Module 21: Types of Phonological Processes-I Linguistics

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Module 21: Types of Phonological Processes-I Linguistics Module 21: Types of Phonological processes-I Linguistics Introduction to Phonetics and Phonology Types of Phonological Processes-I Principal Investigator Prof. Pramod Pandey Centre for Linguistics, SLL&CS, Jawaharlal Nehru University, New Delhi-110067 Paper Coordinator Prof. Pramod Pandey Module ID & Name Lings_P2_M21; Types of Phonological Processes- I Content Writer Pramod Pandey Email id [email protected] Phone 011-26741258, -9810979446 Objectives: • To give a comprehensive account of the typology of phonological processes • To make students aware of the wide use of the processes in various fields dealing with phonology Contents: Introduction Types of phonological processes Whole segment phonological processes Co-occurrence Modification types of phonological processes 1 Syllable-based phonological processes Articulatory phonological processes: Strengthening and Weakening processes Summary 21.1 Introduction This module introduces the various types of phonological processes that are found in world languages. These are presented with definitions and examples. Although an attempt has been made to present the processes as comprehensively as possible, they are by no means exhaustive. A full treatment of the processes will take up more space than we have available in this course. Besides, there is need to distinguish between individual phonological processes and the classes of phonological processes. Many of the specific phonological processes can be grouped into more than one class. Essentially then it is the individual phonological processes that should be kept in mind. 21.2 Need for the study of phonological Processes Why should we study phonological processes? The following are some of reasons for the study of phonological processes: • Each language has a unique phonological system that makes use of phonological process that are specific to itself. The types of processes, however, are finite. This explains the universality of the processes and the phonological systems they give rise to. • Many disciplines, such as psycho-neurolinguistics, historical linguistics, language acquisition, etc. deal with phonology. Although they differ in their approaches, the processes are common to them. For a phonologist, it is thus necessary to know them in order to describe them in those disciplines. • In the study of the phonological system of a language, students are expected to name the phonological processes that they discover in the course of their study. 2 • It is assumed that students are familiar with the types of phonological processes that are discussed in phonological grammars and studies. 21.3 Types of Phonological Processes There are two main types of phonological processes- Whole Segment processes and Modification type processes. Whole segment processes are those processes that affect the segmental structure of the word, by deleting, inserting or changing segments from one contrasting segment type to another. These operate at the level of phonemes 21.4 Whole segment processes The whole segment processes are the following: • Insertion • Deletion • Syllabication • Desyllabication • Coalescence • Metathesis The whole-segment processes are discussed below. 21.4.1 Insertion Insertion takes place when one or more segments are added to a morpheme or a word. For example, /u/ insertion in Tamil. [u] insertion in Tamil (Vasanthakumari 1989) /u/ is inserted at the end of stems ending a consonant other than the palatal approximant /j/. E.g. (21/1) a. /u:t̪/ [u:t̪u] ‘to blow’ /ko:r/ [ko:ru] ‘to collect’ /a:ɭ/ [a:ɭu] ‘to rule’ /ʋe:r/ [ʋe:ru] ‘root’ /tu:n/ [tu:nu] ‘pillar’ /e:ʋ/ [e:ʋu] ‘to send’ 3 b. /pa:l/ [pa:lu] ‘milk’ /pe:r/ [pe:ru] ‘name’ /self/ [selfu] ‘self’ /te:n/ [te:nu] ‘honey’ Stem-final vowel insertion is found in many languages, for example, Japanese and Kannada. Depending on where in the word a segment is inserted, we have three different types of insertion Insertion at the beginning of words is called Prothesis, , e.g. /i/ epenthesis in Hindi dialects. Words beginning with a sC- cluster are pronounced with and initial /i/ in many dialects of Hindi as well as in other languages. For example, 21/2 /sku:l/ [isku:l] ‘school’ /sma:rək/ [isma:rək] ‘memorial’ /stri:/ [istri:] ‘woman’ Insertion in the middle of words is called Epenthesis. More precisely, epenthesis of vowels is called Anaptyxis, and epenthesis of consonants is called Excrescence. Examples of both are given below: Anaptyxis in borrowed words in Punjabi Borrowed words in Punjabi that have initial consonant clusters undergo /a/- Anaptyxis, e. g. 21/3 /sku:l/ [səku:l] ‘school’ /sma:rak/ [səma:rak] ‘memorial’ /praka:sh/ [pərka:sh] ‘light’ Insertion at the end of words is called Paragoge. /u/- insertion in Tamil is an example of paragoge. 4 An example of excrescence is found in Gondi: /son + u:s/ → /sondu:s/ ‘to lose’. 21.4.2 Deletion or Elision Deletion takes place when a segment is elided in a given context. For example, in Hindi, a schwa is deleted when it is occurs in an unstressed open syllable and flanked by syllables, of t which the preceding syllable is stressed, e.g. 21/4 /təbəla:/ [ˈtəbla:] ‘a percussion instrument’ /məŋɡəla:/ [ˈməŋɡla:] ‘(a name)’ /titəli:/ [ˈtitli:] ‘butterfly’ Depending on the position in the word where a segment is deleted, there are three types of Deletion. When a segment is deleted word-initially, we have aphesis oraphaeresis. This process is commonly found in historical change. For example- amend >mend, acute>cute, and alone>lone. When a segment is deleted word-medially, we have syncope. Schwa deletion in Hindi is an instance of syncope. When a segment is deleted word-finally, we have apocope. For example, in the present-day Hindi, the word-final schwa (kept in Sanskrit) is deleted: 21/5 /ra:ma/ [ra:m] ‘(a name)’ /kamala/ [kamal] ‘lotus’ /na:ma/ [na:m] ‘name, noun’ There are many example of apocope in British English. Two of them are Final /ɡ/-Deletion and Medial /r/- Deletion. Thus, in words ending in /ŋɡ/, the final /ɡ/ is elided: /sɪŋɡ/ > [sɪŋ], /brɪŋɡ/ > [brɪŋ], etc. And /r/ at the end of words is deleted, e.g. /fa:r/ [fa:] ‘far’, /rɪvər/ [rɪvə] ‘river’, etc. 5 The /r/-Deletion rule in English also takes place when /r/ is followed by a consonant, as in bird, turning. What type of deletion is the /r/-Deletion rule in this context? 21. 4.3 Syllabication Syllabication is the process whereby a consonant comes to occupy the position of the nucleus of a syllable. This typically happens when a vowel is deleted between a sonorant consonant another consonant. The sonorant consonant then becomes syllabic. A syllabic consonant is transcribed with a subsript [ ̩] below the consonant, as for example, in Chokri: 21/6 /məbo4/ [m̩ bo4] ‘kiss’ /mədi4/ [m̩ di4] > [n̩di4] ‘to loose’ English has /m̩ n̩ l̩/ and /r̩/ (in American English) as syllabic consonants in words such as button, bottom, bottle and butter, pronounced as /bɐtn̩/, /bɔtm̩ /, /bɔtll̩/ and /bɑtr̩/, respectively. In second language varieties like those of Indian English, there are no syllabic consonants. The syllabic consonants are pronounced as non-syllabic consonants with a following vowel /ə/, as in Hindi English /bəʈən/, /bɔ:ʈəm/, /bɔ:ʈəl/ and /bəʈər/. 21.4.4 Desyllabication Desyllabication takes place when a vowel or a syllabic consonant becomes non- syllabic. In many languages, that do not permit a sequence of two vowels, one of the vowels becomes non-syllabic (and transcibed with a subscript [ ̯ ] or changes to a semi-vowel, among other possible processes. Thus, in Bhili (Kulkarni 1976: 44), a stem-final /i/ changes to /j/ when followed by /a:/, e.g. kaʈhi + a/ [kaThja/ ‘sticks’, /ʈopi + a/ [ʈopja] ‘caps’. In Ao, an/i/ changes to [j] after an /a/, e.g /pá + i/ [páj/ ‘3PER-M-S-NOM’, i.e ‘he’/lá + i/ [láj] ‘3PER-F-S-NOM’ , i.e. ‘she’. Examples of a syllabic consonant becoming non-syllabic in related words are commonly found in English, as in [krɪpling] ‘crippling’ (< /krɪpl/̩ ‘cripple’), /smɐɡling/ ‘smuggling’ (</smɐɡl,/̩ ‘smuggle’). The process applies optionally in these words in many varieties of English, which means both the pronunciations, with syllabic as well as non-syllabic sonorants, are found to exist. 6 21.4.5 Coalescence Coalescence takes place when two segments are fused to give rise to a third segment. In a lot of languages, vowel coalescence takes place as a result of a prohibition against the occurrence of a sequence of vowels, as one of the options (deletion of one of the vowels or insertion of an approximant or glottal stop, being other options). In these situations, the phenomenon is commonly known as Vowel Sandhi, as in Sanskrit, for example, /ə +i/ > /e:/ , /a: +i/ > /e:/, /a: +i:/ > /e:/, /ə +u/ > /o:/, /a: +u/ > /o:/, /a: +u:/ > /o:/. E.g. /rəma: +i:sh/ > /rame:sh/ ‘lord of Rama’, /məha: + utsəʋ/ /məho:tsəʋ/ ‘ a great celebration’. In Paite, before any consonant except glottal stop /ʔ/ diphthongs /ua/ and /ia/ change to single vowels /o/ and /e/, respectively. This is an example of phonetic coalescence. 21/7 Paite vowel coalescence Stem Derived Form /ɡuā/ ‘bamboo’ /pʰěl/ ‘(to) untie’ /ɡó pʰěl/ ‘catapult’ /vuâ/‘(to) beat’ /kə̄p/ ‘(to) cry’ /vōkə̄p/ ‘(to) beat to cry’ /kiâ/ ‘(to) fall’ /sūk/ ‘steep’ /kēsūk/ ‘(to) fall downwards’ /biâ/ ‘(to) worship’ /t̪â:/ ‘non – future’ /bét̪â:/ ‘has worshipped’ A well-known example of the process of consonantal coalescence is found Indonesian (Halle & Clements 1983: 125) 21/8 Indosian consonant coalscence /məŋ-pilih/ →məmilih ‘to choose’ /məŋ-tulis/ →mənulis ‘to write’ /məŋ-kasih/ →məŋasih ‘to give’ Coalescence also takes place between tones in tonal languages. For example, in Hausa (Leben 2009) , when a word ends in a falling tone and the following word begins with a rising tone, although the first vowel is deleted, a new tone emerges, as if to preserve the tones of the independent words.
Recommended publications
  • Chapter One Phonetic Change
    CHAPTERONE PHONETICCHANGE The investigation of the nature and the types of changes that affect the sounds of a language is the most highly developed area of the study of language change. The term sound change is used to refer, in the broadest sense, to alterations in the phonetic shape of segments and suprasegmental features that result from the operation of phonological process es. The pho- netic makeup of given morphemes or words or sets of morphemes or words also may undergo change as a by-product of alterations in the grammatical patterns of a language. Sound change is used generally to refer only to those phonetic changes that affect all occurrences of a given sound or class of sounds (like the class of voiceless stops) under specifiable phonetic conditions . It is important to distinguish between the use of the term sound change as it refers tophonetic process es in a historical context , on the one hand, and as it refers to phonetic corre- spondences on the other. By phonetic process es we refer to the replacement of a sound or a sequenceof sounds presenting some articulatory difficulty by another sound or sequence lacking that difficulty . A phonetic correspondence can be said to exist between a sound at one point in the history of a language and the sound that is its direct descendent at any subsequent point in the history of that language. A phonetic correspondence often reflects the results of several phonetic process es that have affected a segment serially . Although phonetic process es are synchronic phenomena, they often have diachronic consequences.
    [Show full text]
  • (1'9'6'8'""159-83 and in Later Publica- (1972 539
    FINAL WEAKENING AND RELATED PHENOMENA1 Hans Henrich Hock University of Illinois at Urbana-Champaign 1: Final devoicing (FD) 1.1. In generative phonology, it is a generally accepted doctrine that, since word-final devoicing (WFD) is a very common and natural phenomenon, the ob- verse phenomenon, namely word-final voicin~ should not be found in natural language. Compare for instance Postal 1968 184 ('in the context----'~ the rules always devoice rather than voice'), Stampe 1969 443-5 (final devoicing comes about as the result of a failure t~ suppress the (innate) process of final devoicing), Vennemann 1972 240-1 (final voicing, defined as a process increasing the complexity of affected segment~ 'does not occur.')o 1.2 One of the standard examples for WFD is that of German, cf. Bund Bunde [bUnt] [bUndeJ. However Vennemann (1'9'6'8'""159-83 and in later publica- tions) and, following him, Hooper (1972 539) and Hyman (1975 142) have convincingly demonstrated that in Ger- man, this process applies not only word-finally, but also syllable-finally, as in radle [ra·t$le]3 'go by bike' (in some varieties of German). The standard view thus must be modified so as to recognize at least one other process, namely syllable-final devoicing (SFD). (For a different eA""Planation of this phenomenon compare section 2.3 below.) 2· Final voicing (or tenseness neutralization) 2 1 A more important argument against the stan- dard view, however, is that, as anyone with any train- ing in Indo-European linguistics can readily tell, there is at least one 5roup, namely Italic, where there is evidence for the allegedly impossible final voicing, cf PIE *siyet > OLat.
    [Show full text]
  • Phonological Processes
    Phonological Processes Phonological processes are patterns of articulation that are developmentally appropriate in children learning to speak up until the ages listed below. PHONOLOGICAL PROCESS DESCRIPTION AGE ACQUIRED Initial Consonant Deletion Omitting first consonant (hat → at) Consonant Cluster Deletion Omitting both consonants of a consonant cluster (stop → op) 2 yrs. Reduplication Repeating syllables (water → wawa) Final Consonant Deletion Omitting a singleton consonant at the end of a word (nose → no) Unstressed Syllable Deletion Omitting a weak syllable (banana → nana) 3 yrs. Affrication Substituting an affricate for a nonaffricate (sheep → cheep) Stopping /f/ Substituting a stop for /f/ (fish → tish) Assimilation Changing a phoneme so it takes on a characteristic of another sound (bed → beb, yellow → lellow) 3 - 4 yrs. Velar Fronting Substituting a front sound for a back sound (cat → tat, gum → dum) Backing Substituting a back sound for a front sound (tap → cap) 4 - 5 yrs. Deaffrication Substituting an affricate with a continuant or stop (chip → sip) 4 yrs. Consonant Cluster Reduction (without /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (grape → gape) Depalatalization of Final Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the end of a word (dish → dit) 4 - 6 yrs. Stopping of /s/ Substituting a stop sound for /s/ (sap → tap) 3 ½ - 5 yrs. Depalatalization of Initial Singles Substituting a nonpalatal for a palatal sound at the beginning of a word (shy → ty) Consonant Cluster Reduction (with /s/) Omitting one or more consonants in a sequence of consonants (step → tep) Alveolarization Substituting an alveolar for a nonalveolar sound (chew → too) 5 yrs.
    [Show full text]
  • Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents
    EmanQuotah Linguistics Senior Paper Hadass Sheffer, Advisor Swarthmore College December 9, 1994 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation Table of Contents Introduction 1 TheQuran 3 Recitation and Tajwi:d 7 Nasal Assimilation in Quranic Recitation 10 Arabic geminates 12 Nasal assimilation rules 15 Blocking of assimilation by pauses 24 Conclusion 26 Bibliography Grateful acknowledgements to my father, my mother and my brothers, and to Hadass Sheffer and Donna Jo Napoli. Introduction This paper is concerned with the analysis of certain rules governing nasality and nasal assimilation during recitation of the holy Quran. I These rules are a subset of tajwi:d, a set of rules governing the correct prescribed recitation and pronunciation of the Islamic scriptures. The first part of the paper will describe the historical and cultural importance of the Quran and tajwi:d, with the proposition that a tension or conflict between the necessity for clarity and enunciation and the desire for beautification of the divine words of God is the driving force behind tajwi:d's importance. Though the rules are functional rather than "natural," these prescriptive rules can be integrated into a study lexical phonology and feature geometry, as discussed in the second section, since prescriptive rules must work within those rules set by the language's grammar. Muslims consider the Quran a divine and holy text, untampered with and unchangeable by humankind. Western scholars have attempted to identify it as the writings of the Prophet Muhammad, a humanly written text like any other. Viewing the holy Quran in this way ignores the religious, social and linguistic implications of its perceived unchangeability, and does disservice to the beliefs of many Muslims.
    [Show full text]
  • The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective
    Passage2013, 1(2), 35-44 The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective By: Novita Arsillah English Language and Literature Program (E-mail: [email protected] / mobile: +6281312203723) ABSTRACT This study is a historical linguistic investigation entitled The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective which aims to explore the types of sound changes found in Palembang Malay. The investigation uses a historical linguistic comparative method to compare the phonemic and syllabic changes between an ancestral language Standard Malay and its decent language Palembang Malay. Standard Malay refers to the Wilkinson dictionary in 1904. The participants of this study are seven native speakers of Palembang Malay whose ages range from 20 to 40 years old. The data were collected from the voices of the participants that were recorded along group conversations and interviews. This study applies the theoretical framework of sound changes which proposed by Terry Crowley in 1997 and Lily Campbell in 1999. The findings show that there are nine types of sound changes that were found as the results, namely assimilation (42.35%), lenition (20%), sound addition (3.53%), metathesis (1.18%), dissimilation (1.76%), abnormal sound changes (3.53%), split (13.53%), vowel rising (10.59%), and monophthongisation (3.53%). Keywords: Historical linguistics, standard Malay, Palembang Malay, comparative method, sound change, phoneme, syllable. 35 Novita Arsillah The Phonemic − Syllabic Comparisons of Standard Malay and Palembang Malay Using a Historical Linguistic Perspective INTRODUCTION spelling system in this study since it is considered to be the first Malay spelling This study classifies into the system that is used widely in Malaya, field of historical linguistics that Singapore, and Brunei (Omar, 1989).
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Sound Change
    An articulatory theory of sound change An articulatory theory of sound change Hypothesis: Most common initial motivation for sound change is the automation of production. Tokens reduced online, are perceived as reduced and represented in the exemplar cluster as reduced. Therefore we expect sound changes to reflect a decrease in gestural magnitude and an increase in gestural overlap. What are some ways to test the articulatory model? The theory makes predictions about what is a possible sound change. These predictions could be tested on a cross-linguistic database. Sound changes that take place in the languages of the world are very similar (Blevins 2004, Bateman 2000, Hajek 1997, Greenberg et al. 1978). We should consider both common and rare changes and try to explain both. Common and rare changes might have different characteristics. Among the properties we could look for are types of phonetic motivation, types of lexical diffusion, gradualness, conditioning environment and resulting segments. Common vs. rare sound change? We need a database that allows us to test hypotheses concerning what types of changes are common and what types are not. A database of sound changes? Most sound changes have occurred in undocumented periods so that we have no record of them. Even in cases with written records, the phonetic interpretation may be unclear. Only a small number of languages have historic records. So any sample of known sound changes would be biased towards those languages. A database of sound changes? Sound changes are known only for some languages of the world: Languages with written histories. Sound changes can be reconstructed by comparing related languages.
    [Show full text]
  • Assimilation, Reduction and Elision Reflected in the Selected Song Lyrics of Avenged Sevenfold
    Dwi Nita Febriyanti Assimilation, Reduction and Elision Reflected in the Selected Song Lyrics of Avenged Sevenfold Dwi Nita Febriyanti [email protected] English Language Studies, Sanata Dharma University Abstract This paper discusses the phenomena of phonological rules, especially assimilation, reduction and elision processes. In this paper, the writer conducted phonological study which attempts to find the phenomena of those processes in song lyrics. In taking the data, the writer transcribed the lyrics of the songs, along with checking them to the internet source, then observed the lyrics to find the phenomena of assimilation, reduction, and elision. After that, she classified the observed phenomena in the lyrics based on the phonological processes. From the data analysis, the results showed that there were three processes found both in the first and second songs: assimilation, reduction and elision. The difference is that in the first song, it has four kinds of assimilation, while from the second song only has three kinds of assimilation. Keywords: assimilation, reduction, elision Introduction brothers and sisters’ discussion or even in songs, for which songs are considered as the As English spoken by the native media for the composer to share his feelings. speakers, it sometimes undergoes simplification to ease the native speakers in Assimilation usually happens in the expressing their feelings. That is why, it is double consonants. This is a phenomenon common for them to speak English in high which shows the influence of one sound to speed along with their emotions. As the another to become more similar. While for result, they make a ‘shortcut’ to get ease of the reduction process, it can happen to the their pronunciation.
    [Show full text]
  • Using Coarticulation Rules in Automatic Phonetic Transcription
    Using Coarticulation Rules in Automatic Phonetic Transcription Arlindo Veiga 1, Sara Candeias 1, Luís Sá 1,2 , Fernando Perdigão 1,2 1 Instituto de Telecomunicações, Pólo de Coimbra 2 Universidade de Coimbra – DEEC, Pólo II, P-3030-290 Coimbra, Portugal {aveiga, saracandeias, luis, fp}@co.it.pt Abstract. Phonetic transcription of continuous speech databases is an important and necessary task for speech synthesis and recognition. Manual transcription requires well-trained experts and is time-consuming. So, it becomes more and more usual to implement automatic or semi-automatic transcription procedures. However, it is well known that pronunciation variations and coarticulation phenomena involve serious problems for automatic transcribers. This paper describes an approach to automatically label a continuous speech database at phonetic level using orthographic transcription (word level) and a set of pre- built acoustic models to deal with pronunciation variations. These pronunciation variations arise from coarticulation phenomena. Common intra- word and trans-word coarticulation phenomena will be described as well. Keywords: Automatic phonetic transcription, Coarticulation, Pronunciation variation 1 Introduction Careful manual transcription of a speech database produces best results comparing to automatic transcriptions. However, it requires a lot of resources and is usually a forbidden option to transcribe a large corpus. In [1] the authors refer more than one hundred times real-time to do manual transcriptions. Automatic or semi-automatic methods are often used to perform this task with accuracies that tend to manual transcriptions after several iterations. However, particularly in continuous speech, multi-pronunciations and coarticulation must be considered in the automatic transcriptions. Most of the work in this area, use forced alignment to deal with multi- pronunciation.
    [Show full text]
  • Acquisition of English /S/-Clusters by Brazilian Portuguese Speakers Carolyn June Baker Florida International University
    Florida International University FIU Digital Commons MA in Linguistics Final Projects College of Arts, Sciences & Education 4-12-2017 Acquisition of English /S/-Clusters by Brazilian Portuguese Speakers Carolyn June Baker Florida International University Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/linguistics_ma Part of the Linguistics Commons Recommended Citation Baker, Carolyn June, "Acquisition of English /S/-Clusters by Brazilian Portuguese Speakers" (2017). MA in Linguistics Final Projects. 1. https://digitalcommons.fiu.edu/linguistics_ma/1 This work is brought to you for free and open access by the College of Arts, Sciences & Education at FIU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in MA in Linguistics Final Projects by an authorized administrator of FIU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. FLORIDA INTERNATIONAL UNIVERSITY Miami, Florida ACQUISITION OF ENGLISH /S/-CLUSTERS BY BRAZILIAN PORTUGUESE SPEAKERS A master’s project submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree MASTER OF ART in LINGUISTICS by Carolyn June Baker 2017 FIU LINGUISTICS PROGRAM MA PROJECT FINAL SUBMISSION To: Director, Linguistics Program College of Arts, Sciences and Education This MA Project, written by Carolyn June Baker, and entitled Acquisition of English /s/-clusters by Brazilian Portuguese Speakers, having been approved in respect to style and intellectual content, is referred to you for judgment. We have read this MA Project and recommend that it be approved. _______________________________________ Virginia C. Mueller Gathercole _______________________________________ Feryal Yavaş _______________________________________ Mehmet Yavaş, Major Professor Date of Defense: April 12, 2017 The MA Project of Carolyn June Baker is approved. _________________________________________ Prof. Virginia C.
    [Show full text]
  • PL2 Production of English Word-Final Consonants: the Role of Orthography and Learner Profile Variables
    L2 Production of English word-final consonants... PL2 PRODUCTION OF ENGLISH WORD-FINAL CONSONANTS: THE ROLE OF ORTHOGRAPHY AND LEARNER PROFILE VARIABLES PRODUÇÃO DE CONSOANTES FINAIS DO INGLÊS COMO L2: O PAPEL DA ORTOGRAFIA E DE VARIÁVEIS RELACIONADAS AO PERFIL DO APRENDIZ Rosane Silveira* ABSTRACT The present study investigates some factors affecting the acquisition of second language (L2) phonology by learners with considerable exposure to the target language in an L2 context. More specifically, the purpose of the study is two-fold: (a) to investigate the extent to which participants resort to phonological processes resulting from the transfer of L1 sound-spelling correspondence into the L2 when pronouncing English word-final consonants; and (b) to examine the relationship between rate of transfer and learner profile factors, namely proficiency level, length of residence in the L2 country, age of arrival in the L2 country, education, chronological age, use of English with native speakers, attendance in EFL courses, and formal education. The investigation involved 31 Brazilian speakers living in the United States with diverse profiles. Data were collected using a questionnaire to elicit the participants’ profiles, a sentence-reading test (pronunciation measure), and an oral picture-description test (L2 proficiency measure). The results indicate that even in an L2 context, the transfer of L1 sound-spelling correspondence to the production of L2 word-final consonants is frequent. The findings also reveal that extensive exposure to rich L2 input leads to the development of proficiency and improves production of L2 word-final consonants. Keywords: L2 consonant production; grapho-phonological transfer; learner profile. RESUMO O presente estudo examina fatores que afetam a produção de consoantes em segunda língua (L2) por aprendizes que foram consideravelmente expostos à língua-alvo em um contexto de L2.
    [Show full text]
  • Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition
    Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 Types of Sound Change Lenition (1) Stronger Weaker voiced voiceless (sometimes) voiceless voiced (sometimes) stops fricatives obstruents sonorants consonants semivowels oral glottal front/back central (2) Lentition: Stronger > Weaker Kara (New Ireland) *bulan > fulan ‘moon’ *tapine > tefin ‘woman’ *punti > fut ‘banana’ (3) Rhotacism: C > r Latin *ami:kosum > ami:korum ‘of friends’ *genesis > generis ‘of the type’ Sound Loss (4) Aphaeresis: initial consonant deletion Angkamuthi (Cape York) *maji > aji ‘food’ *nani > ani ‘ground’ *wapun > apun ‘head’ (5) Apocope: final vowel loss S.E. Ambrym (Vanutu) *utu > ut ‘lice’ *aŋo > aŋ ‘fly’ *asue > asu ‘rat’ *tohu > toh ‘sugarcane’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Winter 2013 (6) Syncope: medial vowel loss Lenakel (Vanutu) *namatana > nɨmrɨn ‘his/her eye’ (note other changes) *nalimana > nelmɨn ‘his/her hand’ *masa > mha ‘low tide’ (7) Cluster reduction: CC >C English Melanesian Pidgin dɪstɹɪkt > distrik ‘district’ poʊst > pos ‘post’ peɩnt > pen ‘paint’ tæŋk > taŋ ‘tank’ (8) Haplology: syllable loss (conditioned by adjacent similar syllable) laɩbɹəɹi > laɩbɹi ‘library’ Anglaland > England Sound Addition (9) Excrescence: consonant addition *æmtig > ɛmpti ‘empty’ *θymle > θɪmbl ‘thimble’ (10) Epenthtesis: vowel addition English Tok Pisin blæk > bilak ‘black’ blu: > bulu ‘blue’ sɪks > sikis ‘six’ (11) Prothesis: initial sound addition Moru (Papua New Guinea) *api > lahi ‘fire’ *asan > lada ‘gills’ *au > lau ‘I, me’ Ling 150, Historical Linguistics Moore, Spring 2011 Metathesis Sounds switch places. (12) *brid > *bird > bɚd æsk > æks (13) Tagalog Ilokano taŋis : sa:ŋit ‘cry’ tubus : subut ‘redeem’ tigis : si:git ‘decant’ tamis : samqit ‘sweet’ Fusion Two sounds become one, bearing features of both original sounds.
    [Show full text]
  • Contrast and Post-Velar Fronting in Russian*
    [To appear in Natural Language and Linguistic Theory] Contrast and Post-Velar Fronting in Russian* Jaye Padgett - University of California, Santa Cruz There is a well-known rule of Russian whereby /i/ is said to be realized as [Y] after non-palatalized consonants. Somewhat less well-known is another allophonic rule of Russian whereby only [i], and not [Y], can follow velars within a morphological word. This latter rule came about due to a sound change in East Slavic called post- velar fronting here: kY > kji (and similarly for the other velars). This paper examines this sound change in depth, and argues that it can be adequately explained only by appeal to the functional notions of perceptual distinctiveness of contrast and neutralization avoidance. Further, these notions crucially require a systemic approach to phonology, in which the wellformedness of any form must be evaluated with reference to the larger system of contrasts it enters into. These notions are formalized in a modified version of Dispersion Theory (Flemming 1995a), a systemic theory that incorporates these functional notions into Optimality Theory (Prince and Smolensky 1993). 1. Introduction It is well known that Russian consonants contrast in palatalization. Also well known are certain allophonic rules connected to this palatalization contrast. One such rule requires that the phoneme /i/ be realized as high central unrounded [Y] after non-palatalized consonants, and [i] elsewhere, e.g., bjitj 'to beat' from /bjitj/ versus bYtj 'to be' from /bitj/. Another concerns velar consonants, which are more limited in their ability to contrast in palatalization: within words, velars can be followed only by [i] and not by [Y], e.g., xjitrYj 'clever' from /xitrij/, cf.
    [Show full text]