The Art of Recollection and Renaissance Memory

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Art of Recollection and Renaissance Memory Donald Beecher, Grant Williams, eds.. Ars Reminiscendi: Mind and Memory in Renaissance Culture. Publications of the Centre for Reformation and Renaissance Studies. Toronto: Centre for Reformation and Renaissance Studies, 2009. 440 pp. $37.00, paper, ISBN 978-0-7727-2048-1. Reviewed by Asaph Ben-Tov Published on H-German (January, 2010) Commissioned by Susan R. Boettcher Once upon a time, the poet Simonides of Ceos their loved ones their due funerary rites, but the (557-467 BCE) delivered a panegyric at the ban‐ dead had been so badly disfigured that they could quet of a rich nobleman. Following common prac‐ no longer be identified. Simonides was able to re‐ tice, the patron's praise was coupled with the locate each diner to the place where he had been praise of a deity; in this case, the twin gods Castor seated, thus inaugurating the mnemonic tech‐ and Pollux (the Dioscuri). The stingy nobleman in‐ nique of according mnemonic images to mental formed the bard he would pay him only half the places (topoi/loci)--as well as earning a handsome price for the commissioned poem, and that he commission for his mnemonic service. should ask the twins to pay the rest, since he had Renaissance readers knew this starting point awarded them half the commissioned praise. from Cicero's influential treatise De Oratore (55 Such mythological acts of meanness rarely go un‐ BCE). It posits classical mnemonic techniques in punished, and so, soon after, as the banquet was antiquity and later in a rhetorical context. Memo‐ underway, a message was brought to Simonides ry, the fourth part of rhetoric (after invention, dis‐ that two young men were awaiting him outside. position, and elocution), was a basic requirement Castor and Pollux were no longer there when the for ancient orators, who were expected to deliver poet stepped outside, but as he did so the roof speeches by heart. Rhetorical treatises, such as the over the banquet hall collapsed, killing the ungra‐ pseudo-Ciceronian Ad Herennium (first century cious host and his unfortunate guests. The fairy BCE) suggested that the orator locate striking tale account of Simonides's divine rescue might mental images to remind him of the things he was not have been handed down had it not been for meant to memorize (forensic evidence, rhetorical the story's second part, which is the foundation tropes, and so on) within a mental edifice--most myth of the art of memory. The distraught rela‐ commonly a mental picture of an actual building tives of the banquet's victims were eager to award well known to the memorizer. This theme had H-Net Reviews many variations, with some seeing it as a wonder- welcome. Many contributions offer thought-pro‐ working form of artificial memory, and others voking discussions of various test cases--and in more skeptical about the virtues of artificial mem‐ some cases point at fecund stretches of arable ory. The images could either be striking-- land waiting to be ploughed. Conversely, precisely grotesquely humorous, cruel, or otherwise memo‐ because of the pervasiveness of memory (if not rable--or more lean, and the places range from mnemonics) in any culture, the historian, as recommendations for a fairly simple local system Beecher rightly cautions in his introduction, must to the contested claim of Metrodorus of Scepsis (c. draw some distinctions, lest everything becomes 145-170 BCE) to be able to use the zodiac as his memory, and the term lose any heuristic value. elaborate loci system. Most of the contributors heed this caution. Still, A fairly neglected aspect of western intellec‐ the degree to which various Renaissance phenom‐ tual history, the art of memory has benefited from ena shed light on, and are explained by, mnemon‐ the scholarly attention it deserves in recent ic habits is a question that allows for a legitimate decades, most famously in Frances Yates's beauti‐ difference of opinion. While the essays included ful The Art of Memory (1966), which re-estab‐ in this volume are interesting and worth reading lished the art's central position in antique and in their own right, I fnd some more convincing high medieval learned practices and conceptions than others as guides to mnemonic arts and of the human mind, described its moralization in habits. the high Middle Ages as memory was acknowl‐ The frst section, "Revisioning the Classical edged as part of the secondary virtue of prudence, Art of Memory," opens with Brenda Dunn- and, most significantly, traced the occult recon‐ Lardeau's discussion of Jehan du Pré's Palais des struction of the art in Renaissance thought with nobles Dames (1534). An interesting discussion of Giulio Camillo's mnemonic theater and L'idea del du Pré's work in its own right, this article indi‐ theatro (completed in 1544) and Giordano Bruno's cates a mnemonic substratum of the work but more radical occultization of the art, developing does not, to my understanding, offer a considera‐ in tandem with Pierre de la Ramée's (Ramus) "as‐ tion of the art of memory, or the ways in which its cetic" dialectics, evocatively portrayed by Yates as employment in this work relates to the tradition an act of internal iconoclasm. Significant as the or the revision of the art. Andrea Torre, consider‐ occult aspects of Renaissance mnemonics are, as ing the uses of images as referential and meta‐ Yates herself acknowledged, these reveal but a phoric accompaniments of texts, concentrates on fraction of the role and significance of mnemonics three Italian texts of the late ffteenth, late six‐ in Renaissance culture. Though valuable work has teenth, and seventeenth centuries--three been done on the subject since, rewarding work mnemonic treatises of various cultural back‐ on Renaissance memory and mnemonics remains grounds that remind us of the various Renais‐ to be carried out. The present volume, based on a sance social contexts where such skills would be 2006 conference, offers sixteen essays on a broad required, such as among preachers, merchants, spectrum of Renaissance memory, followed by a and orators. Il memoriale, by Emanuele Tesauro detailed overview by Donald Beecher. It aims pri‐ (1592-1675), a text discussed by Torre, offers a marily to offer a consideration of broader aspects case of a distinctly non-classical choice of loci: the of mnemonics in their rhetorical, literary, politi‐ etymologies of the stigmata (plaga) as a meta‐ cal, and psychological contexts in the Renais‐ phoric model of memory. The section ends with sance. Such an attempt at gauging the broader sig‐ Wolfgang Neuber's thought-provoking argument nificance and cultural validity of mnemonic tech‐ on the link between mnemonic imagery and early niques and attitudes is, needless to say, highly modern collective memory. Pivoting on the 2 H-Net Reviews mnemotechnical injunction to forge striking, cru‐ ian experiences, but a later literary construction, el, or comic mnemonic images, Neuber considers merging genuine recollection with classical erudi‐ the construction of New World cannibalism in six‐ tion, and, most importantly, extensive unacknowl‐ teenth-century German woodcuts. Although the edged translations of Descrittione di tutta Italia "savages" not only acquired a cruel, and therefore by Leonardo Alberti (1479-c.1552). For Hoby, mnemonically successful image, these texts also Bartlett suggests, memory was a literary construct posited the Americas as part of real and imagin‐ meant to establish his own uniqueness and au‐ able postlapsarian reality, rather than a mnemon‐ thority. Perhaps, as Bartlett suggests, Hoby's gen‐ ically pallid paradisiacal utopia, so that the imma‐ uine memories and Alberti's work were not clear‐ nence of profane human phenomena, Neuber ar‐ ly distinguished for Hoby or other Renaissance di‐ gues, was being asserted: "Not only the visual im‐ arists. Victoria Burke probes the complex relation‐ ages of mnemonic treatises, but also the structural ship between commonplace books and memory. principles of mnemonics can be held responsible Were commonplace books a superficial collection for the formation of a collective memory" (p. 80). of snippets, avoiding an internalization of moral Whether or not one agrees, this essay offers much precepts? Or were they to some extent mnemonic to think about. props? Considering a broad spectrum of common‐ The second section is dedicated to place usages among different strands of seven‐ manuscripts, commonplace books, and personal teenth-century society, Burke offers a rewarding recollection. James Nelson Novoa discusses and consideration of the complex and ambivalent role offers a transliteration and English translation of and social standing of commonplace books among a short Sephardic art of memory, or possibly a a broad spectrum of seventeenth-century society, sketch of what was meant to be a more elaborate and points to their important gender aspects. manual. The ffteenth-century Jewish mnemonic The third section, "Learning, Rhetoric, and treatise (written in Castilian using Hebrew letters) the Humanist Challenge," opens with Paul Nelles's is not perhaps striking in its own right--interest‐ study of Conrad Gessner's Bibliotheca Universalis ingly, it is devoid of any Jewish (for instance: Kab‐ (1545) and Pandecta (1548) as Renaissance balist) elements, and while demonstrating up-to- Melanchthonian loci communes attempts at di‐ date classical and scholastic learning (itself an in‐ gesting and sorting the vast library of sixteenth- teresting fact) is not original. Novoa, however, of‐ century learning. Unlike the print-manuscript di‐ fers us a rewarding glance both at the decidedly chotomy in much of modern scholarship, describ‐ unheroic trappings of ffteenth-century mnemon‐ ing a transfer from a culture of memory to a cul‐ ic Alltag, and a glimpse at the work, however ture of documentation, Nelles argues through the modest, of a learned member of a Jewish Iberian case of Gessner for a more porous relationship.
Recommended publications
  • Memorization & Practice
    Y O R K U N I V E R S I T Y Department of Theatre MEMORIZATION & PRACTICE THEA 2010 VOICE I Eric Armstrong [email protected] C F T 3 0 6 , 4 7 0 0 K e e l e S t . To r o n t o O N M 3 J 1 P 3 • t e l e p h o n e : 4 1 6 . 7 3 6 - 2 1 0 0 x 7 7 3 5 3 • h t t p : / / w w w. y o r k u . c a / e a r m s t r o Memorization What is Memorization? Committing something to memory is a process that all actors working outside of Improvisation need. Memory is a complicated process whereby images, sounds, ideas, words, phrases, and even times and places are encoded, so we can recall them later. To ef- fectively learn “lines,” one may use several different kinds of memory at different times in the process of encoding, storing and recalling the text at hand. The basic learning of a single line begins with you using your working memory to get the line off the page and into your head. At this stage of the process, you can repeat the line while looking at an acting partner, or moving around the space, but it won’t last more than a few seconds. The next phase is when the text gets encoded into the short-term memory. These kinds of memories will stay in your mind for a few minutes at most.
    [Show full text]
  • The Integrated Nature of Metamemory and Memory
    The Integrated Nature of Metamemory and Memory John Dunlosky and Robert A. Bjork Introduction Memory has been of interest to scholars and laypeople alike for over 2,000 years. In a rather gruesome example from antiquity, Cicero tells the story of Simonides (557– 468 BC), who discovered the method of loci, which is a powerful mental mnemonic for enhancing one’s memory. Simonides was at a banquet of a nobleman, Scopas. To honor him, Simonides sang a poem, but to Scopas’s chagrin, the poem also honored two young men, Castor and Pollux. Being upset, Scopas told Simonides that he was to receive only half his wage. Simonides was later called from the banquet, and legend has it that the banquet room collapsed, and all those inside were crushed. To help bereaved families identify the victims, Simonides reportedly was able to name every- one according to the place where they sat at the table, which gave him the idea that order brings strength to our memories and that to employ this ability people “should choose localities, then form mental images of things they wanted to store in their memory, and place these in the localities” (Cicero, 2001). Tis example highlights an early discovery that has had important applied impli- cations for improving the functioning of memory (see, e.g., Yates, 1997). Memory theory was soon to follow. Aristotle (385–322 BC) claimed that memory arises from three processes: Events are associated (1) through their relative similarity or (2) rela- tive dissimilarity and (3) when they co-occur together in space and time.
    [Show full text]
  • Mnemonics in a Mnutshell: 32 Aids to Psychiatric Diagnosis
    Mnemonics in a mnutshell: 32 aids to psychiatric diagnosis Clever, irreverent, or amusing, a mnemonic you remember is a lifelong learning tool ® Dowden Health Media rom SIG: E CAPS to CAGE and WWHHHHIMPS, mnemonics help practitioners and trainees recall Fimportant lists (suchCopyright as criteriaFor for depression,personal use only screening questions for alcoholism, or life-threatening causes of delirium, respectively). Mnemonics’ effi cacy rests on the principle that grouped information is easi- er to remember than individual points of data. Not everyone loves mnemonics, but recollecting diagnostic criteria is useful in clinical practice and research, on board examinations, and for insurance reimbursement. Thus, tools that assist in recalling di- agnostic criteria have a role in psychiatric practice and IMAGES teaching. JUPITER In this article, we present 32 mnemonics to help cli- © nicians diagnose: • affective disorders (Box 1, page 28)1,2 Jason P. Caplan, MD Assistant clinical professor of psychiatry • anxiety disorders (Box 2, page 29)3-6 Creighton University School of Medicine 7,8 • medication adverse effects (Box 3, page 29) Omaha, NE • personality disorders (Box 4, page 30)9-11 Chief of psychiatry • addiction disorders (Box 5, page 32)12,13 St. Joseph’s Hospital and Medical Center Phoenix, AZ • causes of delirium (Box 6, page 32).14 We also discuss how mnemonics improve one’s Theodore A. Stern, MD Professor of psychiatry memory, based on the principles of learning theory. Harvard Medical School Chief, psychiatric consultation service Massachusetts General Hospital How mnemonics work Boston, MA A mnemonic—from the Greek word “mnemonikos” (“of memory”)—links new data with previously learned information.
    [Show full text]
  • The Spaced Interval Repetition Technique
    The Spaced Interval Repetition Technique What is it? The Spaced Interval Repetition (SIR) technique is a memorization technique largely developed in the 1960’s and is a phenomenal way of learning information very efficiently that almost nobody knows about. It is based on the landmark research in memory conducted by famous psychologist, Hermann Ebbinghaus in the late 19th century. Ebbinghaus discovered that when we learn new information, we actually forget it very quickly (within a matter of minutes and hours) and the more time that passes, the more likely we are to forget it (unless it is presented to us again). This is why “cramming” for the test does not allow you to learn/remember everything for the test (and why you don’t remember any of it a week later). SIR software, notably first developed by Piotr A. Woźniak is available to help you learn information like a superhero and even retain it for the rest of your life. The SIR technique can be applied to any kind of learning, but arguably works best when learning discrete pieces of information like dates, definitions, vocabulary, formulas, etc. Why does it work? Memory is a fickle creature. For most people, to really learn something, we need to rehearse it several times; this is how information gets from short‐term memory to long‐ term memory. Research tells us that the best time to rehearse something (like those formulas for your statistics class) is right before you are about to forget it. Obviously, it is very difficult for us to know when we are about to forget an important piece of information.
    [Show full text]
  • 20 Memory Techniques
    20 Memory Techniques Experiment 1,viththese techniques to develop a.flexible, custom-made memm}" system that fits your stJ:fe qf learning the content <ifyour courses and the skills <~/'your,sport. 17w 20 techniques are divided intnfour categories, each 1-vhichrepresents a general principlefiJr improving mem.ory. Organize it 1. Be selective. To a large degree, the art of memory is the a,t of selecting what to remember in the first place. As you dig into your textbooks, playbooks. and notes, make choices about what is most important to learn. Imagine that you are going to create a test on the material and consider the questions you would ask. When reading, look for chapter previews, summaries, and review questions. Pay attention to anything printed in bold type. A !so notice visual elements, such as charts, graphs, and illustrations. All of these are clues pointing to what's important. During lectures, notice what the instructor emphasizes. During practice, focus on what your coach requires you to repeat. Anything that presented visually-on the board, on overheads, or with slides-is also key. 2. Make it meaningful. One way to create meaning is to learn from the general to the specific. Before tackling the details, get the big picture. Before you begin your next reading assignment, for example, skim it lo locate the main idea. If you're ever lost, step back and recall that idea. The details might make more sense. You can also organize any list of items-even random ones-in a meaningful way to make them easier to remember.
    [Show full text]
  • Memorization Techniques
    MEMORIZATION Memorization BasicsTECHNIQUES Putting information in long-term memory takes TIME and ORGANIZATION. This helps us consolidate information so it remains connected in our brains. Information is best learned when it is meaningful, authentic, engaging, and humorous (we learn 30% more when we tie humor to memory!). Rehearsing or reciting information is the key to retaining it long-term. Types of Memory There are three different types of memory: 1. Long-term: Permanent storehouse of information and can retain information indefinitely 2. Short-term: Working memory and can retain information for 20 to 30 seconds 3. Immediate: “In and out memory” which does not retain information If you want to do well on exams, you need to focus on putting your information into long-term memory. Why is Memorization Necessary? Individuals who read a chapter textbook typically forget: 46% after one day 79% after 14 days 81% after 28 days Sometimes, what we “remember” are invented memories. Events are reported that never took place; casual remarks are embellished, and major points are disregarded or downplayed. This is why it is important to take the time to improve your long-term memory. Memorization Tips 1. Put in the effort and time to improve your memorizing techniques. 2. Pay attention and remain focused on the material. 3. Get it right the first time; otherwise, your brain has a hard time getting it right after you correct yourself. 4. Chunk or block information into smaller, more manageable parts. Read a couple of pages at a time; take notes on key ideas, and review and recite the information.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning Efficiency Correlates of Using Supermemo with Specially Crafted Flashcards in Medical Scholarship
    Learning efficiency correlates of using SuperMemo with specially crafted Flashcards in medical scholarship. Authors: Jacopo Michettoni, Alexis Pujo, Daniel Nadolny, Raj Thimmiah. Abstract Computer-assisted learning has been growing in popularity in higher education and in the research literature. A subset of these novel approaches to learning claim that predictive algorithms called Spaced Repetition can significantly improve retention rates of studied knowledge while minimizing the time investment required for learning. SuperMemo is a brand of commercial software and the editor of the SuperMemo spaced repetition algorithm. Medical scholarship is well known for requiring students to acquire large amounts of information in a short span of time. Anatomy, in particular, relies heavily on rote memorization. Using the SuperMemo web platform1 we are creating a non-randomized trial, inviting medical students completing an anatomy course to take part. Usage of SuperMemo as well as a performance test will be measured and compared with a concurrent control group who will not be provided with the SuperMemo Software. Hypotheses A) Increased average grade for memorization-intensive examinations If spaced repetition positively affects average retrievability and stability of memory over the term of one to four months, then consistent2 users should obtain better grades than their peers on memorization-intensive examination material. B) Grades increase with consistency There is a negative relationship between variability of daily usage of SRS and grades. 1 https://www.supermemo.com/ 2 Defined in Criteria for inclusion: SuperMemo group. C) Increased stability of memory in the long-term If spaced repetition positively affects knowledge stability, consistent users should have more durable recall even after reviews of learned material have ceased.
    [Show full text]
  • Hippocampal–Caudate Nucleus Interactions Support Exceptional Memory Performance
    Brain Struct Funct DOI 10.1007/s00429-017-1556-2 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Hippocampal–caudate nucleus interactions support exceptional memory performance Nils C. J. Müller1 · Boris N. Konrad1,2 · Nils Kohn1 · Monica Muñoz-López3 · Michael Czisch2 · Guillén Fernández1 · Martin Dresler1,2 Received: 1 December 2016 / Accepted: 24 October 2017 © The Author(s) 2017. This article is an open access publication Abstract Participants of the annual World Memory competitive interaction between hippocampus and caudate Championships regularly demonstrate extraordinary mem- nucleus is often observed in normal memory function, our ory feats, such as memorising the order of 52 playing cards findings suggest that a hippocampal–caudate nucleus in 20 s or 1000 binary digits in 5 min. On a cognitive level, cooperation may enable exceptional memory performance. memory athletes use well-known mnemonic strategies, We speculate that this cooperation reflects an integration of such as the method of loci. However, whether these feats the two memory systems at issue-enabling optimal com- are enabled solely through the use of mnemonic strategies bination of stimulus-response learning and map-based or whether they benefit additionally from optimised neural learning when using mnemonic strategies as for example circuits is still not fully clarified. Investigating 23 leading the method of loci. memory athletes, we found volumes of their right hip- pocampus and caudate nucleus were stronger correlated Keywords Memory athletes · Method of loci · Stimulus with each other compared to matched controls; both these response learning · Cognitive map · Hippocampus · volumes positively correlated with their position in the Caudate nucleus memory sports world ranking. Furthermore, we observed larger volumes of the right anterior hippocampus in ath- letes.
    [Show full text]
  • The Art of Loci: Immerse to Memorize
    The Art of Loci: Immerse to Memorize Anne Vetter, Daniel Gotz,¨ Sebastian von Mammen Games Engineering, University of Wurzburg,¨ Germany fanne.vetter,[email protected], [email protected] Abstract—There are uncountable things to be remembered, but assistance during comprehension (R6) of the information, e.g. most people were never shown how to memorize effectively. With by means of verbalization, should follow suit. Due to the this paper, the application The Art of Loci (TAoL) is presented, heterogeneity of learners, the applied process and methods to provide the means for efficient and fun learning. In a virtual reality (VR) environment users can express their study material should be individualized (R7) and strike a productive balance visually, either creating dimensional mind maps or experimenting between user’s choices and imposed learning activities (locus with mnemonic strategies, such as mind palaces. We also present of control) (R8). For example, users should be able to control common memorization techniques in light of their underlying the pace of learning. Learning environments should arouse the pedagogical foundations and discuss the respective features of intrinsic motivation of the user (R9). Fostering an awareness TAoL in comparison with similar software applications. of the learner’s cognition and thus reflection on their learning I. INTRODUCTION process and strategies is also beneficial (metacognition) (R10). Further, a constructivist learning approach can be realized by Before writing was common, information could only be the means to create artefacts such as representations, symbols preserved by memorization. Major ancient literature like The or cues (R11). Finally, cooperative and collaborative learning Iliad and The Odyssey were memorized, passed on through environments can benefit the learning process (R12).
    [Show full text]
  • Memory-Modulation: Self-Improvement Or Self-Depletion?
    HYPOTHESIS AND THEORY published: 05 April 2018 doi: 10.3389/fpsyg.2018.00469 Memory-Modulation: Self-Improvement or Self-Depletion? Andrea Lavazza* Neuroethics, Centro Universitario Internazionale, Arezzo, Italy Autobiographical memory is fundamental to the process of self-construction. Therefore, the possibility of modifying autobiographical memories, in particular with memory-modulation and memory-erasing, is a very important topic both from the theoretical and from the practical point of view. The aim of this paper is to illustrate the state of the art of some of the most promising areas of memory-modulation and memory-erasing, considering how they can affect the self and the overall balance of the “self and autobiographical memory” system. Indeed, different conceptualizations of the self and of personal identity in relation to autobiographical memory are what makes memory-modulation and memory-erasing more or less desirable. Because of the current limitations (both practical and ethical) to interventions on memory, I can Edited by: only sketch some hypotheses. However, it can be argued that the choice to mitigate Rossella Guerini, painful memories (or edit memories for other reasons) is somehow problematic, from an Università degli Studi Roma Tre, Italy ethical point of view, according to some of the theories of the self and personal identity Reviewed by: in relation to autobiographical memory, in particular for the so-called narrative theories Tillmann Vierkant, University of Edinburgh, of personal identity, chosen here as the main case of study. Other conceptualizations of United Kingdom the “self and autobiographical memory” system, namely the constructivist theories, do Antonella Marchetti, Università Cattolica del Sacro Cuore, not have this sort of critical concerns.
    [Show full text]
  • Elaborative Encoding, the Ancient Art of Memory, and the Hippocampus
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2013) 36, 589–659 provided by RERO DOC Digital Library doi:10.1017/S0140525X12003135 Such stuff as dreams are made on? Elaborative encoding, the ancient art of memory, and the hippocampus Sue Llewellyn Faculty of Humanities, University of Manchester, Manchester M15 6PB, United Kingdom http://www.humanities.manchester.ac.uk [email protected] Abstract: This article argues that rapid eye movement (REM) dreaming is elaborative encoding for episodic memories. Elaborative encoding in REM can, at least partially, be understood through ancient art of memory (AAOM) principles: visualization, bizarre association, organization, narration, embodiment, and location. These principles render recent memories more distinctive through novel and meaningful association with emotionally salient, remote memories. The AAOM optimizes memory performance, suggesting that its principles may predict aspects of how episodic memory is configured in the brain. Integration and segregation are fundamental organizing principles in the cerebral cortex. Episodic memory networks interconnect profusely within the cortex, creating omnidirectional “landmark” junctions. Memories may be integrated at junctions but segregated along connecting network paths that meet at junctions. Episodic junctions may be instantiated during non–rapid eye movement (NREM) sleep after hippocampal associational function during REM dreams. Hippocampal association involves relating, binding, and integrating episodic memories into a mnemonic compositional whole. This often bizarre, composite image has not been present to the senses; it is not “real” because it hyperassociates several memories. During REM sleep, on the phenomenological level, this composite image is experienced as a dream scene.
    [Show full text]
  • CHAPTER 6: MEMORY STRATEGIES: MNEMONIC DEVICES in Order to Remember Information, You First Have to Find It Somewhere in Your Memory
    CHAPTER 6: MEMORY STRATEGIES: MNEMONIC DEVICES In order to remember information, you first have to find it somewhere in your memory. To be successful in college you need to use active learning strategies that help you store information and retrieve it. Mnemonic devices can help you do that. Mnemonics are techniques for remembering information that is otherwise difficult to recall. The idea behind using mnemonics is to encode complex information in a way that is much easier to remember. Two common types of mnemonic devices are acronyms and acrostics. Acronyms Acronyms are words made up of the first letters of other words. As a mnemonic device, acronyms help you remember the first letters of items in a list, which in turn helps you remember the list itself. Instructor OLC to accompany 100% Online Student Success 1 Examples The following are examples of popular mnemonic acronyms: HOMES Huron, Ontario, Michigan, Erie, Superior Names of the Great Lakes FACE The letters of the treble clef notes in the spaces from bottom to top spells “FACE”. ROY G. BIV Red, Orange, Yellow, Green, Blue, Indigo, Colors of the spectrum Violet MRS GREN Movement, Respiration, Sensitivity, Growth, Common attributes of living Reproduction, Excretion, Nutrition things Create Your Own Acronym Now think of a few words you need to remember. This could be related to your studies, your work, or just a subject of interest. Five steps to creating acronyms are*: 1. List the information you need to learn in meaningful phrases. 2. Circle or underline a keyword in each phrase. 3. Write down the first letter of each keyword.
    [Show full text]