Aquatic Invasions (2019) Volume 14 Article in press

Special Issue: Behaviour in Aquatic Invasions Guest editors: Kit Magellan, Amy Deacon, Marian Wong and Mhairi Alexander

CORRECTED PROOF

Book Review Highlights and Insights from “Biological Invasions and Behaviour.”

Daniel Sol1 and Judith S. Weis2,* 1CREAF Centre for Ecological Research and Forestry Applications, Barcelona, Catalonia, Spain 2Department of Biological Sciences, Rutgers University, Newark, NJ 07102, USA E-mail: [email protected] (DS), [email protected] (JSW) *Corresponding author

Co-Editors’ Note: This paper is a contri- bution to the Behaviour in Aquatic Abstract Invasions Special Issue of Aquatic Invasions. Papers in this Special Issue Although behavior has long been considered central in understanding the causes and explore how behaviour contributes to consequences of animal invasions, “Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour” invasion success; native species’ represents the first attempt to summarize the major advances in a single book. The behavioural strategies that reduce the book is a clear demonstration that behavior influences almost all facets of the impacts of invasions; how knowledge of behaviour can enhance management invasion process. However, a common theme of the book is that general rules are of invasive species; and potential effects rare. The role of behavior changes through the stages of the invasion process and of climate change on the behavioural within each stage can be highly context-dependent, implying that there may be impacts of aquatic invasive species. several ways of being a successful invader. Despite the scarcity of general rules,

Citation: Sol D, Weis JS (2019) Highlights there are two recurring generalization that emerge from the book. One is the central and Insights from “Biological Invasions importance of behavioral plasticity in facilitating establishment and spread. The and Animal Behaviour.” Aquatic Invasions other is the appreciation that behavior is part of a suite of traits closely linked to life 14 (in press) history, whose effects on population dynamics vary as a function of population size Received: 2 October 2018 and degree of adaptive mismatch. Although aquatic ecologists have largely Accepted: 8 February 2019 contributed to developing these ideas, their research has often been restricted to a Published: 24 May 2019 few study systems and has mostly neglected mechanisms. Aquatic ecologists have, however, led research on the impact of invaders, using invasions as “unintended” Handling editor: Mhairi Alexander experiments for examining the consequences of novel species interactions. This Copyright: © Sol and Weis research unambiguously demonstrates that behavior is central to understanding how This is an open access article distributed under terms invaders alter native communities, notably by shaping predator–prey interactions. of the Creative Commons Attribution License (Attribution 4.0 International - CC BY 4.0). We believe that aquatic can continue providing research opportunities to further improve our understanding of the role of behavior in biological invasions, OPEN ACCESS. and we hope that “Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour” may serve to encourage new research avenues.

Key words: behavioral syndromes, plasticity, learning, foraging, dispersal, boldness, predator/prey interactions

Behavior—the motor response to sensory information—has long been considered essential for understanding why some species succeed in new environments and what impacts they will have once established (Suarez and Cassey 2016). In the classic volume “Genetics of Colonizing Species,” Ernst Mayr (1965) highlighted the importance of behavior in defining the nature of successful invaders. For Mayr, behaviors such as the tendency to discover unoccupied habitats and the ability to shift habitat preferences were defining features of invasive species. Over 40 years later, researchers

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

have not only confirmed Mayr’s speculations on the central importance of behavior in invasions but have also found that the role of behavior is pervasive in almost all facets of biological invasions. The role of behavior in animal invasions is more complex than previously thought, however. For example, we now know that the behaviors that help negotiate particular stages of the invasion process (Figure 1) may have little impact, or even be detrimental, at other stages (Chapple and Wong 2016). Our edited book “Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour”, published in 2016 by Cambridge University Press, summarizes the major advances. It contains contributions from researchers throughout the world who study the role of behavior in the invasion process from a variety of perspectives, approaches and study systems. Here, we summarize the most relevant contributions from the perspective of aquatic animals. Specifically, we discuss four major themes: 1) how the plastic nature of behavior assists individuals to thrive in novel environments, 2) how variation across individuals in less plastic behavior may also contribute to establishment by increasing the chances that some individuals are suitable to deal with the novel challenges and by facilitating evolutionary adjustments, 3) whether behavior should be considered as part of a life history syndrome to cope with environmental changes, and 4) how behavior helps predict the impact of biological invasions.

The importance of behavioral plasticity to thrive in novel environments Novel environments confront animals with a wide variety of challenges, such as the need to adopt new foods or to avoid unfamiliar enemies. As behavior is an important way in which animals interact with their environment, the likelihood of establishment is expected to largely depend on the ability of individuals to adjust behavior to the new conditions. One way to do so is through decision-making. By choosing resources and habitats that better suit their phenotype, animals may enhance their fitness in novel environments (Sol and Maspons 2016; Sol et al. 2013). While aquatic ecologists have fundamentally contributed to our current understanding of the causes and consequences of such “Environmental matching choice”, the importance of this process in biological invasions has been under- appreciated. Instead, the importance of broad habitat and resource tolerances has attracted more interest, perhaps because many introduced animals are ecological generalists. In chapter 16, Tricarico and Acquiloni (2016) describe how alien crayfish occupy diverse habitats and tolerate a wide range of environmental conditions, features that are crucial for actively spreading to new habitats. Crayfish are omnivorous and opportunistic feeders, consuming a wide range of food items such as macrophytes, live macroinvertebrates, fish carrion, periphyton, detritus,

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Figure 1. Stages of invasion and corresponding behavioural traits in the guppy. From Deacon and Magurran (2016) courtesy of Cambridge University Press.

molted conspecifics, small live fish and tadpoles (Gherardi 2007). The red swamp crayfish Procambarus clarkii (Girard, 1852) for example, feeds on the diverse items in an invaded habitat in proportion to their availability so that its diet can change with habitats.

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Although environmental matching choice should generally improve fitness in novel environments, it may also lead animals to prefer to settle in poor-quality habitats if the choice reflects the expression of a prewired genetic program and the cues used to make decisions have changed in the new environment. The existence of such ecological traps has been documented in sea turtles. Their hatchlings rely on light cues from the open horizon to orient after emerging, but light pollution from beachfront structures may lead them to migrate inland instead of towards the ocean (Witherington 1997). The existence of ecological traps highlights that even though the expression of innate behavioral responses provides certain plasticity to cope with environmental changes, it may not be enough to cope with many of the challenges of novel environments. However, animals can respond to novel challenges in yet another way, through learning. In a novel environment, animals need to learn things such as which foods are edible and safe, which habitats are safe and appropriate for their needs, where good breeding sites are located or which species are predators and how to avoid them (Griffin et al. 2016; Sol and Maspons 2016; Sol et al. 2005). Crayfish quickly learn to feed on unknown prey: in the laboratory, naive individuals of Procambarus clarkii require less than 12 hrs to learn to maximize capture rate of unfamiliar larvae (Ramalho and Anastacio 2011). Learning may thus facilitate establishment in novel environments by reducing environmental uncertainties and adaptive mismatches between the phenotype and the environment (Sol and Maspons 2016). Once acquired, novel learned behaviors may be rapidly transmitted to other individuals through social learning (Griffin et al. 2016; Sol and Maspons 2016). Focusing on guppies, Poecilia reticulata, Deacon and Magurran (2016) show how behavioral flexibility resulting from both individual innovation and social learning contributes to the invasion success of this species (Figure 1). As an omnivore, the guppy has dietary flexibility and can capitalize on whatever food is available in a new habitat. They can learn to navigate a maze to reach food, and to consume novel foods (Laland and Reader 1999). Using social learning, an individual guppy can learn the route to a new food source by following conspecifics through a maze. This ability might be related to their evolutionary history in variable environments. In their native habitat, guppies repeatedly move from downstream sites with higher predation to upstream sites with lower predation, and then wash back down again. Deacon and Magurran (2016) argue that this exposure to conditions of naturally varying risk might have selected for the flexibility to cope with different environments and predator regimes. Learning can also facilitateinvaders acquiring knowledge from more experienced native species on key issues such as how to exploit or to avoid enemies (see Sol and Maspons 2016). In aquatic animals, the use of such

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

public information has been documented in the context of predator avoidance. The invasive P. clarkii decreases its risk of predation in new environments by quickly learning predator avoidance behaviors from heterospecifics. The species appears to be more sensitive to predator presence than native species, using a broader range of information about predation risk and reacting more strongly to heterospecific alarm cues that elicit the typical alarm reaction (i.e. stop movements to avoid being detected). Individuals of the invasive virile crayfish, Orconectes virilis respond similarly to alarm cues from conspecifics, sympatric heterospecifics and novel heterospecifics, suggesting that these animals use a wider range of information about their environment, and enter a new habitat with the ability to respond adaptively to a wide range of predators (Hazlett et al. 2002, 2003).

The personality of successful invaders While the importance for invasions of behavioral plasticity is without doubt, the common view that successful invaders should be characterized by particular behavioral types is less clear. For example, the view that boldness and aggressiveness should be major features of successful invaders has proved difficult to demonstrate empirically, in part because the contexts in which they apply may not be so common (e.g. Sol et al. 2012). Still, the book presents convincing evidence that certain personality traits, i.e. behavioral traits that are consistent within individuals over time (Sih et al. 2004, 2012), are highly relevant in particular contexts (Chapple and Wong 2016). For example, a spreading population encounters novel conditions and dispersal barriers (e.g. obstacles, natural geographic and human-made features, unsuitable habitat) that must be overcome in order to be successful and become established in new areas. Behaviors that may be relevant to cope with these difficulties include dispersal tendencies, aggressiveness, boldness, activity, exploratory behavior and sociability. Rehage et al. (2016) draw from their studieson the mosquitofish, one of the 100 worst invasive species in the world, to discuss the importance of personality in spreading. Only some species of Gambusia are, in fact, invasive. The invasive ones exhibit greater exploratory tendency (Rehage and Sih 2004) and foraging efficiency but do not differ in boldness from their noninvasive congeners. The existence of individual variation in behavioral types within a population can also be highly relevant to understand how animals negotiate the different stages of the invasion process, as this increases the likelihood that at least some individuals have the appropriate behavior to negotiate the sequential filters of the invasion process (Wong and Candolin 2015). In mosquitofish, among individual variation in aggressiveness and sociability has been related to range expansion of successfully established

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Figure 2. Diagram of how personality type can affect dispersal. From Rehage et al. (2016), with permission from Cambridge University Press.

populations. Sociability affects the tendency to form shoals (Cote et al. 2010) and is likely to reduce dispersal, while asocial, aggressive individuals are more likely to disperse (Figure 2). Thus, more aggressive and asocial individuals are more likely to invade new habitats and establish populations. While the existence of variation in behavior among individuals has been amply documented for aquatic animals, the general importance of this variation for the diferent stages of the invasion process is generally unknown due to the paucity of empirical evidence.

Behavior as part of a life history syndrome It is increasingly appreciated that behavior should not be considered in isolation but as part of a broader complex of adaptive traits that mediate the response of individuals to new environments. Although the importance of behavioral syndromes is discussed in several chapters, Sol and Maspons (Chapter 5; see also Phillips 2016) extend the idea to consider behavioral responses as part of a pace-of-life history syndrome to cope with responses to environmental changes (Figure 3). In a context of environmental uncertainties and displacement from the adaptive optimum, theory predicts that a life- history strategy that gives priority to future rather than current reproduction either through a long reproductive life or reproducing several times in a same season should be particularly beneficial. Such a future returns strategy reduces the costs of a reproductive failure by spreading reproductive effort over many events (i.e. avoiding “putting all the eggs in a single basket,”) and by enabling delaying reproduction if conditions are unfavorable. These expectations from life history theory acquire even more relevance when considering the role of behavior. The ability to delay reproduction increases, for instance, the opportunities for acquiring environmental information and, through learning, improve behavioral performance in exploiting resources and avoiding enemies.

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Figure 3. Mechanisms linking behavior into life history strategies to explain the likelihood that an animal establishes itself in a novel environment under demographic stochasticity, environmental uncertainty and adaptive mismatches. From Sol and Maspons (2016) with permission from Cambridge University Press.

When the population is small, which is often initially the case for introduced species, and there is sufficient phenotype-environment match, a future returns strategy may not be the best solution. In such a scenario, it may pay to have a high fecundity that allows rapid population growth, thereby reducing the period at which the founder population remains small and hence more vulnerable to extinction by demographic stochasticity (Lockwood, 2009). In Chapter 17, Mark Albins examines the range of life- history and behavioral traits that have facilitated the rapid invasion of introduced lionfish ( spp.) in the Atlantic. Lionfish, which were probably initially released from aquariums in South Florida, have become one of the worst fish predators, having major impacts on native coral reef fishes throughout the Caribbean, Gulf of Mexico, and South Atlantic. The rapid population growth and spread of invasive lionfish have been primarily attributed to their life history characterized by fast growth and maturation, and a high fecundity, fertilization success, and survival of eggs and larvae. It can also be attributed in part to the lack of dispersal barriers in the open water. Their population growth and spread have also been facilitated by behavioral mechanisms. The eggs and larvae are able to travel long distances, while after settlement, the fish tend to stay around the same area. While in their native range they tend to be solitary except during spawning periods, in the invasive range they are either more social or are continually ready to spawn. This finding contrasts with the previously discussed studies of Gambusia, where asocial individuals are more likely to invade new habitats and establish populations, illustrating the idiosyncrasies of the process.

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Behavioral syndromes associated with life history can evolve in the novel environment if the underlying components are heritable and together provide fitness advantages in the new context. Surprisingly, however, there has been little research on eco-evolutionary dynamics of behavior in invading populations. The studies of cane toads (Rhinella marina Linnaeus, 1758), a species that is primarily terrestrial but that rely on water to lay the eggs, are an exception. Ben Phillips (2016) describes in Chapter 6 how eco-evolutionary dynamics changes the spreading rates of cane toads introduced in Australia. As only the most dispersive individuals get to the invasion front of the population in any given generation, there will be assortative mating by dispersal ability (Phillips et al. 2008, 2010). This spatial sorting causes the runaway evolution of the dispersal trait. In addition, individuals in the front of the expanding range find themselves in an exponentially growing population which, according to r/K theory, should select for higher fecundity. Conditions at the invasion front therefore favor the evolution of greater dispersal and reproductive rates, which should accelerate spreading rate over time. In cane toads, the rate of spread has indeed been accelerating since the original introduction and now is five times faster than the original rate.

Behavioral interactions between invaders and native species As highlighted by Suarez and Cassey (2016), invasions provide a powerful “unintended” experimental system for examining novel species interactions, including predator-prey and host-parasite co-evolution. Chapters in this section discuss a variety of interactions, including predator-prey, host- parasite, and mutualistic networks. In the chapters dealing with aquatic systems, however, there is an emphasis on predator–prey interactions. In Chapter 11, Weis (2016) examines freshwater and marine ecosystems, discussing the wide-ranging impacts of invasive predators on both native prey species specifically and invaded ecosystems generally. Among the highlighted crustaceans are green crabs (Carcinus maenas Linnaeus, 1758) which manage to outcompete many native species for bivalve prey (MacDonald et al. 2007), and rusty crayfish (Orconectes rusticus Girard, 1852) which also outcompete native crayfish in the US and are less affected by predation by largemouth bass (Hill and Lodge 1999). Among the fishes discussed are rainbow trout (Oncorhynchus mykiss Suckley, 1869) that have been introduced for food or sport fishing in many continents and have spread throughout the world. By feeding on native salmonids and other fish species as well as on amphibians and terrestrial insects, rainbow trout reduce the population of their prey and have complex effects on both aquatic and nearby terrestrial food webs (Fausch 2007). Behavior is central to understand these novel interactions between predators and prey. In their chapter, Grosholz and Wells (2016) focus on

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

the role of predators in recognizing, selecting, handling and consuming novel prey, whether as native predators consuming novel prey, or as non- native predators consuming native prey. It is believed that introduced predators prefer familiar prey and would need time to become accustomed to novel prey, so there should be time lags before interacting with novel prey that should attenuate over time (Hastings et al. 2005). To explore these ideas, Grosholz and Wells (2016) examined introduced predatory whelks and their new oyster and mussel prey. Experiments with both native and introduced populations of oyster drills, Urosalpinx cinera (Say, 1822), showed that shared evolutionary history does not appear to be a major mechanism underlying choice of oysters or mussels as prey. The preferred oyster was either the familiar Crassostrea virginica (Gmelin, 1791) or the novel Ostrea lurida (Carpenter, 1864), depending on circumstances, whereas the preferred mussel was the novel M. senhousia (Benson, 1842). Naïve U. cinerea immediately preferred M. senhousia despite lack of any prior experience. For the population of U. cinerea from the introduced range, the lack of any experience with C. virginica for several decades did not reduce their preference for C. virginica. Thus, the choices made by this predator with novel vs. familiar prey do not seem to follow predictions. The authors concluded that if the cues used in these systems are general and not species specific, there may be little functional naïveté, and if the new prey species are similar to familiar species, the level of naïveté will be low and easily overcome by learning. Behavioral responses of natives to the invaders are also important to consider. When the invader is a predator, native prey are generally naïve and more susceptible to predation, which contributes to the success and impact of the invader. This is particularly clear in the case of invasive lionfish (Pterois volitans Linnaeus, 1758) in the Atlantic and Caribbean, where they consume large numbers of juveniles of many coral reef fish species that are naïve and do not react to the predator, reducing their populations (Albins 2013, 2015). To make things worse, in their invaded range, lionfish consume far more food than in their native range and become larger (Fishelson 1997; Cote et al 2013). In some cases, the invader becomes a new food source for native species. Invasive Pacific oysters (Magellanica gigas Thunberg, 1793) develop reefs which become feeding areas for coastal birds in Europe (Herbert et al. 2018). In cases where the native predators are very abundant, they may be able to slow down or arrest the invasion. In the Chesapeake Bay, US, blue crabs (Callinectes sapidus Rathbun, 1896) are very abundant, and appear to provide some “biotic resistance” to the invasion of green crabs (Carcinus maenas), which are much more abundant in areas further north where there are fewer blue crabs (deRivera et al. 2005). Blue crab predation may similarly be responsible for reducing the numbers and impacts of the

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

invasive rapa whelk, Rapana venosa (Valenciennes, 1846), in Chesapeake Bay (Harding 2003). In some cases, the invasive species is toxic and can be a hazard to its potential predators. Venomous spines of lionfish can be a major deterrent to predation, and low predation rates enhance its success as an invader (Albins 2016). In other cases, toxic invaders can be consumed by naïve predators. Hatchlings of the toxic invasive toad, Bufo japonicus formosus (Temminck and Schelgel, 1838) in its invaded range are preyed upon by native amphibians, including carnivorous salamander larvae Hynobius retardatus (Dunn, 1923) and omnivorous frog tadpoles, Rana pirica (Matsui, 1991). In mesocosm studies, Kazila and Kishida (2019) found that both predator species were adversely affected by consuming toxic toad hatchlings, but that the frog tadpoles were more greatly affected because they had lower toxin resistance. Regarding toxic cane toads in Australia, mass mortality was seen in predatory crocodiles (Crocodilus johnstoni Krefft, 1873) coincident with the arrival of the toads, causing the crocodile populations to decrease by as much as 77% (Letnic et al. 2008). Some mammalian predators have apparently learned to refrain from attacking the toxic toads. While naïve individuals of the marsupial, Planigale maculata (Gould, 1851), would initially seize the toads, they rapidly learned to avoid them (Webb et al. 2008). However, another native marsupial, the northern quoll Dasyuris hallucatus (Gould, 1842) apparently did not learn to avoid cane toads and became locally extinct in Northern Australia following the invasion. Native species may change their habitat or location in order to avoid predation or competition from the invasive species. In many cases, this relocation is detrimental. In response to the invasive predatory cladoceran, Bythotrephes longimanus (Leydig, 1860), Daphnia galeata mendotae (Birge, 1918) were found to move deeper into the water column in lakes to avoid the predators, but this resulted in a reduction in their growth rate (Pangle and Peacor 2006). Invading rainbow (Oncorhynchus mykiss) and brown (Salmo trutta Linnaeus, 1758) trout in Patagonia caused the native galaxiid fish, Galaxias platei (Steindachner, 1898), to similarly move down to feed at greater depths in the lakes, which was interpreted as a response to depletion of their prey in lakes that had high trout density (Correa et al. 2012). In response to the invasion of the benthic round goby (Neogobius melanostomus Pallas, 1814) in the US Great Lakes, juvenile smallmouth bass (Micropterus dolomieu Lacépède, 1802) were found to move up from the bottom to find food in the water column, which forces them to eat small plankton rather than larger benthic macroinvertebrates. This location in the water column also makes them more vulnerable to predators (Winslow 2010). Native sculpins, Cottus beldingi (Eigenmann and Eigenmann, 1891) in California streams live in pools and low velocity habitats. However, when invasive signal crayfish (Pacifasticus leniusculus Dana, 1852) arrive the sculpins move to more rapidly flowing water, where

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

they have reduced feeding and growth (Light 2005). Native Dungeness crabs (Cancer [Metacarcinus] magister Dana, 1852) in the Pacific coast of the US are displaced from their shelters by invasive green crabs (Carcinus maenas), enhancing their risk of predation (McDonald et al. 2001). However, native shore crabs, Hemigrapsus oregonensis (Dana, 1851), in turn, were able to displace the green crabs from their habitat of rock and shell cover (Jensen et al. 2001).

Management implications Understanding the behavior of animals during the invasion process can offer management opportunities. Prevention is widely considered a more efficient way to reduce the impact of biological invasions than control and eradication. In chapter 18, Carrete and Tella (2016) discuss the role of behavior in preventing escapes from the pet trade, one of the major pathways of animal introductions. As behaviors related to anti-predator and foraging abilities may be eroded in captivity, in theory, animals that have been bred for the pet trade should be less likely to escape and establish breeding populations, thereby failing to complete the two main transition stages (release and establishment) of the invasion pathway. Some examples of successful invasions in aquatic pets, such as lionfish, show nonetheless that this is not always the case. Although bans may represent a successful management action against invasions, the relevance of behavior during the early stages of the invasion process is unfortunately poorly understood for aquatic animals because their presence tends to be discovered later in the invasion process. A variety of management options that use behavioral information to control invaders are also discussed throughout the book, although their validity remains to be tested. The chapter on the spread of cane toads in Australia illustrates this (Phillips 2016). Because toads at the leading edge of the invasion are more dispersive, physical barriers such as rivers and mountains, that might have acted as barriers, are less effective. If individuals from long-established populations are moved to the invasion front, those physical barriers might remain effective and the highly dispersive phenotypes might never reach the barrier because the individuals moved to the invasion front are less dispersive and less likely to penetrate the barrier. Understanding hormone-behavior relationships in invaders may also reveal new ways to control pests; yet, it is rare that endocrinological interventions are used to stop or slow invasions (Martin et al. 2016). A final management option that is being considered for some invasive species is to add human predators to the top of the food web to consume the invasive species (Weis 2016). This is being pursued throughout the Caribbean and South Atlantic for the lionfish, Pterois spp.

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Concluding Remarks The chapters of the book reflect the current vitality of the discipline, illustrating the many ways behavior influences biological invasions. A common theme in these chapters is that general rules for predicting invasion success are rare. This may indicate that there is no single strategy to employ in order to be a successful invader, reflecting the variety of mechanisms by which behavior affects population dynamics in novel environments. It also indicates that the role of behavior often depends on the invasion context. The study of aquatic systems has been essential to unravel the idiosyncratic forms invaders may use to invade and impact ecosystems. Arguing that general rules are rare is not to say that there are no rules. In fact, the book highlights a general consensus that behavioral plasticity is a key feature of many successful invaders. However, even in this case the context matters, and being plastic is less relevant under certain contexts, such as when demographic stochasticity is high and/or the species is only weakly displaced from their ancestral adaptive optimum. Understanding the contexts in which particular behaviors or behavioral mechanisms are beneficial is complicated by the fact that they are part of a suite of traits that influence the way animals negotiate the invasion process, from capture to spread. Making the link between behavioral syndromes and invasion contexts is crucial because at an ultimate level, these suites of traits are closely tied to evolving life history strategies that governs the population dynamics in novel environments. Therefore, the consequences of behavior for population dynamics cannot be simply inferred by examining behaviors in isolation. Again, aquatic systems have provided some of the best model systems (e.g. cane toads and guppies) to study the eco-evolutionary dynamics of behavioral syndromes during invasions. Reading Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour, however, it becomes clear that there are still important gaps in our knowledge about how behavior affects and is affected by biological invasions. Some of these gaps are general to the discipline. We have, for instance, an insufficient understanding of how behavioral plasticity interacts with life history to influence population dynamics in novel environments. In a similar vein, the importance of evolution in shaping behaviors is almost absent in the book, despite ample evidence that much among-individual variation in behaviour is heritable and that, by acting on such variation, natural selection can alter the invasion process itself (Phillips 2016). Other knowledge gaps are particularly important for aquatic animals. One is the hormonal basis of the behaviors that are relevant at different stages of the invasion process (see Martin and colleagues in Chapter 4). Martin and colleagues (2016) predict that successful invaders should be those species that are better able to up- and down-regulate circulating hormone levels to salient stimuli,

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

adjust hormone receptor expression among tissues, and/or couple and uncouple other processes (e.g., immune, growth, reproductive) to hormones. However, these ideas have been mostly developed for terrestrial animals, and it is currently unclear whether they should also apply to aquatic systems. Another topic poorly known for aquatic animals is the proximate cognitive basis of behavioural responses to new ecological challenges. This topic, which is widely discussed by Andrea Griffin and colleagues in Chapter 3 (Griffin et al. 2016), is better known for terrestrial invaders than for aquatic invaders. Finally, discussions on role of behaviour in the early stages of the invasion process are notoriously rare for aquatic animals. This is unexpected because some early stages where behavior should be highly relevant, such as escapes from the pet trade, are major pathways of introduction of aquatic organisms. By suggesting ideas, methods and study systems, we hope that “Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour” serves to encourage aquatic ecologists to continue investigating how behavior allows animals to negotiate the wide range of hurdles animals encounter during the invasion of novel regions.

Acknowledgements

We are grateful to the authors of the chapters that are cited here for their hard work in producing them, to the editors at Cambridge University Press who assisted us in getting the book published, and especially to Mark Hauber of the City University of New York, whose idea was the genesis of the book initially. We are also grateful to the reviewers whose comments made this a better article. D.S. was supported by the Spanish Ministry of Economy, Industry and Competitivity (CGL2017-90033-P).

References

Albins M (2013) Effects of invasive Pacific red lionfish Pterois volitans versus a native predator on Bahamian coral-reef fish communities. Biological Invasions 15: 29–43, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-012-0266-1 Albins M (2015) Invasive Pacific red lionfish Pterois volitans reduce abundance and species richness of native Bahamian coral-reef fishes. Marine Ecology Progress Series 522: 231– 243, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps11159 Albins M (2016) Behaviors of Pacific lionfish facilitate invasion of the Atlantic In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge UK, pp 309–323, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.018 Carrete M, Tella JL (2016) Wildlife trade, behaviour and avian invasions. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UK, pp 7–25, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.019 Chapple D, Wong BM (2016) The role of behavioural variation across different stages of the introduction process. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UK, pp 7–25, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo97811399394 92.003 Correa C, Bravo A, Hendry A (2012) Reciprocal trophic niche shifts in native and invasive fish: salmonids and galaxiids in Patagonian lakes. Freshwater Biology 57: 1769–1781, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2427.2012.02837.x Côté IM, Green SJ, Hixon MA (2013) Predatory fish invaders: Insights from Indo-Pacific lionfish in the western Atlantic and Caribbean. Biological Conservation 164: 50–61, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2013.04.014 Cote J, Fogarty S, Weinersmith K, Brodin T, Sih A (2010) Personality traits and dispersal tendency in the invasive mosquitofish Gambusia affinis. Proceedings of the Royal Society B-Biological Sciences 277: 1571–1579, https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.2128 Deacon A, Magurran A (2016) How behaviour contributes to the success of an invasive poeciliid fish: the Trinidadian guppy (Poecilia reticulata) as a model species. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge UK, pp 266–290, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.016

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

DeRivera C, Ruiz G, Hines A, Jivoff P (2005) Biotic resistance to invasion: Native predator limits abundance and distribution of an introduced crab. Ecology 86: 3364–3376, https://doi.org/10.1890/05-0479 Fausch KD (2007) Introduction, establishment and effects of non-native salmonids: Considering the risk of rainbow trout invasion in the United Kingdom. Journal of Fish Biology 71: 1–32, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1095-8649.2007.01682.x Fishelson L (1997) Experiments and observations on food consumption, growth and starvation in Dendrochirus brachypterus and Pterois volitans, (, ). Environmental Biology of Fishes 50: 391–403 Gherardi F (2007) Understanding the impact of invasive crayfish. In: Gherardi F (ed), Biological invaders in inland waters: profiles, distribution, and threats. Invading Nature: Springer Series in Invasion Ecology, Springer, Dordrecht, The Netherlands, pp 507–542, https://doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4020-6029-8_28 Griffin AS, Gues D, Federspiel I, Diquelou M, Lermite F (2016) In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge UK pp 26–46, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.004 Grosholz E, Wells E (2016) Evolutionary novelty and the behavior of introduced predators. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge UK, pp 199–220, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.013 Harding, JM (2003). Predation by blue crabs, Callinectes sapidus, on rapa whelks, Rapana venosa: possible natural controls on an invasive species. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 297: 161–177, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jembe.2003.07.005 Hastings A, Cuddington K, Davies KF, Dugaw C, Elmendorf S, Freestone A, Harrison S, Holland M, Lambrinos J, Malvadkar U, Melbourne B, Moore K, Taylar C, Thomson D (2005) The spatial spread of invasions: new developments in theory and evidence. Ecology Letters 8: 91–101, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2004.00687.x Hazlett BA, Acquistapace P, Gherardi F (2002) Differences in memory capabilities in invasive and native crayfish. Journal of Crustacean Biology 22: 439–448, https://doi.org/10.1651/0278- 0372(2002)022[0439:dimcii]2.0.co;2 Hazlett BA, Burba A, Gherardi F, Acquistapace P (2003) Invasive species use a broader range of predation-risk cues than native species. Biological Invasions 5: 223–228, https://doi.org/ 10.1023/a:1026114623612 Herbert R, Davies C, Bowgen K, Hatton J, Stillman R (2018) The importance of nonnative Pacific oyster reefs as supplementary feeding areas for coastal birds on estuary mudflats. Aquatic Conservation 28: 1294–1307, https://doi.org/10.1002/aqc.2938 Hill AM, Lodge D (1999) Replacement of resident crayfishes by an exotic crayfish: the roles of competition and predation. Ecological Applications 9: 678–690, https://doi.org/10.1890/1051- 0761(1999)009[0678:rorcba]2.0.co;2 Jensen G, McDonald P, Armstrong D (2001) East meets West: Competitive interactions between green crab Carcinus maenas, and native and introduced shore crab Hemigrapsus spp. Marine Ecology Progress Series 225: 251–262, https://doi.org/10.3354/meps225251 Kazila A, Kishida O (2019) Foraging traits of native predators determine their vulnerability to a toxic alien prey. Freshwater Biology 64: 56–70, https://doi.org/10.1111/fwb.13194 Laland KN, Reader SM (1999) Foraging innovation in the guppy. Animal Behaviour 57: 331– 340, https://doi.org/10.1006/anbe.1998.0967 Letnic M, Webb J, Shine R (2008) Invasive cane toads (Bufo marinus) cause massive mortality of freshwater crocodiles (Crocodylus johnstoni) in tropical Australia. Biological Conservation 141: 1773–1782, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.biocon.2008.04.031 Light T (2005) Behavioral effects of invaders: alien crayfish and native sculpin in a California stream. Biological Invasions 7: 353–367, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-004-2510-9 Lockwood JL, Cassey P, Blackburn TM (2009) The more you introduce the more you get: the role of colonization pressure and propagule pressure in invasion ecology. Diversity and Distributions 15: 904–910, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1472-4642.2009.00594.x MacDonald JA, Roudez R, Glover T, Weis JS (2007) The invasive green crab and Japanese shore crab: behavioral interactions with a native crab species, the blue crab. Biological Invasions 9: 837–848, https://doi.org/10.1007/s10530-006-9085-6 McDonald P, Jensen G, Armstrong D (2001) The competitive and predatory impacts of the nonindigenous crab Carcinus maenas (L) on early benthic phase Dungeness crab, Cancer magister Dana. Journal of Experimental Marine Biology and Ecology 258: 39–54, https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-0981(00)00344-0 Martin LB, Brace AJ, Kilvites HJ, Gervasi SS (2016) Invader Endocrinology: The Regulation of Behaviour in Pesky Phenotypes. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press Cambridge, UK, pp 82–95, https://doi.org/10. 1017/cbo9781139939492.005 Mayr E (1965) The nature of colonising birds. In: Baker HD, Stebbins GL (eds), The Genetics of Colonizing Species. Academic Press, New York, pp 29–43

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press) Highlights from Invasion Book

Pangle K, Peacor S (2006) Non‐lethal effect of the invasive predator Bythotrephes longimanus on Daphnia mendotae. Freshwater Biology 51: 1070–1078, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365- 2427.2006.01555.x Phillips B (2016) Behaviour on invasion fronts and the behavior of invasion fronts. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 82–95, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.007 Phillips B, Brown G, Travis J, Shine R (2008) Reid’s paradox revisited: the evolution of dispersal in range-shifting populations. The American Naturalist 172: S34–S48, https://doi.org/10.1086/588255 Phillips B, Brown G, Shine R (2010) Life-history evolution in range-shifting populations. Ecology 91: 1617–1627, https://doi.org/10.1890/09-0910.1 Ramalho R, Anástacio P (2011) Crayfish learning abilities: how does familiarization period affect the capture rate of a new prey item? Ecological Research 26: 53–58, https://doi.org/ 10.1007/s11284-010-0754-7 Rehage J, Sih A (2004) Dispersal behavior, boldness, and the link to invasiveness: a comparison of four Gambusia species. Biological Invasions 6: 379–391, https://doi.org/10. 1023/b:binv.0000034618.93140.a5 Rehage J, Cote J, Sih A (2016) The role of dispersal behavior and personality in post- establishment spread: lessons from mosquitofish. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 96–115, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.008 Sih A, Bell A, Johnson J (2004) Behavioral syndromes: an ecological and evolutionary overview. Trends in Ecology and Evolution 19: 372–378, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tree.2004. 04.009 Sih A, Cote J, Evans M, Fogarty S, Pruitt J (2012) Ecological implications of behavioural syndromes. Ecology Letters 15: 278–289, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1461-0248.2011.01731.x Sol D, Maspons J (2016) Life history, behaviour and invasion success. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 63–81, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.006 Sol D, Duncan RP, Blackburn TM, Cassey P, Lefebvre L (2005) Big brains, enhanced cognition, and response of birds to novel environments. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences USA 102: 5460–5465, https://doi.org/10.1073/pnas.0408145102 Sol D, Bartomeus I, Griffin AS (2012) The paradox of invasion in birds: Competitive superiority or ecological opportunism? Oecologia 169: 553–564, https://doi.org/10.1007/ s00442-011-2203-x Sol D, Lapiedra O, González-Lagos C (2013) Behavioural adjustments for a life in the city. Animal Behaviour 85: 1101–1112, https://doi.org/10.1016/j.anbehav.2013.01.023 Suarez A, Cassey P (2016) Introduction. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 1–4, https://doi.org/ 10.1017/cbo9781139939492.002 Tricarico E, Aquiloni L (2016) How behaviour has helped invasive crayfish to conquer freshwater ecosystems. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 291–308, https://doi.org/10. 1017/cbo9781139939492.017 Webb J, Brown G, Child T, Brownlees M, Phillips B, Shine R (2008) A native dasyurid predator (common planigale, Planigale maculata) rapidly learns to avoid a toxic invader. Austral Ecology 33: 821–829, https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-9993.2008.01847.x Weis JS (2016) Eat or be eaten: Invasion and predation in aquatic ecosystems. In: Weis JS, Sol D (eds), Biological Invasions and Animal Behaviour. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, UK, pp 180–198, https://doi.org/10.1017/cbo9781139939492.012 Winslow C (2010) Competitive interactions between young-of-the-year smallmouth bass (Micropterus dolomieu) and round goby (Apollonia melanostomus). Ph.D. Thesis Bowling Green State University Witherington B (1997) The problem of photopollution for sea turtles and other nocturnal animals. In: Clemmons J, Bucholz R (eds), Behavioral approaches to conservation in the Wild. Cambridge University Press, Cambridge, pp 303–328 Wong B, Candolin U (2015) Behavioral responses to changing environments. Behavioral Ecology 26: 665–673, https://doi.org/10.1093/beheco/aru183

Sol and Weis (2019), Aquatic Invasions (in press)