Saturation and Adaptation in the Rod System

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Saturation and Adaptation in the Rod System Vision Res. Vol. 22, pp. 1299 to 1312, 1982 0042-6989/82/101299-14$03.00/0 Printed in Great Britain Pergamon Press Ltd SATURATION AND ADAPTATION IN THE ROD SYSTEM Edward H. Adelson* Vision Research Laboratory, University of Michigan, Department of Psychology, Ann Arbor, MI 48109, U.S.A. (Received 16 July 1881; in revised form 5 March 1982) Abstract—A background that is briefly flashed to a dark-adapted eye saturates the rod system. This transient saturation occurs with backgrounds that are as much as 2 log units dimmer than those producing saturation under steady viewing. Rod threshold is highest when the background is first turned on, and falls as adaptation proceeds. The nature of the adaptive processes are studied by presenting flashed backgrounds on pre-adapting fields. The data can be interpreted in terms of two adaptive processes: the first is multiplicative, and occurs rapidly; the second is subtractive, and occurs more slowly. INTRODUCTION have traced out the time-course of the effects of light adaptation. Crawford (1947) measured foveal incre- The retina is required to function under light levels ment thresholds against a flashed field, and found a that span an enormous range, and to do this effec- large transient threshold elevation during the first tively it must adapt to whatever ambient light level is 100 msec of background presentation (indeed, the present. In the cone system an important source of flashed background paradigm employed in the adaptation at high light levels is photopigment present study is sometimes known as "Crawford bleaching: as the pigment is depleted, the cones catch masking"). Boynton and Kandel (1957) measured fewer quanta, and so give smaller responses. Indeed, thresholds with flashed backgrounds, and found that pigment depletion seems to prevent cone signals from if the eye was adapted to a "pre-adapting" level before saturating under steady adaptation, as was shown by the flash appeared, threshold elevation could be Alpern et al (1970b). Those investigators also pointed reduced (but see Sperling, 1965). Although Boynton out that, since the bleaching process takes time to and Kandel did not reach saturating conditions, the come to equilibrium, one might expect that cone adaptive effects they observed are no doubt related to saturation could occur if thresholds were measured those that Hood and Geisler found would reduce the in a background that was flashed, rather than being tendency to saturation in cones steadily present. This effect had actually been ob- The present study is concerned with rods, rather served by Brindley, who hinted at it in Brindley than cones, and so the situation is somewhat different. (1959), without showing the relevant data points, and The rod system does show saturation in steady state then showed it clearly in Brindley (1963), where he (Aguilar and Stiles, 1954) and so one must conclude replotted some of his 1959 data. Brindley found cone that whatever adaptive mechanisms are present in the saturation to occur with flash intensities of about rod pathway are insufficient to prevent the signals 200 cd/m2, with a 4 mm pupil, which converts to from saturating. Photopigment depletion will not about 3.4 log td-sec. This value is similar to that later affect rod sensitivity via reduced quantum catch, since measured by Shevell (1977). (See also King-Smith and only in significant amounts of pigment are bleached Webb, 1974: Geisler, 1978.) below saturation. There is some evidence that verte- Hood and his co-workers (Hood, 1978; Hood et al, brate rods show no adaptation at all (Dowling, 1967; 1978), and Geisler (1978a, 1980) have found evidence Penn and Hagitts, 1972), although the evidence on that other adaptive mechanisms, besides pigment de- this is uncertain (see appendix 11), and in any case pletion, can also help prevent cone saturation they leaves open the question of adaptation beyond the have related these findings to the large body of elec- receptors trophysiological evidence on adaptation in photo- One way of studying adaptation in the rod system receptors (e g Kleinschmidt and Dowling, 1975; Nor- is to ask whether rod saturation is any different when man and Werblin, 1974) the backgrounds are flashed rather than steady. As Earlier psychophysical workers have also inves- was first reported in a preliminary description of the tigated the initial effects of a background flash, and present work (Adelson, 1977), the rod system can saturate with flashed backgrounds that are some 100 *Present address: RCA, David Sarnoff Research Center, times dimmer than those that cause saturation in Princeton, NJ 08540, U. S. A. steady state in the experiments that will be described, 1299 1300 Edward H. Adelson rod saturation was studied at various times after the The second beam followed a similar path, except onset of a back-ground, in an attempt to understand that it emerged from the lower pinhole at P, and was ° some of the adaptive processes that occur between the immediately reflected at 90 by mirror M0; it then onset of the background and the ultimate state of continued its route through S2, A2, W2, M2, FH2, FS adaptational equilibrium. (the field stop, defining the test patch), BS, FH4 and ML. METHODS When a third channel was needed, the optional beam splitter BS' was inserted, and the extra tungsten All experiments were done on the Maxwellian view lamp T' was turned on. This lamp (Mazda F-58) had system shown in Fig. 1. The system normally had 2 its filament arranged in a compact square, and the channels, but a third channel could be added by the filament itself was imaged in the plane of the subject's insertion of the beam splitter BS', shown in dashed entrance pupil (without the lens-pinhole system that lines. was used with the other channels). A tungsten ribbon filament, T, was run from a con- The observer stabilized his head by biting on a bite stant voltage power supply. Lens L imaged the fila- bar, which had an impression of his teeth in dental ment onto pinhole P, which actually consisted of two compound. With his right eye, he fixed on a fixation pinholes, one above the other, each selecting one por- point (FP) which was generated by a tiny light emit- tion of the elongated ribbon image. The beam from ting diode, imaged at optical infinity by a small lens, the top pinhole continued in a straight line, and and encased in a metal tube. encountered shutter S1 (Uniblitz; Vincent Associates), For most of the experiments, the author was the aperture A1, and neutral density wedge W, (Inconel; subject; one other subject was sometimes used. Both ° Bausch & Lomb). Mirror M1 introduced a 90 turn, subjects were well-corrected myopes, and wore their and the beam proceeded through the filters in filter spectacles during the experiments. holder FH1, the optional beam splitter BS' (if used), the combining beam splitter BS, and then to the Calibration of filters and light levels observer's eye (O) through filter holder FH4 and Four types of filters were used: neutral density Maxwellian lens ML. ML imaged the pinhole P in the inconels (Bausch & Lomb); neutral density Wratten plane of the eye's entrance pupil, and imaged the field No. 96 gelatines (Eastman Kodak, Rochester, NY); stop FS at optical infinity. colored gelatines (Eastman Kodak and Ilford Ltd., Es- Fig 1. (a) The Maxwellian view system. See text for details. (b) The observer's view in the apparatus. The test patch was 4.5° square, and was centered on the 11° background field. The fixation point (F P.) was 12°·to the right of the field. Saturation and adaptation 1301 sex, U. K.); narrow band inference filters (Baird The subject triggered all trials with a hand-opera- Atomic). The spectral transmittances of all filters were ted switch. He could control wedge W2 with a servo calibrated using a Beckman Acta II spectrophoto- motor, and wedge W1 with a mechanical linkage. meter. In all the experiments to follow, an attempt was The scotopic densities of the colored gelatins were made to isolate the rod system, and to prevent the measured in the apparatus by the method of Flamant cones from detecting the test flash. The conditions and Stiles (1948). The scotopic effectiveness of the adopted were similar to those of Aguilar and Stiles filtered light was compared with that of white light, (1954). by determining the amount of light needed to raise the As shown in Fig. 1b, the background subtended 11° scotopic threshold by l.0 log unit. The relative scoto- and appeared in the nasal half-field of the retina, cen- pic densities of the interference filters were measured tered 12° from the point of fixation. This background in the apparatus with a PIN-10 photodiode, using the was red (Wratten No. 26 gelatin; dom. wave. 621 nm). quantized CIE scotopic luminosity function. The ab- To adapt the cones maximally, the background light solute scotopic densities of two interference filters entered the subject's eye through the center of the were then measured by the method of Flamant and pupil. The background could either be steadily Stiles, and all the other interference filter densities present (as in Aguilar and Stiles' experiment), or were normalized against these. flashed. Absolute light levels, in scotopic trolands, were The test was a square, 4.5° on a side, and fell in the measured in two ways. First, the absolute calibration center of the background. To excite the rods preferen- of the PIN-10, supplied by the manufacturer, was tially, rather than the cones, it entered through the used to derive quanta sec-1 deg-1 with a given inter- nasal edge of the subject's pupil.
Recommended publications
  • Shedding New Light on the Generation of the Visual Chromophore PERSPECTIVE Krzysztof Palczewskia,B,C,1 and Philip D
    PERSPECTIVE Shedding new light on the generation of the visual chromophore PERSPECTIVE Krzysztof Palczewskia,b,c,1 and Philip D. Kiserb,d Edited by Jeremy Nathans, Johns Hopkins University School of Medicine, Baltimore, MD, and approved July 9, 2020 (received for review May 16, 2020) The visual phototransduction cascade begins with a cis–trans photoisomerization of a retinylidene chro- mophore associated with the visual pigments of rod and cone photoreceptors. Visual opsins release their all-trans-retinal chromophore following photoactivation, which necessitates the existence of pathways that produce 11-cis-retinal for continued formation of visual pigments and sustained vision. Proteins in the retinal pigment epithelium (RPE), a cell layer adjacent to the photoreceptor outer segments, form the well- established “dark” regeneration pathway known as the classical visual cycle. This pathway is sufficient to maintain continuous rod function and support cone photoreceptors as well although its throughput has to be augmented by additional mechanism(s) to maintain pigment levels in the face of high rates of photon capture. Recent studies indicate that the classical visual cycle works together with light-dependent pro- cesses in both the RPE and neural retina to ensure adequate 11-cis-retinal production under natural illu- minances that can span ten orders of magnitude. Further elucidation of the interplay between these complementary systems is fundamental to understanding how cone-mediated vision is sustained in vivo. Here, we describe recent
    [Show full text]
  • Control of Rhodopsin Activity in Vision
    Control of rhodopsin DENIS A. BAYLOR, MARIE E. BURNS activity in vision Abstract high concentration in the cytoplasm in darkness and that binds to cationic channels in the Although rhodopsin's role in activating the surface membrane, holding them open. phototransduction cascade is well known, the Hydrolysis of cGMP allows the channels to processes that deactivate rhodopsin, and thus close, interrupting an inward current of sodium, the rest of the cascade, are less well calcium and magnesium ions and producing a understood. At least three proteins appear to hyperpolarisation of the membrane. The play a role: rhodopsin kinase, arrestin and hyperpolarisation reduces the rate at which recoverin. Here we review recent physiological neurotransmitter is released from the synaptic studies of the molecular mechanisms of terminal of the rod. rhodopsin deactivation. The approach was to The purpose of this paper is to review recent monitor the light responses of individual work on the important but still poorly mouse rods in which rhodopsin was altered or understood mechanisms that terminate the arrestin was deleted by transgenic techniques. light-evoked catalytic activity of rhodopsin. Removal of rhodopsin's carboxy-terminal These mechanisms, which fix the intensity and residues which contain phosphorylation sites duration of the activation of the transduction implicated in deactivation, prolonged the flash cascade, need to satisfy strong functional response 20-fold and caused it to become constraints. Rhodopsin activity must be highly variable. In rods that did not express arrestin the flash response recovered partially, terminated rapidly so that an absorbed photon but final recovery was slowed over lOO-fold.
    [Show full text]
  • Molecular Basis of Dark Adaptation in Rod Photoreceptors
    1 Molecular basis of dark c.s. LEIBROCK , T. REUTER, T.D. LAMB adaptation in rod photoreceptors Abstract visual threshold (logarithmically) against time, following 'bleaching' exposures of different Following exposure of the eye to an intense strengths. After an almost total bleach light that 'bleaches' a significant fraction of (uppermost trace) the visual threshold recovers the rhodopsin, one's visual threshold is along the classical bi-phasic curve: the initial initially greatly elevated, and takes tens of rapid recovery is due to cones, and the second minutes to recover to normal. The elevation of slower component occurs when the rod visual threshold arises from events occurring threshold drops below the cone threshold. within the rod photoreceptors, and the (Note that in this old work, the term 'photon' underlying molecular basis of these events was used for the unit now defined as the and of the rod's recovery is now becoming troland; x trolands is the illuminance at the clearer. Results obtained by exposing isolated retina when a light of 1 cd/m2 enters a pupil toad rods to hydroxylamine solution indicate with cross-sectional area x mm2.) that, following small bleaches, the primary intermediate causing elevation of visual threshold is metarhodopsin II, in its Questions and observations phosphorylated and arrestin-bound form. This The basic question in dark adaptation, which product activates transduction with an efficacy has not been answered convincingly in the six about 100 times greater than that of opsin. decades since the results of Fig. 1 were obtained, Key words Bleaching, Dark adaptation, is: Why is one not able to see very well during Metarhodopsin, Noise, Photoreceptors, the period following a bleaching exposure? Or, Sensitivity more explicitly: What is the molecular basis for the slow recovery of visual performance during dark adaptation? In considering the answers to these questions, there are three long-standing observations that need to be borne in mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Temporal and Spatial Dependence of Adaptation on Ganglion Cells
    Temporal and Spatial Dependence of Adaptation on Ganglion Cells Amin, A.P.1, Moazzezi, R.2, Werblin, F.S.2 1University of California, Berkeley; Department of Integrative Biology 2University of California, Berkeley; Department of Molecular and Cellular Biology Keywords: adaptation, contrast sensitivity, ganglion, inter-stimuli interval, ISI, lateral interneurons, luminosity, MicroElectrode Array (MEA), photoreceptor, rabbit, retina, spatial, temporal ABSTRACT BSJ One of the visual system’s many tasks is to be able to the interval between two circular stimuli (inter-stimuli distinguish objects from the background. The ability interval; ISI) of the same diameter, and by changing the to do this is limited and affected by the relationship diameter over a common ISI, we measured a ganglion between the object (or stimulus) of interest and the cell’s output for one stimulus relative to another background. Adaptation in retinal neurons is the process stimulus. The results show saturation (loss of output to of changing the cell’s response to a stimulus according the 2nd stimulus) of stimuli at lower ISIs. Also, the degree to that stimulus’s background. When the stimulus is of saturation for a given ISI depends on the diameter hard to discern from the background, the retina adapts of the stimulus. These combinations of results illustrate by improving its sensitivity to low contrast. The large the temporal and spatial dependence of adaptation response range maintained by adaptation comes at a on ganglion cells. A larger-diameter stimulus involves cost, however. Adaptation complicates neural coding by multiple neurons surrounding the ganglion cell being making the brain interpret identical stimuli as different recorded so various pathways most likely influence based on differences in background.
    [Show full text]
  • The Eye and Night Vision
    Source: http://www.aoa.org/x5352.xml Print This Page The Eye and Night Vision (This article has been adapted from the excellent USAF Special Report, AL-SR-1992-0002, "Night Vision Manual for the Flight Surgeon", written by Robert E. Miller II, Col, USAF, (RET) and Thomas J. Tredici, Col, USAF, (RET)) THE EYE The basic structure of the eye is shown in Figure 1. The anterior portion of the eye is essentially a lens system, made up of the cornea and crystalline lens, whose primary purpose is to focus light onto the retina. The retina contains receptor cells, rods and cones, which, when stimulated by light, send signals to the brain. These signals are subsequently interpreted as vision. Most of the receptors are rods, which are found predominately in the periphery of the retina, whereas the cones are located mostly in the center and near periphery of the retina. Although there are approximately 17 rods for every cone, the cones, concentrated centrally, allow resolution of fine detail and color discrimination. The rods cannot distinguish colors and have poor resolution, but they have a much higher sensitivity to light than the cones. DAY VERSUS NIGHT VISION According to a widely held theory of vision, the rods are responsible for vision under very dim levels of illumination (scotopic vision) and the cones function at higher illumination levels (photopic vision). Photopic vision provides the capability for seeing color and resolving fine detail (20/20 of better), but it functions only in good illumination. Scotopic vision is of poorer quality; it is limited by reduced resolution ( 20/200 or less) and provides the ability to discriminate only between shades of black and white.
    [Show full text]
  • Effect of Rhodopsin Phosphorylation on Dark Adaptation in Mouse Rods
    The Journal of Neuroscience, June 29, 2016 • 36(26):6973–6987 • 6973 Cellular/Molecular Effect of Rhodopsin Phosphorylation on Dark Adaptation in Mouse Rods X Justin Berry,1 XRikard Frederiksen,1 Yun Yao,2 Soile Nymark,3 Jeannie Chen,2 and Carter Cornwall1 1Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Boston University School of Medicine, Boston, Massachusetts 02118, 2Zilkha Neurogenetic Institute, Department of Cell and Neurobiology, Keck School of Medicine, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, California 90033, and 3Department of Electronics and Communications Engineering, BioMediTech, Tampere University of Technology, 33520 Tampere, Finland Rhodopsin is a prototypical G-protein-coupled receptor (GPCR) that is activated when its 11-cis-retinal moiety is photoisomerized to all-trans retinal. This step initiates a cascade of reactions by which rods signal changes in light intensity. Like other GPCRs, rhodopsin is deactivatedthroughreceptorphosphorylationandarrestinbinding.Fullrecoveryofreceptorsensitivityisthenachievedwhenrhodopsin is regenerated through a series of steps that return the receptor to its ground state. Here, we show that dephosphorylation of the opsin moiety of rhodopsin is an extremely slow but requisite step in the restoration of the visual pigment to its ground state. We make use of a novel observation: isolated mouse retinae kept in standard media for routine physiologic recordings display blunted dephosphorylation of rhodopsin. Isoelectric focusing followed by Western blot analysis of bleached isolated retinae showed little dephosphorylation of rhodopsin for up to4hindarkness, even under conditions when rhodopsin was completely regenerated. Microspectrophotometeric determinations of rhodopsin spectra show that regenerated phospho-rhodopsin has the same molecular photosensitivity as unphos- phorylated rhodopsin and that flash responses measured by trans-retinal electroretinogram or single-cell suction electrode recording displayed dark-adapted kinetics.
    [Show full text]
  • A Local Model of Eye Adaptation for High Dynamic Range Images
    A Local Model of Eye Adaptation for High Dynamic Range Images Patrick Ledda Luis Paulo Santos Alan Chalmers University of Bristol, UK University of Minho, Portugal University of Bristol, UK [email protected] [email protected] [email protected] ABSTRACT In the real world, the human eye is confronted with a wide range of luminances from bright sunshine to low night light. Our eyes cope with this vast range of intensities by adapta- tion; changing their sensitivity to be responsive at different illumination levels. This adaptation is highly localized, al- lowing us to see both dark and bright regions of a high dy- namic range environment. In this paper we present a new model of eye adaptation based on physiological data. The model, which can be easily integrated into existing render- ers, can function either as a static local tone mapping oper- ator for single high dynamic range image, or as a temporal adaptation model taking into account time elapsed and in- tensity of preadaptation for a dynamic sequence. We finally validate our technique with a high dynamic range display and a psychophysical study. Figure 1: On the left, the Kalabsha temple tone- mapped with our local model. On the right, linear mappings at different exposure times. Categories and Subject Descriptors I.3.3 [Computer Graphics]: Picture/Image Generation| Display Algorithms response range. Our visual system copes with the large lu- Keywords minance range by a process called adaptation. At the retina Human Visual System, Eye Adaptation, High Dynamic Range, level, eye adaptation is highly localized allowing us to see Tone Mapping both dark and bright regions in the same high dynamic range environment.
    [Show full text]
  • Dual Involvement of G-Substrate in Motor Learning Revealed by Gene Deletion
    Dual involvement of G-substrate in motor learning revealed by gene deletion Shogo Endoa,1, Fumihiro Shutohb,1, Tung Le Dinhc,1, Takehito Okamotob, Toshio Ikedad, Michiyuki Suzukie, Shigenori Kawaharae, Dai Yanagiharaf, Yamato Satof, Kazuyuki Yamadag, Toshiro Sakamotoa, Yutaka Kirinoe, Nicholas A. Hartellh, Kazuhiko Yamaguchic, Shigeyoshi Itoharad, Angus C. Nairni, Paul Greengardj, Soichi Nagaob, and Masao Itoc,2 aUnit for Molecular Neurobiology of Learning and Memory, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology, Uruma 904-2234, Japan; bLaboratory for Motor Learning Control, cLaboratory for Memory and Learning, dLaboratory for Behavioral Genetics, and gSupport Unit for Animal Experiment, Research Resources Center, RIKEN Brain Science Institute, Wako 351-0198, Japan; eLaboratory for Neurobiophysics, School of Pharmaceutical Sciences, University of Tokyo, Tokyo 113-0033, Japan; fDepartment of Life Sciences, Graduate School of Arts and Sciences, University of Tokyo, Tokyo 153-8902, Japan; hDepartment of Cell Physiology and Pharmacology, University of Leicester, Leicester LE1 9HN, United Kingdom; iDepartment of Psychiatry, Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT 06519; and jLaboratory for Molecular and Cellular Neuroscience, The Rockefeller University, New York, NY 10021-6399 Contributed by Masao Ito, December 30, 2008 (sent for review December 7, 2008) In this study, we generated mice lacking the gene for G-substrate, pathway acts as a modulator whose requirement for LTD may a specific substrate for cGMP-dependent protein kinase
    [Show full text]
  • Neural and Photochemical Mechanisms of Visual Adaptation in the Rat
    Neural and Photochemical Mechanisms of Visual Adaptation in the Rat JOHN E. DOWLING From the BiologicalLaboratories, Harvard University, Cambridge, Massachusetts ABSTRACT The effects of light adaptation on the increment threshold, rhodopsin content, and dark adaptation have been studied in the rat eye over a wide range of intensities. The electroretinogram threshold was used as a measure of eye sensitivity. With adapting intensities greater than 1.5 log units above the absolute ERG threshold, the increment threshold rises linearly with increasing adapting intensity. With 5 minutes of light adaptation, the rhodopsin content of the eye is not measurably reduced until the adapting in- tensity is greater than 5 log units above the ERG threshold. Dark adaptation is rapid (i.e., completed in 5 to I0 minutes) until the eye is adapted to lights strong enough to bleach a measurable fraction of the rhodopsin. After brighter light adaptations, dark adaptation consists of two parts, an initial rapid phase followed by a slow component. The extent of slow adaptation depends on the fraction of rhodopsin bleached. If all the rhodopsin in the eye is bleached, the slow fall of threshold extends over 5 log units and takes 2 to 3 hours to com- plete. The fall of ERG threshold during the slow phase of adaptation occurs in parallel with the regeneration of rhodopsin. The slow component of dark adaptation is related to the bleaching and resynthesis of rhodopsin; the fast component of adaptation is considered to be neural adaptation. INTRODUCTION It is a matter of common experience that the eye loses sensitivity in the light and regains sensitivity in the dark.
    [Show full text]
  • Early Scotopic Dark Adaptation
    Early Scotopic Dark Adaptation A dissertation presented by Rebecca J. Grayhem To The Department of Psychology In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy In the field of Psychology Northeastern University Boston, Massachusetts December, 2010 1 EARLY SCOTOPIC DARK ADAPTATION by Rebecca J. Grayhem ABSTRACT OF DISSERTATION Submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology in the Graduate School of Arts and Sciences of Northeastern University, December 2010 2 Abstract The human visual system can function over a broad range of light levels, from few photons to bright sunshine. I am interested in sensitivity regulation of the rod pathway in dim light, below the level at which the cones become effective. I obtained thresholds for large (1.3 deg) and tiny (5 min arc) circular test spots, either after 20-30 mins exposure to complete darkness (absolute threshold), or on uniform backgrounds of varying light intensities, or just after the background was removed to plunge the eye into darkness; Maxwellian view optics were used so that variations in pupil size would have no effect. The overall theory is that sensitivity in the rod system is adjusted by a slow process of adaptation in the retina driven by the long-term mean background light level and a rapid process sensitive to variability in the light provided by the background (“photon noise”). My data show that thresholds for tiny test spots primarily reveal the photon-driven noise effect, and those for large test spots reveal both processes. The traditional idea that all components of rod dark adaptation are slow is challenged here, since almost immediately after the background light is removed, thresholds of tiny spots drop to absolute threshold directly, and thresholds of large test spots drop half-way to absolute threshold before continuing to recover back to absolute threshold over the next 25 minutes.
    [Show full text]
  • Interruption of Homologous Desensitization in Cyclic Guanosine 3V,5V-Monophosphate Signaling Restores Colon Cancer Cytostasis by Bacterial Enterotoxins
    Research Article Interruption of Homologous Desensitization in Cyclic Guanosine 3V,5V-Monophosphate Signaling Restores Colon Cancer Cytostasis by Bacterial Enterotoxins Giovanni M. Pitari, Ronnie I. Baksh, David M. Harris, Peng Li, Shiva Kazerounian, and Scott A. Waldman Division of Clinical Pharmacology, Departments of Pharmacology and Experimental Therapeutics and Medicine, Thomas Jefferson University, Philadelphia, Pennsylvania Abstract Bacterial heat-stable enterotoxins induce secretory diarrhea in Bacterial diarrheagenic heat-stable enterotoxins induce colon endemic populations, travelers, and animal herds (8) by serving cancer cell cytostasis by targeting guanylyl cyclase C (GCC) as superagonists for the intestine-specific receptor guanylyl signaling. Anticancer actions of these toxins are mediated by cyclase C (GCC; ref. 9). Heat-stable enterotoxins, which have cyclic guanosine 3V,5V-monophosphate (cGMP)–dependent in- evolved to facilitate bacterial dissemination and propagation, flux of Ca2+ through cyclic nucleotide-gated channels. However, exemplify molecular mimicry of the endogenous hormones prolonged stimulation of GCC produces resistance in tumor guanylin and uroguanylin, which mediate autocrine/paracrine cells to heat-stable enterotoxin–induced cytostasis. Resistance control of intestinal fluid and electrolyte homeostasis by acti- V V reflects rapid (tachyphylaxis) and slow (bradyphylaxis) mech- vating GCC and inducing cyclic guanosine 3 ,5 -monophosphate anisms of desensitization induced by cGMP. Tachyphylaxis is (cGMP)–dependent chloride efflux through the cystic fibrosis regulator channel (10–12). Intriguingly, longitudinal exposure to mediated by cGMP-dependent protein kinase, which limits the conductance of cyclic nucleotide-gated channels, reducing the heat-stable enterotoxin–producing bacteria seems to protect influx of Ca2+ propagating the antiproliferative signal from endemic populations against colon cancer by reducing rates of the membrane to the nucleus.
    [Show full text]
  • Night Vision in a Case of Vitamin a Deficiency Due to Malabsorption
    Br J Ophthalmol: first published as 10.1136/bjo.67.1.37 on 1 January 1983. Downloaded from British Journal ofOphthalmology, 1983, 67, 37-42 Night vision in a case of vitamin A deficiency due to malabsorption IDO PERLMAN,' DAVID BARZILAI,2 TAMARHAIM,3 AND ALFRED SCHRAMEK4 From the 'Department of Physiology and Biophysics, Faculty of Medicine, Technion, Haifa; the 2Department of Internal Medicine, Rambam Medical Center, Haifa; the 3Department of Ophthalmology, Rambam Medical Center, Haifa, and the 4Department of Surgery, Rambam Medical Center, Haifa, Israel SUMMARY Night vision was tested electroretinographically and psychophysically in a vitamin A deficient patient before and after therapy. Vitamin A deficiency resulted from malabsorption due to a jeujunoileal bypass operation. Before therapy the patient had severely reduced cone and rod function. After the reversal operation, accompanied by 5 injections of a total of 500000 units of vitamin A, complete recovery of cone and rod functions was observed within 7 months. Shortly after therapy rod sensitivity reached the normal level, while the time course of rod adaptation remained slower than normal and the dark-adapted electroretinographic (ERG) responses were subnormal. At later stages the ERG responses reached normal amplitudes but rod adaptation stayed slow. Only after 7 months did night vision reach the normal level with regard to the time course of rod adaptation, rod sensitivity, and ERG responses. Disturbances in night vision have long been associated in retinal function due to vitamin A deficiency and its with vitamin A deficiency. Numerous cases of night recovery following vitamin A therapy was also http://bjo.bmj.com/ blindness have been reported to arise from mal- demonstrated experimentally in rats.
    [Show full text]