Number in Classifier Languages

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Number in Classifier Languages Number in Classifier Languages A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL OF THE UNIVERSITY OF MINNESOTA BY Hiroki Nomoto IN PARTIAL FULFILLMENT OF THE REQUIREMENTS FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Advisor: Hooi Ling Soh March, 2013 c Hiroki Nomoto 2013 ALL RIGHTS RESERVED Acknowledgements Looking back at the years I spent for my doctoral studies, I cannot but realize that quite a number of people were there to play various roles. Most of them did not even know who I was, and I do not know who they were either, like those many strangers who said hi to me on the street, which by the way never happens at the places that I have lived other than in the United States. No matter how small their roles may be, they are definitely part of my memory and my life. Those who beat their collective power include the following individuals, to whom I wish to record my sincere gratitude here. The person who deserves the first mention is no one but my advisor, Hooi Ling Soh. If I had not met her in Singapore, my life—both academic and personal—would have been totally different. I greatly appreciate that she encouraged me to pursue my graduate studies at the Uni- versity of Minnesota (U of M). Though the decision making process was a painful experience, I strongly believe that I made the right decision. I have learned a lot through working with Hooi Ling. It is worth noting that my serious take on optionality and perspective towards it have developed in the course of our collaborative research on the verbal prefix meN- in Malay (Soh and Nomoto 2009, 2011, to appear). Brian Reese read various drafts very carefully and gave detailed comments. His comments and questions have helped clarify my thinking and made my work more precise. I thank the other committee members, Jeanette Gundel and Michael Maratsos, for asking important questions. Addressing them, I hope, has made this study accessible to a wider readership. Besides the committee members, special thanks go to Alan Bale and Akira Watanabe, who have read drafts of parts of this dissertation and gave insightful comments and criticisms. Their comments were crucial in developing and sharpening ideas presented in this study. Parts of this dissertation have been presented at the 2010 Annual Meeting of the Linguis- tic Society of America, the 29th West Coast Conference on Formal Linguistics, Workshop on i Indonesian-Type Voice Systems, Institute of Language Research Regular Colloquium and the fourth meeting of the Indonesian Languages Research Group at Tokyo University of Foreign Studies (TUFS). I would like to thank the participants at these meetings for their questions, com- ments and encouragement, especially Bryan James Gordon, Shinjiro Kazama, Kunio Nishiyama and Norvin Richards (in alphabetical order). In addition, I also benefited greatly from discus- sions with the following people: Byeong-Uk Yi, Kartini Abd. Wahab, James Stevens and Eun-Ju Noh. This study includes data from various languages. I am grateful to the following people, who shared with me their knowledge of their languages: Kartini Abd. Wahab, Mohd Izzat Mohd Thiyahuddin, Muhammad Idham Adli bin Musa, Muhammad Nizam Md Zin (Malay); Fatemeh Gharahkhani, Maryam Ataski Golestan (Persian); Bi Xi, Shu Fan, Pamela Peng (Mandarin); Nala Huiying Lee, Qizhong Chang (Singlish); Filipe Santos Kawano (Brazilian Portuguese); Iwan Setiya Budi (Indonesian); Byeong-Uk Yi, Eun-Ju Noh (Korean). I also thank the three Hmong speakers who served as consultants for Hooi Ling Soh’s project supported by the Col- lege of Liberal Arts Imagine Fund Award (2009–10). In this connection, I am grateful to An- drew Simpson for giving me an opportunity to work together on a comparative study of forms and meanings of noun phrases in four classsifier languages (Simpson et al. 2011). The research reported here was financially supported by the U of M and also by the JSPS Grant-in-Aid for Young Scientists (B) (#23720199), without which I could not have traveled for conferences and hired many consultants. While the work started early in my dissertation years at the U of M, I wrote most of this dissertation while teaching at TUFS. I thus could rely on my colleagues with different exper- tises for information, materials and consultant recruitment: Kenji Okano (Burmese), Naotoshi Kurosawa (Portuguese), Keiko Mochizuki (Mandarin), Kyoko Niwa (Bangla) and Satoko Yoshie (Persian). Masanori Ichikawa helped me with statistics in acceptability judgment experiments that I conducted relating to the present study. I also appreciate the constant care and sup- port from my immediate colleagues in the Malay Language Program, Naoki Soda and Faridah Mohamed. Last but not least, my former advisor, Isamu Shoho, has been always supportive, especially at important turning points in my academic life. Needless to say, those listed above are not responsible for the content of this dissertation; any remaining errors are solely mine. Outside the academic life, I would like to thank my fellow linguistics graduate students and also to Asish Abraham for their friendship. Finally, I owe my deepest gratitude to my parents, ii Masazumi and Setsuko Nomoto, for their continuous patience and support for my unusual life path. I dedicate this dissertation to them, hoping that they will understand what this dissertation means to me. iii Abstract Classifier langauges are often described as lacking genuine number morphology and treating all common nouns, including those conceptually count, as an unindividuated mass. This study argues that neither of these popular assumptions is true, and presents new generalizations and analyses gained by abandoning them. I claim that no difference exists between classifier and non-classifier languages regarding the semantics of either nouns or numerals. Common nouns universally denote properties and are individuated, contra Chierchia (1998b). The primary evidence comes from optional classifier languages such as Malay, in which direct numeral modification of nouns without classifiers is generally possible. Moreover, upon closer examination, optional classifier use is also observed in obligatory classifier languages such as Japanese. I propose that classifiers are a sophisticated kind of singular number marker in that they not only assert the restriction of the domain to singularities but also conventionally implicate the characteristics of the noun. Plural markers in classifier languages have the same semantic structure, and hence they are genuine plural number markers. Furthermore, classifiers and plural markers in classifier languages are subject to the same licensing condition, which involves either referential determiners or quantifiers. These similarities endorse their belonging to the same grammatical category, i.e. number. I argue that that general number, which is associated with number-neutral properties, is a universally available basic number category, along with singular and plural. Classifier languages have distinct forms for all three. Optional number marking follows from the three-way distinction number system, where the general is morphologically unmarked. The three basic number categories can be expressed by combinations of two binary fea- tures, i.e. [±Sg] (atomicity) and [±Pl] (divisibility). They create two kinds of general numbers differing in the presence/absence of number morphology: GN+ and GN−. Languages such as Brazilian Portuguese and Singlish indeed have two number-neutral forms, one with a plural marker and the other without. While classifier languages distinguish all basic number categoires, non-classifier languages conflate one or more of them morphologically. Languages can be classified into five types according to this criterion: (i) SG : GN : PL, (ii) SG/GN : GN/PL, (iii) SG/GN : PL, (iv) SG : iv GN/PL, and (v) SG/GN/PL. Classifier languages (type (i)) do not lack number, but instead make the most fine-grained basic number distinction. The difference between classifier and non- classifier languages reduces not to semantics (Krifka 1995; Chierchia 1998b; Wilhelm 2008) or syntax (Li 1999), but to a difference in number morphology. The proposed number system and typology make it possible to account for bare “singular” kind terms in type (ii) languages (e.g. Brazilian Portuguese), a problem to Dayal’s (2004b) theory of number and definiteness marking in kind terms. v Contents Acknowledgements i Abstract iv List of Tables x List of Figures xi List of Abbreviations xii 1 Introduction 1 1.1 Scope of inquiry . 1 1.2 Overview of the remaining chapters . 2 2 Dissociating classifiers from counting 6 2.1 Introduction . 6 2.2 The definition of ‘classifiers’ . 8 2.3 Classifiers are not for counting . 12 2.3.1 Optional classifier languages . 12 2.3.2 Optional classifier use in obligatory classifier languages . 15 2.4 Two little-discussed facts about classifier languages . 19 2.4.1 Three ways of interpreting noun phrases . 19 2.4.2 Fact 1: Classifiers prevent reference to subkinds . 21 2.4.2.1 Optional classifier languages: Malay and Persian . 22 2.4.2.2 Obligatory classifier languages: Japanese and Thai . 24 vi 2.4.2.3 Crosslinguistic generalization . 26 2.4.2.4 Pragmatic factors affecting the ease of object/subkind reading 29 2.4.3 Fact 2: Plurals do not denote kinds . 30 2.5 Summary . 33 3 Analysis of the two little-discussed facts about classifier languages 34 3.1 Introduction . 34 3.2 Background . 36 3.2.1 The ontology of the domain of individuals . 36 3.2.2 The relation between [object] and [kind] individuals . 41 3.2.3 Kinds . 46 3.3 Fact 1: Classifiers prevent reference to subkinds . 48 3.3.1 The idea . 48 3.3.2 An implementation in Linear Optimality Theory . 51 3.4 Fact 2: Plurals do not denote kinds . 57 3.4.1 Proposal . 57 3.4.2 Alternative accounts . 62 3.4.2.1 Individuality: Reduplication in Malay/Indonesian .
Recommended publications
  • Measuring the Comprehension of Negation in 2- to 4-Year-Old Children Ann E
    Measuring the comprehension of negation in 2- to 4-year-old children Ann E. Nordmeyer Michael C. Frank [email protected] [email protected] Department of Psychology Department of Psychology Stanford University Stanford University Abstract and inferential negation (i.e. negation of inferred beliefs of others). Regardless of taxonomy, negation is used in a variety Negation is one of the most important concepts in human lan- guage, and yet little is known about children’s ability to com- of contexts to express a range of different thoughts. prehend negative sentences. In this experiment, we explore The relationship between different types of negation is un- how children’s comprehension of negative sentences changes between 2- to 4-year-old children, as well as how comprehen- known. One possibility is that distinct categories of negation sion is influenced by how negative sentences are used. Chil- belong to a single cohesive concept. Even pre-linguistically, dren between the ages of 2 and 4 years watched a video in nonexistence, rejection, and denial could all fall under a su- which they heard positive and negative sentences. Negative sentences, such as “look at the boy with no apples”, referred perordinate conceptual category of negation. It is also pos- either to an absence of a characteristic or an alternative char- sible, however, that these types of negation represent fun- acteristic. Older children showed significant improvements in damentally different concepts. For example, the situation in speed and accuracy of looks to target. Children showed more rejection difficulty when the negative sentence referred to nothing, com- which a child expresses a dislike for going outside ( ) pared to when it referred to an alternative.
    [Show full text]
  • Classifiers: a Typology of Noun Categorization Edward J
    Western Washington University Western CEDAR Modern & Classical Languages Humanities 3-2002 Review of: Classifiers: A Typology of Noun Categorization Edward J. Vajda Western Washington University, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://cedar.wwu.edu/mcl_facpubs Part of the Modern Languages Commons Recommended Citation Vajda, Edward J., "Review of: Classifiers: A Typology of Noun Categorization" (2002). Modern & Classical Languages. 35. https://cedar.wwu.edu/mcl_facpubs/35 This Book Review is brought to you for free and open access by the Humanities at Western CEDAR. It has been accepted for inclusion in Modern & Classical Languages by an authorized administrator of Western CEDAR. For more information, please contact [email protected]. J. Linguistics38 (2002), I37-172. ? 2002 CambridgeUniversity Press Printedin the United Kingdom REVIEWS J. Linguistics 38 (2002). DOI: Io.IOI7/So022226702211378 ? 2002 Cambridge University Press Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald, Classifiers: a typology of noun categorization devices.Oxford: OxfordUniversity Press, 2000. Pp. xxvi+ 535. Reviewedby EDWARDJ. VAJDA,Western Washington University This book offers a multifaceted,cross-linguistic survey of all types of grammaticaldevices used to categorizenouns. It representsan ambitious expansion beyond earlier studies dealing with individual aspects of this phenomenon, notably Corbett's (I99I) landmark monograph on noun classes(genders), Dixon's importantessay (I982) distinguishingnoun classes fromclassifiers, and Greenberg's(I972) seminalpaper on numeralclassifiers. Aikhenvald'sClassifiers exceeds them all in the number of languages it examines and in its breadth of typological inquiry. The full gamut of morphologicalpatterns used to classify nouns (or, more accurately,the referentsof nouns)is consideredholistically, with an eye towardcategorizing the categorizationdevices themselvesin terms of a comprehensiveframe- work.
    [Show full text]
  • Grammatical Gender and Grammatical Stems
    Grammatical gender and grammatical stems How many grammatical genders exist in Czech? How can we tell the grammatical gender of a noun? What is a grammatical stem and how do we determine it? Why is knowing the grammatical stem important? How does the ending of a noun help us to know whether it is a hard- or soft-stem noun? Like other European languages (German, French, Spanish) but unlike English, Czech nouns are marked for grammatical gender. Czech has three grammatical genders: Masculine (M), Feminine (F), and Neuter (N). M and F partly overlap with the natural gender of human beings, so we have učitel for a male teacher and učitelka for a female teacher, sportovec for a male athlete and sportovkyně for a female athlete… But grammatical gender is a feature of all nouns (names of inanimate things, places, abstractions…), and it plays an important role in how Czech grammar works. To determine the grammatical gender of a noun (Maskulinum, femininum nebo neutrum?), we look at the ending—the last consonant or vowel—of the word in the Nominative singular (the basic or dictionary form of the word). 1. The great majority of nouns that end in a consonant are Masculine: profesor (professor), hrad (castle), stůl (table), pes (dog), autobus (bus), počítač (computer), čaj (tea), sešit (workbook), mobil (cell-phone)… Masculine nouns can be further divided into animate nouns (people and animals) and inanimate nouns (things, places, abstractions). 2. The great majority of nouns that end in -a are Feminine: profesorka (professor), lampa (lamp), kočka (cat), kniha (book), ryba (fish), fotka (photo), třída (classroom), podlaha (floor), budova (building)… 3.
    [Show full text]
  • The Term Declension, the Three Basic Qualities of Latin Nouns, That
    Chapter 2: First Declension Chapter 2 covers the following: the term declension, the three basic qualities of Latin nouns, that is, case, number and gender, basic sentence structure, subject, verb, direct object and so on, the six cases of Latin nouns and the uses of those cases, the formation of the different cases in Latin, and the way adjectives agree with nouns. At the end of this lesson we’ll review the vocabulary you should memorize in this chapter. Declension. As with conjugation, the term declension has two meanings in Latin. It means, first, the process of joining a case ending onto a noun base. Second, it is a term used to refer to one of the five categories of nouns distinguished by the sound ending the noun base: /a/, /ŏ/ or /ŭ/, a consonant or /ĭ/, /ū/, /ē/. First, let’s look at the three basic characteristics of every Latin noun: case, number and gender. All Latin nouns and adjectives have these three grammatical qualities. First, case: how the noun functions in a sentence, that is, is it the subject, the direct object, the object of a preposition or any of many other uses? Second, number: singular or plural. And third, gender: masculine, feminine or neuter. Every noun in Latin will have one case, one number and one gender, and only one of each of these qualities. In other words, a noun in a sentence cannot be both singular and plural, or masculine and feminine. Whenever asked ─ and I will ask ─ you should be able to give the correct answer for all three qualities.
    [Show full text]
  • Annotating Tense, Mood and Voice for English, French and German
    Annotating tense, mood and voice for English, French and German Anita Ramm1;4 Sharid Loaiciga´ 2;3 Annemarie Friedrich4 Alexander Fraser4 1Institut fur¨ Maschinelle Sprachverarbeitung, Universitat¨ Stuttgart 2Departement´ de Linguistique, Universite´ de Geneve` 3Department of Linguistics and Philology, Uppsala University 4Centrum fur¨ Informations- und Sprachverarbeitung, Ludwig-Maximilians-Universitat¨ Munchen¨ [email protected] [email protected] fanne,[email protected] Abstract features. They may, for instance, be used to clas- sify texts with respect to the epoch or region in We present the first open-source tool for which they have been produced, or for assigning annotating morphosyntactic tense, mood texts to a specific author. Moreover, in cross- and voice for English, French and Ger- lingual research, tense, mood, and voice have been man verbal complexes. The annotation is used to model the translation of tense between based on a set of language-specific rules, different language pairs (Santos, 2004; Loaiciga´ which are applied on dependency trees et al., 2014; Ramm and Fraser, 2016)). Identi- and leverage information about lemmas, fying the morphosyntactic tense is also a neces- morphological properties and POS-tags of sary prerequisite for identifying the semantic tense the verbs. Our tool has an average accu- in synthetic languages such as English, French racy of about 76%. The tense, mood and or German (Reichart and Rappoport, 2010). The voice features are useful both as features extracted tense-mood-voice (TMV) features may in computational modeling and for corpus- also be useful for training models in computational linguistic research. linguistics, e.g., for modeling of temporal relations (Costa and Branco, 2012; UzZaman et al., 2013).
    [Show full text]
  • Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: a Case Study from Koro
    Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro By Jessica Cleary-Kemp A dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics in the Graduate Division of the University of California, Berkeley Committee in charge: Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair Assistant Professor Peter S. Jenks Professor William F. Hanks Summer 2015 © Copyright by Jessica Cleary-Kemp All Rights Reserved Abstract Serial Verb Constructions Revisited: A Case Study from Koro by Jessica Cleary-Kemp Doctor of Philosophy in Linguistics University of California, Berkeley Associate Professor Lev D. Michael, Chair In this dissertation a methodology for identifying and analyzing serial verb constructions (SVCs) is developed, and its application is exemplified through an analysis of SVCs in Koro, an Oceanic language of Papua New Guinea. SVCs involve two main verbs that form a single predicate and share at least one of their arguments. In addition, they have shared values for tense, aspect, and mood, and they denote a single event. The unique syntactic and semantic properties of SVCs present a number of theoretical challenges, and thus they have invited great interest from syntacticians and typologists alike. But characterizing the nature of SVCs and making generalizations about the typology of serializing languages has proven difficult. There is still debate about both the surface properties of SVCs and their underlying syntactic structure. The current work addresses some of these issues by approaching serialization from two angles: the typological and the language-specific. On the typological front, it refines the definition of ‘SVC’ and develops a principled set of cross-linguistically applicable diagnostics.
    [Show full text]
  • The “Person” Category in the Zamuco Languages. a Diachronic Perspective
    On rare typological features of the Zamucoan languages, in the framework of the Chaco linguistic area Pier Marco Bertinetto Luca Ciucci Scuola Normale Superiore di Pisa The Zamucoan family Ayoreo ca. 4500 speakers Old Zamuco (a.k.a. Ancient Zamuco) spoken in the XVIII century, extinct Chamacoco (Ɨbɨtoso, Tomarâho) ca. 1800 speakers The Zamucoan family The first stable contact with Zamucoan populations took place in the early 18th century in the reduction of San Ignacio de Samuco. The Jesuit Ignace Chomé wrote a grammar of Old Zamuco (Arte de la lengua zamuca). The Chamacoco established friendly relationships by the end of the 19th century. The Ayoreos surrended rather late (towards the middle of the last century); there are still a few nomadic small bands in Northern Paraguay. The Zamucoan family Main typological features -Fusional structure -Word order features: - SVO - Genitive+Noun - Noun + Adjective Zamucoan typologically rare features Nominal tripartition Radical tenselessness Nominal aspect Affix order in Chamacoco 3 plural Gender + classifiers 1 person ø-marking in Ayoreo realis Traces of conjunct / disjunct system in Old Zamuco Greater plural and clusivity Para-hypotaxis Nominal tripartition Radical tenselessness Nominal aspect Affix order in Chamacoco 3 plural Gender + classifiers 1 person ø-marking in Ayoreo realis Traces of conjunct / disjunct system in Old Zamuco Greater plural and clusivity Para-hypotaxis Nominal tripartition All Zamucoan languages present a morphological tripartition in their nominals. The base-form (BF) is typically used for predication. The singular-BF is (Ayoreo & Old Zamuco) or used to be (Cham.) the basis for any morphological operation. The full-form (FF) occurs in argumental position.
    [Show full text]
  • Two Types of Serial Verb Constructions in Korean: Subject-Sharing and Index-Sharing
    Two Types of Serial Verb Constructions in Korean: Subject-Sharing and Index-Sharing Juwon Lee The University of Texas at Austin Proceedings of the 21st International Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar University at Buffalo Stefan Muller¨ (Editor) 2014 CSLI Publications pages 135–155 http://csli-publications.stanford.edu/HPSG/2014 Lee, Juwon. 2014. Two Types of Serial Verb Constructions in Korean: Subject- Sharing and Index-Sharing. In Muller,¨ Stefan (Ed.), Proceedings of the 21st In- ternational Conference on Head-Driven Phrase Structure Grammar, University at Buffalo, 135–155. Stanford, CA: CSLI Publications. Abstract In this paper I present an account for the lexical passive Serial Verb Constructions (SVCs) in Korean. Regarding the issue of how the arguments of an SVC are realized, I propose two hypotheses: i) Korean SVCs are broadly classified into two types, subject-sharing SVCs where the subject is structure-shared by the verbs and index- sharing SVCs where only indices of semantic arguments are structure-shared by the verbs, and ii) a semantic argument sharing is a general requirement of SVCs in Korean. I also argue that an argument composition analysis can accommodate such the new data as the lexical passive SVCs in a simple manner compared to other alternative derivational analyses. 1. Introduction* Serial verb construction (SVC) is a structure consisting of more than two component verbs but denotes what is conceptualized as a single event, and it is an important part of the study of complex predicates. A central issue of SVC is how the arguments of the component verbs of an SVC are realized in a sentence.
    [Show full text]
  • Tenses and Conjugation (Pdf)
    Created by the Evergreen Writing Center Library 3407 867-6420 Tenses and Conjugation Using correct verb forms is crucial to communicating coherently. Understanding how to apply different tenses and properly conjugate verbs will give you the tools with which to craft clear, effective sentences. Conjugations A conjugation is a list of verb forms. It catalogues the person, number, tense, voice, and mood of a verb. Knowing how to conjugate verbs correctly will help you match verbs with their subjects, and give you a firmer grasp on how verbs function in different sentences. Here is a sample conjugation table: Present Tense, Active Voice, Indicative Mood: Jump Person Singular Plural 1st Person I jump we jump 2nd Person you jump you jump 3rd Person he/she/it jumps they jump Person: Person is divided into three categories (first, second, and third person), and tells the reader whether the subject is speaking, is spoken to, or is spoken about. Each person is expressed using different subjects: first person uses I or we; second person uses you; and third person uses he/she/it or they. Keep in mind that these words are not the only indicators of person; for example in the sentence “Shakespeare uses images of the divine in his sonnets to represent his own delusions of grandeur”, the verb uses is in the third person because Shakespeare could be replaced by he, an indicator of the third person. Number: Number refers to whether the verb is singular or plural. Tense: Tense tells the reader when the action of a verb takes place.
    [Show full text]
  • Double Classifiers in Navajo Verbs *
    Double Classifiers in Navajo Verbs * Lauren Pronger Class of 2018 1 Introduction Navajo verbs contain a morpheme known as a “classifier”. The exact functions of these morphemes are not fully understood, although there are some hypotheses, listed in Sec- tions 2.1-2.3 and 4. While the l and ł-classifiers are thought to have an effect on averb’s transitivity, there does not seem to be a comparable function of the ; and d-classifiers. However, some sources do suggest that the d-classifier is associated with the middle voice (see Section 4). In addition to any hypothesized semantic functions, the d-classifier is also one of the two most common morphemes that trigger what is known as the d-effect, essentially a voicing alternation of the following consonant explained in Section 3 (the other mor- pheme is the 1st person dual plural marker ‘-iid-’). When the d-effect occurs, the ‘d’ of the involved morpheme is often realized as null in the surface verb. This means that the only evidence of most d-classifiers in a surface verb is the voicing alternation of the following stem-initial consonant from the d-effect. There is also a rare phenomenon where two *I would like to thank Jonathan Washington and Emily Gasser for providing helpful feedback on earlier versions of this thesis, and Jeremy Fahringer for his assistance in using the Swarthmore online Navajo dictionary. I would also like to thank Ted Fernald, Ellavina Perkins, and Irene Silentman for introducing me to the Navajo language. 1 classifiers occur in a single verb, something that shouldn’t be possible with position class morphology.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 Noun Classes and Classifiers, Semantics of Alexandra Y
    1 Noun classes and classifiers, semantics of Alexandra Y. Aikhenvald Research Centre for Linguistic Typology, La Trobe University, Melbourne Abstract Almost all languages have some grammatical means for the linguistic categorization of noun referents. Noun categorization devices range from the lexical numeral classifiers of South-East Asia to the highly grammaticalized noun classes and genders in African and Indo-European languages. Further noun categorization devices include noun classifiers, classifiers in possessive constructions, verbal classifiers, and two rare types: locative and deictic classifiers. Classifiers and noun classes provide a unique insight into how the world is categorized through language in terms of universal semantic parameters involving humanness, animacy, sex, shape, form, consistency, orientation in space, and the functional properties of referents. ABBREVIATIONS: ABS - absolutive; CL - classifier; ERG - ergative; FEM - feminine; LOC – locative; MASC - masculine; SG – singular 2 KEY WORDS: noun classes, genders, classifiers, possessive constructions, shape, form, function, social status, metaphorical extension 3 Almost all languages have some grammatical means for the linguistic categorization of nouns and nominals. The continuum of noun categorization devices covers a range of devices from the lexical numeral classifiers of South-East Asia to the highly grammaticalized gender agreement classes of Indo-European languages. They have a similar semantic basis, and one can develop from the other. They provide a unique insight into how people categorize the world through their language in terms of universal semantic parameters involving humanness, animacy, sex, shape, form, consistency, and functional properties. Noun categorization devices are morphemes which occur in surface structures under specifiable conditions, and denote some salient perceived or imputed characteristics of the entity to which an associated noun refers (Allan 1977: 285).
    [Show full text]
  • Nominal Aspect in the Romance Infinitives
    Nominal Aspect in the Romance Infinitives Monica Palmerini Among the parts-of-speech, a special position is occupied by the infinitive, a grammatical category which has generated a rich debate and a vast literature. In this paper we would like to propose a semantic analysis of this subtype of part-of-speech, halfway between a verb and a noun, based on the notion of Nominal Aspect proposed by Rijkhoff (1991, 2002). To capture in a systematic way the crosslinguistic variability in the types of interpretations available to nouns, Rijkhoff proposed the concept of Nominal Aspect as a nominal counterpart of the way verbal aspect encapsulates the way actions and events are conceptualized. He individuated two dimensions of variation, namely divisibility in space and boundedness, dealing with the way referents are formed in the dimension of space. Encoded by the two binary features [±structure] and [±shape], these parameters define four lexical kinds of nouns. (1) + structure +shape collective nouns + structure -shape mass nouns - structure +shape individual nouns - structure - shape concept nouns We argue that the infinitive, as it appears in romance languages such as Spanish and Italian, can be said to match very closely the aspectual profile of the so called concept nouns, those that, typically in classifier languages, refer to “atoms of meaning” (types) that only in discourse become actualized lexemes: similarly, in abstraction from the context, the infinitive is neither a verb nor a noun. If we consider that the first order noun, the default nominal model in the majority of Indo-european languages has the aspect of an individual noun, the infinitive appear to be, as Sechehaye noticed (1950: 169), an original and innovative lexical resource, which displays in the grammatical system of the romance languages a great variety of uses (Hernanz 1999; Skytte 1983).
    [Show full text]