Ewalsh VERSION 3

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Ewalsh VERSION 3 View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by City Research Online Walsh, E. & Ayton, P. (2009). My Imagination Versus Your Feelings: Can Personal Affective Forecasts Be Improved by Knowing Other Peoples' Emotions?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(4), pp. 351-360. doi: 10.1037/a0017984 City Research Online Original citation: Walsh, E. & Ayton, P. (2009). My Imagination Versus Your Feelings: Can Personal Affective Forecasts Be Improved by Knowing Other Peoples' Emotions?. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 15(4), pp. 351-360. doi: 10.1037/a0017984 Permanent City Research Online URL: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/3601/ Copyright & reuse City University London has developed City Research Online so that its users may access the research outputs of City University London's staff. Copyright © and Moral Rights for this paper are retained by the individual author(s) and/ or other copyright holders. All material in City Research Online is checked for eligibility for copyright before being made available in the live archive. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to from other web pages. Versions of research The version in City Research Online may differ from the final published version. Users are advised to check the Permanent City Research Online URL above for the status of the paper. Enquiries If you have any enquiries about any aspect of City Research Online, or if you wish to make contact with the author(s) of this paper, please email the team at [email protected]. My Imagination 1 Running head: SURROGATE EMOTIONS AND AFFECTIVE FORECASTS My Imagination vs. Your Feelings: Can Personal Affective Forecasts Be Improved By Knowing Other Peoples’ Emotions? Emma Walsh & Peter Ayton City University, London UK My Imagination 2 Abstract A proposed remedy for biased affective forecasts is to base judgments on the actual feelings of people (surrogates) currently experiencing the event, rather than using imagination which conjures an inaccurate vision of the future. Gilbert et al. (2009) forced people to use surrogate reports by withholding all event information, resulting in better predictions. However, in life surrogate information rarely supplants event information - can people effectively integrate both types of information into their judgments? In 5 studies, respondents predicted the impact of a health state on their own happiness. Respondents incorporated surrogate information into their judgments both in the presence and absence of event information. However, they inappropriately discounted other people’s experiences as a valid predictor of their own - particularly in the presence of event information – and imagined their happiness would be different to surrogates’ happiness. Excluding pre-existing event knowledge, changing the size of the surrogate sample, or increasing the size of the response scale, did not alter the adjustment. Although surrogate information improved affective forecasts, its influence was diminished by the presence of event information. Key words: Affective forecasting, Surrogation, Impact bias, Health state My Imagination 3 My Imagination vs. Your Feelings: Can Personal Affective Forecasts Be Improved By Knowing Other Peoples’ Emotions? The conclusion that people predict their future emotions inaccurately - by overestimating both the intensity and duration of their emotional response to an event - is clearly demonstrated in a range of empirical studies (Dunn, Wilson, & Gilbert, 2004; Gilbert, Driver-Linn, & Wilson, 2002; Ubel et al., 2001). This effect, known as the impact bias, has been shown for a wide-range of events, from mis-predicting the impact of winning a football match (Wilson, Wheatley, Meyers, Gilbert, & Axsom, 2000), to electoral defeat (Gilbert, Pinel, Wilson, Blumberg, & Wheatley, 1998), to failing a driving test (Ayton, Pott, & Elwakili, 2007). The impact bias has also been measured for more consequential events, such as the influence of chronic health states on well-being: people overestimate the impact of a variety of health states on both their own happiness as well as other peoples’ happiness (Walsh & Ayton, 2009) despite there actually being little variation in the happiness of people living with or without chronic health states (Brickman, Coates, & Janoff-Bulman, 1978; Walsh & Ayton, 2009). This leads to a discrepancy between non-patients’ predictions and patients’ actual experiences (Hurst et al., 1994; Riis et al., 2005). A discrepancy between anticipated and experienced outcomes may lead to suboptimal decisions (Sevdalis & Harvey, 2007) which, in a medical context could have serious long-term consequences. Why do we persistently make these errors in our affective forecasts? Several explanations suggest that we do not accurately imagine the future event. One proposal, known as the focusing illusion, suggests that people incorrectly focus on the target event and disregard other events which could also impact and moderate their future emotions, resulting in extreme forecasts My Imagination 4 (Kahneman, Krueger, Schkade, Schwarz, & Stone, 2006; Schkade & Kahneman, 1998; Wilson et al., 2000). A further proposed error with the imagination process is that people fail to imagine that they would adapt to the future event, a process of unforeseen adaptation (Gilbert et al., 1998). They imagine their immediate affective reaction and fail to take into consideration that over time their reaction to the event would diminish and they would return to their baseline emotional state. If we can not accurately imagine our reaction to a future event, how about recalling memories of our past experiences as a guide to predicting our future happiness? Studies have shown that not only can we not accurately remember how we felt (Wirtz, Kruger, Scallon, & Diener, 2003) but that we also tend to recall atypical instances of the event, thereby biasing our future predictions (Kahneman, Fredrickson, Schreiber, & Redelmeier, 1993; Morewedge, Gilbert, & Wilson, 2005). It would therefore seem that our imaginations, and our memories, lead us down the garden path when it comes to anticipating our future emotions. Furthermore, endeavours to reduce the impact of these effects on predictions have produced mixed results. Ubel, Loewenstein, and Jepson (2005) compared different methods designed either to reduce the impact of the focusing illusion or of unforeseen adaptation. They found that exercises designed to reduce the focusing illusion had limited effect (a finding consistent with Baron et al., 2003; Ubel et al., 2001; and Walsh & Ayton, 2009). However, exercises which drew attention to the process of adaptation successfully reduced the bias, leading Ubel et al. (2005) to conclude that unforeseen adaptation was a more appropriate explanation for the impact bias shown for health states. My Imagination 5 Another proposed remedy to overcome the failings in our imagination is to disregard imagination entirely and instead base judgments on the actual feelings of people currently experiencing the event - a process termed surrogation (Gilbert, 2006). When people are denied the information that imagination requires to make a future prediction, and are only given information about the feelings of others’, they make more accurate forecasts. Gilbert, Killingsworth, Eyre, and Wilson (2009) demonstrated that women more accurately predicted their enjoyment of a speed-date when they were provided with the enjoyment ratings of another woman who had experienced that date (surrogate information), than when provided with profile information about the date (event information). This was despite 75% of respondents believing that receiving event information would lead them to more accurate forecasts than receiving surrogate information. Similarly, Norwick, Gilbert, and Wilson (2006) recorded how a group of people felt after they had received a prize and then taken part in a tedious task. A second group was informed that they would also receive the prize and participate in the task, but half of the respondents were told what the prize was and the other half were given the reported feelings of a member of the first group. Respondents who only knew what the prize was did not accurately predict their future feelings; arguably because they had used their imaginations to envisage receiving the prize but had failed to anticipate how the tedious task would make them feel. In contrast, respondents who could only base their judgments on the reported feelings of a member of the previous group made accurate forecasts: although they had no information about the future event, by basing their judgments on the experiences of the surrogates they correctly anticipated their future feelings. These findings suggest that for people to accurately predict their future happiness, they should not contemplate information about the future event – thereby disabling their imaginations My Imagination 6 - and instead base their judgments on the feelings of others. Although a fitting suggestion in theory, it will, arguably, not often be feasible in practice. Outside of the laboratory, when making predictions about the impact of a future event, people rarely know how that event is making someone else feel without at the same time knowing something about the event itself. For example, we can read reports of how much critics enjoyed a movie but also know who stars in the film, or we can read reviews of hotels but see pictures of its location, making it difficult to separate
Recommended publications
  • A Task-Based Taxonomy of Cognitive Biases for Information Visualization
    A Task-based Taxonomy of Cognitive Biases for Information Visualization Evanthia Dimara, Steven Franconeri, Catherine Plaisant, Anastasia Bezerianos, and Pierre Dragicevic Three kinds of limitations The Computer The Display 2 Three kinds of limitations The Computer The Display The Human 3 Three kinds of limitations: humans • Human vision ️ has limitations • Human reasoning 易 has limitations The Human 4 ️Perceptual bias Magnitude estimation 5 ️Perceptual bias Magnitude estimation Color perception 6 易 Cognitive bias Behaviors when humans consistently behave irrationally Pohl’s criteria distilled: • Are predictable and consistent • People are unaware they’re doing them • Are not misunderstandings 7 Ambiguity effect, Anchoring or focalism, Anthropocentric thinking, Anthropomorphism or personification, Attentional bias, Attribute substitution, Automation bias, Availability heuristic, Availability cascade, Backfire effect, Bandwagon effect, Base rate fallacy or Base rate neglect, Belief bias, Ben Franklin effect, Berkson's paradox, Bias blind spot, Choice-supportive bias, Clustering illusion, Compassion fade, Confirmation bias, Congruence bias, Conjunction fallacy, Conservatism (belief revision), Continued influence effect, Contrast effect, Courtesy bias, Curse of knowledge, Declinism, Decoy effect, Default effect, Denomination effect, Disposition effect, Distinction bias, Dread aversion, Dunning–Kruger effect, Duration neglect, Empathy gap, End-of-history illusion, Endowment effect, Exaggerated expectation, Experimenter's or expectation bias,
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: a Systematic Mapping Study
    Cognitive Biases in Software Engineering: A Systematic Mapping Study Rahul Mohanani, Iflaah Salman, Burak Turhan, Member, IEEE, Pilar Rodriguez and Paul Ralph Abstract—One source of software project challenges and failures is the systematic errors introduced by human cognitive biases. Although extensively explored in cognitive psychology, investigations concerning cognitive biases have only recently gained popularity in software engineering research. This paper therefore systematically maps, aggregates and synthesizes the literature on cognitive biases in software engineering to generate a comprehensive body of knowledge, understand state of the art research and provide guidelines for future research and practise. Focusing on bias antecedents, effects and mitigation techniques, we identified 65 articles (published between 1990 and 2016), which investigate 37 cognitive biases. Despite strong and increasing interest, the results reveal a scarcity of research on mitigation techniques and poor theoretical foundations in understanding and interpreting cognitive biases. Although bias-related research has generated many new insights in the software engineering community, specific bias mitigation techniques are still needed for software professionals to overcome the deleterious effects of cognitive biases on their work. Index Terms—Antecedents of cognitive bias. cognitive bias. debiasing, effects of cognitive bias. software engineering, systematic mapping. 1 INTRODUCTION OGNITIVE biases are systematic deviations from op- knowledge. No analogous review of SE research exists. The timal reasoning [1], [2]. In other words, they are re- purpose of this study is therefore as follows: curring errors in thinking, or patterns of bad judgment Purpose: to review, summarize and synthesize the current observable in different people and contexts. A well-known state of software engineering research involving cognitive example is confirmation bias—the tendency to pay more at- biases.
    [Show full text]
  • Infographic I.10
    The Digital Health Revolution: Leaving No One Behind The global AI in healthcare market is growing fast, with an expected increase from $4.9 billion in 2020 to $45.2 billion by 2026. There are new solutions introduced every day that address all areas: from clinical care and diagnosis, to remote patient monitoring to EHR support, and beyond. But, AI is still relatively new to the industry, and it can be difficult to determine which solutions can actually make a difference in care delivery and business operations. 59 Jan 2021 % of Americans believe returning Jan-June 2019 to pre-coronavirus life poses a risk to health and well being. 11 41 % % ...expect it will take at least 6 The pandemic has greatly increased the 65 months before things get number of US adults reporting depression % back to normal (updated April and/or anxiety.5 2021).4 Up to of consumers now interested in telehealth going forward. $250B 76 57% of providers view telehealth more of current US healthcare spend % favorably than they did before COVID-19.7 could potentially be virtualized.6 The dramatic increase in of Medicare primary care visits the conducted through 90% $3.5T telehealth has shown longevity, with rates in annual U.S. health expenditures are for people with chronic and mental health conditions. since April 2020 0.1 43.5 leveling off % % Most of these can be prevented by simple around 30%.8 lifestyle changes and regular health screenings9 Feb. 2020 Apr. 2020 OCCAM’S RAZOR • CONJUNCTION FALLACY • DELMORE EFFECT • LAW OF TRIVIALITY • COGNITIVE FLUENCY • BELIEF BIAS • INFORMATION BIAS Digital health ecosystems are transforming• AMBIGUITY BIAS • STATUS medicineQUO BIAS • SOCIAL COMPARISONfrom BIASa rea• DECOYctive EFFECT • REACTANCEdiscipline, • REVERSE PSYCHOLOGY • SYSTEM JUSTIFICATION • BACKFIRE EFFECT • ENDOWMENT EFFECT • PROCESSING DIFFICULTY EFFECT • PSEUDOCERTAINTY EFFECT • DISPOSITION becoming precise, preventive,EFFECT • ZERO-RISK personalized, BIAS • UNIT BIAS • IKEA EFFECT and • LOSS AVERSION participatory.
    [Show full text]
  • A Motivational Account of the Impact Bias DISSERTATION Presented In
    A Motivational Account of the Impact Bias DISSERTATION Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy in the Graduate School of The Ohio State University By Gina M. Hoover, M.A. Graduate Program in Psychology The Ohio State University 2011 Committee: Dr. Gifford Weary, Advisor Dr. Kentaro Fujita Dr. Jennifer Crocker Copyrighted by Gina M. Hoover 2011 Abstract The impact bias is the tendency for people to overestimate the intensity and duration of their feelings in response to some future outcome (Gilbert, Driver-Linn, & Wilson, 2002; Wilson & Gilbert, 2003; Wilson & Gilbert, 2005). Most explanations for the bias conceptualize it as an undesirable side effect of how the cognitive system goes about making predictions. Yet, perhaps such mispredictions can serve an important self- regulatory function, helping people achieve their goals. This research argued for a motivated impact bias. That is, there are times when people may be driven to predict feelings more extreme than they actually will experience. By overestimating one’s feelings to some future outcome, the stakes of succeeding or failing at gaining that outcome are raised. As such, this may energize people to pursue their goals to achieve the desired outcomes. The more one wants to gain (or avoid) some outcome, the more likely s/he will be to exhibit such an impact bias. To investigate this question, participants were asked to predict their feelings in response to the outcome of a performance task (visual responsiveness task in studies 1-3, creativity task in study 4, and verbal test in study 5) they would complete.
    [Show full text]
  • Integrating Temporal Biases the Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences Lawrence J
    PSYCHOLOGICAL SCIENCE Research Article Integrating Temporal Biases The Interplay of Focal Thoughts and Accessibility Experiences Lawrence J. Sanna1 and Norbert Schwarz2 1University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill and 2University of Michigan ABSTRACT—We provide an integrative account of temporal bi- come to mind. This interaction drives the emergence of these biases, ases (confidence changes, planning fallacy, impact bias, and as well as debiasing. hindsight bias). Students listed either 3 or 12 thoughts about success or failure before an upcoming real-life exam or im- TEMPORAL BIASES mediately after learning their grades. Previous explanations had focused on how thought content alone (what comes to mind) Figure 1 illustrates the time course of the four temporal biases that are influences temporal biases. We found, however, an interaction our focus here. between thought content and accessibility experiences (how eas- ily or difficultly thoughts come to mind). Thinking about 3 ways to succeed (success was easy to bring to mind) was equivalent to Confidence Changes thinking about 12 ways to fail (failure was difficult to bring to People are less confident in success when events draw near than they mind), and conversely, thinking about 3 ways to fail was are at a more distant time. For example, participants taking an im- equivalent to thinking about 12 ways to succeed. In no case was mediate test were less confident than those taking a test in 4 weeks thought content alone sufficient to predict the biases. These re- (Nisan, 1972; see also Gilovich, Kerr, & Medvec, 1993). Similarly, sults have implications for debiasing strategies and other judg- college seniors were more muted in estimating their first-job salaries ments over time.
    [Show full text]
  • John Collins, President, Forensic Foundations Group
    On Bias in Forensic Science National Commission on Forensic Science – May 12, 2014 56-year-old Vatsala Thakkar was a doctor in India but took a job as a convenience store cashier to help pay family expenses. She was stabbed to death outside her store trying to thwart a theft in November 2008. Bloody Footwear Impression Bloody Tire Impression What was the threat? 1. We failed to ask ourselves if this was a footwear impression. 2. The appearance of the impression combined with the investigator’s interpretation created prejudice. The accuracy of our analysis became threatened by our prejudice. Types of Cognitive Bias Available at: http://en.wikipedia.org/wiki/List_of_cognitive_biases | Accessed on April 14, 2014 Anchoring or focalism Hindsight bias Pseudocertainty effect Illusory superiority Levels-of-processing effect Attentional bias Hostile media effect Reactance Ingroup bias List-length effect Availability heuristic Hot-hand fallacy Reactive devaluation Just-world phenomenon Misinformation effect Availability cascade Hyperbolic discounting Recency illusion Moral luck Modality effect Backfire effect Identifiable victim effect Restraint bias Naive cynicism Mood-congruent memory bias Bandwagon effect Illusion of control Rhyme as reason effect Naïve realism Next-in-line effect Base rate fallacy or base rate neglect Illusion of validity Risk compensation / Peltzman effect Outgroup homogeneity bias Part-list cueing effect Belief bias Illusory correlation Selective perception Projection bias Peak-end rule Bias blind spot Impact bias Semmelweis
    [Show full text]
  • How Politicians Make Decisions: a Political Choice Experiment
    Vol. 92 (2007), No. 2, pp. 167–196 DOI 10.1007/s00712-007-0264-4 Journal of Economics Printed in The Netherlands How Politicians Make Decisions: A Political Choice Experiment Enrique Fatas, Tibor Neugebauer, and Pilar Tamborero Received May 5, 2003; revised version received January 19, 2007 Published online: July 5, 2007 Ó Springer-Verlag 2007 The present paper reports on a political choice experiment with elected real-world politicians. A questionnaire on political and public issues is used to examine whether prospect theory predicts the responses of experts from the field better than rational choice theory. The results indicate that framing effects do not dis- appear with expertise. Keywords: subject pool effect, subject surrogacy, expected utility theory, prospect theory. JEL Classifications: C91, D72, D81. 1 Introduction Rationality and consistency are crucial assumptions to most theories in the social sciences. Particularly, in neoclassical economics and political sci- ences it is common to assume that all agents make their decisions coherently with the utility maximization doctrine. Rational choice theories under certainty and under risk have been established as descriptive mod- els for the decisions of consumers, producers, voters, politicians, etc. Experiments and empirical observations have revealed that actual behavior and decisions frequently deviate from the neoclassical predic- tions. This evidence has led social scientists to develop descriptive anal- yses of choice, based on observed behavior and decisions. Kahneman and Tversky’s prospect theory (1979; 1992) is an outstanding outcome of 168 E. Fatas et al. this research. In contrast to rational choice theory, it allows people’s preferences to depend on the circumstances they face.
    [Show full text]
  • Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-Being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Walsh, E. (2009). Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments. (Unpublished Doctoral thesis, City, University of London) This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/18257/ Link to published version: Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] Predicting the Impact of Health States on Well-being: Explanations and Remedies for Biased Judgments Emma Walsh Thesis submitted in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy City University, London Table of Contents Table of Contents ........................................................................................................ 2 List of Tables ..............................................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Impact Bias in Student Evaluations of Higher Education Anthony Grimes*, Dominic Medway, Adrienne Foos† and Anna Goatman
    Studies in Higher Education, 2017 Vol. 42, No. 6, 945–962, http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03075079.2015.1071345 Impact bias in student evaluations of higher education Anthony Grimes*, Dominic Medway, Adrienne Foos† and Anna Goatman Manchester Business School, University of Manchester, Booth Street West, Manchester M15 6PB, UK In the context of higher education, this study examines the extent to which affective evaluations of the student experience are influenced by the point at which they are made (i.e. before the experience begins, whilst it is happening and after it has ended). It adopts a between-groups quantitative analysis of the affective evaluations made by 360 future, current and past postgraduate students of a UK business school. The study validates the proposition that affective forecasts and memories of the student experience are considerably inflated in prospect and retrospect; a finding that implies a significant impact bias. It is concluded that the impact bias may have important implications for influencing the effectiveness of student decision-making, the timing and comparability of student course evaluations, and understanding the nature and effects of word-of-mouth communication regarding the student experience. Keywords: student attitudes; student evaluation; student expectations; student experience; students’ perceptions Introduction In recent years, the student experience has become a topic of growing interest and importance in higher education (HE). Accordingly, student evaluations of courses and institutions are now commonplace, with results feeding into various metrics, per- formance indicators and rankings of institutional excellence. The increased focus on the student experience in HE has been accompanied by significant academic research on the matter.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias
    1 Embrace Your Cognitive Bias http://blog.beaufortes.com/2007/06/embrace-your-co.html Cognitive Biases are distortions in the way humans see things in comparison to the purely logical way that mathematics, economics, and yes even project management would have us look at things. The problem is not that we have them… most of them are wired deep into our brains following millions of years of evolution. The problem is that we don’t know about them, and consequently don’t take them into account when we have to make important decisions. (This area is so important that Daniel Kahneman won a Nobel Prize in 2002 for work tying non-rational decision making, and cognitive bias, to mainstream economics) People don’t behave rationally, they have emotions, they can be inspired, they have cognitive bias! Tying that into how we run projects (project leadership as a compliment to project management) can produce results you wouldn’t believe. You have to know about them to guard against them, or use them (but that’s another article)... So let’s get more specific. After the jump, let me show you a great list of cognitive biases. I’ll bet that there are at least a few that you haven’t heard of before! Decision making and behavioral biases Bandwagon effect — the tendency to do (or believe) things because many other people do (or believe) the same. Related to groupthink, herd behaviour, and manias. Bias blind spot — the tendency not to compensate for one’s own cognitive biases. Choice-supportive bias — the tendency to remember one’s choices as better than they actually were.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Perspective How People Decide What They Want To
    PERSPECTIVE HOW PEOPLE DECIDE WHAT THEY WANT TO KNOW Tali Sharot1* & Cass R. Sunstein2 1Affective Brain Lab, Department of Experimental Psychology, University College London, London, UK 2Harvard Law School, Harvard University, Cambridge, MA, USA * To whom corresponds should be addressed [email protected] 1 Abstract Immense amounts of information are now accessible to people, including information that bears on their past, present and future. An important research challenge is to determine how people decide to seek or avoid information. Here, we propose a framework of information- seeking that aims to integrate the motives that drive information-seeking and its avoidance. Our framework rests on the idea that information can alter people’s action, affect and cognition in both positive and negative ways. The suggestion is that people assess these influences and integrate them into a calculation of the value of information that leads to information-seeking or avoidance. The theory offers a framework for characterizing and quantifying individual differences in information-seeking, which we hypothesize may also be diagnostic of mental health. We consider biases that can lead to both insufficient and excessive information-seeking and discuss how the framework offered can help government agencies to assess the welfare effects of mandatory information disclosure. 2 How do people decide what they want to know? As massive amounts of information are becoming available to people, this question is more pertinent today than ever. People must now decide whether they want to receive highly personalized information about their health, genetic make-up, and financial prospects, as well as those of their children.
    [Show full text]
  • Strategy Map Effects on Managers’
    STRATEGY MAP EFFECTS ON MANAGERS’ STRATEGY REVISION JUDGMENTS by Brian Dean Knox Bachelor of Science in Accountancy, Arizona State University, 2011 Master of Accountancy, Southern Utah University, 2013 Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of Joseph M. Katz Graduate School of Business in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Business Administration University of Pittsburgh 2017 UNIVERSITY OF PITTSBURGH JOSEPH M. KATZ GRADUATE SCHOOL OF BUSINESS This dissertation was presented by Brian Dean Knox It was defended on June 30, 2017 and approved by Jongwoon (Willie) Choi Assistant Professor of Business Administration and Fryrear Faculty Fellow in Accounting University of Pittsburgh Marc N. Coutanche Assistant Professor University of Pittsburgh Department of Psychology John H. Evans III Katz Alumni Professor and Area Director of Accounting University of Pittsburgh Dhinu Srinivasan Associate Professor of Accounting University of Pittsburgh Dissertation Advisor: Donald V. Moser Professor of Business Administration and Dean’s Excellence Faculty Fellow in Accounting University of Pittsburgh ii Copyright © by Brian Dean Knox 2017 iii ABSTRACT STRATEGY MAP EFFECTS ON MANAGERS’ STRATEGY REVISION JUDGMENTS Brian Dean Knox, PhD University of Pittsburgh, 2017 Managers make strategy revision judgments, which are judgments that affect how well the firm can revise its strategy when new information comes to light. Using two studies, my dissertation examines how formatting the firm’s strategy as a strategy map affects two types of strategy revision judgments. First, I study middle managers’ judgments on passing along new information to upper management. Second, I study managers’ judgments of the relevance of new information and the appropriateness of the firm’s strategy.
    [Show full text]