Why Do Workers of Myrmica Ruginodis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) Navigate by Relying Mainly on Their Vision?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Why Do Workers of Myrmica Ruginodis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) Navigate by Relying Mainly on Their Vision? Bulletin de la Société royale belge d’Entomologie/Bulletin van de Koninklijke Belgische Vereniging voor Entomologie, 148 (2012) : 42-52 Why do workers of Myrmica ruginodis (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) navigate by relying mainly on their vision? Marie-Claire C AMMAERTS & Stéphanie N ÉMEGHAIRE Faculté des Sciences, D B O, CP 160/12, Université Libre de Bruxelles, 50, Av. F. Roosevelt, 1050 Bruxelles, Belgium (e-mail: [email protected]). Abstract In semi-natural conditions, Myrmica ruginodis foragers were submitted to collective differential operant visual then olfactory conditioning and were individually tested in the course of time. They acquired visual conditioning in 60h, reached a score of 82% after 72h and kept then a permanent score of 74%. When training ceased, they lost their learning in 2 days and kept thereafter a permanent score of 61%. If trained for a second time, these ants were again visually conditioned in 36h and kept then a permanent score of 77%. When training once more ceased, they never entirely lost their visual conditioning but kept 75% of it. M. ruginodis workers have thus a very long term visual memory. At 300-500 lux, these ants never acquired olfactory conditioning. In near-darkness, they were quickly conditioned but forgot what they had learnt as soon as training ceased. When trained in near-darkness a second time, they acquired olfactory conditioning at the same speed and reached the same score as when trained for the first time. When the second training period stopped, they once more rapidly and entirely forgot what they had learnt. So, these ants recognized olfactory cues only in darkness and have almost no olfactory memory. Such observations explain the species’ navigation system: these ants primarily use visual cues located above them to navigate, relying on odours only in darkness or in the absence of visual cues. Keywords: conditioning; learning, memory; operant conditioning. Introduction Learning has been widely studied in insects and, among others, in ants. However, the While foraging, ants rely on their trail and animals are generally trained and tested in area marking pheromones as well as on visual poorly natural conditions. Research has cues (according to a snapshot and/or a sketch concentrated on individual conditioning (f.i. map model), to a lesser extent on odours present JOSENS et al ., 2009), restrained ants (G UERRIERI in the environment and on some other egocentric et al ., 2007), simple conditioning (M ACQUART et elements (P ASSERA &ARON , 2005). The ant al ., 2008), only olfactory conditioning (D UPUY Myrmica ruginodis NYLANDER 1846 was shown et al ., 2006), classical conditioning to use primarily visual cues from above for (C AMMAERTS , 2004a) and/or only first navigation and to use odours only in the absence conditioning (H ARRIS et al ., 2005; N ARENDRA et of visual cues (C AMMAERTS et al ., 2012). Why al ., 2007). do these ants rely so heavily on visual cues? The We aimed to examine the first acquisition and kinetics of M. ruginodis foragers’ acquisition loss, as well as the second acquisition and loss and loss of visual and olfactory conditioning of a collective differential visual and olfactory may answer this question. Visual and olfactory operant conditioning in foragers of M. ruginodis conditionings occur continuously and repeatedly maintained in the laboratory in semi-natural in nature. This means that, on field, ants are conditions. The ants were collectively trained as collectively differentially conditioned essentially naturally as possible, were then subjected to a according to an operant method. In our study of period of no training followed by a second the M. ruginodis navigation system, we period of training and thereafter a second period experimented in such conditions and went on of no training, their conditioning score being using the same experimental conditions in the sequentially assessed in the course of these present work. Fig.1. Experimental design and apparatus. A: the six colonies of Myrmica ruginodis collectively trained to find their food under a green cube (set above a source of sugared water) vs. a yellow cube. B: testing system: a Y apparatus, set in a small tray, in which the ants had to choose between a green and a yellow cube, both of which being identical to those encountered during training. Ten ants of each colony were tested, one by one, in this apparatus; they gave a response when crossing the pencil drawn line which signified the beginning of a branch. The ants’ choices were recorded and the mean conditioning score so assessed. Fig. 2. Experimental design and apparatus. A: the four experimented colonies and, in front of their tray, their testing apparatus, set in a small tray. The black arrows indicate the pieces of thyme set in the ants’ tray. B: training system: pieces of onion (indicated by two white arrows) were set on the left and the right of a tube containing sugared water. C: testing system: a Y maze, deposited into a small tray, with pieces of onion on the sides of one branch and pieces of thyme on the sides of the other branch. Ten ants of each colony were tested, their response recorded and their mean score assessed as detailed in the text. Fig. 3. Instructions for the construction of the experimental apparatus used in the present work: A: cube of green or yellow strong paper, used to collectively train foragers, the green apparatus being associated with a reward. B: Y-apparatus of white strong paper, used to individually test ants. The three pieces of paper (right of the figure) allowed closing the Y-maze at each of its three angles. During the test, either a green or a yellow cube, covered with a piece of identically colored paper (Fig. 1 B) or pieces of onion or of thyme (Fig. 2 C), were set at the beginning of a branch of the Y- apparatus. More information is given in the text. experiments. Finally, the results were examined the trays (Figs 1A, 2A). to explain the species’ travelling system; Temperature was maintained at 20° ± 2° C. comparisons were made with the species M. Humidity was about 80% and remained constant sabuleti Meinert 1861 partly similarly studied over the course of an experiment. The lighting (C AMMAERTS et al ., 2011) and general had an intensity of 300 - 500 lux while caring conclusions were drawn concerning animal for the ants (e.g. providing food, renewing conditioning. nesting tubes), training and testing them. Training was also performed in near-darkness (< Material and Methods 5 lux). Chopped cockroach was served twice a week on a glass-slide. Sugar-water was offered Collection and maintenance of ants ad libitum in a small glass tube plugged with The experiments were conducted on six (for cotton. During training, this sugar-water was the visual conditioning) or on four (for the provided to the ants such as it served as a reward olfactory conditioning) experimental colonies (see ‘training’) (Figs 1A, 2A, B). derived from natural populations collected in the Aise valley (Ardenne, Belgium). These colonies Experimental apparatus; ants’ training (= were demographically similar, each containing a conditioning) and testing queen, brood and about 500 workers. They were The ants were collectively trained on their maintained in the laboratory in artificial nests foraging area using a simple apparatus, each made of two to four glass tubes half-filled with colony having its own training apparatus (Figs water. A cotton-plug separated the ants from the 1A, 2A, B). They were tested individually and water. The glass tubes were deposited in trays sequentially in a test apparatus, with a separate (47 x 32 x 7 cm), the sides of which were test apparatus for each colony (Figs 1B, 2A, C). covered with talc. The trays served as foraging areas: food was placed and ants were trained in Fig.5. Kinetics of the conditioning score of Myrmica ruginodis foragers olfactory trained at diurnal light intensity (300 - 500 lux). Times are given on the x-axis, the conditioning scores on the y-axis. For each three assays, some tendency of conditioning occurred but conditioning obviously failed. Training Fig. 4. Kinetics of the first and second acquisition and Ants of six experimental colonies were loss of a collective visual operant conditioning in collectively visually trained using an Myrmica ruginodis foragers. The ants of six colonies experimental apparatus consisting of two kinds were collectively trained and 120 (control) or 60 of cubes, a green cube and a yellow one both (test) of them were individually tested in the course constructed of strong paper (Canson®). The of time in a Y-maze. Their mean conditioning scores colors were previously analyzed to determine are given on the y-axis, time being on the x-axis. their wavelength reflection (C AMMAERTS , 2007; Results of non-parametric ² tests between the CAMMAERTS &CAMMAERTS , 2009). The cubes obtained values and the randomly expected ones are were constructed according to the instructions schematically given: NS: no symbol; = P < 0.05; given in Fig. 3A, and were hold together with = P < 0.02; = P < 0.01; = P < 0.001. The cello tape®. The ceiling of each cube was filled species obviously presents a strong visual memory. contrary to the four vertical faces. Ants could so easily go underneath the cubes. The experimental apparatus were placed in the ants’ tray so that they could easily be seen by the foragers. The green-colored apparatus of each Fig. 6. Kinetics of the first and second acquisition and loss of a collective olfactory operant conditioning in Myrmica ruginodis foragers trained in darkness. Training and testing were conducted as summarized in Fig. 4. Time is given on the x-axis, the ants’ score along the y-axis.
Recommended publications
  • Succession in Ant Communities (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) in Deciduous Forest Clear-Cuts – an Eastern European Case Study
    EUROPEAN JOURNAL OF ENTOMOLOGYENTOMOLOGY ISSN (online): 1802-8829 Eur. J. Entomol. 114: 92–100, 2017 http://www.eje.cz doi: 10.14411/eje.2017.013 ORIGINAL ARTICLE Succession in ant communities (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) in deciduous forest clear-cuts – an Eastern European case study IOAN TĂUŞAN 1, JENS DAUBER 2, MARIA R. TRICĂ1 and BÁLINT MARKÓ 3 1 Department of Environmental Sciences, Lucian Blaga University of Sibiu; Applied Ecology Research Centre, Dr. Raţiu 5-7, 550012 Sibiu, Romania; e-mails: [email protected], [email protected] 2 Thünen Institute of Biodiversity, Federal Research Institute for Rural Areas, Forestry and Fisheries, Bundesallee 50, D-38116 Braunschweig, Germany; e-mail: [email protected] 3 Hungarian Department of Biology and Ecology, Babeş-Bolyai University, Clinicilor 5-7, 400006 Cluj-Napoca, Romania; e-mails: [email protected], [email protected] Key words. Hymenoptera, Formicidae, ants, deciduous forests, secondary succession, clear-cutting, community structure, pitfall traps Abstract. Clear-cutting, the main method of harvesting in many forests in the world, causes a series of dramatic environmental changes to the forest habitat and removes habitat resources for arboreal and epigeal species. It results in considerable changes in the composition of both plant and animal communities. Ants have many critical roles in the maintenance and functioning of forest ecosystems. Therefore, the response of ants to clear-cutting and the time it takes for an ant community to recover after clear- cutting are important indicators of the effect of this harvesting technique on the forest ecosystem. We investigated ground-dwelling ant communities during secondary succession of deciduous forests in Transylvania, Romania.
    [Show full text]
  • The Large Blue Butterfly Maculinea Alcon in Belgium: Science and Conservation
    BULLETfN DE L'JNSTITUT ROYAL DES SCIENCES NATURELLES DE BELGIQUE BIOLOGIE, 72-S VPPL. : 183-1 85, 20Q ~ BULLETIN VAN HET KONINKLJJK BELGISCH INSTITVUT VOOR NATUURWETENSC HAPPEN BIOLOGLE, 72-S UPPL.: 183-1 85, 2002 The large blue butterfly Maculinea alcon in Belgium: science and conservation W. V ANREUSEL, H. VAN DYCK & D. MAES Introduction According to the Flemish Red List, M. alcon is threa­ tened. It is one of the few legally protected butterflies. In Belgium, the obligate ant-parasitic butterfly Maculinea Since 1996, both its complex life history and conserva­ alcon is confined to NE-Flanders (Kempen) and has tion biology have been studied rather intensively by our decreased considerably in distribution (fig. 1) and abun­ research team. Several populations went extinct, no less dance. than 9 did so in the 1990s including extinctions in nature reserves. Our analyses indicate that populations of smal- . ler, more isolated sites have a significantly higher extinc­ tion probability. At present, only 12 populations remain in 8 areas, including 5 nature reserves and 3 military areas (table I). Based on detailed egg counts, most populations show negative trends. We have recently finalised a Spe­ cies Action Plan for Maculinea a/con funded by the Flemish Ministry of Nature Conservation (VAN REUSEL et al. 2000). It was the first action plan for an invertebrate, but Flanders has only little experience with the imple­ mentation of such plans and with the integration of spe­ before 1991 cies-specific knowledge into site-oriented conservation. Hence, the M. a/con plan will be an important test-case.
    [Show full text]
  • Review and Phylogenetic Evaluation of Associations Between Microdontinae (Diptera: Syrphidae) and Ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae)
    Hindawi Publishing Corporation Psyche Volume 2013, Article ID 538316, 9 pages http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2013/538316 Review Article Review and Phylogenetic Evaluation of Associations between Microdontinae (Diptera: Syrphidae) and Ants (Hymenoptera: Formicidae) Menno Reemer Naturalis Biodiversity Center, European Invertebrate Survey, P.O. Box 9517, 2300 RA Leiden, The Netherlands Correspondence should be addressed to Menno Reemer; [email protected] Received 11 February 2013; Accepted 21 March 2013 Academic Editor: Jean-Paul Lachaud Copyright © 2013 Menno Reemer. This is an open access article distributed under the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. The immature stages of hoverflies of the subfamily Microdontinae (Diptera: Syrphidae) develop in ant nests, as predators ofthe ant brood. The present paper reviews published and unpublished records of associations of Microdontinae with ants, in order to discuss the following questions. (1) Are all Microdontinae associated with ants? (2) Are Microdontinae associated with all ants? (3) Are particular clades of Microdontinae associated with particular clades of ants? (4) Are Microdontinae associated with other insects? A total number of 109 associations between the groups are evaluated, relating to 43 species of Microdontinae belonging to 14 genera, and to at least 69 species of ants belonging to 24 genera and five subfamilies. The taxa of Microdontinae found in association with ants occur scattered throughout their phylogenetic tree. One of the supposedly most basal taxa (Mixogaster)isassociatedwith ants, suggesting that associations with ants evolved early in the history of the subfamily and have remained a predominant feature of their lifestyle.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence for a Thoracic Crop in the Workers of Some Neotropical Pheidole Species (Formicidae: Myrmicinae)
    Arthropod Structure & Development 59 (2020) 100977 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Arthropod Structure & Development journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/asd Evidence for a thoracic crop in the workers of some Neotropical Pheidole species (Formicidae: Myrmicinae) * A. Casadei-Ferreira a, , G. Fischer b, E.P. Economo b a Departamento de Zoologia, Universidade Federal do Parana, Avenida Francisco Heraclito dos Santos, s/n, Centro Politecnico, Curitiba, Mailbox 19020, CEP 81531-980, Brazil b Biodiversity and Biocomplexity Unit, Okinawa Institute of Science and Technology Graduate University, 1919-1 Tancha, Onna, Okinawa, 904-0495, Japan article info abstract Article history: The ability of ant colonies to transport, store, and distribute food resources through trophallaxis is a key Received 28 May 2020 advantage of social life. Nonetheless, how the structure of the digestive system has adapted across the Accepted 21 July 2020 ant phylogeny to facilitate these abilities is still not well understood. The crop and proventriculus, Available online xxx structures in the ant foregut (stomodeum), have received most attention for their roles in trophallaxis. However, potential roles of the esophagus have not been as well studied. Here, we report for the first Keywords: time the presence of an auxiliary thoracic crop in Pheidole aberrans and Pheidole deima using X-ray micro- Ants computed tomography and 3D segmentation. Additionally, we describe morphological modifications Dimorphism Mesosomal crop involving the endo- and exoskeleton that are associated with the presence of the thoracic crop. Our Liquid food results indicate that the presence of a thoracic crop in major workers suggests their potential role as Species group repletes or live food reservoirs, expanding the possibilities of tasks assumed by these individuals in the colony.
    [Show full text]
  • Myrmica Ants
    Biological Journal of the Linnean Socicly (1993),49: 229-238. With 2 figures Comparison of acoustical signals in Maculinea butterfly caterpillars and their obligate host Myrmica ants P. J. DEVRIES* AND R. B. COCROFT Department of ,zbology, University of Texas, Austin, Texas 78712, U.S.A. AND J. THOMAS Institute of Terrestrial Ecology, Furzebrook Research Station, Wareham, Dorset BH20 5AS Received 6 February 1992, accepted for publication I2 May I992 An acoustical comparison between calls of parasitic butterfly caterpillars and their host ants is presented for the first time. Overall, caterpillar calls were found to be similar to ant calls, even though these organisms produce them by different means. However, a comparison of Maculinca caterpillars with those of Mpica ants produced no evidence suggesting fine level convergence of caterpillar calls upon those of their species specific host ants. Factors mediating the species specific nature of the Maculincn-Myrmica system are discussed, and it is suggested that phylogenetic analysis is needed for future work. ADDITIONAL KEY WORDS:-Lycaenidae - Formicidae - symbiotic association - evolution CONTENTS Introduction ................... 229 Materials and methods ................ 231 Results. ................... 232 Discussion ................... 235 Acknowledgements ................. 237 References ................... 237 INTRODUCTION Arthropods from a diversity of phylogenetic lineages form symbiotic associations with ants that may range from parasitism to mutualism. Some arthropod symbionts produce semiochemical secretions that aid in maintaining the symbioses (Holldobler, 1978; Vander Meer & Wojcik, 1982), while others produce food secretions to ants that are also considered important in maintaining these symbioses (Way, 1963; Maschwitz, Fiala & Dolling, 1987; Fiedler & Maschwitz, 1988, 1989; DeVries, 1988; DeVries & Baker, 1989). Ants *Current address for correspondence: Museum of Comparative Zoology, Harvard University, Cambridge MA 02138, U.S.A.
    [Show full text]
  • Functional Role of Extrafloral Nectar in Boreal Forest Ecosystems Under
    Review Functional Role of Extrafloral Nectar in Boreal Forest Ecosystems under Climate Change Jarmo K. Holopainen * , James D. Blande and Jouni Sorvari Department of Environmental and Biological Sciences, University of Eastern Finland, P.O. Box 1627 Kuopio, Finland; james.blande@uef.fi (J.D.B.); jouni.sorvari@uef.fi (J.S.) * Correspondence: jarmo.holopainen@uef.fi Received: 15 December 2019; Accepted: 2 January 2020; Published: 6 January 2020 Abstract: Carbohydrate-rich extrafloral nectar (EFN) is produced in nectaries on the leaves, stipules, and stems of plants and provides a significant energy source for ants and other plant mutualists outside of the flowering period. Our review of literature on EFN indicates that only a few forest plant species in cool boreal environments bear EFN-producing nectaries and that EFN production in many boreal and subarctic plant species is poorly studied. Boreal forest, the world’s largest land biome, is dominated by coniferous trees, which, like most gymnosperms, do not produce EFN. Notably, common deciduous tree species that can be dominant in boreal forest stands, such as Betula and Alnus species, do not produce EFN, while Prunus and Populus species are the most important EFN-producing tree species. EFN together with aphid honeydew is known to play a main role in shaping ant communities. Ants are considered to be keystone species in mixed and conifer-dominated boreal and mountain forests because they transfer a significant amount of carbon from the canopy to the soil. Our review suggests that in boreal forests aphid honeydew is a more important carbohydrate source for ants than in many warmer ecosystems and that EFN-bearing plant species might not have a competitive advantage against herbivores.
    [Show full text]
  • Distribution Maps of the Outdoor Myrmicid Ants (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) of Finland, with Notes on Their Taxonomy and Ecology
    © Entomol. Fennica. 5 December 1995 Distribution maps of the outdoor myrmicid ants (Hymenoptera, Formicidae) of Finland, with notes on their taxonomy and ecology Michael I. Saaristo Saaristo, M. I. 1995: Distribution maps of the outdoor myrmicid ants (Hy­ menoptera, Formicidae) of Finland, with notes on their taxonomy and ecol­ ogy.- Entomol. Fennica 6:153-162. Twentytwo species of myrmicid ants maintaining outdoor populations are listed for Finland. Four of them, Myrmica hellenica Fore!, Myrmica microrubra Seifert, Leptothorax interruptus (Schenck), and Leptothorax kutteri Buschinger are new to Finland. Myrmica Zonae Finzi is considered as a good sister species of Myrmica sabuleti Meinert, under which name the species has earlier been known in Finland. Myrmica specioides Bondroit is deleted from the list as no samples of this species could be located in the collections. The distribution of each species in Finland is presented on 10 X 10 km square grid maps. The ecology of the various species is discussed. Also some taxonomic problems are dealt with. Michael I. Saaristo, Zoological Museum, University of Turku, FIN-20500 Turku, Finland This study was started during the autumn of 1983, Anergates atratulus. One of them, viz. H. sublaevis, by determing the extensive pitfall material of is a dulotic or slave keeping-species with a special­ ants in the collections of the Zoological Museum ized soldier-like worker caste, while others have no of the University of Turku (MZT). About the workers at all. As usual with workerless social same time the ant collections of the Finnish Mu­ parasites, there are quite few records of them be­ seum of Natural History, Helsinki (MZH) and cause they are difficult to collect except during the the Department of Applied Zoology, University relatively short time when they have alates.
    [Show full text]
  • Microgynous Queens in the Paleartic Ant, Manica Rubida: Dispersal Morphs Or Social Parasites?
    Journal of Insect Science: Vol. 10 | Article 17 Lenoir et al. Microgynous queens in the Paleartic ant, Manica rubida: Dispersal morphs or social parasites? Downloaded from https://academic.oup.com/jinsectscience/article/10/1/17/828587 by guest on 24 September 2021 Alain Lenoir1a*, Séverine Devers1b, Philippe Marchand1, Christophe Bressac1c, and Riitta Savolainen2d 1Université François Rabelais, IRBI, UMR CNRS 6035, Faculté des Sciences, Parc de Grandmont, 37200 Tours, France 2Department of Biosciences, P.O. Box 65, 00014 University of Helsinki, Finland. Abstract In many ant species, queen size is dimorphic, with small microgynes and large macrogynes, which differ, for example, in size, insemination rate, ovary development, and dispersal tactics. These polymorphic queens often correspond with alternative reproductive strategies. The Palearctic ant, Manica rubida (Latreille) (Hymenoptera: Formicidae), lives mostly in mountainous regions in either monogynous colonies, containing one macrogynous queen or polygynous colonies, containing a few large macrogynous queens. In 1998, a colony of M. rubida was discovered containing macrogynes and many small alate microgynes that did not engage in a nuptial flight but, instead, stayed in the home nest the following winter. These microgynes were studied more closely by investigating their size, behavior, and spermatheca in relation to M. rubida macrogynes and workers. Mitochondrial DNA of macrogynes, microgynes and workers from four nests was sequenced to detect possible genetic differences between them. The microgynes were significantly smaller than the macrogynes, and the head width of the gynes was completely bimodal. The microgynes behaved like workers of the macrogynes in every experiment tested. Furthermore, the microgynes had a normal spermatheca and could be fecundated, but rarely (only one in several years).
    [Show full text]
  • In a Novel Situation, Ants Can Learn to React As Never Before - a Preliminary Study
    Journal of Behavior Original Research Article *Corresponding author Marie-Claire Cammaerts, 27 square du Castel Fleuri, B 1170 Bruxelles, Belgium In a Novel Situation, Ants can Tel: +32-322-673-4969 Email: [email protected] Submitted: 04 May 2017 Learn to React as Never Before Accepted: 19 July 2017 Published: 18 August 2017 - a Preliminary Study Copyright: © 2017 Cammaerts OPEN ACCESS Marie-Claire Cammaerts* University of Brussels, Brussels, Belgium Keywords • Cognition • Memory • Conditioning • Myrmica ruginodis • Food collection Abstract It has been known for a long time that ants can acquire behavioral reactions by conditioning and by imitation. The aim of the present work was to examine if they could, through such learning processes, acquire never previously exhibited behavior or learn to use new methods. Myrmica ruginodis workers were successively confronted with solid sugar and water set apart, a double door shutting their sugar water tube entrance, or a thin cotton barrier plugging their nest entrance. Each time, they could not find an appropriate method to solve the problem. They were then presented with the ‘solution’, i.e., the sugar wetted, the double door slightly opened, and the cotton barrier partly removed. Thereafter, when again confronted with the initial situation, old ants could act appropriately to solve the problem. Ants can thus learn by conditioning and by imitation and can acquire through these processes novel methods or behavioral acts even if they are unable to innovate spontaneously by themselves, to improvise, or to behave correctly in an unknown situation. INTRODUCTION if they could learn behaviors not included in their repertory and never exhibited, such as the use of a novel technique.
    [Show full text]
  • Population Structure of Myrmica Rubra (Hymeno­Ptera: Formicidae) in Part of Its Invasive Range Revealed by Nuclear DNA Markers and Aggression Analysis Barry J
    ISSN 1997-3500 Myrmecological News myrmecologicalnews.org Myrmecol. News 28: 35-43 doi: 10.25849/myrmecol.news_028:035 30 October 2018 Original Article Population structure of Myrmica rubra (Hymeno ptera: Formicidae) in part of its invasive range revealed by nuclear DNA markers and aggression analysis Barry J. Hicks & H. Dawn Marshall Abstract Myrmica rubra (Linnaeus, 1758) is considered an invasive ant in North America. The genetic relatedness and its dispersive abilities were investigated using microsatellite analysis of colonies on the island of Newfoundland, Canada. The genetic diversity of Newfoundland M. rubra was low, likely as the result of founder effect. Colonies located near each other (< 1 km) generally had low pairwise FST values, which we interpret as considerable mixing of colonies, while populations that are spatially separated showed higher FST values and very little mixing. Aggression analysis was also used to determine the relatedness of populations. In this analysis, considerable aggression was observed among M. ru- bra worker ants from the different localities, and it depended on the level of genetic relatedness. Calculation of Pearson correlation coefficients showed a positive correlation in aggressive behavior with genetic relatedness. The molecular and aggression analyses of M. rubra populations from Newfoundland localities indicate that these ants are not unicolonial over large areas but supercolonial with colonies genetically similar and with aggression reduced toward con-specifics at the local scale (< 1 km). Key words: Invasive ant, Myrmica rubra, dispersal, aggression, Formicidae. Received 20 July 2018; revision received 24 September 2018; accepted 9 October 2018 Subject Editor: Philip J. Lester Barry J. Hicks (contact author), College of the North Atlantic, 4 Pike’s Lane, Carbonear, Newfoundland and Labrador, Canada, A1Y 1A7; Department of Biology, Memorial University, St.
    [Show full text]
  • 111. Om the Alzatomy of Ants. B Y Sir JOHN
    111. Om the Alzatomy of Ants. By Sir JOHNLUBBOCK, Bart., M.P., F.R.S., F.L.S., D. C.L., LLD., Vice-Chancellor of the University of London, President of the Entomological Society. (Plates XI. & XII.) Read February 6th, 1879. Introductory Remnrks. IN conjunction with the observations on the habits of Ants, which the Society has done me the honour of publishing fyom time to time in the Journal, I have also been studying their anatomy, especially with reference to the muscular system. Of the anatomy of the head I have already given some account in the ‘Transactions of the Microscopical Society,’ 1877”. The present paper is devoted to the thorax, with special reference to Lusius Jlavus. Though j t is founded on numerous dissections, and on more than 1000 sections beautifully prepared for me by Mr. Newton, of the School of Mines, and Mr. Robertson, of Oxford, it is still very imperfect ; and I am only induced to bring it before the Society in its present incomplete state because, while I hope it will be found to add somewhat to our knowledge, I see little prospect of being able to work out the subject as thoroughly as I could wish. As a general rule, the thorax of insects is considered to consist of three more or less well-marked segments, usually known by the names suggested by Nitzsch-prothorax, mesothorax, and metathorax. Dr. Ratzeburg, however, published in 1832 a memoir (c Ueber Entwickelung der fuss- losen Hymenopteren-Larven, mit besonderer Rucksicht auf die Gattung Fool*mica ’), in which he maintained that the fifth segment of the larva forms, not the so-called cc scale,” or fist abdominal segment, but the hinder part of the thorax.
    [Show full text]
  • Ants' Ability in Solving Simple Problems
    International Journal of Biology; Vol. 9, No. 3; 2017 ISSN 1916-9671 E-ISSN 1916-968X Published by Canadian Center of Science and Education Ants’ Ability in Solving Simple Problems Marie-Claire Cammaerts1 1 Département de Biologie des organismes, Faculté des Sciences, Université Libre de Bruxelles, 50, Av F.D. Roosevelt, 1050 Bruxelles, Belgium Correspondence: Marie-Claire Cammaerts, Département de Biologie des organismes, Faculté des Sciences, Université Libre de Bruxelles, Belgium. Tel: 32-2673-4969. E-mail: [email protected] Received: April 24, 2017 Accepted: May 9, 2017 Online Published: May 12, 2017 doi:10.5539/ijb.v9n3p26 URL: https://doi.org/10.5539/ijb.v9n3p26 Abstract Aiming to know the extent of the ants’ cognitive abilities, we set Myrmica ruginodis workers in four problematic situations. We discovered that these ants could walk round a barrier, by foraging and navigating as usual, using known visual cues. They could walk preferentially on smooth substrates instead of rough ones, but did not memorize their choice. This behavior may be due to the easier deposit of pheromones on a smooth substrate. The ants could establish a single way when having only two narrow paths for going in and out of their nest. This was the consequence of the ants’ traffic and of the distinct pheromonal deposits while going in and out of the nest. The oldest ants needing sugar water could push a door for getting such water. They did so by having the audacity to go on walking, whatever the presence of a door. Such a door is not a tool sensu stricto.
    [Show full text]