The Hotelling T 2 tests are extensions of univariate model following either the approach outlined in Muller and t tests to the multivariate case, in which more than one Peterson (1984; Muller, LaVange, Landesman-Ramey, & dependent variable is measured: Instead of two single Ramey, 1992) or, alternatively, the approach of O’Brien and means, two mean vectors are compared, and instead of a Shieh (1999; Shieh, 2003). Both approaches approximate the single variance, a variance–covariance matrix is consid- exact distributions of Wilks’s U (Rao, 1951), the Hotelling– ered (Rencher, 1998). In the one-sample case, H0 posits Lawley T1 (Pillai & Samson, 1959), the Hotelling–Lawley 184 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Table 4 Tests for Mean Vectors (Multivariate Case) Test Null Noncentrality Parameter Test Family Hypothesis Effect Size Other Parameters and Degrees of Freedom ______2   T 21  2 Hotelling T : F tests m 5 c D 5 ​ v S v ​ Number of l 5 D N √  difference from v 5 m 2 c response df1 5 k constant mean variables k df2 5 N 2 k vector ______2   Hotelling T : F tests m 5 m T 21  Number of 2 n1 n 2 1 2 D 5 ​√v S v ​ λ = ∆ difference   response v 5 m 1 2 m 2 n1+ n 2 between two variables k df1 5 k mean vectors df2 5 N 2 k 2 1 MANOVA: F tests CM 5 0 Effect size Number of Noncentrality parameter global effects Means matrix fmult groups g and degrees of freedom M depends on the test Number of depend on the test statistic Contrast statistics: response and algorithm used (see matrix C • Wilks’s U variables k Effect Size column and • Hotelling– Table 5). • Lawley T 1 MANOVA: F tests • Hotelling– Number of special effects • Lawley T 2 groups g • Pillai’s V Number of and algorithms: predictors p • Muller & Number of • Peterson response • (1984) variables k • O’Brien & MANOVA: F tests CMA 5 0 • Shieh Levels of repeated Means matrix • (1999) between factor measures, M k between Between Levels of effects contrast repeated MANOVA: F tests matrix C measures factor repeated Within m measures, contrast within effects matrix A MANOVA: F tests repeated measures, between–within interactions

T2 (McKeon, 1974), and Pillai’s V (Pillai & Mijares, 1959) effects and interactions in factorial MANOVA designs. by F distributions and are asymptotically equivalent. Table 5 These procedures are the direct multivariate analogues outlines details of both approximations. The type of statistic of the ANOVA routines described above. Table 5 sum- (U, T1, T2, V ) and the approach (Muller & Peterson, 1984, marizes information that is needed in addition to the or O’Brien & Shieh, 1999) can be selected in an Options formulas given above to calculate effect size f from hy- dialog that can be evoked by clicking on the “Options” but- pothesized values for mean matrix M (corresponding to ton at the bottom of the main window. matrix B in the model), covariance matrix S, and contrast The approach of Muller and Peterson (1984) has found matrix C, which describes the effect under scrutiny. The widespread use; for instance, it has been adopted in the “Effect size” drawer can be used to calculate f from known SPSS software package. We nevertheless recommend the values of the statistic U, T 1, T 2, or V. Note, however, that approach of O’Brien and Shieh (1999) because it has a the transformation of T 2 to f depends on the sample size. number of advantages: (1) Unlike the method of Muller Thus, this test statistic seems not very well suited for and Peterson, it provides the exact noncentral F distri- a priori analyses. In line with Bredenkamp and Erdfelder bution whenever the hypothesis involves at most s 5 1 (1985), we recommend V as the multivariate test statistic. positive eigenvalues; (2) its approximations for s . 1 Another group of procedures in G*Power 3 supports eigenvalues are almost always more accurate than those the multivariate approach to power analyses of repeated of Muller and Peterson’s method (which systematically measures designs. G*Power provides separate but very underestimates power); and (3) it provides a simpler form similar routines for the analysis of between effects, within of the noncentrality parameter—that is, l 5 l* N, where effects, and interactions in simple A 3 B designs, where l* is not a function of the total sample size. A is a between-subjects factor and B a within-subjects G*Power 3 provides procedures to calculate the power factor. To illustrate the general procedure, we describe for global effects in a one-way MANOVA and for special in some detail a post hoc analysis of the within effect for G*Power 3 185

Table 5 Approximating Univariate Statistics for Multivariate Hypotheses Effect Size and Statistic Formula Numerator df2 Noncentrality Parameter s Wilks’s U −1 df 5g(N 2 g ) 2 g 1/ g U =1 + φ 2 1 2 f() U 2 = 1 −U MP ∏( k ) 1/ g k=1 g= r + a− c + 1 U 1 2 2 l 5 f (U) df2 s ca − 2 Wilks’s U −1 g2 = 1/ g U =1 + φ* 2 f() U 2 1 −U OS ∏( k ) = 1/ g k=1  1 ca ≤ 3 U  l 5 Ng f (U)2 g =  ()ca 2 − 4  ca ≤ 4  c2+ a 2 − 5

Pillai’s V s df 5 s(N 2 r 2 a 1 s) 2 V V / 1 2 f() V = MP =∏φk( + φ k ) ()s− V k=1 2 l 5 f (V) df2 Pillai’s V s 2 V V **/ 1 f() V = OS =∏φk( + φ k ) ()s− V k=1 l 5 Ns f (V)2 Hotelling– s df 5 s(N 2 r 2 a 2 1) 1 2 f (T)2 5 T/s T 2 Lawley T = ∏φk 5 f (T)2 df 1 k=1 l 2 MP Hotelling– s f (T)2 5 T/s T * Lawley T = ∏φk 5 Ns f (T)2 1 k=1 l OS

Hotelling– s df 5 4 1 (ca 1 2)g f (T)2 5 T/h T = φ 2 Lawley T ∏ k 2 l 5 f (T)2 df 2 k=1 ()N− r − ()N− r g + g 2 MP g = 4 3 ()N− r g2 − g 1 s 2 Hotelling– * 2 f (T) 5 T/h T = φ g1 5 c 1 2a 1 a 2 1 Lawley T ∏ k l 5 Nh f (T)2 2 k=1 g 5 c 1 a 1 1 OS 2 g3 5 a(a 1 3) g4 5 2a 1 3 df − 2 h = 2 N− r − a − 1 Note—MP, Muller–Peterson algorithm; OS, O’Brien and Shieh algorithm. f and f* are eigenvalues of the effect size matrix (for details and the meaning of the variables a, c, r, and N, see text on p. 183). the scenario illustrated in Figure 4, assuming the variance window,” we get a value of 0.1791 for Pillai’s V and the and correlations structure defined in matrix SR2. We first effect size f(V ) 5 0.4672. Clicking on “Calculate” shows choose “F tests,” then “Repeated measures: Within factors, that the power is .980. The analyses of between effects and MANOVA-approach.” In the “Type of power analysis” interaction effects are performed analogously. menu, we choose “Post hoc.” We click on the “Options” button to open a dialog in which we deselect the “Use mean Tests for Proportions correlation in effect size calculation” option. We choose The support for tests on proportions has been greatly Pillai’s V statistic and the O’Brien and Shieh algorithm. enhanced in G*Power 3. Table 6 summarizes the tests that Back at the main window, we set both number of groups are currently implemented. In particular, all tests on pro- and repetitions to 3, total sample size to 90, and a error portions considered by Cohen (1988) are now available, probability to .05. To compute the effect size f(V ) for the including the (chap. 5), the z tests for the differ- Pillai statistic, we open the “Effect size” drawer by clicking ence between two proportions (chap. 6), and the c2 tests on the “Determine” button next to the “Effect size” label. for goodness-of-fit and contingency tables (chap. 7). In the “Effect size” drawer, select, as procedure, “Effect The sign test is implemented as a special case (c 5 .5) of size from mean and variance–covariance matrix” and, as the more general binomial test (also available in G*Power 3) input method, “SD and correlation matrix.” Clicking on that a single proportion has a specified value c. In both “Specify/edit matrices” opens another window, in which we procedures, Cohen’s (1988) effect size g is used and exact specify the hypothesized parameters. Under the “Means” power values based on the are cal- tab, we insert our means matrix M; under the “Cov sigma” culated. Note, however, that, due to the discrete nature of tab, we choose “SD and correlation” and insert the values the binomial distribution, the nominal value of a usually of SR2. Because this matrix is always symmetric, it suf- cannot be realized. Since the tables in chapter 5 of Cohen’s fices to specify the lower diagonal values. After closing book use the a value closest to the nominal value, even if it the dialog and clicking on “Calculate and transfer to main is higher than the nominal value, the tabulated power values 186 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Table 6 Tests for Proportions Test Noncentrality Test Family Hypothesis Effect Size Other Parameters Parameter 2 2 Contingency tests 5 2 5 w N χ p1i p0i k l (π1i− π 0 i ) tables and i 5 1, . . ., k w = k ∑ goodness of fit i=1 π 0i ∑π 0i = 1 i=1 Difference from exact p 5 c g 5 p 2 c constant proportion c constant (one- tests sample case)

Inequality of two exact p12/p21 5 1 odds ratio 5 p12/p21 proportion of discordant dependent tests pairs 5 p12 1 p21 proportions (McNemar) Sign test exact p 5 1/2 g 5 p 2 1/2 tests

Inequality of two z tests p1 5 p2 (A) alternate proportion: p2 (A) null proportion: p1 independent (B) h 5 f1 2 f2 __ proportions (A) fi 5 2 arcsin √​ p i ​

Inequality of two exact p1 5 p2 alternate proportion: p1 null proportion: p2 independent tests proportions (Fisher’s exact test)

Inequality of exact p1 5 p2 (A) alternate proportion: p1 null proportion: p2 two independent tests (B) difference: p2 2 p1 proportions (C) risk ratio: p2/p1 (unconditional) π/ 1 − π (D) odds ratio: 1( 1) π2/ (1 − π 2 ) Inequality with exact 5 1 c (A) alternate proportion: (A) proportion: p1 p2 p1|H1 p1|H0 offset of two tests (B) difference: 2 (B) difference: 2 p2 p1|H1 p2 p1|H0 independent (C) risk ratio: / (C) risk ratio: / p2 p1|H1 p2 p1|H0 proportions / 1 π1|H − π1|H π1|H / 1 − π1|H (unconditional) (D) odds ratio: 1 ( 1 ) (D) odds ratio: 0 ( 0 ) / 1 π2( − π 2 ) π2/ (1 − π 2 ) (A) null proportion: p2 Note—(A)–(D) indicate alternative effect size measures. are sometimes larger than those calculated by G*Power 3. unconditional power. However, despite the highly opti- G*Power 3 always requires the actual a not to be larger than mized algorithm used in G*Power 3, long computation the nominal value. times may result for large sample sizes (e.g., N . 1,000). Numerous procedures have been proposed to test the Therefore, a limiting N can be specified in the Options null hypothesis that two independent proportions are iden- dialog that determines at which sample size G*Power 3 tical (Cohen, 1988; D’Agostino, Chase, & Belanger, 1988; switches to a large sample approximation. Suissa & Shuster, 1985; Upton, 1982), and G*Power 3 A third variant calculates the exact unconditional power implements several of them. The simplest procedure is a z for approximate test statistics T (Table 7 summarizes the test with optional arcsin transformation and optional conti- supported statistics). The logic underlying this procedure nuity correction. Besides these two computational options, is to enumerate all possible outcomes for the 2 3 2 bi- one can also choose whether Cohen’s effect size measure h nomial table, given fixed sample sizes n1, n2 in the two or, alternatively, two proportions are used to specify the respective groups. This is done by choosing, as success alternate hypothesis. With the options “Use continuity cor- frequencies x1 and x2 in the first and the second groups, rection” off and “Use arcsin transform” on, the procedure respectively, any combination of the values 0 , x1 # n1 calculates power values close to those tabulated by Cohen and 0 , x2 # n2. Given the success probabilities p1, p2 in (1988, chap. 6). With both “Use continuity correction” and the two respective groups, the probability of observing a 2 “Use arcsin transform” off, the uncorrected c approxima- table X with success frequencies x1, x2 is tion is computed (Fleiss, 1981); with “Use continuity cor- rection” on and “Use arcsin transform” off, the corrected PX( |π1 , π 2 ) = c2 approximation is computed (Fleiss, 1981).  n  x n− x  n  x n− x A second variant is Fisher’s exact conditional test (Hase- 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 1 2 2.   π1( − π 1 )   π2( − π 2 ) man, 1978). Normally, G*Power 3 calculates the exact  x1  x2  G*Power 3 187

Table 7 Test Statistics Used in Tests of the Difference Between Two Independent Proportions No. Name Statistic 1 z test pooled variance πˆ− π ˆ   n πˆ + n πˆ z 1 2 ; ˆ ˆ1 ˆ 1 1 ; ˆ 1 1 2 2 = ˆ σ= π ( − π ) +  π = σ  n1 n 2  n1+ n 2 2 z test pooled variance   ˆ ˆ k 1 1 with continuity π1− π 2 +  +  2  n1 n 2  −1 lower tail correction z = ; σˆ(see No.1); k =  σˆ +1 upper tail 3 z test unpooled variance πˆ− π ˆ πˆ1− π ˆ πˆ 1 − π ˆ z 1 2 ; ˆ 1( 1) 2( 2 ) = ˆ σ = + σ n1 n2 4 z test unpooled variance   ˆ ˆ k 1 1 with continuity π1− π 2 +  +  2  n1 n 2  −1 lower tail correction z = ; σˆ(see No. 3); k =  σˆ +1 upper tail 5 Mantel–Haenszel test x1− E (x 1) n1( x 1 + x2 ) n1 n 2 (x1+ x 2 )(N− x1 − x 2 ) z = ; E x = ;V x = ( 1) N ( 1) 2 V( x1) NN()− 1 6 Likelihood ratio t x + t x +t1 − x +t 1 − x + t() N ...  lr 2 ( 1) ( 2 ) ( 1) ( 2 ) ;(t x)::x ln( x) (Upton, 1982) = ...  =  − t( n1) − t( n 2 ) −t( x1 + x 2 ) − t(N − x1− x 2 ) 7 t test with df 5 N 2 2 N − 2 (D’Agostino et al., 1988) tN −2= x 1 (1 − x2) − x 2 ( 1 − x1) N n2x 1 (1 − x1) + n 1x2(1 − x 2 )

Note—xi, success frequency in group i; ni, sample size in group i; N 5 n1 1 n2, total sample size; pˆ i 5 xi/ni. The z tests in the table are more commonly known as χ2 tests (the equivalent z test is used to provide two-sided tests).

To calculate power and the actual Type I error a*, the two proportions. The specific choice has no influence on test statistic T is computed for each table and compared the results. In the case of tests with offset, however, each with the critical value Ta. If A denotes the set of all ta- input method has a different set of available test statistics. bles X rejected by this criterion—that is, those with T . The preferred input method (see Table 6) and the test sta- Ta—then the power and the a level are given by tistic to use (see Table 8) can be changed in the Options dialog. As in the other exact procedures, the computation 1 −β = PX |π ,π ∑ XA∈ ( 1 2 ) may be time-consuming, and a limiting N can be specified and in the Options dialog that determines at which sample size α * = PX |π ,π , G*Power switches to large sample approximations. ∑ XA∈ ( 2 2 ) Also new in G*Power 3 is an exact procedure to calcu- where p2 denotes the success probability in both groups late the power for the McNemar test. The null hypothesis of as assumed in the null hypothesis. Note that the actual a this test states that the proportions of successes are identi- level can be larger than the nominal level! The preferred cal in two dependent samples. Figure 5 shows the structure input method (proportions, difference, risk ratio, or odds of the underlying design: A binary response is sampled ratio; see Table 6) and the test statistic to use (see Table 7) from the same subject or a matched pair in a standard con- can be changed in the Options dialog. Note that the test dition and in a treatment condition. The null hypothesis, statistic actually used to analyze the data must be chosen. ps 5 pt, is formally equivalent to the hypothesis for the For large sample sizes, the exact computation may take odd ratio: OR 5 p12/p21 5 1. To fully specify H1, we need too much time. Therefore, a limiting N can be specified in to specify not only the odds ratio but also the proportion the Options dialog that determines at which sample size of discordant pairs (pD)—that is, the expected proportion G*Power switches to large sample approximations. of responses that differ in the standard and the treatment G*Power 3 also provides a group of procedures to test conditions. The exact procedure used in G*Power 3 calcu- the hypothesis that the difference, risk ratio, or odds ratio lates the unconditional power for the exact conditional test, of a proportion with respect to a specified reference pro- which calculates the power conditional on the number of portion p is different under H1 from a difference, risk ratio, discordant pairs (nD). Let p(nD 5 i) be the probability that or odds ratio of the same reference proportion assumed the number of discordant pairs is i. Then, the unconditional in H0. These procedures are available in the “Exact” test power is the sum over all i ∈ {0, . . ., N} of the conditional family as “Proportions: Inequality (offset), two indepen- power for nD 5 i weighted with p(nD 5 i). This procedure dent groups (unconditional).” The enumeration proce- is very efficient, but for very large sample sizes the exact dure described above for the tests on differences between computation may take too much time. Again, a limiting N proportions without offset is also used in this case. In the that determines at which sample size G*Power switches to tests without offset, the different input parameters (e.g., a large sample approximation can be specified in the Op- differences, risk ratio) are equivalent ways of specifying tions dialog. The large sample approximation calculates 188 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Table 8 Test Statistics Used in Tests of the Difference With Offset Between Two Independent Proportions No. Name Statistic 1 z test pooled variance πˆ− π ˆ − δ n πˆ + n πˆ z = 1 2 ; σˆ = πˆ1 − π ˆ 1 //n + 1 n ; πˆ = 1 1 2 2 ˆ ( )( 1 2 ) σ n1+ n 2 2 z test pooled variance with ˆ ˆ π1− π 2 − δ +k//2( 1 n1 + 1 / n 2 ) −1 lower tail continuity correction z = ; σˆ(see No.1); k =  σˆ +1 upper tail 3 z test unpooled variance πˆ− π ˆ − δ z = 1 2 ; σˆ = πˆ(1 − π ˆ / n + πˆ(1 − π ˆ / n σˆ 1 1) 1 2 2 ) 2 4 z test unpooled variance with ˆ ˆ π1− π 2 − δ +k//2( 1 n1 + 1 / n 2 ) −1 lower tail continuity correction z = ; σˆ(see No. 3); k =  σˆ +1 upper tail 5 t test with df 5 N 2 2 tN −2=( x 1 + δn1)(1− x 2 ) −x2( 1 − x 1 − δn1) K; (D’Agostino et al., 1988) KN=( − 2)/ {N n2x 1 (1 − x1) +n 1 x2(1 − x 2 )} 6 Likelihood score ratio (difference) ˆ ˆ π1− π 2 − δ ˆ ~ ~ ~ ~ z = ;σ= π1(1 − π 1 / n1+ π 2 (1 − π 2/ n 2  K Miettinen & Nurminen (1985) σˆ  ) )  Farrington & Manning (1990) Miettinen & Nurminen: K 5 N/(N 2 1); Farrington & Manning: K 5 1 ~ ~ ~ Gart & Nam (1990) p1 5 2u cos(w) 2 b/(3a); p2 5 p1 2 d q 5 n2/n1; a 5 1 1 q; b 5 2[1 1 q 1 pˆ 1 1 q pˆ 2 1 d(q 1 2)] 2 c 5 d 1 d(2 pˆ 1 1 q 1 1) 1 pˆ 1 1 q pˆ 2; d 5 2 pˆ 1d(1 1 d) v 5 b3/(3a)3 ______2 bc/(6a2) 1 d/(2a); w 5 [3.14159 1 cos21(v/u3)]/3 u 5 sgn(v) ​√b 2/(3a)2 2 c/(3a) ​ Skewness______corrected z′ (Gart & Nam, 1990); z according to Farrington & Manning: ~ ~ ~ ~ 21 z′ 5 [√​ 1 1 4j(j 1 z) ​ 2 1]/2j; V 5 [p1(1 2 p1)/n1 1 p2(1 2 p2)/n2] 2/3 ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ ~ j 5 V /6[p1(1 2 p1)(1 2 2p1)/n1 1 p2(1 2 p2)(1 2 2p2)/n2] 7 Likelihood score ratio (risk ratio) πˆ− π ˆ φ 1 2 ˆ ~ ~ 2 ~ ~  z = ;σ= π1(1 − π 1 / n1 + φ π2(1 − π 2 / n2 K Miettinen & Nurminen (1985) σˆ  ) )  Farrington & Manning (1990) Miettinen & Nurminen: K 5 N/(N 2 1); Farrington & Manning: K 5 1 Gart & Nam (1988)  2  −b − b −4Nφ( x1 + x 2 ) π~ = φπ~ ; π~ =   ; b= − n + xφ − x − n 1 2 2 2Nφ ( 1 2) 1 2   Skewness______corrected z′ (Gart & Nam, 1988); z according to Farrington & Manning: ~ ~ ~ ~ z′ 5 [√​ 1 1 4j(j 1 z) ​ 2 1]/2j; V 5 (1 2 p1)/(p1n1) 1 (1 2 p2)/(p2n2) 2/3 ~ ~ ~ 2 ~ ~ ~ 2 j 5 1/(6V )[(1 2 p1)(1 2 2p1)/(n1p1) 1 (1 2 p2)(1 2 2p2)/(n2p2) ]

8 Likelihood score ratio (odds ratio) ˆ ~ ~ ~ ˆ ~ ~ ~ (π1− π 1 ) / π1(1 − π 1 ) −(π2 − π 2 ) / π2(1 − π 2 ) Miettinen & Nurminen (1985) z = ~ ~ ~ ~ 1/ n1π 1 ( 1 − π1 ) +1/ n2π2( 1 − π 2 ) K Miettinen & Nurminen: K 5 N/(N 2 1); Farrington______& Manning: K 5 1 ~ ~ ~ ~ 2 p1 5 p2w/[1 1 p2(w 2 1)]; p2 5 [2b 1 ​√b 1 4a(x1 1 x2) ​]/(2 a) a 5 n2(w 2 1); b 5 n1w 1 n2 2 (x1 1 x2)(w 2 1)

Note—xi, success frequency in group i; ni, sample size in group i; N 5 n1 1 n2, total sample size; pˆ i 5 xi/ni; δ, difference between proportions postulated in H0; f, risk ratio postulated in H0; w, odds ratio postulated in H0. the power on the basis of an ordinary one-sample binomial the null hypothesis that the population variance s2 has a 2 test with Bin(NpD, 0.5) as the distribution under H0 and specified value c is tested. The variance ratio s /c is used Bin[NpD, OR/(1 1 OR)] as the H1 distribution. as the effect size. The central and noncentral distributions, 2 corresponding to H0 and H1, respectively, are central c Tests for Variances distributions with N 2 1 dfs (because H0 and H1 are based Table 9 summarizes important properties of the two on the same mean). To compare the variance distributions procedures for testing hypotheses on variances that are under both hypotheses, the H1 distribution is scaled with currently supported by G*Power 3. In the one-group case, the value r postulated for the ratio s2/c in the alternate

Standard Treatment Yes No Proportion of discordant pairs: pD 5 p12 1 p21 Yes p11 p12 pt Hypothesis: ps 5 pt or, equivalently, p12 5 p21 No p21 p22 1 2 pt

ps 1 2 ps 1

Figure 5. Matched binary response design (McNemar test). G*Power 3 189

Table 9 Tests for Variances Test Null Other Test Family Hypothesis Effect Size Parameters Noncentrality Parameter Difference from χ2 tests 2 Variance ratio l 5 0 σ 1 constant (one = 2 (H : central χ2 distribution, c r = σ 1 sample case) c scaled with r) df 5 N 2 1

Inequality of two F tests σ 2 Variance ratio l 5 0 2 = 1 2 variances 2 σ 2 (H1: central F distribution, σ1 r = scaled with r) σ 2 1 df1 5 n1 2 1 df2 5 n2 2 1

2 hypothesis—that is, the noncentral distribution is rcN21 DCDFLIB (available from www.netlib.org/random/), (Ostle & Malone, 1988). In the two-groups case, H0 states which was slightly modified for our purposes. G*Power 3 that the variances in two populations are identical (s2/ does not provide the approximate power analyses that s1 5 1). As in the one-sample case, two central F distri- were available in the speed mode of G*Power 2. Two ar- butions are compared, the H1 distribution being scaled by guments guided us in supporting exact power calculations the value of the variance ratio s2/s1 postulated in H1. only. First, four-digit precision of power calculations may be mandatory in many applications. For example, both Generic Tests compromise power analyses for very large samples, and Besides the specific routines described in Tables 2–9 error probability adjustments in case of multiple tests of that cover a considerable part of the tests commonly used, significance may result in very small values of a or b G*Power 3 provides “generic” power analysis routines (Westermann & Hager, 1986). Second, as a consequence that may be used for any test based on the t, F, c2, z, or of improved computer technology, exact calculations have binomial distribution. In generic routines, the parameters become so fast that the speed gain associated with ap- of the central and noncentral distributions are specified proximate power calculations is not even noticeable. Thus, directly. from a computational standpoint, there is little advantage To demonstrate the uses and limitations of these generic to using approximate rather than exact methods (cf. Brad- routines, we will show how to do a two-tailed power analy- ley, Russell, & Reeve, 1998). sis for the one-sample t test using the generic routine. The results can be compared with those of the specific rou- Program Availability tine available in G*Power for that test. First, we select the and Internet Support “t tests” family and then “Generic t test” (the generic test option is always located at the end of the list of tests). Next, To summarize, G*Power 3 is a major extension of, and we select “Post hoc” as the type of power analysis. We improvement over, G*Power 2 in that it offers easy-to- choose a two-tailed test and .05 as a error probability. We apply power analyses for a much larger variety of common now need to specify the noncentrality parameter d and the statistical tests. Program handling is more flexible, easier degrees of freedom for our test. We look up the definitions__ to understand, and more intuitive than in G*Power 2, for the one-sample test in Table 3 and find that d 5 d√​ N ​ reducing the risk of erroneous applications. The added and df 5 N 2 1. Assuming a medium effect of d 5 0.5 graphical features should be useful for both research and and N 5 25, we arrive at d 5 0.5 · 5 5 2.5 and df 5 24. teaching purposes. Thus, G*Power 3 is likely to become After inserting these values and clicking on “Calculate,” a useful tool for empirical researchers and students of ap- we obtain a power of 1 2 b 5 .6697. The critical value plied statistics. t 5 2.0639 corresponds to the specified a. In this post hoc Like its predecessor, G*Power 3 is a noncommercial power analysis, the generic routine is almost as simple as program that can be downloaded free of charge. Copies of the specific routine. The main disadvantage of the generic the Mac and Windows versions are available only at www routines is, however, that the dependence of the noncen- .psycho.uni-duesseldorf.de/abteilungen/aap/gpower3. trality parameter on the sample size is implicit. As a con- Users interested in distributing the program in another sequence, we cannot perform a priori analyses automati- way must ask for permission from the authors. Commer- cally. Rather, we need to iterate N by hand until we find an cial distribution is strictly forbidden. appropriate power value. The G*Power 3 Web page offers an expanding Web- based tutorial describing how to use the program, along Statistical Methods and with examples. Users who let us know their e-mail ad- Numerical Algorithms dresses will be informed of updates. Although considerable effort has been put into program development and evalu- The subroutines used to compute the distribution func- ation, there is no warranty whatsoever. Users are asked to tions (and the inverse) of the noncentral t, F, c2, z, and kindly report possible bugs and difficulties in program binomial distributions are based on the C version of the handling to [email protected]. 190 Faul, Erdfelder, Lang, and Buchner

Author Note Farrington, C. P., & Manning, G. (1990). Test statistics and sample size formulae for comparative binomial trials with null hypothesis of Manuscript preparation was supported by Grant SFB 504 (Project non-zero risk difference or non-unity relative risk. Statistics in Medi- A12) from the Deutsche Forschungsgemeinschaft and a grant from the cine, 9, 1447-1454. state of Baden-Württemberg, Germany (Landesforschungsprogramm Field, A. P. (2005). Discovering statistics with SPSS (2nd ed.). London: “Evidenzbasierte Stressprävention”). Correspondence concerning this Sage. article should be addressed to F. Faul, Institut für Psychologie, Christian- Fleiss, J. L. (1981). Statistical methods for rates and proportions (2nd ­Albrechts-Universität, Olshausenstr. 40, D-24098 Kiel, Germany, or ed.). New York: Wiley. to E. Erdfelder, Lehrstuhl für Psychologie III, Universität Mannheim, Frings, C., & Wentura, D. (2005). Negative priming with masked Schloss Ehrenhof Ost 255, D-68131 Mannheim, Germany (e‑mail: ffaul@ distractor-only prime trials: Awareness moderates negative priming. psychologie.uni-kiel.de or [email protected]). Experimental Psychology, 52, 131-139. Gart, J. J., & Nam, J. (1988). Approximate interval estimation of the References ratio in binomial parameters: A review and correction for skewness. Biometrics, 44, 323-338. Akkad, D. A., Jagiello, P., Szyld, P., Goedde, R., Wieczorek, S., Gart, J. J., & Nam, J. (1990). Approximate interval estimation of the Gross, W. L., & Epplen, J. T. (2006). Promoter polymorphism difference in binomial parameters: Correction for skewness and ex- rs3087456 in the MHC class II transactivator gene is not associated tension to multiple tables. Biometrics, 46, 637-643. with susceptibility for selected autoimmune diseases in German pa- Geisser, S., & Greenhouse, S. W. (1958). An extension of Box’s re- tient groups. International Journal of Immunogenetics, 33, 59-61. sults on the use of the F distribution in multivariate analysis. Annals Back, M. D., Schmukle, S. C., & Egloff, B. (2005). Measuring task- of Mathematical Statistics, 29, 885-891. switching ability in the Implicit Association Test. Experimental Psy- Gerard, P. D., Smith, D. R., & Weerakkody, G. (1998). Limits of chology, 52, 167-179. retrospective power analysis. Journal of Wildlife Management, 62, Baeza, J. A., & Stotz, W. (2003). Host-use and selection of differ- 801-807. ently colored sea anemones by the symbiotic crab Allopetrolisthes Gigerenzer, G., Krauss, S., & Vitouch, O. (2004). The null ritual: spinifrons. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology & Ecology, 284, What you always wanted to know about significance testing but were 25-39. afraid to ask. In D. Kaplan (Ed.), The SAGE handbook of quantitative Barabesi, L., & Greco, L. (2002). A note on the exact computation methodology for the social sciences (pp. 391-408). Thousand Oaks, of the Student t, Snedecor F, and sample correlation coefficient dis- CA: Sage. tribution functions. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 51D, Gleissner, U., Clusmann, H., Sassen, R., Elger, C. E., & Helm- 105-110. staedter, C. (2006). Postsurgical outcome in pediatric patients with Berti, S., Münzer, S., Schröger, E., & Pechmann, T. (2006). Differ- epilepsy: A comparison of patients with intellectual disabilities, sub- ent interference effects in musicians and a control group. Experimen- average intelligence, and average-range intelligence. Epilepsia, 47, tal Psychology, 53, 111-116. 406-414. Bradley, D. R., Russell, R. L., & Reeve, C. P. (1998). The accuracy Goldstein, R. (1989). Power and sample size via MS/PC-DOS comput- of four approximations to noncentral F. Behavior Research Methods, ers. American Statistician, 43, 253-262. Instruments, & Computers, 30, 478-500. Hager, W. (2006). Die Fallibilität empirischer Daten und die Notwen- Bredenkamp, J. (1969). Über die Anwendung von Signifikanztests bei digkeit der Kontrolle von falschen Entscheidungen [The fallibility Theorie-testenden Experimenten [The application of significance tests of empirical data and the need for controlling for false decisions]. in theory-testing experiments]. Psychologische Beiträge, 11, 275-285. Zeitschrift für Psychologie, 214, 10-23. Bredenkamp, J., & Erdfelder, E. (1985). Multivariate Varianzanalyse Haseman, J. K. (1978). Exact sample sizes for use with the Fisher–Irwin nach dem V-Kriterium [Multivariate analysis of variance based on the test for 2 3 2 tables. Biometrics, 34, 106-109. V-criterion]. Psychologische Beiträge, 27, 127-154. Hoenig, J. N., & Heisey, D. M. (2001). The abuse of power: The perva- Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., & Faul, F. (1996). Teststärkeanalysen sive fallacy of power calculations for data analysis. American Statisti- [Power analyses]. In E. Erdfelder, R. Mausfeld, T. Meiser, & cian, 55, 19-24. G. Rudinger (Eds.), Handbuch Quantitative Methoden [Handbook of Hoffmann, J., & Sebald, A. (2005). Local contextual cuing in visual quantitative methods] (pp. 123-136). Weinheim, Germany: Psycholo- search. Experimental Psychology, 52, 31-38. gie Verlags Union. Huynh, H., & Feldt, L. S. (1970). Conditions under which mean square Buchner, A., Erdfelder, E., & Faul, F. (1997). How to use G*Power ratios in repeated measurements designs have exact F-distribution. [Computer manual]. Available at www.psycho.uni-duesseldorf.de/ Journal of the American Statistical Association, 65, 1582-1589. aap/projects/gpower/how_to_use_gpower.html. Keppel, G., & Wickens, T. D. (2004). Design and analysis. A research- Busbey, A. B. I. (1999). Macintosh shareware/freeware earthscience er’s handbook (4th ed.). Upper Saddle River, NJ: Pearson Education software. Computers & Geosciences, 25, 335-340. International. Cohen, J. (1988). Statistical power analysis for the behavioral sciences Kornbrot, D. E. (1997). Review of statistical shareware G*Power. Brit- (2nd ed.). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum. ish Journal of Mathematical & Statistical Psychology, 50, 369-370. D’Agostino, R. B., Chase, W., & Belanger, A. (1988). The appropri- Kromrey, J., & Hogarty, K. Y. (2000). Problems with probabilistic ateness of some common procedures for testing the equality of two in- hindsight: A comparison of methods for retrospective statistical power dependent binomial populations. American Statistician, 42, 198-202. analysis. Multiple Linear Regression Viewpoints, 26, 7-14. Erdfelder, E. (1984). Zur Bedeutung und Kontrolle des b-Fehlers bei Lenth, R. V. (2001). Some practical guidelines for effective sample size der inferenzstatistischen Prüfung log-linearer Modelle [Significance determination. American Statistician, 55, 187-193. and control of the b error in statistical tests of log-linear models]. Levin, J. R. (1997). Overcoming feelings of powerlessness in “aging” Zeitschrift für Sozialpsychologie, 15, 18-32. researches: A primer on statistical power in analysis of variance de- Erdfelder, E., Buchner, A., Faul, F., & Brandt, M. (2004). GPOWER: signs. Psychology & Aging, 12, 84-106. Teststärkeanalysen leicht gemacht [Power analyses made easy]. In McKeon, J. J. (1974). F approximations to the distribution of Hotel- 2 E. Erdfelder & J. Funke (Eds.), Allgemeine Psychologie und deduktivis­ ling’s T 0. Biometrika, 61, 381-383. tische Methodologie [Experimental psychology and deductive method- Mellina, E., Hinch, S. G., Donaldson, E. M., & Pearson, G. (2005). ology] (pp. 148-166). Göttingen: Vandenhoeck & Ruprecht. Stream habitat and rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss) physiologi- Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (1996). GPOWER: A general cal stress responses to streamside clear-cut logging in British Colum- power analysis program. Behavior Research Methods, Instruments, & bia. Canadian Journal of Forest Research, 35, 541-556. Computers, 28, 1-11. Miettinen, O., & Nurminen, M. (1985). Comparative analysis of two Erdfelder, E., Faul, F., & Buchner, A. (2005). Power analysis for rates. Statistics in Medicine, 4, 213-226. categorical methods. In B. S. Everitt & D. C. Howell (Eds.), Encyclo- Müller, J., Manz, R., & Hoyer, J. (2002). Was tun, wenn die Test- pedia of statistics in behavioral science (pp. 1565-1570). Chichester, stärke zu gering ist? Eine praktikable Strategie für Prä–Post-Designs U.K.: Wiley. [What to do if statistical power is low? A practical strategy for pre– G*Power 3 191

post-designs]. Psychotherapie, Psychosomatik, Medizinische Psy- Shieh, G. (2003). A comparative study of power and sample size cal- chologie, 52, 408-416. culations for multivariate general linear models. Multivariate Behav- Muller, K. E., & Barton, C. N. (1989). Approximate power for repeated- ioral Research, 38, 285-307. measures ANOVA lacking sphericity. Journal of the American Statistical Smith, R. E., & Bayen, U. J. (2005). The effects of working memory Association, 84, 549-555. resource availability on prospective memory: A formal modeling ap- Muller, K. E., LaVange, L. M., Landesman-Ramey, S., & Ramey, proach. Experimental Psychology, 52, 243-256. C. T. (1992). Power calculations for general linear multivariate models Steidl, R. J., Hayes, J. P., & Schauber, E. (1997). Statistical power including repeated measures applications. Journal of the American analysis in wildlife research. Journal of Wildlife Management, 61, Statistical Association, 87, 1209-1226. 270-279. Muller, K. E., & Peterson, B. L. (1984). Practical methods for com- Suissa, S., & Shuster, J. J. (1985). Exact unconditional sample sizes puting power in testing the multivariate general linear hypothesis. for 2 3 2 binomial trial. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society A, Computational Statistics & Data Analysis, 2, 143-158. 148, 317-327. Myers, J. L., & Well, A. D. (2003). Research design and statistical Thomas, L., & Krebs, C. J. (1997). A review of statistical power analysis analysis (2nd ed.). Mahwah, NJ: Erlbaum. software. Bulletin of the Ecological Society of America, 78, 126-139. O’Brien, R. G., & Kaiser, M. K. (1985). MANOVA method for analyz- Upton, G. J. G. (1982). A comparison of alternative tests for the 2 3 2 ing repeated measures designs: An extensive primer. Psychological comparative trial. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society A, 145, Bulletin, 97, 316-333. 86-105. O’Brien, R. G., & Muller, K. E. (1993). Unified power analysis for Westermann, R., & Hager, W. (1986). Error probabilities in educa- t-tests through multivariate hypotheses. In L. K. Edwards (Ed.), Ap- tional and psychological research. Journal of Educational Statistics, plied analysis of variance in behavioral science (pp. 297-344). New 11, 117-146. York: Dekker. Zumbo, B. D., & Hubley, A. M. (1998). A note on misconceptions O’Brien, R. G., & Shieh, G. (1999). Pragmatic, unifying algorithm concerning prospective and retrospective power. The Statistician, 47, gives power probabilities for common F tests of the multivariate gen- 385-388. eral linear hypothesis. Available at www.bio.ri.ccf.org/UnifyPow. Ortseifen, C., Bruckner, T., Burke, M., & Kieser, M. (1997). An Notes overview of software tools for sample size determination. Informatik, Biometrie & Epidemiologie in Medizin & Biologie, 28, 91-118. 1. The observed power is reported in many frequently used computer Ostle, B., & Malone, L. C. (1988). Statistics in research: Basic con- programs (e.g., the MANOVA procedure of SPSS). cepts and techniques for research workers (4th ed.). Ames: Iowa State 2. We recommend checking the degrees of freedom reported by Press. G*Power by comparing them, for example, with those reported by the Pillai, K. C. S., & Mijares, T. A. (1959). On the moments of the trace program used to analyze the sample data. If the degrees of freedom do of a matrix and approximations to its distribution. Annals of Math- not match, the input provided to G*Power is incorrect and the power ematical Statistics, 30, 1135-1140. calculations do not apply. Pillai, K. C. S., & Samson, P., Jr. (1959). On Hotelling’s generaliza- 3. Plots of the central and noncentral distributions are shown only tion of T 2. Biometrika, 46, 160-168. for tests based on the t, F, z, c2, or binomial distribution. No plots are Quednow, B. B., Kühn, K.-U., Stelzenmueller, R., Hoenig, K., shown for tests that involve an enumeration procedure (e.g., the McNe- Maier, W., & Wagner, M. (2004). Effects of serotonergic and norad- mar test). renergic antidepressants on auditory startle response in patients with 4. We thank Dave Kenny for making us aware of the fact that the major depression. Psychopharmacology, 175, 399-406. t test (correlation) power analyses of G*Power 2 are correct only in the Rao, C. R. (1951). An asymptotic expansion of the distribution of point–biserial case (i.e., for correlations between a binary variable and Wilks’s criterion. Bulletin of the International Statistical Institute, a continuous variable, the latter being normally distributed for each 33, 177-180. value of the binary variable). For correlations between two continu- Rasch, B., Friese, M., Hofmann, W. J., & Naumann, E. (2006a). ous variables following a bivariate normal distribution, the t test (cor- Quantitative Methoden 1: Einführung in die Statistik (2. Auflage) relation) procedure of G*Power 2 overestimates power. For this reason, [Quantitative methods 1: Introduction to statistics (2nd ed.)]. Heidel- G*Power 3 offers separate power analyses for point–biserial correlations berg, Germany: Springer. (in the t family of distributions) and correlations between two normally Rasch, B., Friese, M., Hofmann, W. J., & Naumann, E. (2006b). distributed variables (in the exact distribution family). However, power Quantitative Methoden 2: Einführung in die Statistik (2. Auflage) values usually differ only slightly between procedures. To illustrate, as- [Quantitative methods 2: Introduction to statistics (2nd ed.)]. Heidel- sume we are interested in the power of a two-tailed test of H0: r 5 .00 for berg, Germany: Springer. continuously distributed measures derived from two Implicit Association Rencher, A. C. (1998). Multivariate statistical inference and applica- Tests (IATs) differing in content. Assume further that, due to method- tions. New York: Wiley. specific variance in both versions of the IAT, the true Pearson correlation Richardson, J. T. E. (1996). Measures of effect size. Behavior Research is actually r 5 .30 (effect size). Given a 5 .05 and N 5 57 (see Back, Methods, Instruments, & Computers, 28, 12-22. Schmukle, & Egloff, 2005, p. 173), an exact post hoc power analysis for Scheffé, H. (1959). The analysis of variance. New York: Wiley. “Correlations: Differences from constant (one sample case)” reveals the Schwarz, W., & Müller, D. (2006). Spatial associations in number- correct power value of 1 2 b 5 .63. Choosing the incorrect “Correla- related tasks: A comparison of manual and pedal responses. Experi- tion: point biserial model” procedure from the t test family would result mental Psychology, 53, 4-15. in 1 2 b 5 .65. Sheppard, C. (1999). How large should my sample be? Some quick guides to sample size and the power of tests. Marine Pollution Bul- (Manuscript received December 8, 2006; letin, 38, 439-447. accepted for publication January 23, 2007.)