Pdf/22/10/2289/1770340/Jocn.2009.21320.Pdf by Guest on 18 May 2021

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Pdf/22/10/2289/1770340/Jocn.2009.21320.Pdf by Guest on 18 May 2021 Characterizing the Spatio-temporal Dynamics of the Neural Events Occurring prior to and up to Overt Recognition of Famous Faces Boutheina Jemel1, Anne-Marie Schuller2, and Valérie Goffaux3 Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/22/10/2289/1770340/jocn.2009.21320.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract ■ Although it is generally acknowledged that familiar face recog- of familiar faces. Although the N170 and the N250 face-sensitive nition is fast, mandatory, and proceeds outside conscious control, responses displayed an abrupt activity change at the moment of it is still unclear whether processes leading to familiar face recog- overt recognition of famous faces, later ERPs encompassing the nition occur in a linear (i.e., gradual) or a nonlinear (i.e., all-or- N400 and late positive component exhibited an incremental in- none) manner. To test these two alternative accounts, we recorded crease in amplitude as the point of recognition approached. In scalp ERPs while participants indicated whether they recognize addition, famous faces that were not overtly recognized at one as familiar the faces of famous and unfamiliar persons gradually trial before recognition elicited larger ERP potentials than unfamil- revealed in a descending sequence of frames, from the noisier to iar faces, probably reflecting a covert recognition process. Overall, the least noisy. This presentation procedure allowed us to charac- these findings present evidence that recognition of familiar faces terize the changes in scalp ERP responses occurring prior to and implicates spatio-temporally complex neural processes exhibit- up to overt recognition. Our main finding is that gradual and all- ing differential pattern activity changes as a function of recogni- or-none processes are possibly involved during overt recognition tion state. ■ INTRODUCTION The functional model put forward by Bruce and Young Many of our everyday behaviors, such as recognizing (1986) has promoted the idea that face familiarity takes acquaintances in the street, reading a book, appropriately place at the level of face-specific representation, referred to manipulating objects, and so forth, appear to be discon- as face recognition units (FRUs) that store structural per- certingly effortless and automatic. For instance, we cannot ceptual information of familiar faces. When the visual input look at a face and decide whether or not we recognize it; from a perceived face matches its stored representation, no matter how hard we try. Face recognition is fast, man- the corresponding FRU is activated, therefore generating datory, and proceeds outside conscious control (Bruce & a feeling of familiarity. FRUs are viewed as transmitting a Young, 1986). This phenomenon and a number of other graded signal that is proportional to the degree of resem- observations have been taken as evidence that face recog- blance between stored representation and the current nition may be achieved in an all-or-none fashion. Paradoxi- percept (Bruce & Young, 1986). FRU activation, in turn, in- cally, other everyday situations like the types of slips and stigates the access to all sorts of identity-specific and seman- errors in recognizing faces and several empirical findings tic information (person–identity nodes, PINs) about the from laboratory experiments (Hay, Young, & Ellis, 1991; face, and finally, retrieval of his or her name. Moreover, in Young, Hay, & Ellis, 1985) strongly suggest that several Burton et al.ʼs (1990) model, face recognition is supported states of face recognition are possible and that the under- by thresholded devices located at PINs, the activation of lying mechanisms would operate in a continuous and which rapidly triggers retrieval of associated person-specific graded manner. These two views have been conceptualized information. Accordingly, these PINs operate in an “all-or- in theoretical frameworks of face recognition (Burton, none” (i.e., binary) fashion (Burton et al., 1990); if their Bruce, & Johnston, 1990; Bruce & Young, 1986) but there signal strength exceeds a set threshold criterion, then a is as yet no clear-cut evidence for an all-or-none or a gradual recognition response is given, and if it is below threshold, account of the process underlying familiar face recognition. the face is judged as unfamiliar. Based on these two models, face recognition entails two memory processes that might be related but are, by no 1Université de Montréal, Canada, 2Public Research Center for means, synonymous. The first computes similarity of probe Health, Department of Public Health, Luxemburg, 3University faces to stored traces in long-term memory and generally of Maastricht, The Netherlands refers to a diffuse and undifferentiated feeling of familiarity, © 2009 Massachusetts Institute of Technology Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience 22:10, pp. 2289–2305 Downloaded from http://www.mitpressjournals.org/doi/pdf/10.1162/jocn.2009.21320 by guest on 02 October 2021 that is, a general, context-free sense of knowing that a given over lateral occipito-temporal recording sites, known as face has been encountered at some time in the past (i.e., the N170 face-sensitive component (Bentin, Allison, Puce, Bruce & Young, 1986). The second generally refers to Perez, & McCarthy, 1996). The demonstration that the explicit forms of memory retrieval of specific person iden- N170 is highly sensitive to stimulus manipulations that tity information (Burton et al., 1990), that is, a context- impede perceptual face processing ( Jemel, Schuller, dependent process associated with the experience of not et al., 2003; Rossion et al., 2000; George, Evans, Fiori, only knowing that a face is familiar but also remembering Davidoff, & Renault, 1996) and is not sensitive to memory- some relevant information that allow identification of that related and familiarity-related factors (Tanaka et al., 2006; face. Accordingly, one can argue that face familiarity is a Schweinberger et al., 2002; Bentin & Deouell, 2000; Eimer, memory process that supports recognition without being 2000) provided support for the claim that this ERP com- Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/22/10/2289/1770340/jocn.2009.21320.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 necessarily accompanied by conscious retrieval of detailed ponent reflects a fairly early stage of the visual analysis of memories (Mandler, 1980). Evidence for this argument faces. Other lines of research, however, showed reliable mainly comes from neuropsychological studies on proso- effects of familiarity, repetition priming, and recogni- pagnosia, a face recognition impairment caused by selective tion on N170 responses to faces ( Jemel, Pisani, Rousselle, damage in ventral occipito-temporal cortex and anterior Crommelinck, & Bruyer, 2005; Tanskanen, Näsänen, temporal regions (Damasio, Tranel, & Damasio, 1990). De- Montez, Päällysaho, & Hari, 2005; Jemel, Pisani, Calabria, spite the failure in overt or conscious recognition of famil- Crommelinck, & Bruyer, 2003; Caharel et al., 2002), sug- iar faces, residual effects of facial familiarity can still be gesting that as early as 170 msec after stimulus onset, the demonstrated in some prosopagnosic patients through brain is individuating previously encoded faces. a variety of indirect measures that do not depend on ex- Nonetheless, there is now an increasing number of plicit recognition (Young, 1994), such as implicit learn- studies showing that the earliest time epoch at which ingoftrueanduntrueface–name associations (Bruyer familiar and unfamiliar face ERPs differ occurs circa 200– et al., 1983), semantic priming (Barton, Cherkasova, & 300 msec after stimulus onset, at the level of an occipito- Hefter, 2004), eye movement scan paths (Rizzo, Hurtig, & temporal negative potential known as the N250 (Begleiter, Damasio, 1987), as well as electrophysiological (Renault, Porjesz, & Wang, 1995); N250 familiarity effect typically Signoret, Debruille, Breton, & Bolgert, 1989) and skin- takes the form of enhanced negative potentials in response conductance response measures [SCR] (Tranel & Damasio, to familiar than to unfamiliar faces (Tanaka et al., 2006; 1985). Moreover, the reverse dissociation, that is, explicit Schweinberger, Pfütze, & Sommer, 1995). This result has recognition without familiarity, has been supported by led some authors to suggest that this component reflects some evidence in patients with ventromedial frontal dam- the activation of preexistent internal representation within age (see Tranel, Damasio, & Damasio, 1995) and in pa- the face recognition system (Bruce & Young, 1986). How- tients with delusional misidentification syndromes such as ever, studies investigating repetition effects within the time Capgras delusion (Ellis, Young, Quayle, & DePauw, 1997; range of the N250 (i.e., N250r for repetition) have cast Hirstein & Ramachandran, 1997). Tranel et al. (1995) re- some doubt on this interpretation. Most notably, larger ported the case of some patients with ventromedial frontal N250r for repeated as compared to nonrepeated faces have damage who failed to show discriminatory autonomic been reported for familiar and unfamiliar faces (Begleiter responses to familiar and unfamiliar faces that were other- et al., 1995; Schweinberger et al., 1995), although sometimes wise consciously recognized. Taken collectively, these sets with reduced amplitudes for unfamiliar faces (Herzmann, of findings suggest that the activation of face representation Schweinberger, Sommer, & Jentzsch, 2004). These find- is necessary
Recommended publications
  • P3b Amplitudes Differences in Ultra-Rapid Visual Categorization Task of Food and Non-Food Items
    South Florida Journal of Development, Miami, v.2, n.1, p. 440-457, jan./ mar. 2021. ISSN 2675-5459 P3b amplitudes differences in ultra-rapid visual categorization task of food and non-food items DOI: 10.46932/sfjdv2n1-033 Received in: November 1st, 2020 Accepted in: December 30th, 2020 Tawhida Jahan Chairperson & Assistant professor, Department of Communication Disorders, University of Dhaka, Bangladesh Cognitive Science, TU-Kaiserslautern, Germany E-mail: [email protected] ABSTRACT P300, especially P3b, the third positive wave of EEG signal with near 350ms associated with occipito- patieto-temporal region of the brain, is mainly responsible for categorization of different objects. So, this study investigates the nature of amplitude and reaction time difference in ‘food’ and ‘no-food’ objects categorization task. Object categorization processes were investigated by measuring event-related potentials while participants were categorizing different ‘food’ and ‘no-food’ items. The EEG study of this experiment found no P3b amplitude differences for ‘food’ and ‘no-food’ category in the ultra-rapid categorization task. On the other hand, from the behavioral study we observed no significant difference in both reaction time (RT) and error rate (ER) in the above task. The result of this study is consistent with some previous experiments. For example, regarding the reaction, the findings can be compared with VanRullen & Thrope (2001) who also found no significant longer reaction time for ‘means of transport’ item in comparison with ‘animal’. The result of this study can also be interpreted from the perspective of ‘coarse-to-fine account’ hypothesis (Prass et al. 2013) which indicated that to recognize objects belong to basic level category one needs detail information with sufficient time.
    [Show full text]
  • The Figure Is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in The
    The Figure is in the Brain of the Beholder: Neural Correlates of Individual Percepts in the Bistable Face-Vase Image A Thesis Presented to The Division of Philosophy, Religion, Psychology, and Linguistics Reed College In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Bachelor of Arts Phoebe Bauer May 2015 Approved for the Division (Psychology) Michael Pitts Acknowledgments I think some people experience a degree of unease when being taken care of, so they only let certain people do it, or they feel guilty when it happens. I don’t really have that. I love being taken care of. Here is a list of people who need to be explicitly thanked because they have done it so frequently and are so good at it: Chris: thank you for being my support system across so many contexts, for spinning with me, for constantly reminding me what I’m capable of both in and out of the lab. Thank you for validating and often mirroring my emotions, and for never leaving a conflict unresolved. Rennie: thank you for being totally different from me and yet somehow understanding the depths of my opinions and thought experiments. Thank you for being able to talk about magic. Thank you for being my biggest ego boost and accepting when I internalize it. Ben: thank you for taking the most important classes with me so that I could get even more out of them by sharing. Thank you for keeping track of priorities (quality dining: yes, emotional explanations: yes, fretting about appearances: nu-uh). #AshHatchtag & Stella & Master Tran: thank you for being a ceaseless source of cheer and laughter and color and love this year.
    [Show full text]
  • Event-Related Brain Potentials During Natural Sentence Processing
    BRAIN AND LANGUAGE 11, 354-373 (1980) Reading Between the Lines: Event-related Brain Potentials during Natural Sentence Processing MARTA KUTAS AND STEVEN A. HILLYARD University of California, San Diego Event-related potentials (ERPs) were recorded from subjects as they silently read 160 different seven-word sentences, presented one word at a time. Each sentence was in itself a natural, meaningful phrase. Substantial intersubject var- iability was observed in the ERP waveshapes following the words. More than half of the subjects generated P3 components to word stimuli, but those who did showed similar responses to repeated control presentations of a single word. In addition, it was found that all but the first word in the sentence elicited an ERP with a significant left-greater-than-right asymmetry in the late positivity over temporo-parietal regions. The ERPs to the last words were associated with more late positivity than those to the preceding words. One quarter of the sentences, at random, ended with a word printed in a typeface that was different from that of the preceding words. This infrequent change in typeface elicited a complex ERP having three consistent late positive components. INTRODUCTION Reading is fundamental for the acquisition of knowledge in literate societies and a stalwart member of the three R’s Yet, we know astonish- ingly little about the neurophysiological mechanisms which enable us to read a sentence and comprehend its meaning. The only currently feasible approach to understanding normal human neurophysiology involves the recording of event-related potentials (ERPs) from the scalp. While this technique has the advantage of being nonintrusive, it provides a rather remote index of the complex neuronal activity patterns which underly human language processes.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissociating Activation and Integration of Discourse Referents
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/671933; this version posted June 14, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Dissociating activation and integration of discourse referents: evidence from ERPs and oscillations Cas W. Coopmans1,2 & Mante S. Nieuwland1,3 1Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands 2Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands 3Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, The Netherlands Contact details: Cas Coopmans Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics Wundtlaan 1, 6500 AH Nijmegen, The Netherlands Tel.: +31-24 3521331 Email address: [email protected] Author note: In accordance with the Peer Review Openness Initiative (Morey et al., 2016), all materials, data and analysis scripts are publicly available on the Open Science Framework (anonymized, view-only link for reviewers: https://osf.io/nbjfm/). 1 bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/671933; this version posted June 14, 2019. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder, who has granted bioRxiv a license to display the preprint in perpetuity. It is made available under aCC-BY-NC-ND 4.0 International license. Abstract A key challenge in understanding stories and conversations is the comprehension of ‘anaphora’, words that refer back to previously mentioned words or concepts (‘antecedents’). In psycholinguistic theories, anaphor comprehension involves the initial activation of the antecedent and its subsequent integration into the unfolding representation of the narrated event.
    [Show full text]
  • Event-Related Brain Potentials and Case Information in Syntactic Ambiguities
    Event-Related Brain Potentials and Case Information in Syntactic Ambiguities Jens-Max Hopf, Josef Bayer, Markus Bader, and Michael Meng Friedrich-Schiller-Universität Jena, Germany Downloaded from http://mitprc.silverchair.com/jocn/article-pdf/10/2/264/1758308/089892998562690.pdf by guest on 18 May 2021 Abstract ■ In an ERP study, German sentences were investigated that neuropsychological correlates. The results of this study support contain a case-ambiguous NP that may be assigned accusative a parser design according to which the so-called structural case or dative case. Sentences were disambiguated by the verb in (nominative or accusative) is assigned without any delay in the ªnal position of the sentence. As our data show, sentences absence of morpho-lexical counterevidence. It is argued that ending in a verb that assigns dative case to the ambiguous NP the enhancement of a negative ERP component with a “classi- elicit a clear garden-path effect. The garden-path effect was cal” N400 topography reºects the difªculty of reanalysis due indicated by a broad centro-posterior negative shift that oc- to reaccessing morpho-lexical information that lies outside the curred between 300 and 900 msec after the dative-assigning domain of the parsing module. Consequently, ERP responses to verb was presented. No enhanced P600 following the misana- garden-path effects are not conªned to a late positivity but vary lysis was observed. Noun phrases whose case ambiguity was depending on the level of processing involved in reanalysis. resolved in favor of accusative case and unambiguously dative- The fact that garden-path effects may also elicit an N400 can marked NPs did not trigger signiªcant ERP differences.
    [Show full text]
  • Quantitative EEG in Disorders of Consciousness
    brain sciences Review Narrative Review: Quantitative EEG in Disorders of Consciousness Betty Wutzl 1,2 , Stefan M. Golaszewski 3,4,5,†, Kenji Leibnitz 1,6 , Patrick B. Langthaler 3,7,8, Alexander B. Kunz 3,4, Stefan Leis 3,9 , Kerstin Schwenker 3,4,5,9, Aljoscha Thomschewski 3,5,9 , Jürgen Bergmann 3,5 and Eugen Trinka 3,4,5,9,* 1 Graduate School of Information Science and Technology, Osaka University, Suita 565-0871, Japan; [email protected] (B.W.); [email protected] (K.L.) 2 Symbiotic Intelligent Systems Research Center, Osaka University, Suita 565-0871, Japan 3 Department of Neurology, Christian Doppler Medical Center, and Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Paracelsus Medical University, Affiliated Member of the European Reference Network EpiCARE, 5020 Salzburg, Austria; [email protected] (S.M.G.); [email protected] (P.B.L.); [email protected] (A.B.K.); [email protected] (S.L.); [email protected] (K.S.); [email protected] (A.T.); [email protected] (J.B.) 4 Karl Landsteiner Institute for Neurorehabilitation and Space Neurology, 5020 Salzburg, Austria 5 Neuroscience Institute, Christian Doppler Medical Center, and Centre for Cognitive Neuroscience, Paracelsus Medical University, 5020 Salzburg, Austria 6 Center for Information and Neural Networks, National Institute of Information and Communications Technology, Suita 565-0871, Japan 7 Department of Mathematics, Paris Lodron University of Salzburg, 5020 Salzburg, Austria 8 Team Biostatistics and Big Medical Data, IDA Lab Salzburg, Paracelsus Medical University, 5020 Salzburg, Austria 9 Spinal Cord Injury and Tissue Regeneration Center Salzburg, Paracelsus Medical University, Citation: Wutzl, B.; Golaszewski, 5020 Salzburg, Austria S.M.; Leibnitz, K.; Langthaler, P.B.; * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +43-5-7255-34600 Kunz, A.B.; Leis, S.; Schwenker, K.; † Deceased 7 November 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Marco-Pallarés-Et-Al.2008.Pdf
    This article was published in an Elsevier journal. The attached copy is furnished to the author for non-commercial research and education use, including for instruction at the author’s institution, sharing with colleagues and providing to institution administration. Other uses, including reproduction and distribution, or selling or licensing copies, or posting to personal, institutional or third party websites are prohibited. In most cases authors are permitted to post their version of the article (e.g. in Word or Tex form) to their personal website or institutional repository. Authors requiring further information regarding Elsevier’s archiving and manuscript policies are encouraged to visit: http://www.elsevier.com/copyright Author's personal copy Neuropsychologia 46 (2008) 241–248 Human oscillatory activity associated to reward processing in a gambling task Josep Marco-Pallares a, David Cucurell b, Toni Cunillera b, Rafael Garc´ıa b, Antonio Andres-Pueyo´ c, Thomas F. Munte¨ a, Antoni Rodr´ıguez-Fornells b,d,∗ a Department of Neuropsychology, Otto von Guericke University, 39112 Magdeburg, Germany b Department of Basic Psychology, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Spain c Department of Personality, Faculty of Psychology, University of Barcelona, Spain d Instituci´o Catalana de Recerca i Estudis Avan¸cats (ICREA), Spain Received 25 January 2007; received in revised form 18 July 2007; accepted 19 July 2007 Available online 28 July 2007 Abstract Previous event-related brain potential (ERP) studies have identified a medial frontal negativity (MFN) in response to negative feedback or mone- tary losses. In contrast, no EEG correlates have been identified related to the processing of monetary gains or positive feedback.
    [Show full text]
  • ERP): Exploring the Time-Course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by RERO DOC Digital Library Brain Topogr (2008) 20:265–277 DOI 10.1007/s10548-008-0053-6 ORIGINAL PAPER Beyond Conventional Event-related Brain Potential (ERP): Exploring the Time-course of Visual Emotion Processing Using Topographic and Principal Component Analyses Gilles Pourtois Æ Sylvain Delplanque Æ Christoph Michel Æ Patrik Vuilleumier Accepted: 4 February 2008 / Published online: 13 March 2008 Ó Springer Science+Business Media, LLC 2008 Abstract Recent technological advances with the scalp and qualitative changes in the electric field configuration EEG methodology allow researchers to record electric recorded at the scalp level, which are not apparent when fields generated in the human brain using a large number of using conventional ERP analyses. Additional information electrodes or sensors (e.g. 64–256) distributed over the gained from these approaches include the identification of a head surface (multi-channel recording). As a consequence, sequence of successive processing stages that may not fully such high-density ERP mapping yields fairly dense ERP be reflected in ERP waveforms only, and the segregation of data sets that are often hard to analyze comprehensively or multiple or partly overlapping neural events that may be to relate straightforwardly to specific cognitive or emo- blended within a single ERP waveform. These findings tional processes, because of the richness of the recorded highlight the added value of such alternative analyses when signal in both the temporal (millisecond time-resolution) exploring the electrophysiological manifestations of com- and spatial (multidimensional topographic information) plex and distributed mental functions, as for instance domains.
    [Show full text]
  • Localizing the Neural Generators of Event-Related Brain Potentials
    Chapter 4 Localizing the Neural Generators of Event-Related Brain Potentials Diane Swick, Marta Kutas, and Helen J. Neville I. INTRODUCTION A major approach to neural localization has been inferring the location of cognitive operations within "normal" brains from the behavioral defi- cits and brain damage observed in neurological patient populations (Caramazza, 1992; Kosslyn & Intriligator, 1992). Years of research attest to the difficulty of this enterprise and underscore the need for converging evidence from multiple techniques. This chapter focuses specifically on describing how recordings of the electrical activity of the brain (in par- ticular, transient responses to various events) in patients and in control subjects can enrich our understanding of neuropsychological issues. Al- though the imaging methods currently available for investigating neural and psychological function in awake humans have improved markedly over the last decade, the nature of the inferences that can be drawn is constrained by the limits inherent in each technique. Generally, the trade off is between precision in space and time. At present, no technique pro- vides both very high spatial and very high temporal resolution; however, within 5-10 years we are likely to have developed a technique that does. The measurement of brain electrical activity from the scalp is noninva- sive, has very high temporal resolution (on the order of tenths of millisec- onds), and can provide an on-line record of brain function at the level of large neuronal populations. The electroencephalogram (EEG) consists of continuous voltage fluctuations caused by the summation of graded post- synaptic potentials from thousands of neurons and has long been used by clinicians to monitor behavioral state.
    [Show full text]
  • Dissociating Activation and Integration of Discourse Referents: Evidence from Erps and Oscillations
    cortex 126 (2020) 83e106 Available online at www.sciencedirect.com ScienceDirect Journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/cortex Research Report Dissociating activation and integration of discourse referents: Evidence from ERPs and oscillations * Cas W. Coopmans a,b, and Mante S. Nieuwland a,c a Max Planck Institute for Psycholinguistics, Nijmegen, the Netherlands b Centre for Language Studies, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands c Donders Institute for Brain, Cognition and Behaviour, Radboud University, Nijmegen, the Netherlands article info abstract Article history: A key challenge in understanding stories and conversations is the comprehension of Received 13 June 2019 ‘anaphora’, words that refer back to previously mentioned words or concepts (‘anteced- Reviewed 22 July 2019 ents’). In psycholinguistic theories, anaphor comprehension involves the initial activation Revised 30 August 2019 of the antecedent and its subsequent integration into the unfolding representation of the Accepted 20 December 2019 narrated event. A recent proposal suggests that these processes draw upon the brain’s Action editor Sonja Kotz recognition memory and language networks, respectively, and may be dissociable in pat- Published online 28 January 2020 terns of neural oscillatory synchronization (Nieuwland & Martin, 2017). We addressed this proposal in an electroencephalogram (EEG) study with pre-registered data acquisition and Keywords: analyses, using event-related potentials and neural oscillations. Dutch participants read Reference resolution two-sentence mini stories containing proper names, which were repeated or new (ease of Anaphora activation) and semantically coherent or incoherent with the preceding discourse (ease of Proper names integration). Repeated names elicited smaller N400 and Late Positive Component ampli- Theta tude than new names, and also an increase in theta-band (4e7 Hz) synchronization, which Gamma was largest around 240e450 ms after name onset.
    [Show full text]
  • Optimal Sustained Attention Is Linked to the Spectral Content of Background
    European Journal of Neuroscience, Vol. 25, pp. 900–907, 2007 doi:10.1111/j.1460-9568.2007.05324.x Optimal sustained attention is linked to the spectral content of background EEG activity: greater ongoing tonic alpha (10 Hz) power supports successful phasic goal activation Paul M. Dockree,1,5 Simon P. Kelly,2,3,5 John J. Foxe,1,3,4,5 Richard B. Reilly2,5 and Ian H. Robertson1,5 1Department of Psychology & Trinity College Institute of Neuroscience, Trinity College Dublin, Dublin 2, Ireland 2Department of Electronic and Electrical Engineering, University College Dublin, Dublin, Ireland 3The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, Nathan S. Kline Institute for Psychiatric Research, 140 Old Orangeburg Road, Orangeburg, NY, USA 4Program in Cognitive Neuroscience, Department of Psychology, The City College of the City University of New York, 138th St. & Convent Avenue, New York, USA 5The Cognitive Neurophysiology Laboratory, St Vincent’s Hospital, Fairview, Dublin, Ireland Abstract Efficient executive control frequently requires the timely activation or re-activation of a task-goal to enable purposeful behaviour. Additionally, more generalized factors such as alertness or neurological health will influence the efficiency with which control can be implemented. Goal-directed processes have been investigated by examining event-related potentials (ERPs), but much less is known about the involvement of background or ‘tonic’ processes reflected in the ongoing electroencephalogram (EEG), and how these affect the phasic processes expressed in the broad-band ERP. Here, we investigate the relationship between a key attention- sensitive tonic process – the alpha rhythm – and relevant phasic processes observed during a sustained attention paradigm in neurologically healthy subjects.
    [Show full text]
  • ERP Evidence for Consumer Evaluation of Copycat Brands
    RESEARCH ARTICLE ERP evidence for consumer evaluation of copycat brands Qian Shang1,2, Guanxiong Pei3, Jia Jin4,5, Wuke Zhang3, Yuran Wang3, Xiaoyi Wang3* 1 Chinese Academy of Science and Education Evaluation, Hangzhou Dianzi University, Hangzhou, China, 2 Management School, Hangzhou Dianzi University, Hangzhou, China, 3 School of Management, Zhejiang University, Hangzhou, China, 4 Business School, Ningbo University, Ningbo, China, 5 Academy of Neuroeconomics and Neuromanagement at Ningbo University, Ningbo, China * [email protected] a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 a1111111111 Abstract a1111111111 Copycat brands mimic brand leaders to free ride on the latter's equity. However, little is known regarding if and how consumers confuse copycat as leading brand in purchasing. In this study, we applied a word-pair evaluation paradigm in which the first word was a brand name (copycat vs. normal brand both similar with a leading brand in category), followed by a OPEN ACCESS product name (near vs. far from the leading brand's category). Behavioral results showed Citation: Shang Q, Pei G, Jin J, Zhang W, Wang Y, that, when the product is near the leader's category, the copycat strategy (CN) was more Wang X (2018) ERP evidence for consumer preferred compared to the normal brand (NN) but not different in the far product condition evaluation of copycat brands. PLoS ONE 13(2): e0191475. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal. (CF and NF). Event-related potential (ERP) data provided further insight into the mecha- pone.0191475 nism. The N400 amplitude elicited by the CN condition was significantly smaller than NN. Editor: Hengyi Rao, University of Pennsylvania, However, when products are far from the leader's category, there was no significant differ- UNITED STATES ence in N400 amplitudes.
    [Show full text]