The Loss of V2 Clause Structure in the : A Review of the Literature

Heather Burnett and Elizabeth Ferch

Queen’s University

August 24, 2005

© Summer 2005

Cette recherche a bénéficié de l’appui du projet Modéliser le changement : les voies du français (GTRC/CRSH), dirigé par France Martineau. La recherche a été supervisée par Mireille Tremblay, Queen’s University. Table of Contents ______

Section 1: Review of the Literature

0. Introduction…………………………………………………………………... 3 1. Theories of Language Change……………………………………………….. 3 2. Synchronic Analyses of Old English……………………………………….. 14 3. Synchronic Analyses of Early Middle English……………………………... 27 4. Late Middle English and the Loss of V2…………………………………… 30 5. Conclusion………………………………………………………………….. 33

Section 2: Article Summaries…………………………………………………………... 35

Section 3: Bibliography of V2 in English……………………………………………... 114 Section 1: Review of the Literature

0. Introduction

Historical and the study of language change was once the major area of study for linguists. With the advent of generative grammar, it has taken on a new role.

The study of the way in which language changes, and the way in which it doesn’t, offers a unique window into the constitution of the language faculty. In this paper, we will present a comprehensive and critical review of the literature surrounding a major change in the history of the : the loss of verb second from Old

English to Middle English. We will begin by describing some of the main theories of language change which have been used to study the loss of V2. We will then examine the debate on the synchronic description of word order in Old English. Finally, we will review the various analyses of the synchronic state of Middle English, as well as the proposals for the causes and time course of the loss of V2. The goal of this paper is to make accessible the vast amount of literature available on the subject, to fuel further research into the history of English and diachronic in general.

1. Theories of Language Change

In this first section, we will present a brief description of some of the major theories of language change that the discussion on the history of V2 has been couched in.

Although they cannot be entirely separated, we feel that it is very important to make a distinction between the synchronic descriptions of various stages of a language, and the way in which the transition between those stages is expressed. This is because a certain theory of change can hold valuable insights, even if the particular synchronic analysis used to exemplify it may not be 100% correct. Roberts (1993) distinguishes three components in his theory of language change.

A step, defined as “the appearance of a new construction, or a significant change in the frequency of a construction” (Roberts 1993: 158), is a diachronic relation between E- languages; a parametric shift is a diachronic relation between I-languages; and a

Diachronic Reanalysis (DR), the reanalysis of a given structure, is a relation between the

E-language of one generation and the I-language of the next. Diachronic Reanalyses are caused by the Least Effort Strategy, which prompts learners to choose the grammatical representation containing the shortest possible set of chains, and they often provoke parametric shifts by eliminating the evidence for a given parameter setting; Roberts suggests that they are always minimal, that is, that each reanalysis reduces the structure as little as possible. Although steps and DRs can reduce the frequency of constructions, only parametric shifts can make them ungrammatical. However, as Lightfoot (1997) points out, the distinction between a parametric shift and a Diachronic Reanalysis is not entirely clear; Roberts himself allows that “[a]ll DRs may turn out to be instances of

Parametric Change” (1993: 159). As well, this theory does not explain why the LES should provoke a reanalysis in one generation of learners and not another.

Lightfoot (1991, 1995, 1997) proposes a theory of language change based on sudden reanalysis. He postulates that parameter settings are acquired based on specific triggering experiences in the linguistic environment. Based on data from Dutch and

German regarding the acquisition of OV word order (Klein (1974); Clahsen & Smolka

(1986), cited in Lightfoot (1991)), and data from its loss in Old English (Bean (1983) and others), Lightfoot argues for “degree-0 learnability,” in other words, the idea that triggering experiences must be present in simple matrix clauses. Triggers must also be expressed robustly, that is, in a sufficient proportion of sentences, in order to cause the parameter to be set. Lightfoot believes that the linguistic environment is always changing, due to grammar-external factors; in cases of language change, a particular construction increases or decreases in frequency until the triggering experience has changed sufficiently for the parameter it expresses to be set a different way. Thus,

Lightfoot’s theory involves a gradual drift in usage followed by a sudden change in grammar. However, Kroch (1999) takes issue with this notion of “drift”, saying that empirical evidence for its existence is “at the least, uncertain” (Kroch, 1999:6). He claims that the problem lies in the fact that so-called “drifting” has no obvious motivation; therefore, it is unclear why the frequency of a certain form undergoing change should be incorrectly learnt in one case, but correctly learnt in practically all the other cases. He cites cases such as the stability of the frequency of the placement of adverbs in the history of English as an example where drift would have most likely occurred, but did not

(Kroch, 1999:6-8). According to him, if the drift model was correct, the loss of V to I movement in Middle English would have caused an increase in the frequency of preverbal adverbs. However, the frequency of preverbal adverbial placement versus postverbal placement remained the same throughout time, thus rendering the motivation for the change in frequencies that bring about a reanalysis extremely unclear.

In order to avoid postulating drifts in usage frequencies that occur prior or independently of grammar change, Kroch (1989; 1999, 2003 a.o.) proposes the

“imperfect transmission” (IT) model of language change. In this model, instead of the grammatical change occurring at the end of a long period of drift, it is the first stage in the process of change. According to the “IT”, change occurs when, for a variety of reasons, a child “mislearns” the grammar of its parents. They create a grammar that is different from the one they are exposed to, and it is this difference in grammar that creates a difference in the patterns of usage.

Basing the model in the principles and parameters (P&P) framework (for an overview, see Chomsky, 1995: chapter 1), Kroch (2003), following Clahsen (1991) and

Penner (1992; both cited in Kroch, 2003), assumes that language learners set linguistic parameters in an irreversible way. Therefore, when the learner first encounters some input that makes reference to a certain parameter, she will set it accordingly. If all the following input that she is exposed to is consistent with this parameter setting, the learner will simply assume that this is the target grammar and nothing further will happen.

However, if, later on, she comes into contact with data which contradicts the first parameter setting, seeing as she cannot simply re-set the parameter, nor can she put the parameter on both settings, the learner creates a separate grammar which has the parameter in question set in the opposite way. The learner is now “bilingual” or

“bidialectal”, in the sense that she has two (or more) separate grammars in her head and can use either one of them. The two grammars do not, however, coexist completely harmoniously; they are in what Kroch (1989 and later work) and Pintzuk (1991 and later work) call “grammar competition”. In a first language acquisition sense, this term corresponds to a situation where a child mistakenly acquires a parameter setting that is clearly different from the majority of input sentences she is exposed to. According to

Kroch (2003), if the learner is young enough and the primary linguistic data conclusively support the second, newer grammar, then she will begin using the second grammar at the expense of the first. After a period of variation/competition, the second grammar will

“win out” over the first, i.e. it will become the dominant one all but eradicating the first.

Kroch (2003) claims that a concrete example of this type of grammar competition is found in the study of Labov & Labov (1976). In this study, the Labovs recorded thousands of their four-year old daughter’s wh-questions and note that the acquisition of questions formed with “why” undergoes a radically different pattern with respect to the presence of inversion than the other Wh-words:

(1)

Acquisition of Wh-questions by Labov daughter (cited in Kroch (2003:his figure1)

Kroch interprets these results in the following way: until roughly December 1976, the

Labovs’ daughter had inaccurately set the “inversion” parameter as – in the case of why- questions, but as + in the case of the other wh-questions. Having been exposed to overwhelming evidence of the existence of a grammar with the +inversion setting, she then creates one which enters into competition with the existing –inversion grammar. The

+inversion grammar ends up replacing the original grammar by June, 1976.

Kroch (1989 and subsequent work) proposes a model of language change which essentially mirrors the process of grammar competition in acquisition, but on a larger scale. According to this model, language change occurs when the primary linguistic data available to learners consistently requires the creation of two grammars, the competition between which is not eradicated within the critical period of acquisition. Speakers can, therefore, live their entire lives with multiple grammars, and they make choices as to frequencies at which they use one grammar over the other that are “probabilistically influenced by features of context and situation” (Kroch, 1989:3).

Following authors such as Weinreich, Labov and Herzog (1968, cited in Kroch,

1989) and Osgood and Sebeok (1954 cited in Kroch, 1989), Kroch assumes that the time course of language change follows an ‘S’-shaped curve, with “new forms replacing established ones only slowly in the beginning of a change, then accelerating their replacement in the middle stages of a change and finally, as the old forms become rare, slowing their advance once again” (Kroch, 1989:3). Kroch (1982, 1989) goes even further in his characterization of the shape that language change takes by proposing a specific mathematical function, the logistic1, as the underlying form of a syntactic change. Although, as Kroch (1989:4) notes, it is impossible to verify exactly which function is the proper characterization of the time course of language change, other

1 The equation of the logistic curve is where p is the fraction of the advancing form, t is the time variable, and s and k are constants. models that have been used, such as the cumulative function of the normal distribution

(Aldrich and Nelson, 1984, cited in Kroch, 1989), “generally differ so little from the logistic that they can provide no improvement in fit to empirical data.” Also, the logistic has been widely used in other fields to model population behaviour, such in epidemiology and population biology (ex. Spiess, 1989, cited in Kroch (1989); therefore, the logistic seems to be a plausible model to apply to grammar competition and replacement.

Further support for parameter-based grammar competition comes from the rate that the time course of language change takes. If one assumes that the grammars in competition differ in the setting of an abstract parameter, one would expect to see the result of a parametrical difference to take the form not of a single difference in form, but as a series of correlated changes that instantiate the abstract change. Therefore, we would expect all the elements affected by the parametrical change to be change at the same rate.

This is known in Kroch (1989 and subsequent work) as the “constant rate hypothesis”.

More specifically, it is the claim that, although the rate of use of different grammatical options in competition will most likely differ across contexts at each period in time, the rate of change will be the same across contexts (Kroch, 1989:6). The realization of this hypothesis can be illustrated through the case of the loss of V2 in the history of French.2

As shown in (2), the losses of -dropped subjects, full NP subject inversion, and -subject inversion proceed at the same rate, as would be expected if they were the result of a change in a certain “V2 parameter”.

2 We bring up this case, as Kroch does, simply for explanatory purposes. At this point we do not support or deny this claim about the actual nature of V2 in French; however, for a deeper discussion about the issues involved in the history of French V2 see Burnett & Ferch (in prep). (2)

(Kroch, 1989: 12, his Figure 3)

Another approach that strongly links language change with acquisition is that of

Yang (2000). Yang compares the tension that exists between the discrete and symbolic nature of grammatical competence and the variable, heterogeneous nature of performance with the tension that exists between the discrete basis of Mendelien genetic and the continuous distribution of genotypes in populations. He views language acquisition as being similar to evolution: it is as a “variational process in which the distribution of grammars changes as an adaptive response to the linguistic evidence in the environment”

(Yang, 2000:234). As in Kroch’s model, this “variational process” is realized through multiple grammar competition and replacement. In this model, within the finite collection of grammars that make up UG, each grammar Gi is associated with a weight pi, which denotes the probability with which the learner will access the grammar. Acquisition works as follows: when an input sentence is presented, a grammar is selected. The probability that certain grammar is selection is determined by its weight. The grammar is then used to analyze the sentence. If the sentence can be successfully parsed using the selected grammar, it is rewarded: i.e. its weight is increased, and the weights of all the other grammars are indirectly punished: i.e. their weights are decreased. If the selected grammar fails to parse the input, it is punished and the other grammars are rewarded. This process continues until a single grammar has a weight that ensures that it will be exclusively selected.

If we assume, as is common, that UG remains constant from generation to generation, then language change must be due to a situation where two generations are exposed to different linguistic evidence, which results in different knowledge of language. According to Yang, the “different linguistic evidence” can arise in a variety of ways such as through the migration of foreign speakers who introduce novel forms into the language, through linguistic innovation, or through more general social and cultural factors (237). These factors will create a situation where the input evidence will seem to reflect two conflicting grammars. The proportion of expressions that belong to Grammar

1 (G1) that are incompatible with Grammar 2 (G2) is called the advantage of G1, and the proportion of expressions in the environment that belong to G2 that are incompatible with

G1 is called the advantage of G2. Therefore, according to Yang’s theory of acquisition,

Grammar 2 will overtake Grammar 1if the advantage of grammar G2 is higher than G1.

The change takes the shape of the logistic, as shown in (3) (3)

(Yang 2000: 240)

Due to the repeated “punishment” of unsuccessful grammars that it proposes, a corollary of Yang’s model is that, once a grammar is on the rise, it is unstoppable (239).

Another theory which takes an evolutionary look at language acquisition and change is that of Clark and Roberts (1993), who characterize language acquisition as natural selection among hypotheses about the target grammar. In their theory, hypotheses are made up of sets of parameters and encoded as strings of binary digits. Each hypothesis translates into a parsing device; these parsing devices are tested against each input datum, and their fitness is judged based on how they interpret the datum3. The most

3 C&R provide a fitness metric for mathematically calculating the relative fitness of the grammars. This metric is shown in (i):

(i) n n n (_j = 1 vj + b_ j = 1 sj + c_ j = 1 ej) – (vi + bsi + cei) n n n (n – 1)( _j = 1 vj + b_ j = 1 sj + c_ j = 1 ej) important factor is the well-formedness of each parse, specifically the number of basic grammatical principles which are violated. Other factors are also considered, including the elegance of the parse, measured as the number of nodes contained in it, and the number of superset parameters; these factors are important when distinguishing among equally grammatical interpretations. A superset parameter is one which, when set a given way, produces a set of sentences which includes all sentences that could be generated with the parameter set the other way. If the learner were to incorrectly postulate the superset setting, there would be no evidence available to disprove this hypothesis, and the language acquired would be a superset of the target language; in order for this not to happen, such hypotheses must be selected against, as per the Subset Condition of

Berwick (1985). When the relative fitness of the hypotheses is determined for a given datum, the most fit hypotheses are selected for special “mating operations”: a crossover mechanism, by which a new hypothesis is formed from parts of two old ones, and a mutation operator, which forms new hypotheses by randomly changing parameter settings. Over time, parameter settings which are frequently expressed in the input will be selected for, while those which are unable to interpret the input will be selected against; thus, the population of hypotheses will tend towards the optimal settings for the target language. This process does not, however, guarantee that the learners’ grammars will be identical to those of their parents. If a parameter is unambiguously expressed in the language, it is stable, but if the evidence becomes ambiguous between various

n In this equation, n represents the number of parsing devices; _j = 1 vj represents the total number of grammatical violations of all parsing devices; n vi represents the number of violations by the parsing device whose fitness is being determined; _j = 1 sj represents the total number of superset settings in the n population of hypotheses; si represents in number of superset settings in the grammar being evaluated; _j = 1 ej represents the total number of nodes generated by all parsers; ei represents the number of nodes (the elegance) of the parser being evaluated; and b and c are constants, greater than 0 and smaller than 1, used to weight the elegance and superset factors so that they have less impact than does failure to parse. settings, the language will be unstable and therefore prone to change. Instability can arise for various reasons: phonological changes may make syntactic parameter settings difficult to interpret; a change in one parameter may destabilize another one; or the language may become “shifted,” with learners setting parameters in such a way that they interact to produce a superset language. Once a language becomes unstable, according to

Clark & Roberts, learners turn to UG-internal factors, specifically elegance and the

Subset Condition, to choose between hypotheses.

This model is similar toYang (2000) in that, as in his model, the outcome of language acquisition is determined by the compatibilities of grammars with the linguistic input data in a “Darwinian selectionist manner” (Yang, 2000:240), the differences between the two models being primarily that Clark and Roberts (1993) do not accept, like

Kroch and Yang, the existence of multiple grammars within the head of a single speaker.

These evolutionary theories are impressive, but as Kroch (1999) notes, theories such as that of Clark and Roberts “are useful hypotheses, no doubt, but unless they can be further specified to make empirically testable predictions, they will remain speculative”

(1999:35).

2. Synchronic Analyses of Old English

The proper characterization of Old English syntax has been a matter of great debate. Due to the large amount of complex variation in its surface word order, a number of scholars have concluded that OE was a free word order language (Kroch & Taylor,

1997). For those who noticed the V2 pattern in OE, it was still uncertain whether or not this was a robust syntactic rule, or just a narrative tendency (see Bean, 1983). However, in more recent years, studies such as those done by van Kemenade (1987) and Pintzuk (1991) have shown that OE word order in general and, more specifically, the V2 pattern is, in fact, highly rule governed. However, the exact nature of the V2 rule remains open to discussion. In this section we will first give a brief overview of the V2 pattern in OE, and then we will present various analyses which have tried to account for the distribution of verb-second order in both main and subordinate clauses.

If, following van Kemenade (1987), Pintzuk (1991;1993), Kiparsky (1994), Kroch

& Taylor (1997), we take the dominant underlying order of late OE to be SOV, then the most common type of V2 sentence would be the one where the finite verb moves to become the second element in the clause, and the NP-subject is the topic, yielding surface

SVO order, as in (4):

(4) Godes _eowas sceoloni unscæ__ignysse healdan ti God’s servants should harmlessness preserve ‘God’s servants should preserve harmlessness…” (AELS 25.846, cited in Pintzuk, 1993: 6)

However, the subject is not the only thing that can appear before the verb. It is also very common to have the “topic” position filled by a non-subject: a direct , a prepositional phrase or an adverb. In this case, the subject appears after the verb, as shown in (5):

(5) a. & of heom twam is eall manncynn cumen (WHom 6.52) and of them two is all mankind come b. pæt hus hæfdon Romane to dæm anum tacne geworht (Or 59.3) that building had Romans with the one feature constructed c. pær wearp se cyning Bagsecg ofslægen (Anglo-Saxon Chron., Parker, 871) there was the king Bagsecg slain (all cited from Kroch & Taylor (1997:302), their

(5)) Another pattern that we see with non-subject topics is “verb third” word order. This arises in cases where the subject of the sentence is a pronoun:

(6) Ælc yfel he mæg don (WHom, 4.62) each evil he can do (cited in Kroch & Taylor, 2000, their (7a))

Although V3 word order was originally taken to indicate the inconsistent nature of the V2 constraint in OE, Kemenade (1987) and Pintzuk (1991) have shown that it reflects not a variable verb movement rule, but particular characteristics of the themselves (to be discussed below). Evidence to suggest that V3 is a result of the “-like” nature of the pronoun and not of the V2 constraint comes from the fact that object pronouns can appear in the same preverbal position as their subject counterparts.

(7) & seofon ærendracan he him hæfde to asend (ASC, Parker, 905) and seven messengers he him had to sent ‘and he had sent seven messengers to him’ (cited in Kroch, Taylor & Ringe, 2000, their (8b))

V3 order can also occur when some adverbs are topicalized, as in (8):

(8) Her Oswald se eadiga arcebisceop forlet pis lif. (ASC, Laud, 992) in-this-year Oswald the blessed archbishop forsook this life (cited in Kroch, Taylor & Ringe, 2000, their (10c))

V2 order also appears in a small number of exceptional constructions such as non- subject wh-questions, sentences introduced by the elements _a and _onne, and negation- initial sentences, examples of which are found in (9):

(9) a. hwi sceole we opres mannes niman? (AELS 24.188) why should we another man's take b. pa ge-mette he sceadan (AELS 31.151) then met he robbers c. ne mihton hi nænigne fultum æt him begitan (Bede 48.9–10) not could they not-any help from him get (cited in Kroch, Taylor & Ringe, 2000, their (9a-c)) These constructions are customarily analyzed as involving verb movement to C (Pintzuk

(1991, 1993), Kroch & Taylor (1997), Kemenade (1987, 1997), Fischer et al (2000)).

As for subordinate clauses, the data are much less clear. According to Pintzuk

(1991), Old English exhibits V2 order in a variety of subordinate clauses. In her view, this makes OE different from other V2 languages such as German and Dutch which allow verb second only in main clauses4. This significant difference leads Pintzuk to postulate that OE is an IPV2 language (like Icelandic and Yiddish) rather than a CPV2 language, such as German and the Mainland Scandinavian languages. It is generally assumed that in the latter languages the verb raises to the complementizer position, and the topic moves to

SPEC, CP. However, when the C position is filled by a complementizer, as is the case in subordinate clauses, verb movement to C is blocked, yielding a non-V2 word order. In an IPV2 language, the verb raises to the inflectional head I, and the topic moves to SPEC

IP. In subordinates, V2 order is still possible since the I head is not affected by the presence of a Comp in C. In order to explain the V3 effect that arises when the subject is a pronoun, Pintzuk proposes that pronouns in OE are IP (Wackernagel) , which means that they attach at the border between CP and IP. She also postulates the existence of a phonological rule which moves them to the right of the first phrasal constituent, yielding the order found in (6).

Pintzuk’s analysis is reviewed in Kroch & Taylor (1997) (K&T), and, although they largely support her claims, these authors raise a number of concerns with her proposal. Primarily, they take issue with the phonological rule that is needed to derive the surface XP Pro V order. As they say, “the special clitic movement rule needed by Pintzuk

4 This does not take into account special cases which can be analysed as instances of “CP recursion” as discussed in Iatridou & Kroch (1992). to account for the placement of pronouns between topic and verb in V2 clauses has no counterpart elsewhere among the and does not have clear theoretical justification” (Kroch & Taylor, 1997: 305). We would also like to point out that the

“phonological” nature of this postposing rule would predict that that the final word of the first constituent and the subject clitic would form a phonological word. For example, in

(6) (repeated here as (10)), Pintzuk’s rule would have “he” forming a phonological word with “yfel”, which seems to us to be counterintuitive.

(10) Ælc yfel he mæg don (WHom, 4.62) each evil he can do (cited in Kroch & Taylor, 2000, their (7a))

In order to remedy these inconsistencies, K&T propose that, in Old English main clauses, the topic moves first to SPEC, IP but goes on to land in SPEC, CP. As such, there is now no need to propose a phonological rule that moves the clitic from the Wackernagel position at the CP/IP border. K& T do, however, support Pintzuk’s claim that the topic moves only to SPEC, IP in subordinate clauses.

The second difficulty that K&T have with Pintzuk (1991) is that it fails to account for a striking generalization made from data presented in van Kemenade (1997): rather than being relatively productive (as Pintzuk’s theory predicts), V2 word order with a non- subject topic in subordinate clauses is possible only in cases where “the nominative subject is absent or is licensed to appear in a position other than SPEC, IP, as in passive sentences or in sentences with experiencer dative “subjects” ” (K&T: 307). These two kinds of V2 subordinates are shown in (11):

(11) a. passive: _onne ælce dæge beo_ manega acennede _urh hys mihte on woruld when each day are many(NOM) given-birth through his power on world (AEHP.VI.120, cited in K&T:307) b. experiencer dative “subjects”: gif us ne lyst _æra ærrena yfela _e we ær worhton if us(DAT) not pleases the earlier evils (GEN) that we earlier wrought (CP.445.29, cited in van Kemenade (1997: 335))

Thus, according to K&T, SPEC, IP is available for topics only when the subject does not have to be there. Both Pintzuk (1991)’s analysis and K&T’s refinement are immediately confronted with the following questions: why is it that the subject is not required to move to SPEC, IP in order to check case and EPP features in main clauses? Conversely, if the subject is not required to move to SPEC IP in main clauses, why is it that it must raise in subordinates?

K&T’s solution to these problems is the following: If one takes the view that the essence of the V2 constraint is the establishment of a SPEC/head relationship between the topic and the verb, and if the verb only moves to I, it is then imperative that the topic occupy the specifier position of IP.5 Since the topic or its trace is in SPEC, IP, the subject cannot move there. In order to satisfy the Principle of Full Interpretation (Chomsky

(1986; 1993), it must check the finite agreement features in another way. According to

K&T, this is accomplished through the incorporation of a null expletive which is coindexed with the subject into the feature complex of I. This expletive checks the active inflectional and EPP features in I, making it unnecessary for the subject to move into

SPEC, IP. In subordinate clauses, the movement of the subject to SPEC, IP is not driven by inflectional or EPP considerations; it is, in fact, due to the topicalization that is necessary to satisfy the V2 constraint. Although this scenario addresses the theoretical problem of the licensing of the subject inside the VP, K&T themselves note that “since the licensing of topics in main and subordinate clauses is identical, we are now without a

5 K&T note that It is also possible, assuming a split IP comprised of AgrS and T, that the verb moves to AgrS. (K&T, 1997:319). simple syntactic explanation for the greatly reduced range of topicalizations in subordinate as opposed to matrix clauses” (K&T: 309). However, K&T suggest that this restriction may in fact not be syntactic but pragmatic: they claim that due to information structure considerations, the subject will be topicalized as much as possible in subordinate clauses, the only exceptions to this being the cases, such as in (11), where there seems to be no subject. Although it is impossible to verify this hypothesis for OE, evidence from Icelandic and Yiddish (Rognvaldsson & Santorini, p.cs to K&T) suggests that this generalization holds in living IPV2 languages.

Another analysis of the Old English syntax is that of van Kemenade (1987;1997) who proposes that OE is, in fact, a CPV2 rather than IPV2 language. She claims that, in main clauses, the topic moves to SPEC,CP and the verb moves to the complementizer position. Pronouns cliticize on the left of the finite verb, as in (12a), but the cliticization is blocked if the first constituent is an operator (i.e. contexts where the verb independently raises to C, such as in wh-questions) (12b):

(12) a.Be _æm we magon swi_e swutule oncnawan _æt… (CP.181.16) by that we may very clearly perceive that b. for hwam noldest _u _e sylfe me gecy_an (AESLXXXIII.307) for what not-wanted you yourself me make known ‘wherefore would you not make yourself known to me’ (cited in van Kemenade (1997:333)

By saying that the topic moves to SPEC, CP in main clauses, van Kemenade, like Kroch

& Taylor (1997), eliminates the need to postulate the existence of a special phonological clitic postposition rule of the type argued for in Pintzuk (1991). However, unlike Pintzuk

(1991) and K&T, van Kemenade does not suggest that the topic is ever in SPEC, IP. This placement of the topic predicts that V2 word order would be impossible in subordinate clauses. Van Kemenade (1997) argues that this is, in fact the case, and she shows that apparent V2 order in subordinates only occurs in constructions which have no external argument. This distribution was taken by K&T to reflect a pragmatic constraint on embedded topicalization rather than a syntactic one, and they analyze cases such as the ones in (11), repeated here as (13), as real instances of V2 in subordinates.

(13) a. passive: _onne ælce dæge beo_ manega acennede _urh hys mihte on woruld when each day are many(NOM) given-birth through his power on world (AEHP.VI.120, cited in K&T:307) b. experiencer dative “subjects”: gif us ne lyst _æra ærrena yfela _e we ær worhton if us(DAT) not pleases the earlier evils (GEN) that we earlier wrought (CP.445.29, cited in van Kemenade (1997: 335))

However, van Kemenade (1997) argues that these cases do not constitute valid examples of V2 in subordinates as it is impossible to tell that what K&T are calling the “topic” (i.e.

“each day” in (13a) and “us” in (13b)) is actually in SPEC, IP. For example, it is not clear that “each day” is necessarily in SPEC, IP, since, as shown in (14), there is a position between SPEC,IP and I available for temporal adverbials.

(14)

Kroch (2005:L6:3) In order to properly conclude that the V2 constraint is active in subordinate clauses, one would have to come across a case where the external argument was postverbal with a non-subject XP occupying the position of SPEC, IP. Van Kemenade (1997) says that, since cases such as in (13) do not have an external argument, they cannot be used as evidence for the presence of the V2 constraint in subordinates. We also think that it is possible that the constituents which K&T classify as “non-subject topics” in sentences such as (13b) are actually subjects. K&T conclude that the DP “us” is not generated in

SPEC, vP based on its dative case marking. However, it has been shown for Icelandic

(Harley, 1995) that there are a certain group of verbs that lexically assign dative case to their subject and nominative case to their object, such as the verb “liki” in (15):

(15) Calvini liki verki_ Calvin-D like the job-N “Calvin likes the job” (Harley, 1995, her (1))

Despite their “quirky” case marking, these DPs display properties of subjecthood (for a complete discussion, see Harley (1995); Bobaljik (2005)). As shown in (16), it seems likely that there is a similar group of verbs in OE, of which “licode” is a member:

(16) ac Gode ne licode na heora geleafleast but God(DAT) not pleased not their faithlessness(NOM) ‘but their faithlessness did not please God’ (AEHP.XX.71, cited in van Kemendade (1997:334))

In these sentences, SPEC,IP would be occupied by the subject, and thus examples such as

(13b) give us no information at all as to the presence or absence of V2 in subordinates.

Given the ambiguous evidence, it is difficult to uphold an IPV2 analysis of Old English; however, until these constructions are examined in more depth it cannot be completely ruled out.

Van Kemenade’s (1987;1997) analysis is slightly revised in Fischer et al. (2000).

This revision concerns the treatment of pronouns in OE. They propose that OE pronouns are Germanic-style weak pronouns and not clitics, as claimed by Pintzuk (1991;1993),

Kroch & Taylor (1997), Van Kemenade (1987; 1997), and Cardinaletti & Roberts (1991).

Following Koopman (1997), they claim that these pronouns are housed in the specifier of a functional projection (FP) that is below C, but above IP and the high negation phrase. In main clauses, the topic moves to SPEC, CP and the verb moves to the head of FP to join the pronouns.

(17)

Based on Fischer et al. (2000:126)

Treating the pronouns as independent syntactic objects rather than as clitics allows

Fischer et al. (2000) to account for the fact that pronouns appear after the verb in contexts such as Wh-questions. In these cases, the verb raises past the pronouns to C yielding the WH V Pro order. There is thus no need for an extra rule concerning the direction of cliticization, such as that in van Kemenade (1987;1997). Haeberli (2002) has a very similar proposal to the one made in Fischer et al. (2000). The structure is the same; the only difference being that Haeberli calls FP AgrP. He proposes that subjects below AgrP are licensed by an empty expletive in SPEC, AgrP.

A final, somewhat different, analysis comes from Cardinaletti and Roberts (1991)

(C&R). They argue for a more articulated clause structure than is usually assumed, which includes two subject agreement projections.

(18)

Based on (C&R, their (0)) The lower agreement projection holds the agreement of the verb, and the upper one holds subject clitics. In main clauses, the verb joins the clitic in Agr1; however, in subordinate clauses, it only moves as far as Agr2.

It is difficult to evaluate this analysis in comparison with the others as it is an analysis more of verb movement than it is of the V2 phenomenon. C&R make no explicit mention of the position of topics or what the relationship between the topic and the verb is. Also, the expanded clause structure allows for an easy derivation of a lot more possible word orders than do the analyses of Pintzuk (1991), Kroch & Taylor (1997),

Kemenade (1987), and Fischer et al. (2000). However, this account may not be the most interesting one to pursue, as the existence of agreement projections has been argued against by Chomsky (1995:349-355), who notes that it is motivated solely by theory- internal reasons. In the interests of empirical falsifiability, it is thus desirable to postulate the existence only of projections “with intrinsic properties that are manifested at the interface levels” (Chomsky, 1995:355), not ones, such as the many Agr projections in

C&R, which have no semantic or phonological consequences.

The various approaches to the syntax of Old English examined here are summarized in table (1): Table (1): Analyses of V2 in Old English Pintzuk (1991; Kroch & Taylor (1997) Van Kemenade Fischer, van Cardinaletti & 1993) (1987; 1997) Kemenade, Koopman Roberts (1991) & van der Wurff (2000) Main Topic SPEC, IP TopicSPEC, CP (passing Topic  SPEC, CP Topic  SPEC, CP VerbAgr1 clauses Verb I through SPEC,IP) Verb  C Verb  F Verb I/AgrS Subordinate Topic SPEC, IP Topic SPEC IP Not V2 Not V2 VerbAgr2 clauses VerbI Verb I/AgrS Status of IP clitics + a IP clitics IP clitics Germanic-style weak Clitics in Agr1 pronouns phonological rule pronouns housed in postposing them SPEC, FP, not clitics to the right of the first constituent In conclusion, the analysis given by Fischer et al. (2000) and Haeberli (2002) seems to be best able to account for the various word order patterns in OE in a straight- forward manner; however, we are concerned that, through the addition of the extra functional projection needed to account for the distribution of pronouns, they lose what is often considered the motivation for V2: the establishment of a local (Spec/head) relationship between the topic and the verb (Kroch & Taylor, 1997). It is therefore clear that, at the moment, there is no completely satisfactory analysis of the V2 phenomenon in

Old English, and the question is still open for discussion.

3. Synchronic Analyses of Early Middle English

V2 word order continues to be dominant well into the Early Middle English period. However, in this time period, we can see the development of a dialectal difference in the V2 constraint with respect to the behaviour of pronouns. The Southern dialects exhibit the same pattern as in Old English: subject-inversion occurs with full NPs and not with pronouns. The Northern dialects, however, start to show inversion with all types of subjects, as shown in (19).

(19) O_ir labur sal _ai do other labour shall they do ‘they must do other labour’ (Benet.33.20 cited in Van Kemenade (1997))

For Van Kemenade (1987, 1997), verb movement in Old and Early Middle

English is basically the same. According to her, Old English is a CPV2 language, a pattern which continues into the 14th century. Van Kemenade (1997) attributes the difference in the position of pronouns between the northern and southern dialects to a loss of a clitic position in the North which remains available in the South. The Loss of V2 in the History of English 28

However, Kroch & Taylor (1997) take issue with this simplistic approach, saying that it does not account for another change occurring at the same time in the Northern dialects: the increase in inversion of full NPs with adverbs and adjunct PPs. As shown in

(20), there is a higher rate of subject-inversion in the Northern dialects than in the South and Midlands.

(20a)

Table (2): subject-verb inversion in the Northern Prose Rule of St. Benet (from Kroch & Taylor (1997:313)

(b)

Table (3): subject-verb inversion in seven early Midlands texts (from Kroch & Taylor (1997:311)

This dialectal difference cannot be explained by a simple difference in the cliticization of pronouns. Also, there is reason to believe that pronouns in the “Rule of St. Benet”, a

Northern text, are still clitics. For example, stylistic fronting can occur with pronouns, as in (21), but not with full NPs. The Loss of V2 in the History of English 29

(21) Bot yef it sua bi-tide, _at any falle in mis-trouz; _an sal scho pray gerne to god but if it so betide, that any fall into mistruth; then shall she pray earnestly to God (Benet 19.30, cited in Kroch & Taylor 1997: 314)

In order to account for these differences, Kroch & Taylor propose that the IPV2 system which they postulated for Old English is replaced by a CPV2 system in the Northern dialect, while the Southern dialect remains IPV2. This means that in the North, the verb moves to C, and the topic moves directly to SPEC,CP, not leaving a trace in SPEC,IP as it did in Old English. In this case, the verb in C always precedes pronouns cliticized at the CP/IP boundary; the increased rate of inversion with adverbs and adjunct PPs is explained by the fact that adjunction to CP is less productive than adjunction to IP, so that in the Northern CPV2 dialect, it is more likely that initial adverbs or PPs are topics rather than preverbal adjuncts, and therefore that they trigger inversion.

The fact that a change from IPV2 to CPV2 can account for the differences discussed above seems to provide evidence for an IPV2 characterization of Old English; however, the increased rate of subject inversion with adverbs and PP adjuncts is not necessarily due to such a change, as it might simply reflect an increased preference for topicalizing such constituents in the Northern dialect. Thus, a CPV2 analysis could still be upheld. However, the asymmetry between pronouns and full NPs when it comes to stylistic fronting remains a problem for van Kemenade’s (1997) decliticization analysis.

This asymmetry can be accounted for by the analysis of Fischer et al (2000).

They adopt K&T’s idea of a dialectal difference between North and South, with a CPV2 dialect in the North, but they characterize the Southern dialect as having verb movement to F, as in their analysis of Old English, rather than to I.

The analyses we have discussed are summarized in Table (4): The Loss of V2 in the History of English 30

Van Kemenade Kroch & Taylor Fischer et al (1997) (1997) (2000) Main clauses: North Topic  SPEC, CP Topic  SPEC,CP Topic  SPEC,CP Verb  C Verb  C Verb  C South Topic  SPEC,CP Topic  SPEC,CP Topic  SPEC,CP Verb  C Verb  AgrS Verb  F Subordinates: North Not V2 (Topic  SPEC,CP)6 Not V2 Verb T South Not V2 Topic  SPEC, IP Not V2 Verb T Pronouns: North Not clitics Clitics Weak pronouns South clitics Clitics Weak pronouns Table (4): Synchronic analyses of Early Middle English

4. Late Middle English and the Loss of V2

The number of unambiguously verb second sentences gradually declines starting from the middle of the 14th century (Kroch, Taylor & Ringe, 2000), although it is not completely eradicated until the 17th century (Fisher et al. 2000), as shown in (22):

(22)

Frequency of subject-verb inversion (V2) in the Penn-Helsinki Parsed Corpus of Middle English and Early Modern English (from Johnson & Whitton (2002), cited in Kroch (2003).

6 In Middle English, it is possible to have topicalization across a lexical complementizer in ungoverned subordinate clauses, as in (i).

(i) I sal yu lere _e dute of god, his wille _at Ζe may do I shall you teach the duty of God, his will that ye may do (Benet 2.5, cited in Kroch & Taylor 1997: 315) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 31

There are a number of accounts for the loss of the V2 constraint available in the literature. For example, van Kemenade (1987) explains this loss as a result of a change in the position of Infl. According to her, in Old and Early Middle English, Infl is outside the sentential node “S”. As such, in order to be licensed it must be lexicalized either by a complementizer or by V2 movement. In late Middle English, Infl moves inside the “S” node where it is after the subject position. Due to this change in the phrase structure, we no longer observe the V2 pattern.

According to van Kemenade (1997), the asymmetry between pronominal and full

NP subjects was lost so that “fronting of XP to Spec, CP no longer went hand in hand with V/I to C” (van Kemenade (1997:349). With no positive evidence for the existence of verb movement to C, children would fail to acquire the V2 constraint thus causing it to disappear.

The account given in Fischer et al. (2000) equates the loss of V2 with the loss of verb movement to F, which then eliminates the pronoun/full NP asymmetry since all the subjects now come before the verb. They suggest that this loss may be linked to loss of agreement morphology, but they admit that the motivation behind the loss of V to F movement is still unclear.

Haeberli (2000) attributes the loss of V2 to the loss of empty expletives.

According to him, empty expletives are only licensed if singular forms are distinguished from the infinitive. The erosion of the infinitive ending in late Middle

English caused the first person singular to be identical to the infinitive. The empty expletive could no longer appear in SPEC, Agr; therefore, post verbal subjects could no longer be licensed. The Loss of V2 in the History of English 32

By far the most elaborated account of the loss of V2 in late Middle English is the one given by Kroch & Taylor (1997) (K&T) and Kroch, Taylor & Ringe (2000) (KTR).

They develop a complex scenario of language contact and bilingualism based on the

“double base” hypothesis and the notions of “grammar competition” discussed in section

1. According to these authors, the loss of the V2 constraint was a result of language contact between Northern English speakers and Scandinavian settlers on the one hand, and Northern and Southern English speakers on the other.

Between the 9th and 11th centuries, the Vikings (speakers of ) invaded and settled the North of England. K&T and KTR hypothesize that, during this time period, there was a great deal of Old English/Old Norse bilingualism, the Norse speakers having to learn English (as adults) to properly integrate themselves into the community.

Due to imperfect acquisition by the Vikings, some inflectional endings were levelled in the North of England. Therefore, verb movement to AgrS to attach inflectional endings to the verb would no longer take place. If the Old English V2 constraint was satisfied by the verb in AgrS, it could no longer be so when the verb ceased to move there. Since Old

Norse was also a V2 language, the evidence for V2 in a mixed environment was too robust to allow the constraint to be lost; thus, in order to preserve V2, Northern Early

Middle English gained verb movement to C.

According to K&T and KTR, the loss of V2 was a result of contact between the

CPV2 northern speakers and the IPV2 southern speakers. Since, in the North, both pronouns and full NPs invert with non-subject topics, when northerners hear southerners say a sentence with a preverbal pronoun, they conclude that the sentence comes from a non-V2 grammar. The northerners would then acquire this non-V2 grammar, which The Loss of V2 in the History of English 33 would begin to compete with their original V2 grammar. The “non-V2” parameter setting being the typologically less marked, the V2-less northern grammar would have an advantage in the grammar competition, and would therefore spread at the expense of the

V2 grammars. It is also possible that there was a southern dialect which had independently lost V2 (see Prasad (2000) for discussion). This could have favoured the eradication of V2 in England as a whole.

5. Conclusion

In this paper, we have reviewed the literature on the verb second phenomenon in the history of English. As shown by the variety of analyses presented in the literature, the loss of V2 in the history of English could have come about, from a syntactic point of view, in many different ways. It is for this reason that pure technical analyses are insufficient to choose between the many logical possibilities that syntactic description affords. The answer must therefore lie in research into language acquisition and universals, as well as into the sociolinguistic situation of 14th/15th century England. The research undertaken by Kroch & Taylor (1997) and Kroch, Taylor & Ringe (2000)

(regardless of any particular syntactic analysis presented in their work) is a first step on what we believe to be the right path.

The Loss of V2 in the History of English 35

Section 2: Article Summaries

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Prasad, • dialects • V2 was lost • texts from • Kroch & • refinement Rashmi of from 1350-1500 : Taylor of analysis (2000) Middle southern regions show (1997); in Kroch, English MidE in 2 different Kroch, Taylor & • history different patterns of use Taylor & Ringe of ways: of V2 over time Ringe (1997) English a) eastern a) no drop in (1997) midlands: usage of V2 • wrt nature lost due to during this of V2 contact period constraint with in northern Old/Early dialects Middle b) extreme b) sentences English: south & with Vikner west preposed (1991), midlands: element & Iatridou & independen full NP Kroch t syntactic subject: drop (1992), triggers in V2 around Pintzuk (subject 1400 (1991), movement, sentences Kroch & decliticizat with Taylor ion of preposed (1997), pronouns) element & Kemenade pronoun (1987) subject: no change or slight increase in V2 The Loss of V2 in the History of English 36

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

subject: no change or slight increase in V2 Stockwell, • history • V2 in Swieczkowski Robert P. of English (1962) (1984) English became limited to certain contexts a) late OE: a) 70-80% of a) Andrew optional main clauses (1934), norm have V2 Bean (1983) b) ModE: b) some fronted b) Green reflexes of constituents (1980) earlier V2 obligatorily in certain trigger V2 contexts (wh- constituents, negative & affective adv, AdjP, etc), some optionally (, some adverbials), some disallow it (NPs, bare infinitives) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 37

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

adverbials), some disallow it (NPs, bare infinitives) • V2 with • only V2 with • participles fronted be and a few may have progressive other verbs been or passive reanalyzed VP, AdjP, or as predicative adjectives PP really a case of equational inversion Swan, • Syntax • Old English • With non- • OE • wrt ON: • wrt OE as Toril of Old not a true V2 sentence grammar Faarlund underlyingl (1994) English language, adverbials, includes (1990), y SOV: unlike Old always AVS in several Christoffers Lightfoot Norse ON, but concurrent en (1980, (1977), alternation word-order 1994) Kemenade between AVS options • wrt OE (1987) & ASV in OE; word order: • wrt with sentence Stockwell distinction adverbials (1984), between (speaker Stockwell pronouns comment; not a & Minkova and full NP well-developed (1991), subjects in class in ON), Traugott OE: usually ASV in (1972), Kemenade OE, always Vennemann (1987) AVS in ON (1974) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 38

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

class in ON), Traugott OE: usually ASV in (1972), Kemenade OE, always Vennemann (1987) AVS in ON (1974) • wrt nature of sentence adverbials: Halliday (1970)

Haeberli, • morpho • loss of V2 • evidence from • explanatio • wrt • wrt Eric logy- due to loss of modern n for observed reasons for (2000) syntax empty Germanic remaining word loss of V2: interface expletives languages: V2 orders orders: Fuss • history adjuncts may after empty Kemenade (1998), of intervene expletives (1987), Kemenade English between lost? Pintzuk (1987), • syntax fronted V & Remnants, (1991), Platzack of Old subject iff or a V-to-C Koopman (1995), English subject is a full option (1998), Roberts DP, only in Kroch & (1993), languages Taylor Kroch et which license (1997), al. (2000), expletive pro Baekken Lightfoot loss of V2 (1998) (1997) coincides with • wrt nature loss of of V2 in expletive pro- OE: drop in English Cardinaletti & Roberts (1991), Hulk & Kemenade (1997), Kroch & Taylor The Loss of V2 in the History of English 39

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

drop in English Cardinaletti a) decliticizat a) pronominal & Roberts ion of subjects (1991), subject behaving like Hulk & pronouns non- Kemenade doesn’t pronominal (1997), explain it subjects Kroch & should have Taylor reinforced (1997), V2; no Pintzuk explanation (1991, for why 1993) pronouns lost clitic status; object pronouns not decliticized, contrasts in syntactic behaviour of pronominal and non- pronominal subjects remain in Early ModE • empty • evidence from • loss of V2 expletives modern is a lost because Germanic syntactic of changes in languages: effect of a verbal empty morphologi morphology expletives only cal change licensed in languages where all sg pres verb forms The Loss of V2 in the History of English 40

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

lost because Germanic syntactic of changes in languages: effect of a verbal empty morphologi morphology expletives only cal change licensed in languages where all sg pres verb forms have clear agreement morphology of 1sg & infinitive forms in Eng coincided with loss of empty expletives exceptional MidE text with high frequency of V2 also has high frequency of distinct infinitive form • V-fronting • distribution of targets C V2 in OE: with when an non-operator operator is fronted, fronted and a impossible with lower head pronominal when a non- subjects & operator is optional with fronted; 2 non- subj pronominal positions subjects; with The Loss of V2 in the History of English 41

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

operator is fronted, fronted and a impossible with lower head pronominal when a non- subjects & operator is optional with fronted; 2 non- subj pronominal positions subjects; with possible, one operator above & one fronted, occurs below the with both types lower target; of subjects pronouns must occur above but full DP subjects can stay below if higher position filled by empty expletive Koopman, • syntax • V2 effect • any Willem F. of Old much less constituent can (1997) English strong in OE be topicalized, than in Dutch but different & German constituents cause varying frequencies of inversion The Loss of V2 in the History of English 42

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

cause varying frequencies of inversion Fischer, • syntax • IP consists • root questions • wrt dialect Kemenade Olga; (synchro of FP (hosts have VS order, variation in (1987), Kemenade nic & pronouns), embedded MidE: Pintzuk , Ans van; diachron NegP, TP; questions Kroch & (1991) Koopman, ic) Vf lands in C don’t; Taylor Willem; for questions, embedded (1997) Wurff, neg-initial, topicalization Wim van (th)a-initial, allowed only in der (2000) but in F quoted root ch 4 (with clauses & pronouns in unaccusative specF) in constructions; topic-initial contrast between positions of pronominal and non- pronominal subjects (esp. in negative- initial sentences) • FP has • V2 with • Learners morphology topicalization draw for subj-V lost at the same conclusions agreement; time as some about word V-to-F verb-agreement order based movement (sg- on lost along pl constrasts) inflectional with morpholog agreement y rather than on observed The Loss of V2 in the History of English 43

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

agreement; time as some about word V-to-F verb-agreement order based movement inflections (sg- on lost along pl constrasts) inflectional with morpholog agreement y rather inflection than on observed word order patterns Cardinalet • Nomina • some • evidence from • wrt to • wrt ti, Anna; tive- languages Icelandic (V2 potential A- analysis of Roberts, Case have two in embedded positions: Tobler- Ian (1991) assignm subject clauses), Old Rizzi Mussafia ent AgrP’s (one French (V2 in (1991) effects: • Syntax directly wh-clauses), • wrt Dutch Alberton of Old below Comp, Yiddish clitics: (1990), French one lower); (embedded Zwart Benincà • Syntax higher one topicalization) (1991) (1989) of Old assigns • wrt English nominative Romance case clitics: a) clitics a) evidence Mussafia occupy from German (1983) Agr1E (ordering of • clitics (head of clitics wrt always in higher each other & head AgrP); full subject), positions: verbal medieval Kayne inflection Romance (1991) in Agr2E (Tobler/Muss afia law on position of clitics), Old English and Old High The Loss of V2 in the History of English 44

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

verbal medieval Kayne inflection Romance (1991) in Agr2E (Tobler/Muss • wrt afia law on Nominative position of -Case clitics), Old assignment: English and Koopman Old High & Sportiche German (1991) (interaction • wrt null of clitics & subjects: V2) Rizzi b) Agr1E (or b) possible (1986) AgrE in subject • wrt languages positions in connection with only Icelandic, between V2 one) German, OE, & null assigns OF, Dutch, subjects in Nom under Italian, OF: agreement English Vanelli, where it is Renzi & a clitic Benincà position, or (1986) where it • wrt contains an distribution inflected V of which is referential coindexed null with the subjects in NP in the OF texts: specifier; Hirschbuhle possibility r (1990) of assigning under governmen The Loss of V2 in the History of English 45

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

with the subjects in NP in the OF texts: specifier; Hirschbuhle possibility r (1990) of assigning under governmen t is a parametric choice c) pro c) distribution licensed in of pro in configurati Icelandic, ons where German, OF Nom can be assigned, except with Spec-head coindexati on • OF does not • embedded V2 have rare with overt generalized subjects (except embedded in very early topicalization texts), although (does have common with Stylistic null subjects Fronting): subj can be in either Agr The Loss of V2 in the History of English 46

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Stylistic null subjects Fronting): subj can be in either Agr a) after 13th a) in embedded c., only one clauses, Agr, which referential cannot null subjects license null not seen in subj under prose after agreement 12th c; clitic- (inflectiona first orders in l yes/no morpholog questions y too appear in 13th poor): c prose; OF related to case system loss of lost between Tobler- 12th & 14th c; Mussafia among effect & Germanic morphologi lang, cal case morphologica l case correlates with double Agr • OE clitics • distribution of are in Agr1E; clitic pronouns V can be in wrt V and subject Agr1E or CE in main clauses, Agr2E in embedded clauses The Loss of V2 in the History of English 47

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

are in Agr1E; clitic pronouns V can be in wrt V and subject Agr1E or CE in main clauses, Agr2E in embedded clauses Kohonen, • syntax • shift from • quantitative • Lightfoot Viljo of Old SOV to SVO data (OE): new (1977), (1978) English partly due to information Johansson Ch 5 • history tendency to more often (1968), of place new after V than Thompson English information given (1978) • relation after V • wrt ships • topicalizatio • hierarchies between n: of word a) triggered a) given givenness: order by information Chafe phenom connection most often (1970), ena with fronted; Sgall et al. preceding fronting of (1973) discourse, long or new • wrt emphasis; constituents, topicalizatio affected by negators, n in distinction verbs gives dependent between emphatic clauses: obligatory effect; Hooper & & consecutive Thompson peripheral sentences (1973), elements, may have Green weight of similar (1976) constituent pattern s, stylistic factors The Loss of V2 in the History of English 48

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

peripheral sentences (1973), elements, may have Green weight of similar (1976) constituent pattern • wrt triggers s, stylistic of factors topicalizatio b) inversion b) VS order n: used with more Johansson long/new common with (1968), subjects to longer Enkvist solve subjects and (1975, conflict with new 1976) between subjects • wrt informatio developmen n and t of grammatic passives: al Lightfoot structures; (1977) XSV order usual with short, given subjects c) when SVX c) proportio order fixed n of for longer topicaliza subjects, tion topicalizati should on blocked decrease as word order becomes fixed The Loss of V2 in the History of English 49

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

on blocked decrease as word order becomes fixed • developmen • Subjectless • if t of passives decline topicalization passivization between 10th & also related connected to 13th c to SVO change from order, SOV to SVO passivization should increase as topicalization decreases • extrapositio • hit and thaer n of clausal optional, but subjects/object increases from s: insertion of 10th to 13th c dummy subject developed along with SVX syntax Hulk, • syntax: • languages • V2 languages • Lightfoot • Wrt OF as Aafke; verb may be C- or have V-to-C (1979) IV2: Kemenade second, I-oriented; when no • Wrt OF: Hirschbuhl , Ans van pro-drop dominant complementize Adams er & (1995) • history head must be r in C (1987) Junker of lexicalised (1988), English Lemieux & Dupuis (1995) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 50

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

of lexicalised • Wrt (1988), English a) I licensed a) impersonal embedded Lemieux & • history by having sentences: I V2 in OF: Dupuis of ϕ-features not fully Vance (1995) French identified licensed, so (1988) (syntactical V cannot ly or assign Case morpologic (but P or ally); V morphology can assign can) Case iff governed by fully licensed I b) pro-drop: b) cross- pro must linguistic be distribution governed of full and by expletive designated pro-drop Case- assigning head (C or I); content must be identified • OF: C- • referential pro- oriented drop only in root language; clauses referential pro licensed when V+I moves to C The Loss of V2 in the History of English 51

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

language; clauses referential pro licensed when V+I moves to C • MidF: • V2 becomes increase of V3 allowed in structures, complements of embedded V2 non-bridge V a) cliticizatio a) V1 structures n of subject & embedded pronouns: V2 XP- (characteristi pronoun- cs of IV2 Vf lang) become ambiguous more between common; CP and IP pro-drop interpretati available in on; this root and leads to nonroot shift from contexts C- orientation to I- orientation b) later I loses b) V2 & pro- morphologi drop cal ϕ- disappear features; subj NP in Spec,IP becomes obligatory The Loss of V2 in the History of English 52

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

cal ϕ- disappear features; subj NP in Spec,IP becomes obligatory • OE: C- • Root/nonroot oriented lang asymmetry a) pronominal a) data from subjects Wycliffite usually sermons before V; (MidE) when discrepanc y between nominal and pronominal subjects lost, nominal subjects use pronominal pattern in many dialects b) as V2 b) loss of declines, expletive pro- Eng drop changes from C- oriented to I-oriented The Loss of V2 in the History of English 53

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

declines, expletive pro- Eng drop changes from C- oriented to I-oriented • I has ϕ- • distribution of features in OF referential pro- • Eng: but not in OE drop, subjects Spec,IP obligatorily subject position; cannot be IV2 Platzack, • Syntax: • English & • ME: • wrt • wrt loss of • theoretical Christer verb French lost pronominal theoretical V2: Vance framework: (1995) second, V2 because subjects keep framework: (1989), V2 lang pro-drop they had same patterns Holmberg Weerman have • History SVO & after object & Platzack (1989) finiteness of subject clitics lost; (1988, ([±F]) English clitics: inversion 1995(?)), feature in C, • History unambiguous optional in 14th Platzack & non-V2 lang of V2 sentences c Holmberg in I French were rare MF: subject (1989) a) [+F] must pronouns • wrt reasons govern become clitics for loss of Nominativ • Nominal • ME: subject-V V2: e; Agr agreement Kemenade Nominativ (connected to weakened in (1987), e must be agreement 14th c; 14th c Adams governed morphology, sentences with (1987) by head V-to-I & pro- do have tense hosting drop) lost at on both verbs [+F] or by the same (Infl-lowering); chain time as V2 in pro-drop whose Eng, but later declines head is The Loss of V2 in the History of English 54

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(connected to weakened in (1987), e must be agreement 14th c; 14th c Adams governed morphology, sentences with (1987) by head V-to-I & pro- do have tense • wrt loss of hosting drop) lost at on both verbs V-to-I in [+F] or by the same (Infl-lowering); Eng: chain time as V2 in pro-drop Pollock whose Eng, but later declines (1989) head is in Fr MF: pro-drop governed extended to by [+F] more contexts b) Agr after loss of V2 (hosted in I) may be nominal or not; nominal Agr bears Nominativ e Case & allows pro-drop

Kemenade • Syntax: • Old English • Data from OE • Movement • wrt theory • wrt word • first two , Ans van verb underlyingly compared with rules of language order in points from (1987) second SOV (θ-roles Dutch: needed: change: OE: Malsh ch. 2, the • History assigned distribution of extrapositio Lightfoot (1976), rest from ch. of from right to V and particles n of objects (1979) Reddick 6 English left) to right of • wrt word (1982) V, V2, V- order in raising to OE: form verbal Lightfoot clusters (1976, 1977), Canale (1978), Koopman The Loss of V2 in the History of English 55

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

V, V2, V- order in • wrt raising to OE: structure of form verbal Lightfoot V2: Travis clusters (1976, (1984), • In V2 lang, • Evidence from 1977), Weerman INFL is in Dutch, West Canale (1986), COMP & Flemish (1978), Safir triggers Koopman (1982) move V; (1984) head of S is • wrt V in V2 lang diagnostics and INFL in for SOV other lang; order: topicalization Koster is optional (1975) • 2 major • V2 orders • wrt changes in found after extrapositio MidE: change to VO n: order complete Stockwell a) change a) Position of (1977), from SOV particles Kuno to SVO relative to V; (1974), (change in V-raising & Pintzuk & direction of extraposition Kroch θ-marking, give surface (1985) ca 1200), VO order, • wrt because of loss of structure of common oblique V2: Evers surface VO (dat/gen) (1981, patterns & case & loss 1982), loss of case of clitics on Cremers & morpholog VP date to ca Sassen y 1200 (1983), Platzack (1983), Koopman The Loss of V2 in the History of English 56

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

surface VO (dat/gen) (1981, patterns & case & loss 1982), loss of case of clitics on Cremers & morpholog VP date to ca Sassen y 1200 (1983), b) loss of V2 b) Decrease in b) INFL Platzack (ca 1400), subj-V comes to (1983), because of inversion in assign case Koopman difficulty the early 15th from right to (1984), de in c, changes in left, opposite Haan & interpretin complementi to V & P at Weerman g subject- zers; subject this stage (1985), initial pronouns Taraldsen sentences cease to (1983) as V2 & behave as • wrt clitics: reduction clitics Borer in verbal (1983), agreement Aoun morpholog (1985) y • wrt change from SOV to SVO: Canale (1978), Hiltunen (1983) Anderson, • optimal • placement • evidence from • wrt • wrt Stephen R. ity of special Hittite, Serbo- distinction Macedonia (2000) theory clitics based Croatian, between n: Tomi_ on Macedonian, simple (1996) morphologic Kwakw’ala, (phonologic al processes: French, ally clitics are dependent) on the clitics & phrase special (syntacticall The Loss of V2 in the History of English 57

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(2000) theory clitics based Croatian, between n: Tomi_ • morpho on Macedonian, simple (1996) syntax: morphologic Kwakw’ala, (phonologic • wrt clitics, al processes: French, ally stylistic verb clitics are Sanskrit dependent) fronting: second affixes on the clitics & Maling phrase special (1980), • clitics are • evidence from (syntacticall Jónsson introduced Serbo-Croatian, y unusual) (1991) simultaneous Italian, clitics: ly (rather Bulgarian, Zwicky than by a set Warlpiri, (1977) of ordered European • wrt clitics rules) & Portuguese as arranged morphology based on : Anderson element- (1992, specific 1993, 1995, constraints 1996) (some • wrt Hittite: stronger than Garrett others): (1990) edgemost, • wrt Serbo- non-initial, Croatian: integrity Browne (prevents (1974), word or Bo_kovi_ phrase from (1995) being interrupted) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 58

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

being • wrt interrupted) Warlpiri: • V2 based on • evidence from Simpson the same Icelandic • V need not (1991) constraints: always move: non- V2 can be initial(Vfin,S) satisfied by & subject in its edgemost(Vfi normal n,L,S) causes position or by the verb stylistic movement fronting Roberts, • syntax: • in V2 • evidence from • wrt • wrt to • first point Ian (1993) verb contexts, C German (pro- structure of structure of from ch. 1, second, morphologic drop, clitics), I: Pollock OF V2: next 4 from null ally selects West Flemish (1989), Adams ch. 2, last subjects Agr to (complementiz Cardinaletti (1988), point from • history incorporate er agreement), & Roberts Dupuis 3.4 of with it; head Dutch (clitics) (1991) (1989) French containing • wrt Case- • wrt pro- • history Agr must assignment: drop in of have filled Sportiche OF: English spec (1988), Adams • languag • Fr lost • distribution of • possibility Koopmann (1987), e option to simple & of V2 & & Sportiche Dupuis acquisiti assign complex null (1990) (1988, on Nominative inversion subjects • wrt 1989) under depends on incorporatio • wrt MidF government setting of n: Baker subject Nominative (1988), pronouns -assignment Rizzi & as clitics: parameter Roberts Adams (1989) (1987) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 59

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

government setting of n: Baker subject a) OF null a) distribution Nominative (1988), pronouns subjects of null -assignment Rizzi & as clitics: were subjects parameter Roberts Adams licensed (1989) (1987) under • wrt governmen interrogativ t e inversion: b) V2 lost b) distribution Rizzi because of SV and (1990) Nominativ VS orders • wrt e became licensing of only pro : Rizzi assigned (1986), by Jaeggli & agreement Safir (1989) (subject • wrt had to be stylistic before V) fronting in • OF • distribution of OF: Dupuis originally V2, free (1989), had 2 AgrPs, inversion, null Lema & V2 subjects Rivero movement to (1990) the higher • wrt left one; Agr1 dislocation lost in 13th c., in OF: V2 became Foulet movement to (1921), C Schulze The Loss of V2 in the History of English 60

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

movement to (1921), C Schulze • MidF: • wrt null a) more a) evidence subjects in elements from MidF, MidF: became Swedish, Vance able to Icelandic: V (1989), appear movement, Hirschbuhle initially possibility of r (1991) without V3 orders • Subset triggering Principle in inversion; acquisition: CP Berwick reanalysed (1985) as AgrP • Wrt V2 in b) Agr able to b) distribution OE: license pro of pro Kemenade under (1987) agreement when C not present • Least Effort Strategy in acquisition: sentences interpreted to contain shortest possible chains The Loss of V2 in the History of English 61

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

possible chains • MidE lost V2 because it acquired Nominative- assignment under agreement, after subject pronouns decliticized Lightfoot, • languag • children • evidence from • model of • models of David e scan Creoles, sign acquisition: acquisition (1999) ch. acquisiti environment language Dresher & : Gibson & 6 on & (simple Kaye Wexler change sentences) (1990), (1994), • history for Dresher Clark of designated (1998), (1992) English cues on how Fodor • wrt loss of to set (1998) V2: Clark parameters • wrt dialects & Roberts • V2 of MidE: (1993) sentences Kroch & begin with Taylor arbitrary XP (1997) in Spec,CP; • wrt sign material in language: Spec,CP Goldin- must be Meadow & licensed by Mylander lexical C (1990), Newport (1998), Supalla (1990) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 62

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Spec,CP Goldin- must be Meadow & licensed by Mylander lexical C (1990), a) cue for V2 Newport is (1998), sentences Supalla beginning (1990) with nonsubject s; V2 acquired if cue is robust b) two dialects of MidE, one with V2 b) lower and one percentage of without; nonsubject-initial cue is not V2 clauses in robust for MidE than in children modern V2 lang, who hear V3 orders with both pronouns Pintzuk, • syntax • Kemenade’s • arguments for • wrt • wrt syntax • first point Susan of Old (1987) verb projection problems of OE: from ch. 2, (1999) English analysis raising don’t with radical Kemenade next three doesn’t work work with reanalysis: (1987) from ch. 3, different deep Allen last point structure; (1990) from ch. 6 clitics assumed to behave differently in subordinate & main clauses; The Loss of V2 in the History of English 63

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

• languag doesn’t work work with reanalysis: (1987) from ch. 3, e change different deep Allen last point structure; (1990) from ch. 6 clitics assumed • wrt syntax to behave of OE: differently in Kiparsky subordinate & (1995) main clauses; • wrt not all surface nominative verb positions vs neutral accounted for; AGR: frequency of Santorini verb-medial (1992), order in main Platzack & & subordinate Holmberg clauses (1990) analysed as unrelated, although they increase at the same rate • Clauses vary • distribution of between particles, object INFL-medial pronouns, & INFL-final monosyllabic adverbs in subordinate clauses; frequency of verb raising in INFL-final clauses The Loss of V2 in the History of English 64

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

frequency of verb raising in INFL-final clauses • Double base • variation hypothesis: within texts by variation in individual the authors grammars of individual speakers • V2 in OE • distribution of usually clitics movement to clause- medial INFL a) V moves to COMP in questions, V1 clauses, & clauses beginning with certain adverbs • V2 lost because AGR changed from inherently nominative to neutral (so came to need to be c- commanded The Loss of V2 in the History of English 65

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

changed from inherently nominative to neutral (so came to need to be c- commanded by a thematic antecedent) Kroch, • dialects • 2 types of • evidence from • wrt 2 types Anthony; of borrowing: code-switching of V2 Taylor, Middle ordinary in native languages: Ann; English borrowing Yiddish Diesing Ringe, • languag (of content speakers with (1990), Donald e words from fluent English; Pintzuk (1997) contact 2nd language Norse function (1991), • history into 1st) & words in Santorini of imposition English (1992) English (borrowing • wrt of Scandinavia grammatical n influence features from in England: 1st language Geipel into 2nd) (1971), • V2 lost Stenton because of (1967), dialect Ekwall contact: (1936), Morse- Gagné (1992, 1993) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 66

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

contact: (1936), a) southern a) distribution Morse- dialects of subject Gagné preserve inversion in (1992, OE IP-V2 13th c. 1993) pattern Midlands • wrt (movement texts imposition: to Agr) van b) northern b) distribution Coetsem dialects are of subject (1988), CP-V2 inversion in a Appel & northern text Muysken c) northern c) comparison (1987), speakers of northern & Prince & interpret southern Pintzuk many manuscripts (1984) southern of one text; • wrt syntax sentences inversion in of OE : as non-V2 Chaucer Pintzuk & acquire (southern) (1991), competing Haeberli & grammar; Haegeman in south, (1995), IP-V2 first Kroch & replaced Taylor with CP- (1997) V2 and • wrt then lost connection between morphologi cal agreement and V-to-I movement : Platzack & Holmberg The Loss of V2 in the History of English 67

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

• northern • placement of between dialect (originally null) morphologi becomes CP- subject cal V2 because pronouns in agreement of Norse Northumbrian and V-to-I influence glosses of movement : text Platzack & a) Norse a) evidence Holmberg settlers from (1989), learning Northumbria Roberts English n texts (1993), simplify Rohrbacher verbal (1994) agreement • wrt split paradigm INFL: due to Pollock phonologic (1989) al issues b) V-to-Agr b) evidence movement from lost due to negation in reduction MidE and of Modern agreement; Scandinavian verb movement reanalysed as V-to-C The Loss of V2 in the History of English 68

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Yang, • model • variational • research in • wrt • wrt theory Charles D. of approach: mathematical competition of (2000) languag language psychology; -based learning: e acquisition is gradualness of learning: Gibson & acquisiti a acquisition Atkinson, Wexler on & competition Bower & (1994) change process Crothers • wrt final among a (1965), state of population of Bush & acquisition grammars Mosteller : Clark & • language (1951, Roberts change is 1958) (1993) change in the • wrt what distribution determines of grammars outcome of a) caused by a) competence acquisition: change in of children Clark & the acquiring Roberts linguistic language; (1993) evidence, scale of • wrt not by language variation in individual change grammars: mislearnin Kroch g (1989), b) if one b) mathematical Kroch & grammar evidence Taylor has an (1997), advantage Kroch, over Taylor, & another, it Ringe will (2000), overtake it Pintzuk (1991), Santorini (1992) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 69

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

over Taylor, & another, it Ringe will (2000), overtake it Pintzuk • advantage of • distribution of • same (1991), V2 lost in subject analysis Santorini French inversion & may extend (1992) because of pro-drop in to other • wrt to null subjects MidFr Western dialects of and increase Romance MidE: in V>2 languages Kroch & patterns Taylor • in MidE: • distribution of (1997) a) south: with inversion with • wrt link loss of nominal & between morpholog pronominal subject ical case, subjects clitics & clitics V2: reanalyzed Kemenade as NPs, (1987) increasing • wrt link the between advantage clitics & of SVO morphologi over V2 cal case: b) north: Kiparsky contact (1997) with south leads to mixture of grammars & weakening of V2 The Loss of V2 in the History of English 70

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

leads to mixture of grammars & weakening of V2 Koopman, • theoreti • pied-piping • evidence from • Kayne Hilda cal is common, Bantu lang., (1994), (1997) syntax even with Eng (DP- Koopman heavy internal); Fr (1994) constituents (IP); Nweh • wrt Nweh: (complement of Nkemnji NegP) (1995) • linear order • wrt Double of overt Voice filter: terminal Sportiche elements (1992) corresponds • wrt to developmen asymmetric ts of c-command English : a) projection a) doubly filled a) some Kemenade can’t have Comp filter, projectio (1987), W. terminal Double ns, such Koopman elements in Voice filter, as CP, (1990), both Spec evidence must be Roberts and head; from Dutch, more (1993) only one is Eng, Fr wrt articulate overt apparent d than violations; previousl evidence y thought from English, Dutch, Irish The Loss of V2 in the History of English 71

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

overt apparent d than violations; previousl evidence y thought from English, Dutch, Irish b) if head & b) evidence Spec both from Italian, overt, one Catalan, may Spanish become (negative silent head); Dutch & German (topic drop), pro-drop • projection • evidence from interpretable English (CP in iff activated declaratives, by lexical wh-movement) material: lexical material must be associated with either Spec or head during derivation • ModE has more Spec marking than OE The Loss of V2 in the History of English 72

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

more Spec marking than OE a) OE a) apparent (apparent) loss of V2 V2 really involved due to topic changes movement in to Spec,CP distributi with V on of staying lexical lower items lower in the clause b) VS order b) coexistence has head of V movement movement & pied-piping, of V, SV rather than order has sudden pied-piping reanalysis with of an XP loss of verbal containing morphology the V; overt bound head forces head movement, but lack of it doesn’t prevent it The Loss of V2 in the History of English 73

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

but lack of it doesn’t prevent it c) weak c) evidence from c) need a pronouns Fr, Bantu clitics theory for not clitics; what preverbal determines object whether pronouns small XP or have heavy XP moved moves independen tly, postverbal objects have heavy XP movement Lightfoot, • theory • acquisition • evidence from • wrt nature • wrt David of of V2 should children of V2: motivation (1995) acquisiti be possible learning Paardekoop for V2: den on without German, er (1971), Besten • history access to Dutch, English den Besten (1983), de of negative & French (1983), Haan & English data: inverted forms Rizzi Weerman children (1990) (1986), learn that V2 • wrt Haider sentences acquisition (1986), begin with an of Koopman arbitrary auxiliaries (1984), phrasal in English: Platzack category; Newport, (1986), initial Gleitman & Safir category Gleitman (1981), isn’t (1977), Evers complement Richards (1982), The Loss of V2 in the History of English 74

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

begin with an of Koopman arbitrary auxiliaries (1984), phrasal in English: Platzack category; Newport, (1986), initial Gleitman & Safir category Gleitman (1981), isn’t (1977), Evers complement Richards (1982), so must be (1990) Taraldsen specifier; • wrt loss of (1986) lexical V2 in E: specifier Schmidt requires (1980), lexical head, Kemenade so V+I (1987) moves there • loss of V2: • sudden decline gradual in V2 in 14th c. increase in English & subject- French; initial V2 increase in sentences, subject-initial followed by clauses in 11th- reanalysis & 12th c. E sudden loss of V2 • loss of V-to- I: The Loss of V2 in the History of English 75

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

a) loss of V- a) be undergoes to-C, V-to-I when verbal richly inflection inflected but are factors not in but not a dialects complete where it’s explanatio uninflected; n V-to-I retained in E & F after loss of V2, Scandinavian languages have V-to-I without verbal inflection b) alternatives b) frequency of of do optional do support & support in late Middle- hopping early Modern removed English need for V- to-I Bækken, • history • V2 patterns • Evidence • Nevalainen Bjørg of persisted into from 15th-18th (1997) (2000) English Early c. prose texts Modern English: subject-verb inversion gradually decreases with most The Loss of V2 in the History of English 76

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(2000) English Early c. prose texts • wrt initial • influen Modern adverbials: ce of English: Breivik & pragmat subject-verb Swan ic inversion (1994), factors gradually Kohonen on decreases (1978) syntax with most initial elements & increases with negative initial elements • inversion • full NP due to end subjects invert focus/end more often than weight: new pronouns; information distribution of tends to new subjects occur at the end of the clause Bean, • history • OE not • data from the • wrt theory: • wrt Marian C. of verb-second Anglo-Saxon Firbas theory: (1983) English or topic- Chronicle & (1964), Li Lehmann verb; other texts & (1973), changed Thompson Venneman directly from (1976), n (1973), SXV to Greenberg Bartsch & X’SV (1963), Venneman Givón n (1972), (1977) Hawkins (1979), Lakoff (1972), The Loss of V2 in the History of English 77

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

directly from (1976), n (1973), SXV to Greenberg Bartsch & X’SV (1963), Venneman a) V2 orders Givón n (1972), due to (1977) Hawkins narrative • wrt OE: (1979), style Smith Lakoff b) preverbal (1893), (1972), pronominal McKnight Koch objects (1897), (1974), give Saitz Haiman appearance (1955), (1974), of topic- Carlton Lightfoot verb (1959) (1979), ordering McKnight (1897), Hyman (1975), Li & Thompson (1974) • wrt OE: Brown (1970), Johansson (1968), Kohonen (1976) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 78

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Haukenes, • history • OE not • Data from OE, • Venneman • Ella of consistently Old Nordic n (1974) (2004) English V2: TVX • wrt • syntax- • ModE • Data from emotive pragmat negative MidE, early principle: ics inversion, ModE Firbas interface restrictions (1992) on type of • wrt subject not pragmatic directly renewal: descended Traugott from OE (1989), • when SVO • Data from Brinton & order became ModE, Stein grammaticali Norwegian (1993) sed, V2 order came to encode pragmatic information de Haas, • history • pre-contact • evidence from • wrt origins • wrt degree Nynke of Northumbria 7th-9th c. poems & dialects of (2004) English n: no of OE: comprehen • languag loanwords, Nielsen sion e negation (1998, between contact expressed 1986) ON and with • wrt contact OE: independent with Townend ni or ne, no Vikings: (2002), generalized Loyn Hansen –s verb (1994), (1984) ending Baugh & Cable (1993), Nielsen (1998), The Loss of V2 in the History of English 79

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

ni or ne, no Vikings: (2002), generalized Loyn Hansen –s verb (1994), (1984) ending • evidence from Baugh & • wrt • post-contact 8th-11th c texts: Cable influence Northumbria more (1993), of ON: n shows loanwords, but Nielsen Samuels evidence of no borrowed (1998), (1989), borrowing, pronouns; Burnley Burnley but not of double (1992), (1992) structural negation begins Townend • wrt V2: interference to appear (2002), Kroch & • evidence from Pons-Sanz Taylor • MidE: MidE texts: (2001, (1997), southern texts from 2004), Trips texts more Danelaw show Barnes (2002) conservative many (2000), • wrt verbal & show less loanwords, Ekwall inflection: ON influence borrowed (1930), Kroch & pronouns Samuels Taylor a) double (1989) (1997), a) changes in negation • wrt dialects Kroch, negation increases of MidE: Taylor & not due to during period Baugh & Ringe interferenc of contact Cable (2000), e • verbal (1993), Samuels • generalized paradigms of Trips (1989) –s ending not Northumbrian/ (2002) due to ON Northern MidE influence vs Mercian/East Midlands MidE The Loss of V2 in the History of English 80

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

due to ON Northern MidE • wrt influence vs changes in Mercian/East OE & ME: Midlands MidE Lass • evidence from (1992), • evidence Northern texts Fischer et al that V2 (2000), changed in Baugh & the North, Cable but no clear (1993) link to ON • wrt influence contact- induced change: Thomason (2001), Sebba (1997), Arends, Muysken & Smith (1995) • wrt borrowing: Burnley (1992), Townend (2002), Baugh & Cable (1993), Serjeantson (1935) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 81

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Baugh & Cable (1993), Serjeantson (1935) • wrt pronouns: Lass (1992) • wrt negation: Kemenade (2000) • wrt V2: Fischer et al (2000), Kemenade (2000) • wrt Old Northumbri an texts: Sweet (1975), Campbell (1959) • wrt post- contact OE texts: Kuhn (1965), Skeat (1871), Hogg (2004), Pons-Sanz (2004) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 82

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

Skeat (1871), Hogg (2004), Pons-Sanz (2004) • wrt MidE texts: Morris (1874, 1878), Iyeiri (2001), Kock (1902), Kroch & Taylor (1997), Clark (1970), Bennet & Smithers (1968), Fischer et al (2000), Holt (1878), Trips (2002), Hall (1920), Holthausen (1921), Jack (1978), Dance (2003), Stadlmann (1921) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 83

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(1920), Holthausen (1921), Jack (1978), Dance (2003), Stadlmann (1921) Henry, • syntacti • V2 in • Evidence from • Restricted • wrt Alison c change imperatives: Belfast choice of embedded (1997) loss goes English: 3 grammars: inversion in through stage dialects, one learners BE: where certain with V2, one check McCloskey verbs appear with inversion possibilities (1992) to permit V2 with against • wrt object but don’t unaccusatives data, rather shift: (subject inside than Holmberg VP), one with working (1986, no inversion out rules 1992), • Individual • Speakers of Vikner grammars different ages (1990) are have different • wrt pro- categorical, dialects drop: but a pool of Burzio examples (1981), from Bouchard speakers (1983) with • wrt different negation in grammars imperatives: would seem Beukema & to show Coopmans gradual (1989), decline Zhang (1991) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 84

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

different negation in grammars imperatives: would seem Beukema & to show Coopmans gradual (1989), decline Zhang (1991) Heycock, • syntax: • licensing • evidence from • wrt • Haider Caroline; moveme relation German, German: (1988) Kroch, nt & between two Dutch, English Höhle • wrt SLF- Anthony licensin elements is (coordination); (1983) coordinatio (1993) g satisfied by Eng subject • wrt Dutch: n: Van highest questions; Zwart Valin position in a subject-initial (1991) (1986) chain at clauses in V2 • wrt OE • wrt which lang; data: analysis of licensing distribution of Kemenade V2: Travis configuration topics in (1987, (1984) occurs; Yiddish (and 1992), structure OE) Pintzuk projected (1991) from heads is the minimum within which all licensing relations can be satisfied: unnecessary traces/project ions are deleted The Loss of V2 in the History of English 85

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

traces/project ions are deleted a) V2 always a) evidence involves from movement German, OE, to C, but if Dutch: IP is distribution deleted, C of unstressed takes on subject functions pronouns, of I different behaviour of subject-initial & non- subject-initial matrix clauses Kemenade • history • late • evidence from • wrt data: • wrt loss of , Ans van of MidE change OE & MidE Schmidt impersonal (1993) English from C- texts; decrease (1981), van s: • syntax: orientation to in doubly filled der Gaaf Lightfoot verb I-orientation COMP (1904), (1981), second, leads to Elmer Bennis pro-drop several (1981) (1986) changes: loss • wrt of V2, loss of sentence impersonal structure: constructions Chomsky without (1986) nominative, loss of expletive pro-drop The Loss of V2 in the History of English 86

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

constructions Chomsky without (1986) nominative, • wrt pro- loss of drop: expletive Taraldsen pro-drop (1978), • languages • evidence from Chomsky may be C- Dutch, Italian (1982), oriented or I- Rizzi oriented; C- (1982), oriented Platzack lang. can’t (1987) have full pro- drop because C can’t identify referential pro Kiparsky, • history • in English & • Evidence from • ModE • wrt OE • wrt Paul of early ModE, OE, Old more data: Allen origins of (1995) English Germanic, Icelandic, Old conservativ (1977), V2 : Spec-C is a High German: e than other Mitchell Wackernag focus interaction of Germanic (1985), el (1892), position topicalization lang. in Kemenade Hock (hosts wh- & wh- distinguishi (1987), (1982), phrases, phrases/negatio ng topic & Pintzuk & Anderson demonstrativ n, placement of wh landing Kroch (1992), es, negation); clitic pronouns, sites (1989), Lenerz can be word order in Pintzuk (1984, preceded by conjoined (1991) 1985) a topic clauses, position differences adjoined to between CP relative clauses with a bare complementize The Loss of V2 in the History of English 87

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

can be word order in Pintzuk (1984, preceded by conjoined (1991) 1985) a topic clauses, • wrt Old position differences Icelandic: adjoined to between Maling & CP relative clauses Zaenen with a bare (1981), complementize Maling r & with a (1976) demonstrative • wrt Old pronoun plus High complementize German: r Behaghel • Indo- • Evidence from (1932) European Vedic Sanskrit, • wrt theory also had two Hittite, early of left- Greek, Latin: cliticization peripheral topicalization : Halpern operator with wh- (1992) positions, but elements, • wrt IE lacked the placement of phrase category of clitics, adjoined structure: complementi rather than Hale (1987, zer subordinated 1989), relative clauses Hock a) Germanic a) early (1989), (& others) Germanic Garrett gained languages (1992) compleme have ntizers complementi zers (but OE still allows adjoined relatives) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 88

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

compleme have ntizers complementi zers (but OE still allows adjoined relatives) b) V-to-C movement first triggered by elements in Spec-C • OE: V-to-C • Evidence from movement OE: possible obligatory, verb positions but C optional when not required Koopman, • syntax • pronominal • distribution of • • Mitchell • wrt Willem F. of Old subjects not inversion after (1985) subject (1996) English always clitics various initial pronouns elements in OE as clitics: • topicalizatio • rates of Kemenade n signals inversion after (1987), main clause; adverbs which Pintzuk pronominal can also be (1991) subjects subordinating invert to conjunctions: disambiguate high when alone, lower when another topic present The Loss of V2 in the History of English 89

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

subjects subordinating invert to conjunctions: disambiguate high when alone, lower when another topic present • difficult to • position of explain verbs in non- variable inverted inversion of clauses, nominal distribution of subjects: resumptive can’t always pronouns be failure of V2, left- dislocation Kroch, • languag • once a • evidence from • wrt settings • wrt loss of • draft Anthony e parameter is child language of V-to-I: (2005) acquisiti set, it can’t acquisition: parameters: Lightfoot on & be unset; if errors in case Clahsen (1993, change it’s set marking in (1991), 1999) • history wrong, the German & Penner of correct Russian, wh- (1992) English setting is questions in • wrt FLA: learned as English, V2 in Clahsen another German, tough- (1984), dialect movement in Tracy Eng (1986), Babyonyshe v et al (1994), Polinsky (to appear), W & T Labov (1976), Müller The Loss of V2 in the History of English 90

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

• in both • similar time Babyonyshe acquisition course for v et al errors & correction of (1994), language acquisition Polinsky (to change, errors & appear), W speakers historical & T Labov have change (1976), competing Müller settings; over (1996), time, one Anderson wins out (2001), • acquisition • evolution of McKee errors may Yiddish from (1997) cause German (SLA), • wrt OE language Icelandic change to change by “dative VO order: skewing sickness” Pintzuk linguistic among children (1997, data 2002) available to • wrt children Yiddish: • grammar • evidence from Santorini change OE (shift from (1993) precedes OV to VO), • wrt frequency Yiddish (shift frequency change to INFL- of medial), MidE periphrastic (loss of V-to-I)\ do: Ellegård (1953) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 91

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

• V2 lost from • evidence from (1953) OE because 13th-17th c. • wrt V2 in of 3-way texts: different OE: Kroch competition syntax in & Taylor between Northern & (1997), Northern Southern texts; Haeberli dialect, rates of (2000), Southern inversion for Kemenade dialect & nominal & (1999, non-V2 pronominal 1987), dialect subjects Johnson & created by Whitton contact (2002) Lightfoot, • theory • number of • mathematical • wrt • wrt David of parameters evidence: distinction Swedish: (1997) acquisiti should be number of between Andersson on restricted to possible root & & Dahl 30-40 grammars subordinate (1974) • degree-0 • evidence from clauses: • ‘subordina learnability: Dutch & Emonds te clause children German (1976), strategy’: acquire (acquisition of Ross (1973) Roeper & grammar OV order), • wrt Weissenbo based on Swedish acquisition rn (1990) main clauses (deletion of ha) in • wrt • language • evidence from • historical Germanic: theories of change: Tongan data give Clahsen & change: parameters (ergative case insight into Smolka Andersen are reset system), what (1986), (1973), because English (loss of triggers Wexler Weerman trigger V-to-I, V2) parameter (1994) (1993), experiences settings Roberts change (1993), Keyser & O’Neil (1985) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 92

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

are reset system), what (1986), (1973), because English (loss of triggers Wexler Weerman trigger V-to-I, V2) parameter (1994) (1993), experiences settings • wrt Roberts change Swedish: (1993), a) trigger for a) evidence Platzack Keyser & V2 is from Dutch, (1986) O’Neil sentences German, • wrt (1985) beginning Norwegian, ergative with Swedish, case: arbitrary MidE: Anderson phrase in distribution (1988), Spec,CP of initial Chung (non- subjects & (1978) subject- non-subjects • wrt MidE: initial V2); Kroch & V2 lost Taylor when not (1997) enough initial non- subjects • no theory • random needed to variation explain always present changes in trigger experiences • IP-V2 • evidence from grammar not OE: data learnable leading to IP- V2 analysis not robust & not degree-0 The Loss of V2 in the History of English 93

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

grammar not OE: data learnable leading to IP- V2 analysis not robust & not degree-0 Koopman, • syntax • verb-final • evidence from • Pintzuk Willem F of Old main clauses texts: position (1991), (1995) English allowed in of finite verbs Kemenade OE (1987) a) stylistically a) distribution marked; of verb-final perhaps clauses immature across texts style by same author b) Latin b) frequency of influence verb-final in only in translations direct compared to translations other works Minkova, • Syntax • verse & • evidence from • wrt data: • Campbell Donka; of Old prose share OE verse & Kuhn (1970) Stockwell, English the same prose texts: (1933), Robert syntax, but frequency of Bean (1992) verse has verb-initial (1983), special clauses, Denison conventions inversion after (1986), a) V2 less ambiguous Mitchell strict in verse adverb/subordi (1964) nator, V2 order The Loss of V2 in the History of English 94

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

strict in verse adverb/subordi (1964) nator, V2 order Venneman • Theory • principle of • Behaghel • wrt n, Theo of word natural (1923, reasons for (1974) order serialization: 1932), language change languages Trubetzkoy change: are either (1939), Lehmann head final or Greenberg (1973) head initial; (1966), inconsistenci Bartsch es are due to (1972) change • wrt • XV negation in languages: French: a) disadvanta a) unmarked Regula ge: require SOV & (1966) subject and object- object NPs focused OSV to be clauses have clearly same order of distinguish phrasal ed categories; case marking in Japanese, Quechua, Hindi, b) advantage: Persian sentence b) auxiliaries operators etc. usually don’t after the main interrupt verb in XV the clause and before it in VX lang The Loss of V2 in the History of English 95

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

operators etc. usually don’t after the main interrupt verb in XV the clause and before it • languages in VX lang with S-O • history of morphology English, generally Romance lang; XV; become verb-final VX when subordinates in phonological German, OE; change sentence brace weakens construction in morphology German, OE, (first topic- Early Italian; initial, then coexistence of subject- head-final and initial); if head-initial new patterns in morphology TVX develops, languages; change back development of to XV negation in Fr, Eng, German Clark, • theoreti • change • examples of • wrt nature • wrt nature Brady Z cal always gradual change of change: of change: (2004) syntax involves a in English: Weinreich Lightfoot • languag period of raising of short et al (1968), (1991), e change variation; a, shift from Bender Vincent language OV to VO, loss (2001) (2000), change based of V-to-I Fries & on inherent Pike variation (1949), within Kroch grammars (1989), Yang The Loss of V2 in the History of English 96

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

e change variation; a, shift from Bender Vincent language OV to VO, loss (2001) (2000), change based of V-to-I • wrt Fries & on inherent stochastic Pike variation OT: (1949), within Boersma Kroch grammars (1998), (1989), • theory: • degrees of Boersma & Yang a) LFG: ungrammaticali Hayes (2000), C(onstitue ty, open (2001), Tesar & nt)- lexicon Bresnan et Smolensky structure al (2001) (2000) (trees), • wrt LFG: • wrt OE: F(unctional Kaplan & Pintzuk )-structure Bresnan (1999), (features), (1995), Kroch & & Kaplan Taylor correspond (1995), (2001), ence Bresnan Haeberli relation (2001), (2002), b) OT-LFG: b) example: loss Dalrymple Hulk & potential of V-to-I in (2001), Kemenade structures English Kuhn (1997), for (2001, Kemenade semantic 2003) (1999, informatio • wrt model- 2000), n theoretic Fischer et considered syntax: al (2000) wrt Blackburn constraints; et al (1993), optimal Blackburn candidate & Gardent is chosen (1995), based on Blackburn ranking of & Meyer- constraints Viol (1997), The Loss of V2 in the History of English 97

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

wrt Blackburn • wrt constraints; et al (1993), methodolo optimal Blackburn gy: candidate & Gardent Newmeyer is chosen (1995), (2003) based on Blackburn ranking of & Meyer- constraints Viol (1997), violated Potts (2002) c) Stochastic c) constraints • wrt order OT: may be theory: constraints categorical in Davey & ranked on one language Priestley a & only (1990), continuous preferences Samek- scale, with in another; Lodovici & the ranking stable Prince perturbed variation is (1999) by random possible; • wrt factors; stylistic methodolog constraints variation y: Mohanan associated (2003), with a Boersma range of (2001, values 2004), (may Keller & overlap Asudeh with other (2002) constraints ) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 98

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

constraints ) d) Gradual d) gradualness Learning of language Algorithm: learning compares clause heard to what grammar would produce for assumed underlying form; if mismatche d, constraint values are adjusted slightly • 3 word • evidence from orders in OE OE & MidE & MidE texts subordinate clauses (SOViVf, SVfViO, SVfOVi); all-final most common in OE, all- medial most common in MidE The Loss of V2 in the History of English 99

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

SVfOVi); all-final most common in OE, all- medial most common in MidE a) two a) generates all alignment attested constraints: orders; HEAD- ungrammatic L(eft) & ality of SPINE-R SViOVf (no (aligns ordering of heads & constraints their where it’s projections optimal) to the right); two structural markednes s constraints, *S and *I (to choose between structures headed by S or headed by I) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 100

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

S or headed by I) b) between b) application of OE & GLA to one MidE, OE and one SPINE-R MidE text became lower- ranked & *S became higher- ranked • OE • evidence from pronouns are OE & MidE: non- distribution of projecting subject and appear in inversion, the I2 position of domain; full subjects wrt V2 NP subjects -adjoined can appear negative either in adverbs, Spec,V2 or distribution of Spec,I2 modified a) during pronouns, null MidE, expletives subject pronouns become projecting (due to loss of one of OE’s 2 series of pronouns) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 101

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

pronouns become projecting (due to loss of one of OE’s 2 series of pronouns) & full NP subjects appear more often in Spec,I2 b) 2 structural markednes s constraints, CANON- X’ (c- structure categories must carry a bar level) & *XP (category should not project to a full phrase); 4 hierarchica l constraints, SUBJECT (requires overt subject in The Loss of V2 in the History of English 102

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

phrase); 4 hierarchica l constraints, SUBJECT (requires overt subject in Spec,I2) & dominance constraints *Dom(C,Pr onon- projecting), *Dom(I,Pr onon- projecting), *Dom(V,P ronon- projecting) (determine position of non- projecting pronouns); 2 alignment constraints FINITE-L, NEG-L (prevent elements preceding finite V, neg adverb); The Loss of V2 in the History of English 103

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

FINITE-L, NEG-L (prevent elements preceding finite V, neg adverb); *EXPLETI VE c) CANON- X’ & SUBJECT become higher- ranked; *XP, FINITE-L, *EXPLETI VE become lower- ranked Kiparsky, • languag • OV changed • evidence from • wrt time • wrt Paul e change to VO in directionality course of grammatic (1996) • syntax Germanic of change: alization: of Old languages, complementati Pintzuk Venneman English where verb on in (1991) n (1975), fronting Germanic, Roberts made distribution of (1994) evidence restricted (main opaque clause) and (enabling general verb- cause), fronting, because of change in generalizatio English, The Loss of V2 in the History of English 104

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

fronting Germanic, • wrt Greek: Roberts made distribution of Taylor (1994) evidence restricted (main (1994) • wrt opaque clause) and reanalysis: (enabling general verb- Stockwell cause), fronting, (1977), because of change in Lightfoot generalizatio English, (1979, n of Yiddish, Greek 1991), rightward Kemenade complementa (1987), tion Stockwell (effective & cause) Minkova • OE: (1991) a) Wh- a) position of a) Kemenad • wrt V2 in movement clitics, e’s OE: and availability in (1987) Kemenade topicalizati verb-final analysis (1987), on involve main clauses, not Pintzuk different verb-fronting correct (1991) verb in positions subordinate clauses b) topicalizati b) no b) Pintzuk’s on not to topicalization (1991) Spec,IP; no in analysis such thing subordinate not as I-final clauses; correct structure evidence from Dutch & other OV languages The Loss of V2 in the History of English 105

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

as I-final clauses; correct structure evidence from Dutch & other OV languages c) Spec,CP is c) evidence a focus from OE: position; distribution where C is of preverbal present, constituents must be lexically filled (by compleme ntizer or finite verb); where C filled by compleme ntizer or not present, V moves to I; Spec,IP is subject position, unless no external argument; adjunction possible to both CP and IP The Loss of V2 in the History of English 106

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

external argument; adjunction possible to both CP and IP Kemenade • history • OE & MidE • Evidence from • wrt nature • Pintzuk , Ans van of are CV2 OE, MidE: of V2: Hulk (1991, (1997) English root/non-root & 1993), asymmetry; Kemenade Kroch & embedded (1993) Taylor topicalization (1997) only in subjectless contexts, MidE topics precede negated Vf a) pronouns a) position of are clitics; pronouns cliticizatio n blocked by an operator in first position b) V2 lost in b) evidence late MidE from MidE: due to loss inversion in of Chaucer & discrepanc Wycliffe y between pronominal & nominal subjects The Loss of V2 in the History of English 107

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

of Chaucer & discrepanc Wycliffe y between pronominal & nominal subjects Warner, • languag • change • evidence from • wrt loss of • wrt nature Anthony e change motivated by loss of V-to-I V-to-I: of (1997) • history properties of in Eng Lightfoot parameters of UG (1997), : Zaring & English • V2 began to • evidence from • loss of V2 Roberts Hirschbühl decline in early MidE: may be due (1993), er (1997) early MidE rates of to loss of I- Kroch • wrt V2: inversion, final (1989) Kroch & cliticization of structure • wrt Taylor subject imperative (1997) pronouns inversion: • varying • northern Rule Henry rates of of St. Benet (1995, inversion in may be 1997) MidE may be stylistically • wrt V2: due to marked Kemenade stylistic (1997) factors rather than dialect variation Fontana, • syntax : • modern • OR & • wrt clitics : • wrt Old Josep M clitics Romance Germanic Taylor Spanish: (1997) clitics have clitics may (1990), Rivero different attach Halpern (1997) properties enclitically to (1992) from Old almost any type Romance & of constituent; Germanic Modern clitics; 2P Spanish clitics clitics are must attach to The Loss of V2 in the History of English 108

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(1997) clitics have clitics may (1990), Rivero different attach Halpern (1997) properties enclitically to (1992) from Old almost any type • wrt V2: Romance & of constituent; Diesing Germanic Modern (1990), clitics; 2P Spanish clitics Santorini clitics are must attach to (1989), phrasal verb Rögnvaldss categories, in on & Spec,IP or Thráinsson adjoined to (1990), den IP or to a Besten projection in (1978) Spec,IP • double • Cardinaletti base & Roberts hypothesis: (1991) : Santorini a) accounts (1989), for varying a) evidence Kroch positions from German (1994) of clitics & Dutch • wrt OF as (position of IV2: subject wrt Dupuis clitics), OF (1989) (Tobler- Mussafia effects), OE (position of clitics wrt verb) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 109

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(position of clitics wrt b) obscures verb) similarities b) differing between analyses of Germanic similar facts & OR in OE & OF languages Kroch, A. Language • Constant • Loss of V2 in • Grammatic Weinreich, Bailey (1973) “The change (1989) Change rate French al analysis Labov & from one hypothesis: (Fontaine, that Herzog (1968). grammar to when one 1985): the rate defines the Stein (1986) another is grammatical of loss of contexts of Adams (1987) necessarily option inversion and change is instantaneous replaces null subjects is quite and its causes another with roughly the abstract, as are necessarily which it is same as the synchronic external” (p.2) in loss of V2 syntactic “the process of competition order. studies language across a set • The rate of the suggest. change is not a of linguistic rise of reprise • Quantitativ fact of contexts, the (left e studies grammar but a rate of dislocation) is can fact of replacement, the same as provide language use properly the decline of direct and so must be measured, is V2. evidence studied with the same in as to the tools all of them. causation appropriate to of change. that domain.” (p.3) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 110

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

• The set of of change. that domain.” contexts of (p.3) change is “Variation not defined often reflects by a shared choices that are surface not property categorically (like a determined by particular linguistic word or principles at ), any level but but by instead are syntactic only structure. probalistically • Linguistic influenced by changes features of follow and contaxt and ‘S-shaped’ situation” (p.3) curve. • The loss of V2 in French was due to a change in phrasal accent which forces proposed constituents to move from the topicalizatio n position to the position of left- dislocation. The Loss of V2 in the History of English 111

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

constituents to move from the topicalizatio n position to the position of left- dislocation. • The dislocated NPs don’t count for the V2 constraint, and as sentences of this type increase in frequency, positive evidence for V2 decreases. Pintzuk V2 in • Verb • Presence of • EPP can Iatridou & Verb moves (1993) English seconding in V2 in be satisfied Kroch (1992) to comp in main clauses subordinate through finite main involves clauses verbal Santorini clauses: verb morpholog (1992) Van movement y Kemenade to Infl rather (1987) than to Comp The Loss of V2 in the History of English 112

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

movement y (double base Kemenade to Infl rather hypothesis) (1987) than to Comp Kiparsky • Verb (1990) seconding to Infl applies Verb moves to to clause- subordinate medial clauses (not functional only main head in clauses) subordinate • OE clauses: exhibited Cardinaletti & both Infl- Roberts final and (1991) Infl-medial Haeberli phrase (1991, 1992), structure Haeberli and Haegeman (1992), Higgins (1991), Koopman (1985), Lightfoot (1991) Tomaselli (1990) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 113

Article Context Major claims Major Arguments Implications Authors Authors Comments following against

(1990) The Loss of V2 in the History of English 114

Section 3: Bibliography of V2 in English

Allen, Cynthia. Obsolescence and sudden death in syntax: The decline of verb-final order in early Middle English. Generative Theory and Corpus Studies: A Dialogue from 10 ICEHL , 3-25. 2000. mouton de gruyter.

Anderson, John. On Variability in Old English Syntax, and Some Consequences Thereof. Transactions of the Philological Society 95[1], 9-40. 1997. Malden, MA, Blackwell Publishers.

Anderson, Stephen R. Towards an Optimal Account of Second-Position Phenomena. Optimality Theory: , Syntax and Acquisition , 302-333. 2000. Oxford U Press.

Baekken, Bjorg. Inversion in Early Modern English. English Studies 5, 393-421. 2000.

Bean, Marian C. The Development of Word Order Patterns in Old English. 1983. Totowa, NJ, Barnes & Noble.

Bobaljik, J. Why Phi? paper presented at the Phi Workshop. 2005.

Burnett, H. and Ferch, E. V2 in the History of French: A review of the Literature. in preparation.

Cardinaletti, Anna and Roberts, Ian. Clause Structure and X-Second. 11/1991. Universita de Venezia//University of Wales.

N. Chomsky. 1995. The Minimalist Program. Cambridge, Mass., MIT Press.

Clark, Brady Z. A Stochastic Optimality Theory Approach to Syntactic Change. 07/2004. Stanford University. de Haas, Nynke. Dialect Variation and Language Contact: The Influence of Old Norse on Medieval English. 24/12/2004. University of Groningen.

Fischer, Olga, van Kemenade, Ans, Koopman, Willem, and van der Wurff, Wim. The Verb-Second constraint and its loss. The Syntax of Early English , 104-137. 2000. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Fontana, Josep M. On the integration of second position phenomena. Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change , 207-249. 1997. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Haeberli, Eric. Inflectional Morphology and the Loss of V2 in English. University of Reading. 09/2000.

Harley, H. Abstracting away from abstract case. Proceedings of NELS 25. 1995. University of Massachusetts, Amherst.

Haukenes, Ella. `Pragmatic aspects of word order developments in English and Scandinavian languages. NAES 2004 . 2004. Aarhus.

Henry, Alison. Viewing change in progress: the loss of V2 in Hiberno-English imperatives. Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change . 1997. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Heycock, Caroline and Kroch, Anthony. Verb movement and coordination in a dynamic theory of licensing. The Linguistic Review 11, 257-283. 1994. The Loss of V2 in the History of English 115

Heycock, Caroline and Kroch, Anthony. Verb Movement and the Status of Subjects: Implications for the Theory of Licensing. Groninger Arbeiten zur germanistischen Linguistik 36, 75-102. 1993.

Hulk, Aafke. Residual verb-second and the licensing of functional features. Probus 5, 127-154. 1993.

Hulk, Aafke and van Kemenade, Ans. Verb Second, Pro-drop, Functional Projections and Language Change. Clause Structure and Language Change , 227-256. 1995. New York, Oxford University Press.

Iatridou, Sabine and Kroch, Anthony. The Licensing of CP-recursion and its Relevance to the Germanic Verb-Second Phenomenon. Working papers in Scandinavian syntax 50. 10/1992 . Trondheim, Linguistics Dept., University of Trondheim.

Kiparsky, Paul. Indo-European Origins of Germanic Syntax. Clause Structure and Language Change , 140- 169. 1995. New York, Oxford University Press.

Kiparsky, Paul. The Shift to Head-Initial VP in Germanic. Comparative Germanic Syntax . 1996. Kluwer.

Kohonen, Viljo. Thematic Structure and Transformations. On the Development of English Word Order in Religious Prose around 1000 and 1200 A.D. 138-196. 1978.

Koopman, Hilda. The Doubly filled C filter, the Principle of Projection Activation and Historical Change. 1997. UCLA.

Koopman, Willem F. Inversion after single and multiple topics in Old English. Advances in English , 135-150. 1996. Mouton de Gruyter.

Koopman, Willem F. Old English Clitic Pronouns: Some Remarks. Evidence for Old English , 44-87. 1992. Edinburgh, John Donald Publishers.

Koopman, Willem F. Topicalization in Old English and its effects. Some remarks. Language History and Linguistic Modelling: A Festschrift for Jacek Fisiak on his 60th Birthday , 307-321. 1997. Berlin, Mouton de Gruyter. Hickey, Raymond and Puppel, Stanislaw.

Koopman, Willem F. Verb-Final Main Clauses in Old English Prose. Studia Neophilologica 67, 129-144. 1995.

Kroch, A. Syntactic Change. Notes: Course notes for LSA139. 2005 LSA Institute. Harvard/MIT.

Kroch, Anthony. If at first you don't succeed: the time course of language acquisition and its implications for language change. presentation at LSA . 2003. University of Pennsylvania. Notes: draft

Kroch, Anthony. Morphosyntactic Variation. Papers from the 30th Regional Meeting of the Chicago Linguistics Society: Parasession on Variation and Linguistic Theory . 1994 .

Kroch, Anthony. Reflexes of Grammar in Patterns of Language Change. Language Variation and Change 1, 199-244. 1989.

Kroch, Anthony and Taylor, Ann. Remarks on the XV/VX Alternation in Early Middle English. 03/1994. University of Pennsylvania.

Kroch, Anthony and Taylor, Ann. Verb movement in Old and Middle English. in Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change. van Kemenade & Vincent (eds.). Cambridge University Press. 1997 The Loss of V2 in the History of English 116

Kroch, Anthony and Taylor, Ann. Verb-Object Order in Early Middle English. 2000.

Kroch, Anthony, Taylor, Ann, and Ringe, Donald. The Middle English verb-second constraint: a case study in language contact and language change. 1997. University of Pennsylvania.

Lightfoot, David. Cue-Based Acquisition and Change in Grammars. The Development of Language , 144- 177. 1999. Malden, MA, Blackwell Publishing.

Lightfoot, David. How to Set Parameters: Arguments from Language Change. 1991. Cambridge, MA, The MIT Press. Notes: ch 1, 3

Lightfoot, David. triggers and diachronic reanalyses. Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change , 253-272. 1997. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Lightfoot, David. Why UG Needs a Learning Theory: Triggering Verb Movement. Clause Structure and Language Change , 31-51. 1995. New York, Oxford University Press.

Minkova, Donka and Stockwell, Robert. Poetic Influence on Prose Word Order in Old English. Evidence for Old English , 142-154. 1992. Edinburgh, John Donald Publishers.

Pintzuk, Susan. Phrase Structures in Competition: Variation and Change in Old English Word Order. 1999. New York, Garland Publishing. Notes: ch. 1, 2, 3.1, 6

Pintzuk, S. Verb seconding in Old English: verb movement to Infl. The Linguistic Review 10 (1993), 5-35

Pintzuk, Susan and Taylor, Ann. Objects in Old English: Why and How Early English Is Not Icelandic. York Papers in Linguistics 2[1], 137-150. 2004. Heslington, York.

Platzack, Christer. The Loss of Verb Second in English and French. Clause Structure and Language Change , 200-226. 1995. New York, Oxford University Press.

Prasad, Rashmi. A Quantitative Analysis of the Loss of V2 in the History of English. WCCFL 19 Proceedings , 371-384. 2000. Somerville, MA, Cascadilla Press.

Roberts, Ian. Object Movement and Verb Movement in Early Modern English. Studies in Comparative Germanic Syntax , 269-284. 1995. Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Roberts, Ian. Verbs and diachronic syntax: a comparative history of English and French . 1993. Dordrecht, Kluwer Academic Publishers.

Roberts, Ian and Roussou, Anna. The History of the Future.

Stockwell, Robert P. On the history of the verb-second rule in English. Historical Syntax , 575-592. 1984. The Hague, Mouton. Fisiak, Jacek.

Swan, Toril. A note on Old English and Old Norse initial adverbials and word-order with special reference to sentence adverbials. Language change and language structure , 233-270. 1994. Mouton de Gruyter. Morck, E., Swan, T., and Westivik, O. J. van der Wurff, Wim. Deriving object-verb order in late Middle English. Journal of Linguistics 33, 485-509. 1997. UK, Cambridge University Press. van Kemenade, Ans. Syntactic case and morphological case in the history of English. 1987. Dordrecht, The Loss of V2 in the History of English 117

Foris Publications. van Kemenade, Ans. Syntactic Changes in Late Middle English. Historical Linguistics 1989 , 235-248. 1993. van Kemenade, Ans. V2 and embedded topicalization in Old and Middle English. Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change , 326-352. 1997. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Vennemann, Theo. Topics, Subjects, and Word Order: from SXV to SVX via TVX. Historical linguistics I: Proceedings of the First International conference on Historical Linguistics , 339-376. 1974.

Warner, Anthony. The structure of parametric change, and V-movement in the history of English. Parameters of Morphosyntactic Change , 380-393. 1997. New York, Cambridge University Press.

Weerman, Fred. The V2 Conspiracy: A synchronic and a diachronic analysis of verbal positions in Germanic languages. 1989. Providence RI, Foris Publications. Publications in Language Sciences.

Yang, Charles D. Internal and external forces in language change. Language Variation and Change 12, 231-250. 2000. U.S.A., Cambridge University Press. 2001.