Sound Localization

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Sound Localization L24. Mate Calling in Animal Communication Crickets: a model system for studies of neuroethology of rhythmic behavior Oct. 5, 2011 BioNB 4240 Cornell University BioNB424 Neuroethology 1 2 © Carl D. Hopkins Why Study Animal Communication Signals? Why Invertebrates? Why Insects? Why Crickets? 1) Readily accessible nervous systems 1) The signals (=Displays) tend to be “Fixed” Action Patterns (FAPs) - all-or-none, highly stereotyped - large cells (because they need to be redundant and quickly recognized) - small number of cells often released by social stimuli; signals also act as releasers - cells may be consistently identified in each individual 2) Complexity vs. Tractability 2) Relatively simple behavior - Am. Comm. signals are interesting (more complex than most - more reliable than many vertebrates FAPs) - stereotyped: reliable and repeatable. - greater stereotypy than many vertebrate signals 3) Ubiquity and Functionality - nearly all animals communicate - adaptive significance of signals can be readily assessed 4) Sensory, Motor, and Sensory-Motor Integration 3 4 3 Different Song Types Calling Acheta domesticus Gryllus rubens Songs Chirps 1) Calling song is for 5 advertisement 0 Trill 2) Aggression song 10 kHz occurs when a Allonemobius sparsalus Oecanthus californicus male enters another male‟s territory and a fight ensues 3) Courtship song 0 Hz occurs when a Eunemobius confusus Gryllotalpa major female enters a male‟s territory in response to his call 2 sec 5 T. Walker Singing crickets of North America 6 http://buzz.ifas.ufl.edu/index.htm 1 Acheta domesticus Teleogryllus texensis Divergence between the Courtship Songs of the Field Crickets Gryllus texensis and Gryllus rubens (Orthoptera, Gryllidae) Mark J. Fitzpatrick & David A. Gray Ethology Volume 107 Issue 12 Page 1075-1085, December 2001 7 8 Song Variation Song Variation Pulses Pulses - number of cycles - number of cycles - duration - duration - carrier frequency - carrier frequency Chirps/Trills Chirps/Trills - number of pulses - number of pulses - duration - duration - pulse period (rate) - pulse period (rate) - inter-pulse interval - inter-pulse interval Chirp Rates Chirp Rates 9 10 What Song Features Matter to Females? Sound Production 1) Females prefer a narrow range of pulse rates 2) Preference matches pulse rates of conspecific song F M 11 12 Gryllus wings are sexually dimorphic 2 Sounds are Generated 1) The right wing lays with the Forewings over the left wing (Tegmen; ; tegmina) 2) As the wings close, the plectrum of the left wing scrapes The plectrum serves as a against the file of the scraper, which rubs against right wing the file of the opposite wing 3) Each notch in the file corresponds to a single cycle within the The harp serves as a pulse resonator that amplifies the sound at the carrier 4) Full closure of the frequency wings corresponds to a single pulse 5) No sound on opening 13 14 H. Bennett-Clark. J. Exp. Biol. 206:1479. 15 16 Gryllus campestris close Neuromuscular Sound Calling Activity During open Songs Closer Song Production Opener Gryllus bimaculatus -Studies of David close open Bentley closer -Wings opened with depressor muscles opener (M99) - Wings closed with Gryllus campestris elevator muscles Courtship (M90) Songs - Pulse characteristics determined by MUSCLE Gryllus bimaculatus CONTRACTION and WING MORPHOLOGY 17 18 3 Songs can be triggered or Stimulating at different inhibited by stimulating in frequencies has direct certain parts of the brain effects on chirping or in the connectives frequency, but no effects going from the brain to the on characteristics of thoracic ganglia pulses or chirps Stimulation Stimulation Frequency This suggests that song This suggests that chirp production is INITIATED characteristics are IN THE BRAIN CONTROLLED BY THE GANGLIA 19 20 Hierarchical Song Control Central Pattern Generators (CPG) 1) Neural circuits that drive stereotyped, programmed, rhythmic 3) Sound Familiar? movements 4) Unlike original FAP concept, 2) Activation results in output patterns are not completely fixed output patterns in stereotyped. Different patterns the absence of can be produced through continued input or neuromodulatory inputs feedback 5) Generally a result of both cellular Chirp Decision Chirp Parameters Pulse Parameters (pulse (ion channels) and network (yes/no, chirp rate) (pulse rate, number duration, number of cycles, properties of pulses, chirp carrier frequency) duration) 21 22 Central Pattern Ion Channel Central Pattern Post-Inhibitory Generators (CPG) Properties Generators (CPG) Rebound Rhythmic Burster Pacemaker Inhibitory Input No Post-Inhibitory Rebound Post-Inhibitory Rebound 1) Resting Potential > Threshold Hyperpolarization-activated Na+ channels 2) Ca2+-activated K+ Channels 23 24 4 Central Pattern Post-Inhibitory Rebound Central Pattern Generators (CPG) plus Generators (CPG) Reciprocal Inhibition Dep Open Wing Motor Neuron Start Elev + - To Depressor Sound - + End Rhythmic Activity in Rate of cycling can determine + Phase with Song, To Elevator the pulse rate within chirps - Alternating with Interneuron Inhibition Close Wing 25 26 Central Pattern Central Pattern Generators (CPG) Generators (CPG) Interneurons that There are interneurons in the ganglia that are rhythmically active in show rhythmic phase with chirps: they most likely start and stop the cycling of pulses excitation within chirps, determining pulse number and chirp duration Interneurons that show rhythmic inhibition at the same time This can explain pulse rate, but CPG must also control pulse number/chirp duration 27 28 Command Neurons Identification of the (Kupferman and Weiss 1978) Cricket Song Command Neuron (Hedwig 1996, 2000) Criteria for Designation as a „Command Neuron‟: 1) Correlation: Neuron‟s activity should be directly related to production of the behavior 2) Sufficiency: Stimulating neuron should induce behavior 3) Necessity: Inhibiting neuron should eliminate behavior 29 30 5 Identification of the Cricket Song Command Neuron (Hedwig 1996, 2000) Command neuron stimulation. 31 32 The criteria of both necessity and Genetics of sufficiency have been Cricket Song met. Thus, it is likely the (Bentley and Hoy, 1972; command neuron that Hoy et al. 1977) drives chirping. Hybrids have calls that As predicted, command are intermediate in neuron activity only several features influences chirp Females prefer the calls production. It has NO of their hybrid crosses EFFECT on pulses within chirp, which are Calls and preferences controlled by the CPG in are genetically the meta- and meso- controlled and thoracic ganglia correlated 33 34 Review of Important General Concepts Evolution of a silent crickets on Kauai 1) Sound production involves interactions between peripheral sound- generating structures and central nervous system control of those structures 2) Sound motor output is hierarchically organized. Decision command pattern generation muscle contraction 3) Stereotyped motor output is often under the control of central pattern generators (CPGs) that operate in the absence of any additional input. (CPG is a cellular/circuit oscillator) 4) CPGs may be activated by „command neurons‟, which are defined as both necessary and sufficient for production of the behavior 5) Vocal communication signals may be genetically controlled and tightly linked to the genetics of the auditory system. 6) Cricket sound production is advantageous for communication (but vulnerable to parasitism). Zuk, M., Rotenberry, J. T., and Tinghitella, R. M. (2006). Silent night: adaptive disappearance of a sexual35 36 signal in a parasitized population of field crickets. Biology Letters. DOI:10.1098/rsbl.2006.0539 6 .
Recommended publications
  • Behavioral Choices: How Neuronal Networks Make Decisions Dispatch
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by Elsevier - Publisher Connector Current Biology, Vol. 13, R140–R142, February 18, 2003, ©2003 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved. PII S0960-9822(03)00076-9 Behavioral Choices: How Neuronal Dispatch Networks Make Decisions Ronald L. Calabrese Shaw and Kristan [3] asked how the decision between shortening and swimming is made by the leech — specifically at what level the antagonism To survive, animals must constantly make behavioral between these behavioral networks occurs. What they choices. The analysis of simple, almost binary, found was somewhat surprising. At the trigger level behavioral choices in invertebrate animals with there was no antagonism: stimuli that activated short- restricted nervous systems is beginning to yield ening and swimming both activated trigger neurons. insight into how neuronal networks make such Even at the decision or command neuron level, some decisions. neurons were activated by both types of stimulus. One key neuron — cell 204 — however, was strongly inhibited by stimuli that led to shortening. The neurons Simple behavioral choices often seem binary and of the swim CPG were similarly mixed in their sequential. For example, an animal perceives some- responses. Some elements were strongly inhibited by thing novel in its environment, it chooses approach shortening stimuli and others were excited by short- over withdrawal, and sensing potential food, it chooses ening stimuli. to eat or to reject the item. Such decisions often begin What is to be made of these observations? CPG with a drive that originates in a need that is, in turn, neurons are multifunctional — there is a large body of conditioned by internal state and external stimuli.
    [Show full text]
  • Fixed Action Patterns and the Central Nervous System
    9.20 M.I.T. 2013 Lecture #6 Fixed Action Patterns and the Central Nervous System 1 Scott ch 2, “Controlling behavior: the role of the nervous system” 3. Give an example of a “supernormal stimulus” that acts as a releaser of a fixed action pattern in herring gull chicks. (See p 21) • See the conspicuous red-orange spot on the beak of an adult Herring gull on the next slide. Gull chicks respond to this visual stimulus with a gaping response—which elicits a feeding response from the parent. • A stronger gaping response can be elicited by a human who moves a yellow pencil painted with an orange spot. The spot plus the movement forms a “supernormal” stimulus. • Another example: Triggering the egg-rolling response from an adult gull: A larger-than-normal egg can elicit a stronger response. 2 Courtesy of Bruce Stokes on Flickr. License CC BY-NC-SA. 3 Can you give examples of supernormal stimuli for humans? 4 Supernormal stimuli for humans: • Foods: Sweet in taste, high in fats (Beware of restaurants!) • Stimuli of sexual attraction: The “poster effect” in advertizing • Enhancements of male appearance – Shoulder width, exagerated – Penis prominence enhanced: Sheath in tribal dress, cowl in medieval constumes • Enhancements of female appearance: – Waist-to-hip ratio enhancements: Girdle, bustle – Breast prominence increased – Lip color, size enhanced (How? For what purpose?) – Shoulder size: But what is the purpose of shoulder pads in women’s dress? 5 Scott ch 2, “Controlling behavior: the role of the nervous system” 4. Define: Primary sensory neuron, secondary sensory neuron, motor neuron, interneuron (neuron of the great intermediate net).
    [Show full text]
  • Of the Lateral Giant Escape Neurons in Crayfish Sensory Activation And
    Sensory Activation and Receptive Field Organization of the Lateral Giant Escape Neurons in Crayfish Yen-Chyi Liu and Jens Herberholz J Neurophysiol 104:675-684, 2010. First published 26 May 2010; doi:10.1152/jn.00391.2010 You might find this additional info useful... This article cites 57 articles, 31 of which can be accessed free at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/104/2/675.full.html#ref-list-1 Updated information and services including high resolution figures, can be found at: http://jn.physiology.org/content/104/2/675.full.html Additional material and information about Journal of Neurophysiology can be found at: http://www.the-aps.org/publications/jn This infomation is current as of February 10, 2012. Downloaded from jn.physiology.org on February 10, 2012 Journal of Neurophysiology publishes original articles on the function of the nervous system. It is published 12 times a year (monthly) by the American Physiological Society, 9650 Rockville Pike, Bethesda MD 20814-3991. Copyright © 2010 by the American Physiological Society. ISSN: 0022-3077, ESSN: 1522-1598. Visit our website at http://www.the-aps.org/. J Neurophysiol 104: 675–684, 2010. First published May 26, 2010; doi:10.1152/jn.00391.2010. Sensory Activation and Receptive Field Organization of the Lateral Giant Escape Neurons in Crayfish Yen-Chyi Liu1 and Jens Herberholz1,2 1Department of Psychology, 2Neuroscience and Cognitive Science Program, University of Maryland, College Park, Maryland Submitted 28 April 2010; accepted in final form 26 May 2010 Liu YC, Herberholz J. Sensory activation and receptive field 1999; Herberholz 2007; Krasne and Edwards 2002a; Wine and organization of the lateral giant escape neurons in crayfish.
    [Show full text]
  • Portia Perceptions: the Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping Spider
    Portia Perceptions: The Umwelt of an Araneophagic Jumping 1 Spider Duane P. Harland and Robert R. Jackson The Personality of Portia Spiders are traditionally portrayed as simple, instinct-driven animals (Savory, 1928; Drees, 1952; Bristowe, 1958). Small brain size is perhaps the most compelling reason for expecting so little flexibility from our eight-legged neighbors. Fitting comfortably on the head of a pin, a spider brain seems to vanish into insignificance. Common sense tells us that compared with large-brained mammals, spiders have so little to work with that they must be restricted to a circumscribed set of rigid behaviors, flexibility being a luxury afforded only to those with much larger central nervous systems. In this chapter we review recent findings on an unusual group of spiders that seem to be arachnid enigmas. In a number of ways the behavior of the araneophagic jumping spiders is more comparable to that of birds and mammals than conventional wisdom would lead us to expect of an arthropod. The term araneophagic refers to these spiders’ preference for other spiders as prey, and jumping spider is the common English name for members of the family Saltici- dae. Although both their common and the scientific Latin names acknowledge their jumping behavior, it is really their unique, complex eyes that set this family of spiders apart from all others. Among spiders (many of which have very poor vision), salticids have eyes that are by far the most specialized for resolving fine spatial detail. We focus here on the most extensively studied genus, Portia. Before we discuss the interrelationship between the salticids’ uniquely acute vision, their predatory strategies, and their apparent cognitive abilities, we need to offer some sense of what kind of animal a jumping spider is; to do this, we attempt to offer some insight into what we might call Portia’s personality.
    [Show full text]
  • Command Neurons Are Often Defined As Neurons Which, When Stimulated by the Experimenter, Evoke Some Behavioral Response
    THE BEHAVIORAL AND BRAIN SCIENCES (1978), 1,3-39 Printed in the United States of America The command neuron concept Irving Kupfermann Department of Psychiatry and Division of Neurobiologyand Behavior, College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University, New York, N Y 10032 Klaudiusz R. Weiss Department of Psychiatry and Division of Neurobiology and Behavior, College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University, New York, N Y 10032 Abstract: The notion of the command cell has been highly influential in invertebrate neurobiology, and related notions have been increasingly used in research on the vertebrate nervous system. The term "command neuron" implies that the neuron has some critical function in the generation of a normally occurring behavior. Nevertheless, most authors either explicitly or implicitly use a strictly operational definition, independent of considerations of normal behavioral function. That is, command neurons are often defined as neurons which, when stimulated by the experimenter, evoke some behavioral response. Even when utilizing such an operational definition, investigators frequently differ on what they consider to be the exact characteristics that a neuron must have (or not have) to be considered a command cell. A few authors appear to treat command neurons in relation to normal function, but a precise be- haviorally relevant definition has not been specified. Because of the ambiguity in the definition of command neurons, the term can refer to a wide variety of neurons which may play divergent behavioral roles. In some ways the attempt to label a cell as a command neuron may interfere with the process of discovering the complex causal determinants of behavior.
    [Show full text]
  • Lecture 5 Study Questions: Ethology of Geese; Fixed Action Patterns And
    9.20 Class #5 Study questions: 1. Yawning is a human “fixed action pattern” (FAP). Name three other FAPs shown by humans. Try not to name reflexes, but rather, innate patterns of behavior that have a motivational component (see next question). 2. Unlike Graham Scott, many ethologists distinguish FAPs from reflexes. How do you think these types of actions can be distinguished? Give examples. (Scott uses “reflex” to mean automatic and at least initially unlearned.) 3. Give an example of a “supernormal stimulus” that acts as a releaser of a fixed action pattern in herring gull chicks. (See p 21) 4. Define: Primary sensory neuron, secondary sensory neuron, motor neuron, interneuron (neuron of the great intermediate net). [This textbook is not as clear as I would like in discussing the nervous system. Do not depend on this book for neuroscience information. The terms will be defined in class.] 5. What are the major specializations of nerve cells, compared with other cells of the body? 6. How can a “wandering spider” catch its prey without using a web, by a kind of touch sensitivity that does not involve direct contact? 7. What features of a moving visual stimulus are detected by the visual system of a toad in the triggering of prey-catching behavior? Describe a prey-catching action of a toad or a frog. 8. Where in the central nervous system of a toad could an electrical stimulus elicit a prey-catching fixed action pattern? What would change if the electrode were moved a short distance parallel to the brain surface? 9.
    [Show full text]
  • Neurobiology of Acoustic Behaviour in Crickets
    J Comp Physiol A (2006) 192: 677–689 DOI 10.1007/s00359-006-0115-8 REVIEW Berthold Hedwig Pulses, patterns and paths: neurobiology of acoustic behaviour in crickets Received: 19 October 2005 / Revised: 15 February 2006 / Accepted: 16 February 2006 / Published online: 8 March 2006 Ó Springer-Verlag 2006 Abstract Crickets use acoustic communication for pair formation. Males sing with rhythmical movements of Introduction their wings and the mute females approach the singing males by phonotaxis. Females walking on a trackball Communication strategies to attract or find a mate are rapidly steer towards single sound pulses when exposed central to any sexually reproducing animals. At the to split-song paradigms. Their walking path emerges sender side, they involve a variety of species-specific from consecutive reactive steering responses, which signals such as optical (fireflies, birds), chemical (moths show no temporal selectivity. Temporal pattern recog- and fish), electrical (fish), vibratory (spiders, insects) or nition is tuned to the species-specific syllable rate and acoustical (birds, fish and insects) displays. At the gradually changes the gain of auditory steering. If pat- receiver side, these are processed by matched sensory tern recognition is based on instantaneous discharge rate filters and pathways that in turn may activate specific coding, then the tuning to the species-specific song pat- signalling responses and/or guide locomotion. Signal tern may already be present at the level of thoracic display and processing are shaped by natural selection to interneurons. During the processing of song patterns, environmental constraints but also by sexual selection to changes in cytosolic Ca2+ concentrations occur in phase species-specific conditions (Bradbury and Vehrenkamp with the chirp rhythm in the local auditory interneurone.
    [Show full text]
  • L31. Neural Circuits for Escape and Startle
    Motor Behavior Motor control of escape and startle as examples of: Coordination of: multiple muscle groups L31. Neural Circuits for appendages Escape and Startle limbs Speed …and what it says about decision Stereotyped, repetitive acts making in the Nervous system Decisions Oct. 19, 2011 BioNB 4240 Cornell University © Carl D. Hopkins 1qs c 2 Decision making: a motor hierarchy 3 4 Command Neuron Concept • Keis Wiersma & Ikeda (1964) – interneurons in crayfish tail – electrical stimulation of single cell evoked coordinated movement (swimmeret movement, posture control, escape) – pattern of stimulation was unimportant 5 6 1 Command neuron in cricket brain Escape • Rapid: Latency can be less than 10-20 ms • All or none (decisive!) • Complex: many actions coordinated. • Dominant: once initiated, blocks all other behavioral acts. • Illustrates decision making properties of command neurons. 7 8 Escape: the nervous system of the crayfish Procambarus clarkii 9 10 Flexion Escape Control by Giant Anatomy of Escape Interneurons FLEXOR Muscles EXTENSOR Muscles Giant interneuron fires Implanted electrodes record spikes from ventral nerve cord. Giant motor neuron fires Recorded activity during escape response: giant interneuron fires; flexor muscle in abdominal segments fire. Fast flexor muscles contract Transverse section of the connectives shows two giant interneurons: lateral giant, medial giant. 11 12 2 Lateral Giant Escape Lateral Giant Escape mechanical stim. to tail mechanical stim. to tail rostral segments flex. rostral segments flex. abrupt, single flexion drawing tail abrupt, single flexion drawing tail upward. Somersault. upward. Somersault. Medial giant escape. Medial giant escape. anterior stimulation (visual, anterior stimulation (visual, mechanical) mechanical) All segments flex All segments flex tail flip, propels animal tail flip, propels animal backward.
    [Show full text]
  • Dopamine Modulates the Lateral Giant Neuron and Serotonergic Facilitation in Crayfish Joshua Scott Itlot W [email protected]
    Marshall University Marshall Digital Scholar Theses, Dissertations and Capstones 1-1-2010 Dopamine Modulates the Lateral Giant Neuron and Serotonergic Facilitation in Crayfish Joshua Scott itloT w [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://mds.marshall.edu/etd Part of the Aquaculture and Fisheries Commons, Marine Biology Commons, and the Terrestrial and Aquatic Ecology Commons Recommended Citation Titlow, Joshua Scott, "Dopamine Modulates the Lateral Giant Neuron and Serotonergic Facilitation in Crayfish" (2010). Theses, Dissertations and Capstones. Paper 282. This Thesis is brought to you for free and open access by Marshall Digital Scholar. It has been accepted for inclusion in Theses, Dissertations and Capstones by an authorized administrator of Marshall Digital Scholar. For more information, please contact [email protected]. DOPAMINE MODULATES THE LATERAL GIANT NEURON AND SEROTONERGIC FACILITATION IN CRAYFISH Thesis submitted to the Graduate College of Marshall University In partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science Biological Science By Joshua Scott Titlow, B.S. Approved by Brian L. Antonsen, Ph.D., Committee Chairperson Elmer M. Price, Ph.D., Committee member Eric R. Blough, Ph.D., Committee member Marshall University December 2010 ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This thesis could not have been written without the expertise and guidance of Dr. Brian Antonsen. Thank you for giving me an opportunity in your lab and teaching me how to be a scientist. I am grateful to Dr. Nadja Spitzer for editing this manuscript and being a second mentor. I thank Dr. Eric Blough for his comments on the paper and professional advice, and Dr. Elmer Price for encouragement and thought-provoking conversations.
    [Show full text]
  • Neuronal Coincidence Detection by Voltage-Sensitive Electrical Synapses
    Proc. Natl. Acad. Sci. USA Vol. 95, pp. 7145–7150, June 1998 Neurobiology Neuronal coincidence detection by voltage-sensitive electrical synapses DONALD H. EDWARDS*†,SHIH-RUNG YEH*, AND FRANKLIN B. KRASNE‡ *Department of Biology, Georgia State University, Atlanta, GA 30302-4010; and ‡Department of Psychology, University of California, Los Angeles, CA 90024 Communicated by Donald Kennedy, Stanford University, Stanford, CA, March 25, 1998 (received for review December 4, 1997) 52 52 ABSTRACT Coincidence detection is important for func- mV rest potential), EK 82 mV, EL 60 mV, and C tions as diverse as Hebbian learning, binaural localization, (compartmental capacitance) 5 6 nF. These values are equiv- and visual attention. We show here that extremely precise alent to those of the Hodgkin-Huxley model of the squid axon coincidence detection is a natural consequence of the normal calculated for a temperature of 19°C and are not critical for the function of rectifying electrical synapses. Such synapses open operation of the model. For the rectifying electrical synapses, 5 to bidirectional current flow when presynaptic cells depolarize the synaptic conductance is governed by the equation Gj 1 2 y 1 2 2 2 relative to their postsynaptic targets and remain open until Gmin (Gmax Gmin) (1 exp( A[Vpre Vpost Vo])), 5 y 5 5 m 5 well after completion of presynaptic spikes. When multiple where A 0.15 mV, Vo 70 mV, Gmax 20 S, Gmin 0.2 m input neurons fire simultaneously, the synaptic currents sum S, and Vpre and Vpost are the voltages in the pre- and effectively and produce a large excitatory postsynaptic poten- postsynaptic model neurons, respectively.
    [Show full text]
  • Minireview Neuronal Decision-Making Circuits
    Current Biology 18, R928–R932, October 14, 2008 ª2008 Elsevier Ltd All rights reserved DOI 10.1016/j.cub.2008.07.081 Neuronal Decision-Making Circuits Minireview William B. Kristan being chosen [7]; and third, stimulating localized areas of the cortex centered on that neuron can bias the decisions that the monkey makes [8]. These studies have the distinct Studying the neural basis of decision-making has largely advantage of being close to what springs to mind when we taken one of two paths: one has involved cell-by-cell char- ‘make a decision’ [9], but they have the disadvantage of acterization of neuronal circuits in invertebrates; and the being embedded in complex brains, precluding a detailed other, single-unit studies of monkeys performing cognitive study of the neuronal circuitry and cellular properties under- tasks. Here I shall attempt to bring these two disparate lying the behavior. approaches together. Because a major motivation for the neuroethological/be- havioral approach is to find underlying neuronal circuits re- sponsible for the behaviors, neuroethologists select animals and behaviors for their simplicity, accessibility, and robust- We tend to think about choices and decisions as momen- ness. Such studies start with more natural behaviors and tous, life-changing events: whether to take a job, to get mar- tend to consider choices between qualitatively different ried, to buy a house. We also consider choices and decisions behaviors (such as swimming versus crawling, or feeding to be cognitive processes: we rationally enumerate the versus egg-laying) or between different forms of the same options, weigh them against our likes and dislikes, hold up behavior (such as ingesting food versus rejecting it, or swim- the choices against our moral values, and imagine the conse- ming versus scratching).
    [Show full text]
  • Mechanisms That Underlie Experience-Dependent Assembly of Neural Circuits
    Mechanisms that underlie experience-dependent assembly of neural circuits by Ann Marie Macara A dissertation submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy (Molecular, Cellular, and Developmental Biology) in the University of Michigan 2016 Doctoral Committee: Associate Professor Bing Ye, Co-Chair Professor Kenneth M. Cadigan, Co-Chair Associate Professor Catherine A. Collins Professor John Y. Kuwada Professor Suzanne Moenter DEDICATION To all my animals, past and present. ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS This dissertation was made possible through the collaborations between many people. I would first like to thank my research advisor, Dr. Bing Ye, for allowing me the opportunity to continue my PhD training in his laboratory at the Life Sciences Institute in the University of Michigan. Under his instruction, my research has been elevated to greater levels. I would like to thank my co-advisor, Dr. Ken Cadigan, for all of his support and advice on not only my research but also providing me with valuable insights into navigating graduate school. I would also like to thank my teaching mentor Dr. Tim McKay whose enthusiasm for teaching has greatly influenced my career path. You never really know where you will find a mentor, and I found mine in the Reptiles and Amphibian Division at the University of Michigan Museum of Zoology. Gregory Schneider has been my colleague, friend and advocate; I would like to thank him for all of his help over the past 3 years. I would also like to thank my committee members Drs. Cathy Collins and John Kuwada, who recently joined my committee and Dr.
    [Show full text]