Differences in Negativity Bias Underlie Variations in Political Ideology John R

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Differences in Negativity Bias Underlie Variations in Political Ideology John R University of Nebraska - Lincoln DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln Faculty Publications: Political Science Political Science, Department of 2014 Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology John R. Hibbing University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] Kevin B. Smith University of Nebraska-Lincoln, [email protected] John R. Alford Rice University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/poliscifacpub Part of the Political Science Commons Hibbing, John R.; Smith, Kevin B.; and Alford, John R., "Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology" (2014). Faculty Publications: Political Science. 67. http://digitalcommons.unl.edu/poliscifacpub/67 This Article is brought to you for free and open access by the Political Science, Department of at DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. It has been accepted for inclusion in Faculty Publications: Political Science by an authorized administrator of DigitalCommons@University of Nebraska - Lincoln. BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (2014) 37, 297–350 doi:10.1017/S0140525X13001192 Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology John R. Hibbing Department of Political Science, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588 [email protected] www.unl.edu/polphyslab Kevin B. Smith Department of Political Science, University of Nebraska–Lincoln, Lincoln, NE 68588 [email protected] www.unl.edu/polphyslab John R. Alford Department of Political Science, Rice University, Houston, TX 77005 [email protected] http://politicalscience.rice.edu/ Abstract: Disputes between those holding differing political views are ubiquitous and deep-seated, and they often follow common, recognizable lines. The supporters of tradition and stability, sometimes referred to as conservatives, do battle with the supporters of innovation and reform, sometimes referred to as liberals. Understanding the correlates of those distinct political orientations is probably a prerequisite for managing political disputes, which are a source of social conflict that can lead to frustration and even bloodshed. A rapidly growing body of empirical evidence documents a multitude of ways in which liberals and conservatives differ from each other in purviews of life with little direct connection to politics, from tastes in art to desire for closure and from disgust sensitivity to the tendency to pursue new information, but the central theme of the differences is a matter of debate. In this article, we argue that one organizing element of the many differences between liberals and conservatives is the nature of their physiological and psychological responses to features of the environment that are negative. Compared with liberals, conservatives tend to register greater physiological responses to such stimuli and also to devote more psychological resources to them. Operating from this point of departure, we suggest approaches for refining understanding of the broad relationship between political views and response to the negative. We conclude with a discussion of normative implications, stressing that identifying differences across ideological groups is not tantamount to declaring one ideology superior to another. Keywords: attitudes; conservatives; liberals; negativity; physiology; psychology; politics 1. Introduction or “rightist” may be relatively recent (etymologically they John Stuart Mill called it “commonplace” for political are typically assumed to date to the French Revolution, systems to have “a party of order or stability and a party of but they appear to be much older; see Laponce 1981)but progress or reform” (1991). Ralph Waldo Emerson agreed, the political division they describe is ancient and universal noting that “the two parties which divide the state, the (Bobbio 1996;Jost2006; Jost & Amodio 2012;McCarty party of conservatism and that of innovation, are very old, et al. 2006). Is Emerson right in his claim that this division and have disputed the possession of the world ever since it springs from a deep, possibly innate part of the human con- was made,” and he inferred that this “irreconcilable antagon- dition? Does political temperament vary from person to ism must have a correspondent depth of seat in the human person because the physiology and psychology constituting condition” (1903). The antagonism between two primal human nature also varies from person to person? If so, mindsets certainly pervades human history: Sparta and how are the individuals who support parties of stability and Athens; optimates and populares; Roundheads and Cava- order psychologically and physiologically different from liers; Inquisition and Enlightenment; Protagonus and those who support parties of progress and innovation? Plato; Pope Urban VIII and Galileo; Barry Goldwater and Existing research offers only incomplete answers to George McGovern; Sarah Palin and Hillary Rodham these questions. All too often, the questions are not even Clinton. The labels “liberal” or “leftist” and “conservative” asked. Folk wisdom and much scholarly research assumes © Cambridge University Press 2014 0140-525X/14 $40.00 297 Hibbing et al: Differences in negativity bias underlie variations in political ideology political orientations are products of socialization, learned Though twin studies are valuable for assessing the from parents and family, acquired by osmosis from sociode- general roles of heritability and various categories of mographics, or conditioned exclusively by environmental environmental influence (shared and unshared), they say situations and cultural contexts. The logic here is reason- little about the specific sources of influence within those able; authority figures encountered at impressionable broad categories. Accordingly, efforts are underway to early stages of life, as well as broader circumstances experi- identify particular genetic regions or even particular enced later seem obvious sources of influence on a range of genes that might relate to politics (Fowler & Dawes personal and social orientations including those relating to 2008; Hatemi et al. 2011; McDermott et al. 2009; Settle politics. Yet the effects of parental socialization on political et al. 2010). Yet although intriguing, it is not clear genopo- orientations are fairly meager (bivariate correlations typi- litics research can comprehensively illuminate the source of cally running between 0.1 and 0.3) with the exception of the “irreconcilable differences” that Mill, Emerson, and identification with social groups such as a political party others have long suspected to be the basis of political (Jennings & Niemi 1968; Niemi & Jennings 1991). beliefs. Any given candidate gene (or genetic region) is Adding socidemographic variables such as age, education likely to explain only a small fraction of the variance in a level, and family income to models of political attitudes complex quantitative trait like political temperament and and behavior only modestly increases explanatory horse- statistically isolating meaningful relationships amongst power (Plutzer 2002). Moreover, sociodemographic vari- such marginal impacts is difficult. That situation is reflected ables in and of themselves do not explain the precise in the poor replication record of candidate gene association factors at work in structuring preferences. In sum, political studies, particularly when they involve interactions with any orientations do not seem to be the automatic result of par- of a large number of possible environmental influences. ental socialization and sociodemographic circumstances. For example, Fowler and Dawes (2008) identified allelic To the surprise of many (but see Merelman 1971), it is variation in two genes involved in the serotonin system increasingly clear that Emerson’s intuition was right. Poli- (the transporter 5-HTT and the degrader MAO-A) that sys- tics might not be in our souls, but it probably is in our tematically correlated with political participation. A reana- DNA. More than 25 years ago Nicholas Martin and lysis of the same data by Charney and English (2012) Lindon Eaves (Martin et al. 1986), using a standard twin using different procedures did not reproduce that design on a large sample, produced heritability estimates finding, and replications have fed the controversy as between 0.2 and 0.4 for attitudes on a wide variety of pol- much as resolved it (see Deppe et al. 2013; Fowler & itical issues (e.g., capital punishment, disarmament, abor- Dawes 2013). So although twin studies suggest that political tion). More recent twin studies consistently confirm these orientations may be heritable, identifying the particular findings and extend them to behaviors such as voter genetic pathways that lead to political orientations constitu- turnout (Alford et al. 2005; Bell et al. 2009; Bouchard & tes a daunting challenge. McGue 2003; Fowler et al. 2008; Hatemi et al. 2007; The same could be said, however, about identifying 2009; 2013; Klemmensen et al. 2012; Smith & Hatemi the particular environmental pathways that lead to politi- 2013). Given the many assumptions undergirding twin cal orientation. Twin studies repeatedly point to a strong studies, it is important to note that alternative techniques influence of the unshared environment and a fairly weak for estimating heritability that do not rely on twins report role of the shared environment on political orientations. slightly smaller but still statistically significant effects of Traditional research on the correlates of political
Recommended publications
  • Author's Personal Copy
    Author's personal copy Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews 35 (2011) 1042–1051 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Neuroscience and Biobehavioral Reviews journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/neubiorev Review Human threat management systems: Self•protection and disease avoidance Steven L. Neuberg a,∗, Douglas T. Kenrick a, Mark Schaller b a Department of Psychology, Arizona State University, Tempe, AZ 85287•1104, United States b Department of Psychology, University of British Columbia, Vancouver, BC, Canada V6T 1Z4 article info abstract Article history: Humans likely evolved precautionary systems designed to minimize the threats to reproductive fitness Received 8 March 2010 posed by highly interdependent ultrasociality. A review of research on the self•protection and disease Received in revised form 18 August 2010 avoidance systems reveals that each system is functionally distinct and domain•specific: each is attuned Accepted 18 August 2010 to different cues; engages different emotions, inferences, and behavioral inclinations; and is rooted in somewhat different neurobiological substrates. These systems share important features, however. Each Keywords: system is functionally coherent, in that perceptual, affective, cognitive, and behavioral processes work Self•protection in concert to reduce fitness costs of potential threats. Each system is biased in a risk•averse manner, Disease avoidance Threat management erring toward precautionary responses even when available cues only heuristically imply threat. And Precautionary psychology each system is functionally flexible, being highly sensitive to specific ecological and dispositional cues Evolutionary psychology that signal greater vulnerability to the relevant threat. These features characterize a general template Domain specificity useful for understanding not only the self•protection and disease avoidance systems, but also a broader Fear set of evolved, domain•specific precautionary systems.
    [Show full text]
  • Contact Information
    REBECCA NEEL Department of Psychology Arizona State University Tempe, AZ 85287-1104 602-965-3326 EMPLOYMENT Beginning August, 2013 Assistant Professor, Department of Psychology, University of Iowa EDUCATION Ph.D., Psychology (May, 2013). Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona. Thesis: “Expert in the language of fear”: Stigmatized targets’ perception of others’ emotion-specific prejudice M.A., Psychology (2010). Arizona State University, Tempe, Arizona. Thesis: When does seeing through your eyes help me? Emotions functionally encourage and inhibit perspective taking B.A., English Literature (2002). Carleton College, Northfield, Minnesota. Cum Laude AWARDS AND HONORS 2013 SPSP Graduate Student Travel Award 2012 SPSSI Grant-in-Aid for Dissertation Research 2012 The Robert B. Cialdini Dissertation Project Prize in Social Psychology, ASU 2012 The Darwyn and Marie Linder Fellowship in Experimental Social Psychology, ASU 2012 ASU Graduate and Professional Student Association Grant for Independent Research 2011 Summer Institute in Social Psychology, Princeton, NJ; SPSP program funded by NSF 2009, 2010, 2011, 2012 ASU Graduate and Professional Student Association Grant for Conference Travel 2008 ASU Graduate and Professional Student Association Grant for Independent Research 2007 Departmental Research Excellence Award, ASU Psychology Department Neel CV RESEARCH Publications White, A.E., Kenrick, D.T., Neel, R., & Neuberg, S.L. (in press). From the bedroom to the budget deficit: Mate competition changes men’s attitudes toward economic redistribution. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology. Sadalla, E., Berlin, A., Neel, R., & Ledlow, S. (in press). Priorities in residential water use: A trade-off analysis. Environment and Behavior. Neel, R., Neufeld, S.L., & Neuberg, S.L. (2013). Would an obese person whistle Vivaldi? Targets of prejudice self-present to minimize appearance of specific threats.
    [Show full text]
  • Home Again George Clarke on Why Reviving Britain’S Architectural Past Could Ease the Housing Crisis
    RSA Student Opportunity Journal Spring 2013 Fund Home again George Clarke on why reviving Britain’s architectural past could ease the housing crisis From encouraging young enterprise, to cultural visits and To find out more, or to make a debating contests, the new Student Opportunity Fund will donation and help us reach our provide enrichment activities for students target of £35,000, please visit in our RSA Academies. www.thersa.org/opportunityfund Dieter Helm considers the sustainable practices that will reverse our nation’s natural decline The fund will be used to help to grow students’ confidence and There are two ways to donate: Colin Beard and Ilfryn Price on how space is shaping the way we work encourage creative thinking and problem solving. It will give them new +44 (0)20 7451 6902 opportunities and skills that offer the best possible chance to realise their potential when they leave school. www.thersa.org/opportunityfund RSA_J582_house_journal_advert_7.3.13.indd 1 07/03/2013 14:56 Help future generations Do you know fulfi l their potential someone Journal Spring 2013 1755 RSA awarded its fi rst premiums for new products and who would inventions 2010 RSA Catalyst set up, seed funding Fellows’ make a great projects that make an impact in the UK and internationally Fellow? 1856 RSA launched the fi rst national exams for vocational qualifi cations 2008 The RSA Tipton Academy opened, continuing the RSA’s strong commitment to education Your nominations are a great way to add the expertise and enthusiasm of friends and colleagues to the Fellowship community.
    [Show full text]
  • Committees and Welcomes
    Committees and Welcomes SPSP Execut ive and Com m it t ees Welcome to the Fourth Annual SPSP President Jim BlascovichMeeting! Past President Claude SteeleDear SPSP Colleagues, President Elect Hazel Markus It is my great pleasure to welcome you to the annual meetings Executive Officer Harry Reis of the Society for Personality and Social Psychology. Co-Secretary Treasurers Leslie Zebrowitz Sharon Brehm This is our fourth annual meeting – a remarkably brief history Members-at-Large David Dunning for a convention that is now so firmly established. Our confer- David Funder ences in Nashville, San Antonio, and Savannah each drew crowds that were greatly in excess of expectations. In so doing, Judith Harakiewicz they rapidly established SPSP as the major national or interna- APA Council Representatives Monica Biernat tional scientific meeting for personality and social psycholo- June Tangney gists. Publication Committee John Dovidio Gifford Weary We now look forward to building on this string of success in Universal City/Los Angeles, site of our 2003 meeting. This Joanne Wood year, we once again have an exceptionally strong and diverse Convention Committee Dan Cervone chair set of scientific presentations. Our program is expanded even Lynne Cooper beyond last year’s offerings, thanks in part to funding from the Rick Hoyle Decade of Behavior initiative, which is supporting a plenary Training Committee Lisa Aspinwall address by the renowned biologist and author Jared Diamond. Kim Bartholomew We also have expanded on the lighter side of things; on Satur- Allen Omoto day night, we will end our meetings with a social event featur- PSPB Editor Fred Rhodewalt ing DJ’d music at 8 pm followed by a live performance by the PSPR Editor Eliot Smith LA rock band Lustra.
    [Show full text]
  • Discipline and Murder: Panoptic Pedagogy and the Aesthetics of Detection in J.G
    Discipline and Murder 72 DOI: 10.1515/abcsj-2017-0005 Discipline and Murder: Panoptic Pedagogy and the Aesthetics of Detection in J.G. Ballard’s Running Wild CORNELIA MACSINIUC University of Suceava Abstract My essay proposes a reading of J.G. Ballard’s 1988 novella Running Wild as a cautionary crime story, a parable about the self-fulfilling prophecies of contemporary urban fears and about the “prisons” they create in a consumerist, technology- and media-dominated civilization. Interpreted in the light of Foucault’s concept of panopticism, Ballard’s gated community as a crime setting reveals how a disciplinary pedagogy meant to obtain “docile bodies,” masked under the socially elitist comfort of affluence and parental care, “brands” the inmate-children as potential delinquents and ultimately drives them to an act of “mass tyrannicide.” Ballard uses the murder story as a vehicle for the exploration of the paradoxical effects of a regime of total surveillance and of mediated presence, which, while expected to make “murder mystery” impossible, allows for the precession of the representation to the real (crime). The essay also highlights the way in which Ballard both cites and subverts some of the conventions of the Golden Age detective fiction, mainly by his rejection of the latter’s escapist ethos and by the liminal character of his investigator, at once part of a normalizing panoptic apparatus and eccentric to it, a “poetic figure” (Chesterton) relying on imagination and “aestheticizing” the routines of the detection process. Keywords: J.G. Ballard, Running Wild , gated community, Michel Foucault, Panopticon, surveillance, crime scene, video, forensic psychiatrist, tyrannicide, visibility, escapism J.G.
    [Show full text]
  • Members of the Political Science Department
    MEMBERS OF THE POLITICAL SCIENCE DEPARTMENT FACULTY PROFILES Vera Achvarina B.A. (Moscow), M.A., Ph.D. (Pittsburgh) Primary research interests: international relations, human security, mobilization of people for armed conflict, recruitment of children in wars. Secondary research interests: international norms (promotion, diffusion, effectiveness, commitment and compliance), and research methodology. In 2006 she was a visiting researcher at the International Peace Research Institute, Oslo, Norway. She has published several articles, including in the journal International Security. She is currently working on a book based on her dissertation. Emanuel Adler B.A., M.A. (Hebrew), Ph.D. (Berkeley) Andrea and Charles Bronfman Chair of Israeli Studies and editor of International Organization. Research interests: The international politics of identity and peace, rationality and the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, a constructivist reconsideration of strategic logic, including deterrence, in post-Cold War international security, the role of practice in international relations, European security institutions, and international relations theory, in particular, constructivism, epistemic communities, and security communities. Publications include: The Power of Ideology (1987); Progress in Postwar International Relations (with Beverly Crawford) (ed.) (1991); Security Communities (with Michael Barnett) (ed.) (1998); Communitarian International Relations (2005); Convergence of Civilizations (2006), ed. with Federica Bicchi, Beverly Crawford, and Raffaella Del Sarto, and articles in International Organization, the European Journal of International Relations, and the Review of International Studies. Current projects include a study of rationality and the Israeli-Palestinian conflict, a constructivist reconsideration of strategic logic, including deterrence, in post-Cold War international security, a project on a turn to practice in international relations, and a study of Europe as a civilizational state.
    [Show full text]
  • Preferences Under Pressure
    Eric Skoog Preferences Under Pressure Conflict, Threat Cues and Willingness to Compromise Dissertation presented at Uppsala University to be publicly examined in Zootissalen, EBC, Villavägen 9, Uppsala, Friday, 13 March 2020 at 10:15 for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy. The examination will be conducted in English. Faculty examiner: Associate Professor Thomas Zeitzoff (American University, School of Public Affairs). Abstract Skoog, E. 2020. Preferences Under Pressure. Conflict, Threat Cues and Willingness to Compromise. Report / Department of Peace and Conflict Research 121. 66 pp. Uppsala: Department of Peace and Conflict Research. ISBN 978-91-506-2805-0. Understanding how preferences are formed is a key question in the social sciences. The ability of agents to interact with each other is a prerequisite for well-functioning societies. Nevertheless, the process whereby the preferences of agents in conflict are formed have often been black boxed, and the literature on the effects of armed conflict on individuals reveals a great variation in terms of outcomes. Sometimes, individuals are willing to cooperate and interact even with former enemies, while sometimes, we see outright refusal to cooperate or interact at all. In this dissertation, I look at the role of threat in driving some of these divergent results. Armed conflict is rife with physical threats to life, limb and property, and there has been much research pointing to the impact of threat on preferences, attitudes and behavior. Research in the field of evolutionary psychology has revealed that threat is not a singular category, but a nuanced phenomenon, where different types of threat may lead to different responses.
    [Show full text]
  • Teaching Eighteenth-Century French Literature: the Good, the Bad and the Ugly
    Eighteenth-Century Modernities: Present Contributions and Potential Future Projects from EC/ASECS (The 2014 EC/ASECS Presidential Address) by Christine Clark-Evans It never occurred to me in my research, writing, and musings that there would be two hit, cable television programs centered in space, time, and mythic cultural metanarrative about 18th-century America, focusing on the 1760s through the 1770s, before the U.S. became the U.S. One program, Sleepy Hollow on the FOX channel (not the 1999 Johnny Depp film) represents a pre- Revolutionary supernatural war drama in which the characters have 21st-century social, moral, and family crises. Added for good measure to several threads very similar to Washington Irving’s “Legend of Sleepy Hollow” story are a ferocious headless horseman, representing all that is evil in the form of a grotesque decapitated man-demon, who is determined to destroy the tall, handsome, newly reawakened Rip-Van-Winkle-like Ichabod Crane and the lethal, FBI-trained, diminutive beauty Lt. Abigail Mills. These last two are soldiers for the politically and spiritually righteous in both worlds, who themselves are fatefully inseparable as the only witnesses/defenders against apocalyptic doom. While the main characters in Sleepy Hollow on television act out their protracted, violent conflict against natural and supernatural forces, they also have their own high production-level, R & B-laced, online music video entitled “Ghost.” The throaty feminine voice rocks back and forth to accompany the deft montage of dramatic and frightening scenes of these talented, beautiful men and these talented, beautiful women, who use as their weapons American patriotism, religious faith, science, and wizardry.
    [Show full text]
  • Digital Dystopia∗
    forthcoming, American Economic Review Digital Dystopia∗ Jean Tirole† December 17, 2020 Abstract: Autocratic regimes, democratic majorities, private platforms and religious or professional organizations can achieve social control by managing the flow of information about individuals' behavior. Bundling the agents' political, organizational or religious attitudes with information about their prosocial conduct makes them care about behaviors that they otherwise would not. The incorporation of the individuals' social graph in their social score further promotes soft control but destroys the social fabric. Both bundling and guilt by association are most effective in a society that has weak ties and is politically docile. Keywords: Social behavior, social score, platforms, strong and weak ties, social graph, mass surveillance, divisive issues, community enforcement. JEL numbers: D64, D80, K38. ∗This project received funding from the European Research Council (ERC) under the European Union's Horizon 2020 research and innovation programme (grant agreement no. 669217 - ERC MARK- LIM). Jean Tirole acknowledges funding from the French National Research Agency (ANR) under the Investments for the Future (Investissements d'Avenir) program, grant ANR-17-EURE-0010. The author gratefully acknowledges the financial support of the TSE Digital Center (the list of sponsors is available at https://www.tse-fr.eu/digital). Daron Acemoglu, Amirreza Ahmadzadeh, Roland B´enabou, Aim´e Bierdel, Erik Brynjolfsson, Sylvain Chassang, Bin Cheng, Johannes H¨orner,Paul-Henri Moisson, Charles P´ebereau, two anonymous referees, and participants at conferences (Luohan Academy conference on pri- vacy and data governance, IT & digitization and IO groups at NBER summer institute, 13th Toulouse conference on the digital economy, privacy conference at Princeton University), and at seminars at MIT, Northwestern, Tehran IAS, TSE and University of Auckland provided helpful comments.
    [Show full text]
  • Genopolitics and the Science of Genetics
    American Political Science Review Page 1 of 14 May 2013 doi:10.1017/S0003055413000099 Genopolitics and the Science of Genetics EVAN CHARNEY Duke University WILLIAM ENGLISH Harvard University nanearlierarticlewechallengedthefindingsofFowlerandDawes(FD)thattwogenespredictvoter turnout as part of a more general critique of “genopolitics.” FD now acknowledge that their finding I of a “significant” direct association between MAOA and voting was incorrect, but claim to have replicated their finding of an “indirect” association between 5HTT, self-reported church attendance, and self-reported voting. We show that this finding is likely driven by population stratification and omitted variable bias. We then explain why, from the standpoints of genetics, neuroscience, and evolutionary biology, genopolitics is a fundamentally misguided undertaking; we also respond to FD’s charge that some of our previous statements concerning genetics are “highly misleading,” “extremely disingenuous,” and “even incorrect.” We show that their criticisms demonstrate a lack of awareness of some basic principles in genetics and of discoveries in molecular genetics over the past 50 years. ewouldliketothanktheeditorsofThe Amer- STATISTICS ican Political Science Review for inviting us Wto participate in this Forum by writing a re- Population Stratification sponse (or “rejoinder”) to the articles of Fowler and Dawes and of Deppe, Stoltenberg, Smith, and Hibbing. Fowler and Dawes (FD) acknowledge that their previ- We view this as a welcome and important opportu- ous, highly publicized finding that a polymorphism of nity. Although we address both articles, our emphasis the MAOA gene showed a “significant” direct associ- throughout is on the contribution of Fowler and Dawes, ation with (self-reported) voter turnout was incorrect, which itself is intended, in part, as a rejoinder to our inasmuch as they failed to replicate it using new data earlier article in this journal, “Candidate Genes and from Wave IV of the National Longitudinal Study of Political Behavior” (Charney and English 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose Mcdermott, and Michael
    States in Mind States in Mind Anthony C. Lopez, Rose McDermott, and Evolution, Coalitional Psychology, and Michael Bang International Politics Petersen One of the most com- monly studied puzzles in international politics is the recurrence of coalitional competition and aggression between political groups such as states. Indeed, this pattern constitutes an enduring and central feature of all politics. Yet de- spite the tragic endurance of this leitmotif throughout history, its manifestation varies through time and space. Some wars are fought for honor or revenge, whereas others are ignited for mere opportunism or as a consequence of vari- ous misperceptions, whatever their source. We argue that evolutionary theory provides a theoretical framework that can explain both the stubborn endur- ance and dynamic diversity of coalitional behavior. Debate on the relevance of “human nature” and biological factors for explaining political behavior is not new.1 Yet the comprehensive value of evo- lutionary theory for explaining important aspects of international politics has not been adequately explicated. As we discuss below, this has in part been a consequence of general skepticism about the validity and scope of evolution- ary theory for explaining political behavior. We argue, however, that evolu- tionary psychology can generate falsiªable ex ante predictions that are of central interest to the study of international politics, and we offer several hy- potheses derived from this model to illustrate the depth of this approach. Evo- lutionary psychologists have already generated a large body of work that suggests that the human brain contains webs of psychological mechanisms, or adaptations, each designed to operate in domains relevant to modern politics, and which emerged as a product of natural selection.
    [Show full text]
  • The Human Choice: Individuation, Reason, and Impulse, and Chaos
    nebraska symposium on motivation 1969 The Human Choice: WILLIAM J. ARNOLD and DAVID LEVINE, Editors Individuation, Reason, and Order versus Deindividuation, Dalbir Bindra Professor of Psychology- McGill b'liiversily Impulse, and Chaos Professor of Psychology Edward L. Wike PHILIP G. ZIMBARDO University of Kansas Stanjord University Roger W. Black Professor of Psychology University oj South Carolina Omtrol. That’s what current psychology is all about. The use ol powerful schedules of reinforcement, probing neurophysiological techniques, computer simulation, and the new behavior therapies Elliot Aronson Professor of Psychology University of Texas at Austin (among other advances) enable psychologists to manipulate the responses of a wide range of research subjects in order to improve learning and discrimination; to arouse, rechannel, and satisfy Stuart W. Cook Professor of Psyi hology drives; and to redirect abnormal or deviant behavior. It has, in fact, Univernty of Colorado become the all-consuming task of most psychologists to learn how to bring behavior under stimulus control. Philip G. Zimbardo Prolessor of Psychology It is especially impressive to note (in any volume of the Nebraska Stanford University Symposium on Motivation) the relative case with which laboratory researchers can induce motives which have an immediate and often demonstrably pervasive effect on a vast array of response measures. On the other hand, one must reconcile this with the observation ’ that in the “real world” people often.show considerable tolerance university
    [Show full text]