Dishabituation and Sensitization Emerge As Separate Processes During Development in Ap/Ysia

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Dishabituation and Sensitization Emerge As Separate Processes During Development in Ap/Ysia The Journal of Neuroscience, January 1988, 8(l): 197-211 Dishabituation and Sensitization Emerge as Separate Processes During Development in Ap/ysia Catherine H. Rankin and Thomas J. Carew Departments of Psychology and Biology, Yale University, New Haven, Connecticut 06520 Until recently, dishabituation and sensitization have com- inhibitory component of tail shock that produces reflex monly been considered to reflect a unitary process: Sensi- depression. Moreover, there was a clear progression in the tization refers to a general facilitation produced by strong net effects of tail shock during development: reflex depres- or noxious stimuli that enhances subsequent responding; sion was produced in stages 11 and early stage 12, followed dishabituation has been thought to represent a special in- by a transition to reflex facilitation (sensitization) in late stage stance of sensitization in which the facilitation is simply su- 12. Finally, when sensitization emerged in late stage 12, the perimposed on a habituated response level. The unitary pro- process of dishabituation showed a significant increase cess hypothesis was based on the observation that both compared with previous developmental stages. decremented and nondecremented responses are facilitat- Our results permit 2 principal conclusions. First, in contrast ed by a common noxious or strong stimulus. However, this to a unitary process view, dishabituation and sensitization observation does not rule out the possibility that dishabitua- emerge as separate behavioral processes according to very tion and sensitization could reflect separate processes that different developmental timetables in Aplysia. Second, the are activated in parallel by a strong stimulus. Recent cellular magnitude of dishabituation appears to be determined by experiments by Hochner et al. (1986) suggest that this, in the interaction of 3 underlying processes: (1) the dishabit- fact, occurs in the sensory neurons of the gill withdrawal uation process itself; (2) an inhibitory process that com- reflex in Aplysia. A developmental analysis of learning in petes with dishabituation (see also Rankin and Carew, 1987b); the marine mollusc Aplysia permits a direct behavioral test and (3) a facilitatory process (sensitization) that augments of this hypothesis. If dishabituation and sensitization reflect dishabituation (see also Hochner et al., 1986). a unitary process then they should emerge at the same time ontogenetically. On the other hand, if they reflect different processes, then they might emerge according to different The 3 most commonly observed forms of nonassociativeleam- ontogenetic timetables. ing are habituation, dishabituation, and sensitization. Habit- In the present study we examined the temporal emergence uation involves a progressivedecrement in the amplitude of a of dishabituation and sensitization in the defensive siphon behavioral responseproduced by the repeatedelicitation of that withdrawal reflex in 3 stages of juvenile Aplysia: stage 11, response.Dishabituation involves the facilitation of habituated early stage 12, and late stage 12. Animals received one of responsesby the presentation of a strong or noxious stimulus, 2 kinds of training: D&habituation training, in which the effect and sensitization involves that facilitation of nonhabituated re- of strong tail shock on habituated responses was observed, sponsesby a similar presentation of a strong stimulus (Pavlov, and Sensitization training, in which the effect of strong tail 1927; Grether, 1938; for reviews, seeThompson and Spencer, shock on nondecremented responses was observed. We 1966; Groves and Thompson, 1970; Carew and Kandel, 1974). found that, while dishabituation was present in all stages Until recently, it has commonly been assumedthat dishabitua- examined, sensitization did not emerge until several weeks tion and sensitization reflect a unitary process:Sensitization has later, in late stage 12. These results were confirmed and been viewed asa generalfacilitatory process,and dishabituation extended in a group of animals that were tested twice: first has been thought to representa special instanceof sensitization in stage 11, when they showed no sensitization, and again in which the facilitatory processis simply superimposedon a 13 weeks later, in late stage 12, when they then showed habituated responselevel. This general conclusion has come significant sensitization. from studiesexamining a variety of systems,including the EEG Our analysis of nondecremented responses prior to the arousal response(Sharpless and Jasper, 1956) human abdom- emergence of sensitization also revealed an unexpected inal reflexes (Hagbarth and Kugelberg, 1958), the flexion reflex in the cat (Spencer et al., 1966a, b), and the gill and siphon withdrawal reflex in Aplysia (Carew et al., 1971; for a general Received Mar. 3, 1987; revised June 15, 1987; accepted June 17, 1987. review, seeGroves and Thompson, 1970). We wish to thank Deda Gillespie for her excellent help with scoring the data, In all of these studies, the basic observation that supported and our colleagues Emilie Marcus, Thomas G. Nolen, Mark Stopfer, Allan Wagner, and William Wright for their helpful criticism of an earlier version of the manu- the unitary processview wasthat both decrementedand nondec- script. We are also very grateful to the Howard Hughes Medical Institute, and to remented responseswere simultaneously facilitated by the pre- Tom Capo, for generously supplying juvenile Aplysia. This work was supported sentation of a single strong or noxious stimulus. The most par- by NIH BSRG Grant 507-RR-075 015 and NSF Grant BNS 83 11300 (to T.J.C.). Correspondence should be addressed to Thomas J. Carew, Department of Psy- simonious explanation of this kind of result was that a noxious chology, P.O. Box 11A Yale Station, New Haven, CT 06520. stimulus initiated a general arousal-like processthat was wide- Copyright 0 1988 Society for Neuroscience 0270-6474/88/010197-15$02.00/O spread in the nervous system, facilitating habituated and non- 198 Rankin and Carew - Development of Sensitization in Aplysia habituated responses alike. Although both reasonable and log- obtained from the Howard Hughes Medical Institute (Woods Hole, ically consistent, this explanation does not rule out the possibility MA). Small animals (2.5-8 mm) were maintained at 15°C in groups of 4-8 on seaweed (Gracilluria) in 50 ml plastic centrifuge tubes filled with that dishabituation and sensitization could reflect separate fa- seawater obtained from Marine Biological Laboratories. Larger animals cilitatory processes that are activated in parallel by a strong (18-25 mm) were kept in a 30 gal aquarium of artificial seawater main- stimulus. In fact, direct cellular evidence for 2 separate facili- tained at 15°C (Instant Ocean). The animals used in this study were tatory processes has recently been provided in Aplysia sensory developmental stages 11 and 12, as described by Kreigstein (1977). neurons by Hochner and colleagues(1986) who suggestedthat However, additional morphological criteria were used to further sub- divide stage 12. Our combined criteria were as follows: Stage 11 animals dishabituation and sensitization in adult Aplysia are produced, were at least 47 d posthatching and 1.5-3.0 mm in length. The rhi- at least in part, by different cellular mechanisms. nophores were seen only as rudimentary buds above the eyes. Early One way to addressthe question of whether dishabituation stage 12 animals were at least 60 d posthatching. They were 5.0-8.0 and sensitization reflect a unitary process behaviorally is to mm in length, had well-developed anterior tentacles and rhinophores, and had white spots distributed over their bodies, with large patches of examine how they emergeduring development. If a single pro- white on the parapodia. The genital groove was visible on the right side cessis involved, both forms of learning should be expressed of the animal stretching from the anterior insertion of the parapodia to simultaneouslywhen that processemerges developmentally. Al- the propodium. Late stage 12 animals were 18-25 mm in length, had ternatively, if more than one processis involved, it might be developed mottled colored patterns like adult animals, and weighed possibleto separatethem ontogenetically. The defensive with- 400-600 mg. Late stage 12 animals looked very much like miniature adults. drawal reflex of the gill and siphon in the marine molluscAplysia Behavioralprocedures. Details of the behavioral procedures have been calijbrnica provides an excellent preparation in which to ex- previously described by Rankin and Carew (1987a). Briefly, a Zeiss amine this question directly. First, adult Aplysia exhibit habit- stereomicroscope (model SV8) fitted with a color video camera (JVC uation, dishabituation, and sensitization of this reflex, and these S-62U) was used to videotape behavioral responses. A timing signal from a video timer (Panasonic Time-Date Generator WJ-8 10) was su- processeshave been extensively analyzed on both behavioral perimposed onto the video image. Image and signal were recorded by and cellular levels (for review, seeHochner et al., 1986; Carew, a videotape recorder (Panasonic NV-8950) which permitted slow-mo- 1987; Hawkins et al., 1987). Second,it has recently been shown tion and stop-frame playback analysis when displayed on a color video to be possibleto analyze the development of different forms of monitor (Sony PMV- 12704). Aplysia Stage II animals were restrained with 3 suction micropipettes
Recommended publications
  • A Psychological Approach to Musical Form: the Habituation–Fluency Theory of Repetition
    A Psychological Approach to Musical Form: The Habituation–Fluency Theory of Repetition David Huron With the possible exception of dance and meditation, there appears to be nothing else in common human experience that is comparable to music in its repetitiveness (Kivy 1993; Ockelford 2005; Margulis 2013). Narrative arti- facts like movies, novels, cartoon strips, stories, and speeches have much less internal repetition. Even poetry is less repetitive than music. Occasionally, architecture can approach music in repeating some elements, but only some- times. There appears to be no visual analog to the sort of trance–inducing music that can engage listeners for hours. Although dance and meditation may be more repetitive than music, dance is rarely performed in the absence of music, and meditation tellingly relies on imagining a repeated sound or mantra (Huron 2006: 267). Repetition can be observed in music from all over the world (Nettl 2005). In much music, a simple “strophic” pattern is evident in which a single phrase or passage is repeated over and over. When sung, it is common for successive repetitions to employ different words, as in the case of strophic verses. However, it is also common to hear the same words used with each repetition. In the Western art–music tradition, internal patterns of repetition are commonly discussed under the rubric of form. Writing in The Oxford Companion to Music, Percy Scholes characterized musical form as “a series of strategies designed to find a successful mean between the opposite extremes of unrelieved repetition and unrelieved alteration” (1977: 289). Scholes’s characterization notwithstanding, musical form entails much more than simply the pattern of repetition.
    [Show full text]
  • Modeling the Dishabituation Hierarchy: the Role of the Primordial Hippocampus*
    Biol. Cybern. 67, 535-544 (1992) Biological Cybemetics Springer-Verlag 1992 Modeling the dishabituation hierarchy: The role of the primordial hippocampus* DeLiang Wang and Michael A. Arbib Center for Neural Engineering, University of Southern California, Los Angeles, CA 90089-2520, USA Received January 30, 1992/accepted in revised form April 6, 1992 Abstract. We present a neural model for the organiza- orienting response to a certain stimulus applied at a tion and neural dynamics of the medial pallium, the given location, the response can be released by the same toad's homolog of mammalian hippocampus. A neural stimulus applied at a different retinal locus (Eikmanns mechanism, called cumulative shrinking, is proposed 1955; Ewert and Ingle 1971). (2) Hierarchical stimulus for mapping temporal responses from the anterior tha- specificity. Another stimulus given at the same locus lamus into a form of population coding referenced by may restore the response habituated by a previous spatial positions. Synaptic plasticity is modeled as an stimulus. Only certain stimuli can dishabituate a previ- interaction of two dynamic processes which simulates ously habituated response. Ewert and Kehl (1978) acquisition and both short-term and long-term forget- demonstrated that this dishabituation forms a hierarchy ting. The structure of the medial pallium model plus the (Fig. 1A), where a stimulus can dishabituate the habitu- plasticity model allows us to provide an account of the ated responses of another stimulus if the latter is lower neural mechanisms of habituation and dishabituation. in the hierarchy. Computer simulations demonstrate a remarkable match A model developed by Wang and Arbib (1991a) between the model performance and the original exper- showed how a group of cells in the anterior thalamus imental data on which the dishabituation hierarchy was (AT) could respond to stimuli higher in the hierarchy based.
    [Show full text]
  • Gabab Regulation of Methamphetamine-Induced Associative Learning
    Loyola University Chicago Loyola eCommons Dissertations Theses and Dissertations 2010 Gabab Regulation of Methamphetamine-Induced Associative Learning Robin Michelle Voigt Loyola University Chicago Follow this and additional works at: https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss Part of the Pharmacology Commons Recommended Citation Voigt, Robin Michelle, "Gabab Regulation of Methamphetamine-Induced Associative Learning" (2010). Dissertations. 38. https://ecommons.luc.edu/luc_diss/38 This Dissertation is brought to you for free and open access by the Theses and Dissertations at Loyola eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Dissertations by an authorized administrator of Loyola eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution-Noncommercial-No Derivative Works 3.0 License. Copyright © 2010 Robin Michelle Voigt LOYOLA UNIVERSITY CHICAGO GABAB REGULATION OF METHAMPHETAMINE-INDUCED ASSOCIATIVE LEARNING A DISSERTATION SUBMITTED TO THE FACULTY OF THE GRADUATE SCHOOL IN CANDIDACY FOR THE DEGREE OF DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY PROGRAM IN MOLECULAR PHARMACOLOGY & THERAPEUTICS BY ROBIN MICHELLE VOIGT CHICAGO, IL DECEMBER 2010 Copyright by Robin Michelle Voigt, 2010 All rights reserved ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Without the support of so many generous and wonderful individuals I would not have been able to be where I am today. First, I would like to thank my Mother for her belief that I could accomplish anything that I set my mind to. I would also like to thank my dissertation advisor, Dr. Celeste Napier, for encouraging and challenging me to be better than I thought possible. I extend gratitude to my committee members, Drs. Julie Kauer, Adriano Marchese, Micky Marinelli, and Karie Scrogin for their guidance and insightful input.
    [Show full text]
  • Journal of Neurobiology, Parametric Studies of The
    JOURNAL OF NEUROBIOLOGY,VOL. 1, NO. 3, PP. 345-360 PARAMETRIC STUDIES OF THE RESPONSE DECREMENT PRODUCED BY MECHANICAL STIMULI IN THE PROTOZOAN, Stentor coeruleus DAVID C. WOOD* Brain Research Laboratory, The University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, Michigan 43104 SUMMARY A decrement in the probability of eliciting a response is observed dur- ing the course of repetitive stimulation in many response systems of both metazoa and protozoa. Those forms of metazoan response decre- ment called habituation have recently been characterized behaviorally. In the studies reported here, the response decrement of the contractile protozoan, Stentor coeruleus, to repeated mechanical stimulation was systemically characterized to provide a comparison with metazoan habit- uation. This change in response probability was closely approximated by a negative exponential function after the first few stimuli. Animals responded to parametric variations of stimulus amplitude, interstimulus interval and retention period in a manner paralleling that observed in metazoa. However, neither interpolated large amplitude mechanical stimuli nor suprathreshold electrical stimuli produced dishabituation in Stentor though these stimuli are among the most effective dishabituating stimuli for metazoan tactile response systems. Behavioral analysis of the response decrement proceeded on the assumption that Stentor con- tains receptor and effector mechanisms only. Since the response decre- ment was not correlated with a change in responsiveness to electrical stimuli, the ability of the animal to contract appears to be unaltered by the process producing the response decrement. On the other hand, weak mechanical prestimuli were found to increase the rate of the response decrement which suggests that a process of receptor adaption is opera- tive.
    [Show full text]
  • Orienting Response Reinstatement and Dishabituation: the Effects of Substituting
    Orienting Response Reinstatement and Dishabituation: The Effects of Substituting, Adding and Deleting Components of Nonsignificant Stimuli Gershon Ben-Shakar, Itamar Gati, Naomi Ben-Bassat, and Galit Sniper The Hebrew University of Jerusalem Acknowledgments: This research was supported by The Israel Science Foundation founded by The Academy for Sciences and Humanities. We thank Dana Ballas, Rotem Shelef, Limor Bar, and Erga Sinai for their help in the data collection. We also thank John Furedy and an anonymous reviewer for the helpful comments on an earlier version of this manuscript. The article was written while the first author was on a sabbatical leave at Brandeis University. We wish to thank the Psychology Department at Brandeis University for the facilities and the help provided during this period. Address requests for reprints to: Prof. Gershon Ben-Shakhar Department of Psychology, The Hebrew University, Jerusalem, 91905, ISRAEL Running Title: OR Reinstatement and Dishabituation 2 ABSTRACT This study examined the prediction that stimulus novelty is negatively related to the measure of common features, shared by the stimulus input and representations of preceding events, and positively related to the measure of their distinctive features. This prediction was tested in two experiments, which used sequences of nonsignificant verbal and pictorial compound stimuli. A test stimulus (TS) was presented after 9 repetitions of a standard stimulus (SS), followed by 2 additional repetitions of SS. TS was created by either substituting 0, 1, or 2 stimulus components of SS (Experiment 1), or by either adding or deleting 0, 1, or 2 components of SS (Experiment 2). The dependent measure was the electrodermal component of the OR to both TS (OR reinstatement) and SS that immediately followed TS (dishabituation).
    [Show full text]
  • The Role of Habituation in Social Fear by Suzanne N. Avery Dissertation
    Slow to Warm Up: The Role of Habituation in Social Fear By Suzanne N. Avery Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the Graduate School of Vanderbilt University in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY in Neuroscience August, 2015 Nashville, Tennessee Approved: David Zald, Ph.D. Bunmi Olatunji, Ph.D. Brandon Ally, Ph.D. Jennifer Blackford, Ph.D. Copyright © 2015 by Suzanne N. Avery All Rights Reserved ii To my husband, Stacy, for his unending support, and my daughter, Sophie, for whom I do everything iii ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I would first and foremost like to thank my advisor, Dr. Jennifer Blackford, who is a truly exceptional scientist, teacher and mentor. She has encouraged me to pursue every opportunity for scientific achievement and has mentored me closely through each step. Her enthusiasm to help me become an excellent scientist and her generosity with her time and effort have made my graduate training an incredible experience. She has an incredible passion for science and possesses a brilliant combination of curiosity, creativity, motivation, intelligence and passion that inspires all of those who have the pleasure to work with her. I am eternally grateful for your mentorship—thank you is not enough. I would also like to thank the current and former members of the Blackford lab, including Jacqueline Clauss, Ross VanDerKlok, and Brittany Matthews, whose assistance and encouragement have made this work possible. My gratitude goes out to my dissertation committee, who have provided invaluable feedback and whose suggestions have significantly improved this project. Thank you for your insights and help Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Disrupted Habituation in the Early Stage of Psychosis
    Biological Psychiatry: CNNI Archival Report Disrupted Habituation in the Early Stage of Psychosis Suzanne N. Avery, Maureen McHugo, Kristan Armstrong, Jennifer U. Blackford, Neil D. Woodward, and Stephan Heckers ABSTRACT BACKGROUND: Learning and memory are impaired in schizophrenia. Some theories have proposed that one form of memory, habituation, is particularly impaired. Preliminary evidence suggests that memory impairment is associated with failed hippocampal habituation in patients with chronic schizophrenia. We studied how abnormal habituation of the hippocampus is related to relational memory deficits in the early stage of psychosis. METHODS: We measured hippocampal activity in 62 patients with early psychosis and 70 healthy individuals using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Habituation was defined as the slope of functional magnetic resonance imaging signal change to repeated presentations of faces and objects. Relational memory ability was measured as the slope of preferential viewing during a face-scene pair eye movement task outside the scanner. RESULTS: Patients with early psychosis showed impaired relational memory (p , .001) and less hippocampal habituation to objects (p = .01) than healthy control subjects. In the healthy control group, better relational memory was associated with faster anterior hippocampal habituation (faces, r = 2.28, p = .03). In contrast, patients with early psychosis showed no brain-behavior relationship (r = .12, p = .40). CONCLUSIONS: We found evidence for disrupted hippocampal habituation in the early stage of psychosis along with an altered association between hippocampal habituation and relational memory ability. These results suggest that neural habituation may provide a novel target for early cognitive interventions in psychosis. Keywords: First episode, Hippocampus, Novelty, Relational memory, Schizophrenia, Visual cortex https://doi.org/10.1016/j.bpsc.2019.06.007 Repetition is one of the most familiar memory tools.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning Learning and Reinforcement Simple Learning Habituation and Sensitization Classical Conditioning Classical Conditioning
    Learning Learning and reinforcement Learning A long-lasting change in behavior, or potential to change behavior, that results from experience Reinforcement Anything that affects the probability that a particular behavior will occur Simple learning Habituation and sensitization Aplysia Repeated presentation of stimulus chaffinch Habituation Sensitization Percent birds respondingbirds Percent Playbacks Eric Kandel Classical conditioning Classical conditioning Is there any adaptive significance of classical conditioning? food (US) salivation (UR) food (US) + tone (CS) salivation (UR) tone (CS) salivation (CR) Ivan Pavlov 1 Classical conditioning Classical conditioning ♂ ♀ blue gourami Karen Hollis Mount Holyoke Classical conditioning Classical conditioning How might classical conditioning work in nature? Courtship behavior by female (US) Courtship (UR) Courtship behavior by female (US) + sex-recognition cue (CS) Courtship (UR) Sex-recognition cue (CS) Courtship (CR) Japanese quail Heritability of conditioned response Gene × environment interactions Proboscis extension response conditioned to various odors Evidence of significant heritability Tryon Maze Dull and Bright rats Offspring score Offspring Parent score 2 Operant conditioning Laws of effect and exercise Thorndike Operant conditioning Operant conditioning Operant conditioning = association between Negative behavior and its consequences Positive reinforcement reinforcement Increase likelihood that behavior will occur B.F. Skinner Operant conditioning What factors affect conditioning?
    [Show full text]
  • Habituation As a Neural Algorithm for Online Odor Discrimination
    Habituation as a neural algorithm for online odor discrimination Yang Shena, Sanjoy Dasguptab, and Saket Navlakhaa,1 aSimons Center for Quantitative Biology, Cold Spring Harbor Laboratory, Cold Spring Harbor, NY 11724; and bComputer Science and Engineering Department, University of California San Diego, La Jolla, CA 92093 Edited by Timothy O’Leary, University of Cambridge, Cambridge, United Kingdom, and accepted by Editorial Board Member Gina G. Turrigiano April 13, 2020 (received for review September 5, 2019) Habituation is a form of simple memory that suppresses neural the extinction of the stimulus. This is in contrast with faster forms activity in response to repeated, neutral stimuli. This process is crit- of habituation, such as odorant receptor neuron (ORN) adapta- ical in helping organisms guide attention toward the most salient tion (17, 18) occurring on the order of hundreds of milliseconds, and novel features in the environment. Here, we follow known and longer forms of habituation, such as long-term habituation circuit mechanisms in the fruit fly olfactory system to derive a (16), which lasts for days. Different time scales of habituation simple algorithm for habituation. We show, both empirically and have also been described in the vertebrate olfactory system (1). analytically, that this algorithm is able to filter out redundant infor- Short-term habituation is of interest because it requires a rela- mation, enhance discrimination between odors that share a similar tively fast online mechanism for its initiation and decay, yet it is background, and improve detection of novel components in odor also stable enough to have a lasting effect. mixtures. Overall, we propose an algorithmic perspective on the Prior work has studied the mechanisms of short-term habitu- biological mechanism of habituation and use this perspective to ation in the mouse and honeybee olfactory circuits (19–22), but understand how sensory physiology can affect odor perception.
    [Show full text]
  • Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM IV to 5 Subcategories (4 to 5)
    Emory Medicine: Strategic ImplementingRepositioning an Early 2013- 2017Intervention for the Prevention of PTSD in Emergency Department Patients Barbara Olasov Rothbaum, Ph.D., ABPP Paul A. Janssen Chair in Neuropsychopharmacology Director, Trauma and Anxiety Recovery Program Associate Vice Chair of Clinical Research Professor, Department of Psychiatry Emory University School of Medicine Disclosure This study was supported by National Institute of Mental Health Grant Numbers R34 MH083078 and R01 MH071537, and the Emory Center for Injury Control, Center for Disease Control Grant Number 5R49CE001494 and a NARSAD Distinguished Investigator award to Dr. Rothbaum. The authors report no biomedical financial interests or potential conflicts of interest. Recognition and Description of Posttraumatic Stress Disorder DSM IV to 5 subcategories (4 to 5) Criteria DSM-IV DSM-5 A Trauma Trauma B reexperiencing intrusion C Avoidance and avoidance numbing D hyperarousal Negative alterations in cognitions and mood E - Marked alterations in arousal and reactivity A Prospective Examination of PTSD in Rape Victims Percent of Victims with PTS 100 94 80 79 70 64 59 55 60 53 53 45 48 47 41 40 Percent Percent (%) 20 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Assessment Rothbaum BO et al. J Traumatic Stress. 1992;5(3):463; NIMH Grant No. R01MH42178 Severity of PTSD Symptoms Non-PTS PTS 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 Assessment Rothbaum BO et al. J Traumatic Stress. 1992;5(3):464; NIMH Grant No. R01MH42178 Extinction and Habituation • We view PTSD as a disorder of extinction McSweeney FK, Swindell S.
    [Show full text]
  • 'T: Ottawa
    csf 001 TOWARD A HUMANISTIC-TRANSPERSONAL PSYCHOLOGY OF RELIGION by Peter A. Campbell Thesis presented to the Department of Religious Studies of the University of Ottawa as partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy ^ «<o% BIBUOTHEQUES * ^^BlBi; 'T: Ottawa LlBRAKicS \ Vsily oi <P Ottawa, Canada, 1972 Peter A. Campbell, Ottawa 1972. UMI Number: DC53628 INFORMATION TO USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. Broken or indistinct print, colored or poor quality illustrations and photographs, print bleed-through, substandard margins, and improper alignment can adversely affect reproduction. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if unauthorized copyright material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. UMI® UMI Microform DC53628 Copyright 2011 by ProQuest LLC All rights reserved. This microform edition is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS I wish to express my gratitude to Jean-Marie Beniskos, Ph.D., of the Faculty of Education, University of Ottawa, for his helpful direction of this dissertation. My heartfelt thanks are also due to Edwin M. McMahon, my close collabora­ tor and companion whose constructive criticism and steady encouragement have been a continued source of support through­ out this research project. CURRICULUM STUDIORUM Peter A. Campbell was born January 21, 1935, in San Francisco, California. He received the Bachelor of Arts degree in 1957 and the Master of Arts degree and Licentiate in Philosophy in 1959 from Gonzaga University, Spokane, Washington.
    [Show full text]
  • The Effect of Habituation and Changes in Cognition on Anxious Children's
    Louisiana State University LSU Digital Commons LSU Master's Theses Graduate School 2014 The effect of habituation and changes in cognition on anxious children's performance on the WISC- IV Franziska Noack Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College Follow this and additional works at: https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_theses Part of the Psychology Commons Recommended Citation Noack, Franziska, "The effect of habituation and changes in cognition on anxious children's performance on the WISC-IV" (2014). LSU Master's Theses. 988. https://digitalcommons.lsu.edu/gradschool_theses/988 This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by the Graduate School at LSU Digital Commons. It has been accepted for inclusion in LSU Master's Theses by an authorized graduate school editor of LSU Digital Commons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. THE EFFECT OF HABITUATION AND CHANGES IN COGNITION ON ANXIOUS CHILDREN’S PERFORMANCE ON THE WISC-IV A Thesis Submitted to the Graduate Faculty of the Louisiana State University and Agricultural and Mechanical College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Arts in The Department of Psychology by Franziska Noack B.S., University of Houston-Clear Lake, 2012 May 2014 To my husband Daniel NoackLeSage and my parents Ilona and Ralf Noack. Thank you for your continuous support, kind words of encouragement, and loving patience. ------ An meinen Mann Daniel NoackLeSage und meine Eltern, Ilona und Ralf Noack. Danke für eure grenzenlose Unterstützung, eure freundlichen Worte der Ermutigung, und euer liebevolles Verständnis. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS I would like to express my appreciation for the support and time allocated to this project by my advisor and mentor Thompson E.
    [Show full text]