When Life Imitates Art: Surrogate Decision Making at the End of Life

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

When Life Imitates Art: Surrogate Decision Making at the End of Life Ethics in Practice Kristi L. Kirschner, MD, Editor When Life Imitates Art: Surrogate Decision Making at the End of Life Susan P. Shapiro The privileging of the substituted judgment standard as the gold standard for surrogate decision making in law and bioethics has constrained the research agenda in end-of-life decision making. The empirical literature is inundated with a plethora of “Newlywed Game” designs, in which potential patients and potential surrogates respond to hypothetical scenarios to see how often they “get it right.” The preoccupation with determining the capacity of surrogates to accurately reproduce the judgments of another makes a number of assumptions that blind scholars to the variables central to understanding how surrogates actually make medical decisions on behalf of another. These assumptions include that patient preferences are knowable, surrogates have adequate and accurate information, time stands still, patients get the surrogates they want, patients want and surrogates utilize substituted judgment criteria, and surrogates are disinterested. This article examines these assumptions and considers the challenges of designing research that makes them problematic. Key words: substituted judgment, surrogate decision making, research design here are sometimes moments in an patterns, causes, and places of death. In some respects these changes “have made living easier and dying harder.” academic life when one’s scholarly agenda [1(p14)] More effective treatments may significantly extend life T and personal biography unexpectedly, but may also confront dying individuals, their families, and sometimes tragically, overlap. This collision opens health care providers with a prolonged period of dying that a window into one’s research that had been involves complex choices about end-of-life care. …[T]he previously opaque and affords an opportunity to timing of an individual’s death has also changed. In 1992, it was estimated that 70%[2] of the 6,000 deaths that occur see it with new eyes. It exposes what had been daily in the United States are somehow timed or negotiated invisible and imposes an overlay of shades of gray with family, doctors, and the dying person when compe- over what had formerly appeared black and white. tent, quietly agreeing to not use death-delaying And so it was that, just as I was beginning an technology.3(pp5–6) observational study of surrogate decision making But dying persons are not always able to “negoti- at the end of life, I took up residence – quite ate” their own deaths. Two studies found, for ex- unexpectedly – in a hospital where my father ample, that of all decisions to withhold or with- would spend his last days. This article, which was draw life support in the medical-surgical intensive written when my father was healthy and vibrant, is care units (ICUs) of two urban hospitals, only 3%– now retold through the lens of a recent denizen of 4% were made by the patients themselves; the the world my research hopes to understand. It other patients were not considered competent.2,4 draws on what the scholarly literature tells us Of course, patients who end up in an ICU may lack about surrogate decision making and is overlaid competence more often than those who face death with preliminary observations from my research site and 3 weeks of experience as a research subject in a quite different setting. Susan P. Shapiro, PhD, is Senior Research Fellow, American My experience, however momentous it was to Bar Foundation, Chicago, Illinois. me, is not all that uncommon. Over the last cen- Top Stroke Rehabil 2007;14(4):80–92 tury, advances in science, medicine, and public © 2007 Thomas Land Publishers, Inc. health have yielded dramatic changes, not only in www.thomasland.com the life expectancy of older adults, but also in the doi: 10.1310/tsr1404-80 80 Surrogate Decision Making 81 at home or in other settings. One does not have to mote their well-being, and choose, after weighing do the math to recognize that the final days of an the benefits and burdens, a course of action with extraordinary proportion of patients’ lives are be- the greatest net benefit.7,20,23,24 ing choreographed by others. The most significant The privileging of the substituted judgment life-and-death decisions of our lives – literally – are standard in law and bioethics has inspired much being delegated to others. of the research on surrogate decision making. Some of us plan for the possibility that we will The literature is inundated with scholarly ver- lose the capacity to choreograph our lives or sions of the Newlywed Game25,26 in which re- deaths. We write living wills,5–7 “Ulysses”8–10or searchers test whether surrogates get decsions pre-commitment contracts, or durable powers of “right” and when they are most likely to do so. attorney in which we (1) specify our values, prefer- Most of these contests pose hypothetical medical ences, and decision rules; (2) designate parties to scenarios to potential patients and their potential carry them out and to exercise discretion on our surrogates (and occasionally their physicians as behalf; and (3) delineate the conditions under well) to assess the correspondence in predicted which these documents go into effect or desist. We treatment preferences.20,27–34 (Some studies even opt for contract over trust to protect our future invite conversation on end-of-life issues between interests.11–15 patient and surrogate before they complete the For the majority of us who fail to provide a questionnaire – typically to no effect.) Other contract or whose instructions are too vague or studies focus not on specific wishes or treatment incomplete, legal statutes and court decisions seek preferences, but on general goals for care or pref- to preserve our constitutional rights of autonomy erences about the process by which patients want and self-determination16,17 and to restore our voices end-of-life decisions to be made.34 Without going when they have been otherwise silenced. Most juris- into great detail, the results of several dozen stud- dictions in the United States specify who is to speak ies suggest that some of the assumptions underly- on the patients’ behalf when they have not named a ing the use of substituted judgment may be illu- surrogate18 and enunciate standards by which sur- sory. A recent meta-analysis of 16 of these studies rogates make decisions for the patients and eviden- (encompassing 151 scenarios and 2,595 patient/ tiary rules to guide their judgments. surrogate pairs) found an overall prediction accu- In most states, the substituted judgment standard racy of 68%, which varies somewhat by type of is considered the gold standard and given highest health problem or type of medical intervention priority. It dictates that surrogate decision makers offered in the scenario.35 should choose as the patients would now choose, Individual studies found substantial discrepan- if they were competent and aware of all the rel- cies between subjects and surrogates in their as- evant facts and circumstances, including the fact sessment of the subjects’ satisfaction and quality of that they are incompetent. It is as if the patients life; they also found that spouses and children tend were to miraculously awaken from their coma or to underestimate the subjects’ functional status. attain lucidity for a few moments, knowing that Both these assessments are essential to the they will soon lapse back into their former state.16 surrogate’s ability to make either substituted or Surrogates must “don the mental mantle”19(p545) or best interest judgments. Indeed, in several studies, stand in the patients’ shoes20 – taking into account surrogates did no better than chance (and occa- the patients’ prior statements, actions, instruc- sionally did worse). Nor did the surrogates named tions, personal value system, character, goals, be- by subjects as their preferred decision maker (akin liefs, attitudes, and lifestyle – to try replicate what to a person appointed under a durable power of the patients would have wanted.21,22 Where the attorney) do any better than the others. Moreover, patients’ preferences cannot be determined, legal neither frequency of contact, similarity in age or doctrine instructs surrogates to adopt a best interest gender, nor type of relationship affected the accu- standard to advance the patients’ interests, pro- racy of their predictions. 82 TOPICS IN STROKE REHABILITATION/JULY-AUG 2007 Data collected by the SUPPORT study from wed Game” model makes several problematic as- thousands of “real” hospitalized patients, many of sumptions about that context. them critically ill, and their actual surrogates36–39 found much the same thing. Patients (if able) and Our Wishes Are Knowable surrogates were interviewed on multiple occasions throughout the course of hospitalization and after One reason surrogates guess wrong is that the release/death and asked about care, prognosis, subjects in these studies behave like the rest of us. quality of life, treatment preferences, and decision Few of them had discussed life-sustaining inter- making. As in the hypothetical studies,20,27–35 re- ventions with their physicians or families,20 even searchers compared patient and surrogate prefer- those “at relatively high risk of terminal ences (for those hospitalized patients who were events.”29(pM120) Because the vast majority of the competent and able to speak – a potentially biased surrogates were confident that they knew the sub- sample, of course). They found a slightly higher jects’ preferences, they were probably even less level of agreement (74%) than in the hypothetical likely to confer or check out the accuracy of their scenarios administered to healthy subjects. Again, assumptions and understandings. Family mem- researchers found differences in perceptions of bers, socialized in the same cauldron of values and quality of life between patients and their surro- beliefs, may assume that they share the same gates, for example, in their willingness to give up worldview and confuse their values and prefer- months of life in their current state of health for a ences for those in whose shoes they now stand.
Recommended publications
  • Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: a Systematic Mapping Study
    Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: A Systematic Mapping Study Rahul Mohanani, Iflaah Salman, Burak Turhan, Member, IEEE, Pilar Rodriguez and Paul Ralph Abstract—One source of software project challenges and failures is the systematic errors introduced by human cognitive biases. Although extensively explored in cognitive psychology, investigations concerning cognitive biases have only recently gained popularity in software engineering research. This paper therefore systematically maps, aggregates and synthesizes the literature on cognitive biases in software engineering to generate a comprehensive body of knowledge, understand state of the art research and provide guidelines for future research and practise. Focusing on bias antecedents, effects and mitigation techniques, we identified 65 articles (published between 1990 and 2016), which investigate 37 cognitive biases. Despite strong and increasing interest, the results reveal a scarcity of research on mitigation techniques and poor theoretical foundations in understanding and interpreting cognitive biases. Although bias-related research has generated many new insights in the software engineering community, specific bias mitigation techniques are still needed for software professionals to overcome the deleterious effects of cognitive biases on their work. Index Terms—Antecedents of cognitive bias. cognitive bias. debiasing, effects of cognitive bias. software engineering, systematic mapping. 1 INTRODUCTION OGNITIVE biases are systematic deviations from op- knowledge. No analogous review of SE research exists. The timal reasoning [1], [2]. In other words, they are re- purpose of this study is therefore as follows: curring errors in thinking, or patterns of bad judgment Purpose: to review, summarize and synthesize the current observable in different people and contexts. A well-known state of software engineering research involving cognitive example is confirmation bias—the tendency to pay more at- biases.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • A Motivational Account of the Impact Bias DISSERTATION Presented In
    A Motivational Account of the Impact Bias DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Gina M. Hoover, M.A. Graduate Program in Psychology The Ohio State University 2011 Committee: Dr. Gifford Weary, Advisor Dr. Kentaro Fujita Dr. Jennifer Crocker Copyrighted by Gina M. Hoover 2011 Abstract The impact bias is the tendency for people to overestimate the intensity and duration of their feelings in response to some future outcome (Gilbert, Driver-Linn, & Wilson, 2002; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003; Wilson & Gilbert, 2005). Most explanations for the bias conceptualize it as an undesirable side effect of how the cognitive system goes about making predictions. Yet, perhaps such mispredictions can serve an important self- regulatory function, helping people achieve their goals. This research argued for a motivated impact bias. That is, there are times when people may be driven to predict feelings more extreme than they actually will experience. By overestimating one’s feelings to some future outcome, the stakes of succeeding or failing at gaining that outcome are raised. As such, this may energize people to pursue their goals to achieve the desired outcomes. The more one wants to gain (or avoid) some outcome, the more likely s/he will be to exhibit such an impact bias. To investigate this question, participants were asked to predict their feelings in response to the outcome of a performance task (visual responsiveness task in studies 1-3, creativity task in study 4, and verbal test in study 5) they would complete.
    [Show full text]
  • Integrating Temporal Biases the Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences Lawrence J
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Integrating Temporal Biases The Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences Lawrence J. Sanna1 and Norbert Schwarz2 1University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and 2University of Michigan ABSTRACT—We provide an integrative account of temporal bi- come to mind. This interaction drives the emergence of these biases, ases (confidence changes, planning fallacy, impact bias, and as well as debiasing. hindsight bias). Students listed either 3 or 12 thoughts about success or failure before an upcoming real-life exam or im- TEMPORAL BIASES mediately after learning their grades. Previous explanations had focused on how thought content alone (what comes to mind) Figure 1 illustrates the time course of the four temporal biases that are influences temporal biases. We found, however, an interaction our focus here. between thought content and accessibility experiences (how eas- ily or difficultly thoughts come to mind). Thinking about 3 ways to succeed (success was easy to bring to mind) was equivalent to Confidence Changes thinking about 12 ways to fail (failure was difficult to bring to People are less confident in success when events draw near than they mind), and conversely, thinking about 3 ways to fail was are at a more distant time. For example, participants taking an im- equivalent to thinking about 12 ways to succeed. In no case was mediate test were less confident than those taking a test in 4 weeks thought content alone sufficient to predict the biases. These re- (Nisan, 1972; see also Gilovich, Kerr, & Medvec, 1993). Similarly, sults have implications for debiasing strategies and other judg- college seniors were more muted in estimating their first-job salaries ments over time.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-Being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Walsh, E. (2009). Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments. (Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City, University of London) This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/18257/ Link to published version: Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments Emma Walsh Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy City University, London Table of Contents Table of Contents ........................................................................................................ 2 List of Tables ..............................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Impact Bias in Student Evaluations of Higher Education Anthony Grimes*, Dominic Medway, Adrienne Foos† and Anna Goatman
    Studies in Higher Education, 2017 Vol. 42, No. 6, 945–962, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2015.1071345 Impact bias in student evaluations of higher education Anthony Grimes*, Dominic Medway, Adrienne Foos† and Anna Goatman Manchester Business School, University of Manchester, Booth Street West, Manchester M15 6PB, UK In the context of higher education, this study examines the extent to which affective evaluations of the student experience are influenced by the point at which they are made (i.e. before the experience begins, whilst it is happening and after it has ended). It adopts a between-groups quantitative analysis of the affective evaluations made by 360 future, current and past postgraduate students of a UK business school. The study validates the proposition that affective forecasts and memories of the student experience are considerably inflated in prospect and retrospect; a finding that implies a significant impact bias. It is concluded that the impact bias may have important implications for influencing the effectiveness of student decision-making, the timing and comparability of student course evaluations, and understanding the nature and effects of word-of-mouth communication regarding the student experience. Keywords: student attitudes; student evaluation; student expectations; student experience; students’ perceptions Introduction In recent years, the student experience has become a topic of growing interest and importance in higher education (HE). Accordingly, student evaluations of courses and institutions are now commonplace, with results feeding into various metrics, per- formance indicators and rankings of institutional excellence. The increased focus on the student experience in HE has been accompanied by significant academic research on the matter.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias
    1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias http://blog.beaufortes.com/2007/06/embrace-your-co.html Cognitive Biases are distortions in the way humans see things in comparison to the purely logical way that mathematics, economics, and yes even project management would have us look at things. The problem is not that we have them… most of them are wired deep into our brains following millions of years of evolution. The problem is that we don’t know about them, and consequently don’t take them into account when we have to make important decisions. (This area is so important that Daniel Kahneman won a Nobel Prize in 2002 for work tying non-rational decision making, and cognitive bias, to mainstream economics) People don’t behave rationally, they have emotions, they can be inspired, they have cognitive bias! Tying that into how we run projects (project leadership as a compliment to project management) can produce results you wouldn’t believe. You have to know about them to guard against them, or use them (but that’s another article)... So let’s get more specific. After the jump, let me show you a great list of cognitive biases. I’ll bet that there are at least a few that you haven’t heard of before! Decision making and behavioral biases Bandwagon effect — the tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink, herd behaviour, and manias. Bias blind spot — the tendency not to compensate for one’s own cognitive biases. Choice-supportive bias — the tendency to remember one’s choices as better than they actually were.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Perspective How People Decide What They Want To
    PERSPECTIVE HOW PEOPLE DECIDE WHAT THEY WANT TO KNOW Tali Sharot1* & Cass R. Sunstein2 1Affective Brain Lab, Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK 2Harvard Law School, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA * To whom corresponds should be addressed [email protected] 1 Abstract Immense amounts of information are now accessible to people, including information that bears on their past, present and future. An important research challenge is to determine how people decide to seek or avoid information. Here, we propose a framework of information- seeking that aims to integrate the motives that drive information-seeking and its avoidance. Our framework rests on the idea that information can alter people’s action, affect and cognition in both positive and negative ways. The suggestion is that people assess these influences and integrate them into a calculation of the value of information that leads to information-seeking or avoidance. The theory offers a framework for characterizing and quantifying individual differences in information-seeking, which we hypothesize may also be diagnostic of mental health. We consider biases that can lead to both insufficient and excessive information-seeking and discuss how the framework offered can help government agencies to assess the welfare effects of mandatory information disclosure. 2 How do people decide what they want to know? As massive amounts of information are becoming available to people, this question is more pertinent today than ever. People must now decide whether they want to receive highly personalized information about their health, genetic make-up, and financial prospects, as well as those of their children.
    [Show full text]
  • “The Futile Pursuit of Happiness.” by Jon Gertner, Adapted from the New York Times Magazine
    The Futile Pursuit of Happiness Jon Gertner “The Futile Pursuit of Happiness.” By Jon Gertner, adapted from The New York Times Magazine. September 7, 2003. Used by permission. Copyright laws may prohibit photocopying this document without express permission. The Futile Pursuit of Happiness by Jon Gertner 1 If Daniel Gilbert is right, then you are wrong. That is to say, if Daniel Gilbert is right, then you 2 are wrong to believe that a new car will make you as happy as you imagine. That’s 3 because when it comes to predicting exactly how you will feel in the future, you are most 4 likely wrong. 5 A professor in Harvard’s department of psychology, Gilbert likes to tell people that he studies 6 happiness. But it would be more precise to say that Gilbert—along with the psychologist Tim 7 Wilson of the University of Virginia, the economist George Loewenstein of Carnegie-Mellon 8 and the psychologist (and Nobel laureate in economics) Daniel Kahneman of Princeton—has 9 taken the lead in studying a specific type of emotional and behavioral prediction. In the past 10 few years, these four men have begun to question the decision-making process that shapes 11 our sense of well-being: how do we predict what will make us happy or unhappy—and then 12 how do we feel after the actual experience? . 13 Until recently, this was uncharted territory. How we forecast our feelings, and whether those 14 predictions match our future emotional states, had never been the stuff of laboratory 15 research.
    [Show full text]
  • The Enhancement Bias in Consumer Decisions to Adopt and Utilize Product Innovations
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Marketing Papers Wharton Faculty Research 2003 The Enhancement Bias in Consumer Decisions to Adopt and Utilize Product Innovations Shenghui Zhao Robert J. Meyer University of Pennsylvania Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/marketing_papers Part of the Advertising and Promotion Management Commons, Behavioral Economics Commons, Business Intelligence Commons, Cognitive Psychology Commons, Experimental Analysis of Behavior Commons, Management Information Systems Commons, and the Marketing Commons Recommended Citation Zhao, S., & Meyer, R. J. (2003). The Enhancement Bias in Consumer Decisions to Adopt and Utilize Product Innovations. Retrieved from https://repository.upenn.edu/marketing_papers/329 This is an unpublished manscript. This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/marketing_papers/329 For more information, please contact [email protected]. The Enhancement Bias in Consumer Decisions to Adopt and Utilize Product Innovations Abstract The ability of consumers to anticipate the value they will draw from new product generations that expand the capabilities of incumbent goods is explored. Drawing on previous research in affective forecasting, the work explores a hypothesis that consumers will frequently overestimate the benefits they envision drawing from new added product features and underestimate the learning costs required to realize those benefits. This hypothesis is tested using a computer simulation in which subjects are trained to play a Pacman-like arcade game where icons are moved over a screen by different forms of tactile controls. Respondents are then given the option to play a series of games for money with an incumbent game platform or pay to play with an alternative version that offer either expanded (Experiments 1 and 2) or simplified (Experiment 3) sets of controls.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases Cognitive Biases and Heuristics Are Mental Shortcuts That Aid in Processing of Information, but Can Lead to Suboptimal Decisions
    Cognitive Biases Cognitive biases and heuristics are mental shortcuts that aid in processing of information, but can lead to suboptimal decisions. List of Biases and Heuristics Bias/Effect Description Business Problem Category Advice Ambiguity bias Preference for known Need to We favor options that appear simple risks over unknown risks Act Fast or that have more complete information over more complex, ambiguous options. Anchoring Overreliance on initial Think of each Too Much We notice when something has information piece of Information changed. information as independently as possible, then review them collectively Anecdotal fallacy Overvaluing personal Not We find stories and patterns even in experience when Enough sparse data. compared to data Meaning Anthropomorphism Mistakenly imbuing things Not We find stories and patterns even in with human traits Enough sparse data. Meaning Appeal to novelty Belief that something new Need to In order to stay focused, we favor the is inherently superior to Act Fast immediate, relatable thing in front of status quo us over the delayed and distant. Appeal to probability Mistaking probability for Even the best Not We simplify probabilities and fallacy certainty laid plans can Enough numbers to make them easier to think fail, even when Meaning about. they are more likely than not to succeed Argument from Mistaken belief that a Some Not We fill in characteristics from fallacy fallacious argument arguments are Enough stereotypes, generalities, and prior implies that the better than Meaning histories
    [Show full text]