Differential Diagnosis Diagnostic Process?

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Differential Diagnosis Diagnostic Process? 8/23/2010 What We’ll Cover Today • 1. (Part 1) Differential Diagnosis (defined) The Best Tests for Diagnosis: And Why – The decision making process it's Important – Costs associated with errors in decision making • 3. (Part 2) Diagnostic Accuracy – The language of diagnosis Chad Cook PT, PhD, MBA, OCS, FAAOMPT – Bias (studying diagnosis) Professor and Chair Walsh University • 4. (Part 3) Best tests for diagnosis (Part 1) Differential Diagnosis My Objectives • Differential diagnosis is a systematic 1. Differential Diagnosis is Extremely Important process used to identify the proper (in the early phases of the examination but diagnosis from a competing set of possible less so in the later phases of the examination) diagnoses. 2. Recognizing Bias in Diagnosis is Half the Battle • Diagnosis is one of many necessary 3. It is Critical to Know which tests are best used components during the clinical decision early or late in the examination making process Diagnostic Process? Who Performs Diagnoses? • The Diagnostic process involves identifying or • Anyone who treats determining the etiology of a disease or patients condition through evaluation of patient • Certainly, physical history, physical examination, and review of therapists laboratory data or diagnostic imaging; and the subsequent descriptive title of that finding Whiting et al. J Health Serv Res 2008 1 8/23/2010 PT’s and Diagnosis 1988 Why it’s Important • “Physical Therapists thus must establish • Failure to correctly identify an appropriate diagnostic categories that direct their diagnosis can lead to: treatment prescriptions and that provide a – Negative outcomes (Trowbridge 2008). means of communication both within the – Delays in appropriate treatment (Whiting et al. profession and with other practitioners and 2008) consumers about the conditions that require – Unnecessary healthcare costs (Dohrenend and their particular expertise for effective Skillings) treatment and prognostication” Sahrmann S. Diagnosis by the physical therapist: A prerequisite for treatment. Phys Ther. 1988;68:1703-6. Can Lead to Death Most Common Types and Locations? • 44,000 to 98,000 • Most in physician offices Americans die (primary care 31%, annually as a medical specialty 21%) result of medical • Error types: Diagnostic 36%, surgical 24%, non- errors (1999). surgical procedures 14%, medications 13% Institute of Medicine. To err is human: building a safer health system. Kohn, Corrigan, Woods DM, Thomas EJ, Holl J. Ambulatory care adverse events and preventable adverse events and Donaldson (ed). Washington DC. National Academies Press, 1999, leading to hospital admission. Qual Saf Health Care 2007;16;127-131 ED Missed Diagnoses Worth Noting • Leading breakdowns • Delayed Diagnosis – Failure to order 58% – Diagnosis is made but – Inadequate at a later date history/physical 42% – Incorrect test • Misdiagnosis interpretation 37% – Diagnosis is missed Kachalia A, Gandhi TK, Puopolo AL, et al. Missed and delayed diagnoses in the ambulatory setting: Both cause adverse events a study of closed malpractice claims. Ann Intern Med. 2006;145:488-96. 2 8/23/2010 Diagnosis vs. Treatment Difficult, but Preventable • Acting on Insufficient Information* – (81% High Preventability) • Misapplication of, or failure to apply a rule or use a bad or inadequate rule – (90.3% High • 34.8% resulted in permanent disability • 86.5% of diagnostic delays were highly preventable Preventability) Wilson R, Harrison B, Gibberd R, et al. An analysis of the causes of adverse events from the Quality in Australian Health Care Study. MJA 1999; 170: 411-415 Wilson R, Harrison B, Gibberd R, et al. An analysis of the causes of adverse events from the Quality in Australian Health Care Study. MJA 1999; 170: 411-415 Costs associated with Too Many Lab Healthcare Costs Tests and Imaging • 50 to 53 million US dollars per million members, per year for a Referral• “Incorrect for lumbar diagnoses spine may medical misdiagnosis. radiographylead to incorrect for first and presentation of lowineffective back pain treatment in primary or care • 6.1 to 10.4 billion dollars is notunnecessary associated testing, with improved which is (adjusted) annually (for diagnostic physicalcostly functioning, and sometimes pain errors) or disability.invasive” Medical Misdiagnosis: Overlooked opportunity for meaningful health plan improvement Kerry et al. Br J Gen Pract. 2002;52:469-74. of quality and costs. Managed Care Outlook. 2008;21(8). Reducing Errors in Health Care: Translating Research Into Practice . AHRQ Publication No. 00-PO58, April 2000. Agency for Healthcare Research and Quality, Rockville, MD. Institute of Medicine. To err is human: building a safer health system. Kohn, Corrigan, http://www.ahrq.gov/qual/errors.htm and Donaldson (ed). Washington DC. National Academies Press, 1999, Cost Utility Analysis Old Knowledge • An interdisciplinary early intervention • Seeing a rehab specialists treatment model was the preferred option in results in fewer loss of work over 85% of the samples within the an days per dedicated injury (whether serious or not established range of acceptable costs. serious) (N=2700) • (most of these interventions are underutilized because of physician concerns of cost and applicability) • Rogerson, Gatchel, & Bierner. Pain Pract. 2010;Apr 5. E-pub, ahead of print. Haig et al. Aggressive early medical management by a specialist in physical medicine and rehabilitation: effect on lost time due to injuries in hospital employees. J Occup Med. 1990;32:241-4. 3 8/23/2010 Misdiagnosis: A System Problem? Only Physicians? • < 5% of Primary Care Physicians Routinely Examine for Red Flags during an Initial Screen • Bishop & Wing. Spine J. 2006: 6:282-8. • “Low rates of compliance for the assessment and documentation of yellow and red flags” • Walsh et al. Occup Med. 2008;58:485-9. Orthopedic Surgeons? I’m Immune to your consultations (Bowie D) • “Both orthopedic surgeons and family physicians’ • …Not in my setting knowledge of treating LBP is deficient.” • ….I’ve never been in a direct • “Orthopedic surgeons are less aware of current access environment treatment than family practitioners” • …..I work with good • Finestone et al. Spine. 2009;34:1600-3. physicians • ……The prevalence is so low….. • …. I don’t see patients with those diagnoses .. Missed cancer diagnoses Most Commonly Missed Diagnoses? Kachalia A, Gandhi TK, Puopolo AL, et al. Missed and delayed diagnoses in the ambulatory setting: a study of closed malpractice claims. Ann Intern Med. 2006;145:488-96. 4 8/23/2010 What’s the Best Way to Go About the Diagnosis is Part of a Process “Process” • It is most important when the condition at hand is • A couple of thoughts unknown or a competing – Use a dedicated strategy – Understand the metrics of certain diagnosis is as likely as the findings presumed condition – Never rely too heavily on a single measure – Realize that diagnosis is a “process” not an “event” Tekwani K, Sikka R. High Risk Chief Complaints III: Abdomen and Extremities. Emerg Med Clin N Am. 2009;27:747-65. The Process Three Diagnostic Questions when Addressing a Patient Rule Out Sinister Problems • The first question of diagnosis: Are the patient's symptoms reflective of a visceral disorder or a serious or potentially life-threatening illness? • The second question of diagnosis: From where is Identify the the patient's pain arising? Appropriate Location • The third question of diagnosis: What has gone wrong with this person as a whole that would cause the pain experience to develop and persist? Identify “other” Contributors to the Condition Murphy D, Hurwitz E. A theoretical model for the development of a Murphy D, Hurwitz E. A theoretical model for the development of a diagnosis- diagnosis-based clinical decision rule for the management of patients with based clinical decision rule for the management of patients with spinal pain. BMC spinal pain. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders 2007, 8:75 Musculoskeletal Disorders 2007, 8:75 Early Stages of Diagnosis • Specific to the necessity for accuracy, is the ability to differentiate patients with symptoms that arise from non-mechanical disorders or other potentially life threatening pathology • Tests will require high levels of sensitivity, low LR-, and reasonable reliability Murphy D, Hurwitz E. A theoretical model for the development of a diagnosis- based clinical decision rule for the management of patients with spinal pain. BMC Musculoskeletal Disorders 2007, 8:75 Rubinstein SD, van Tulder M. A best-evidence review of diagnostic procedures for neck And low back pain. Best Practice & Research Clinical Rheum. 2008;22:471-482. 5 8/23/2010 Example Some Foreshadowing • Mr. Smith, who was seen through direct access complained of an aching pain in his left lateral thigh. • “The findings of our study suggest that the The pain was worse at night and was described as a deep, boring, ache. majority of clinical tests for the spine with The comprehensive clinical acceptable diagnostic accuracy are for non- examination yielded no mechanical critical diagnoses.” findings during provocation or reduction testing and no special test that implicated a specific finding. Palpation findings were also inconclusive. Cook C, Hegedus E. Diagnostic utility of clinical tests for spinal dysfunction. Man Ther. 2010. (accepted for publication) Part Two of the Process Ruling Out Location? • The second question of diagnosis: • Using tests with high sensitivity (or From where is the patient's pain overpressures) to rule out a body part region arising? as
Recommended publications
  • Prior Belief Innuences on Reasoning and Judgment: a Multivariate Investigation of Individual Differences in Belief Bias
    Prior Belief Innuences on Reasoning and Judgment: A Multivariate Investigation of Individual Differences in Belief Bias Walter Cabral Sa A thesis submitted in conformity with the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy Graduate Department of Education University of Toronto O Copyright by Walter C.Si5 1999 National Library Bibliothèque nationaIe of Canada du Canada Acquisitions and Acquisitions et Bibliographic Services services bibliographiques 395 Wellington Street 395, rue WePington Ottawa ON K1A ON4 Ottawa ON KtA ON4 Canada Canada Your file Votre rëfërence Our fi& Notre réterence The author has granted a non- L'auteur a accordé une licence non exclusive Licence allowing the exclusive permettant à la National Libraq of Canada to Bibliothèque nationale du Canada de reproduce, loan, distribute or sell reproduire, prêter, distribuer ou copies of this thesis in microforni, vendre des copies de cette thèse sous paper or electronic formats. la forme de microfiche/film, de reproduction sur papier ou sur format électronique. The author retains ownership of the L'auteur conserve la propriété du copyright in this thesis. Neither the droit d'auteur qui protège cette thèse. thesis nor subçtantial extracts fi-om it Ni la thèse ni des extraits substantiels may be printed or otherwise de celle-ci ne doivent être imprimés reproduced without the author's ou autrement reproduits sans son permission. autorisation. Prior Belief Influences on Reasoning and Judgment: A Multivariate Investigation of Individual Differences in Belief Bias Doctor of Philosophy, 1999 Walter Cabral Sa Graduate Department of Education University of Toronto Belief bias occurs when reasoning or judgments are found to be overly infiuenced by prior belief at the expense of a normatively prescribed accommodation of dl the relevant data.
    [Show full text]
  • Thinking and Reasoning
    Thinking and Reasoning Thinking and Reasoning ■ An introduction to the psychology of reason, judgment and decision making Ken Manktelow First published 2012 British Library Cataloguing in Publication by Psychology Press Data 27 Church Road, Hove, East Sussex BN3 2FA A catalogue record for this book is available from the British Library Simultaneously published in the USA and Canada Library of Congress Cataloging in Publication by Psychology Press Data 711 Third Avenue, New York, NY 10017 Manktelow, K. I., 1952– Thinking and reasoning : an introduction [www.psypress.com] to the psychology of reason, Psychology Press is an imprint of the Taylor & judgment and decision making / Ken Francis Group, an informa business Manktelow. p. cm. © 2012 Psychology Press Includes bibliographical references and Typeset in Century Old Style and Futura by index. Refi neCatch Ltd, Bungay, Suffolk 1. Reasoning (Psychology) Cover design by Andrew Ward 2. Thought and thinking. 3. Cognition. 4. Decision making. All rights reserved. No part of this book may I. Title. be reprinted or reproduced or utilised in any BF442.M354 2012 form or by any electronic, mechanical, or 153.4'2--dc23 other means, now known or hereafter invented, including photocopying and 2011031284 recording, or in any information storage or retrieval system, without permission in writing ISBN: 978-1-84169-740-6 (hbk) from the publishers. ISBN: 978-1-84169-741-3 (pbk) Trademark notice : Product or corporate ISBN: 978-0-203-11546-6 (ebk) names may be trademarks or registered trademarks, and are used
    [Show full text]
  • Fixing Biases: Principles of Cognitive De-Biasing
    Fixing biases: Principles of cognitive de-biasing Pat Croskerry MD, PhD Clinical Reasoning in Medical Education National Science Learning Centre, University of York Workshop, November 26, 2019 Case q A 65 year old female presents to the ED with a complaint of shoulder sprain. She said she was gardening this morning and injured her shoulder pushing her lawn mower. q At triage she has normal vital signs and in no distress. The triage nurse notes her complaint and triages her to the fast track area. q She is seen by an emergency physician who notes her complaint and examines her shoulder. He orders an X-ray. q The shoulder X ray shows narrowing of the joint and signs of osteoarthrtritis q He discharges her with a sling and Rx for Arthrotec q She is brought to the ED 4 hours later following an episode of syncope, sweating, and weakness. She is diagnosed with an inferior MI. Biases q A 65 year old female presents to the ED with a complaint of ‘shoulder sprain’. She said she was gardening this morning and sprained her shoulder pushing her lawn mower (Framing). q At triage she has normal vital signs and in no distress. The triage nurse notes her complaint and triages her to the fast track area (Triage cueing). q She is seen by an emergency physician who notes her complaint and examines her shoulder. He orders an X-ray (Ascertainment bias). q The shoulder X ray shows narrowing of the joint and signs of osteoarthrtritis. He explains to the patient the cause of her pain (Confirmation bias).
    [Show full text]
  • Diagnostic Downshift’: Clinical and System Consequences of Extrapolating Secondary Care Testing Tactics to Primary Care
    EBM analysis BMJ EBM: first published as 10.1136/bmjebm-2020-111629 on 7 June 2021. Downloaded from ‘Diagnostic downshift’: clinical and system consequences of extrapolating secondary care testing tactics to primary care Imran Mohammed Sajid ,1,2 Kathleen Frost,3 Ash K Paul4 10.1136/bmjebm-2020-111629 ABSTRACT Several drivers push specialist diagnostic Numerous drivers push specialist diagnostic approaches down to the broader primary care layer of a health system, which we describe as ‘diag- 1NHS West London Clinical approaches down to primary care (‘diagnostic Commissioning Group, downshift’), intuitively welcomed by clinicians nostic downshift’. Aspirations for earlier disease London, UK and patients. However, primary care’s different detection or capacity pressures in specialist and 2University of Global Health population and processes result in under- cancer pathways underlie shifting of tests from Equity, Kigali, Rwanda recognised, unintended consequences. Testing high- cost hospital settings to primary care. Such 3 NHS Central London Clinical performs poorer in primary care, with indication assumptions have led to procurement and growth Commissioning Group, creep due to earlier, more undifferentiated of GP diagnostics, with unfettered direct access to London, UK presentation and reduced accuracy due to physiology tests, endoscopy, ultrasound, MRI, CT, 4NHS South West London spectrum bias and the ‘false-positive paradox’. and biochemical and immunological tests, often Health and Care Partnership only evaluated in secondary care. It is increasingly STP, London, UK In low- prevalence settings, tests without near- 100% specificity have their useful yield eclipsed expected, sometimes mandated, for GPs to perform by greater incidental or false- positive findings. secondary care- based testing strategies prior to Correspondence to: Ensuing cascades and multiplier effects can referral.
    [Show full text]
  • Bias Miguel Delgado-Rodrı´Guez, Javier Llorca
    635 J Epidemiol Community Health: first published as 10.1136/jech.2003.008466 on 13 July 2004. Downloaded from GLOSSARY Bias Miguel Delgado-Rodrı´guez, Javier Llorca ............................................................................................................................... J Epidemiol Community Health 2004;58:635–641. doi: 10.1136/jech.2003.008466 The concept of bias is the lack of internal validity or Ahlbom keep confounding apart from biases in the statistical analysis as it typically occurs when incorrect assessment of the association between an the actual study base differs from the ‘‘ideal’’ exposure and an effect in the target population in which the study base, in which there is no association statistic estimated has an expectation that does not equal between different determinants of an effect. The same idea can be found in Maclure and the true value. Biases can be classified by the research Schneeweiss.5 stage in which they occur or by the direction of change in a In this glossary definitions of the most com- estimate. The most important biases are those produced in mon biases (we have not been exhaustive in defining all the existing biases) are given within the definition and selection of the study population, data the simple classification by Kleinbaum et al.2 We collection, and the association between different have added a point for biases produced in a trial determinants of an effect in the population. A definition of in the execution of the intervention. Biases in data interpretation, writing, and citing will not the most common biases occurring in these stages is given. be discussed (see for a description of them by ..........................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • Systematic Reviews of Diagnostic Test Accuracy
    Systematic reviews of diagnostic test accuracy Karen R Steingart, MD, MPH July 10, 2014 [email protected] Conflicts of interest • Editor Cochrane Infectious Diseases Group • Editor Cochrane Diagnostic Test Accuracy Working Group • Member GRADE Working Group • No financial interests to declare Overview • Describe key steps in a systematic review of diagnostic test accuracy • Describe standard methods for meta-analysis of diagnostic test accuracy • Describe resources for those who consider doing a systematic review of diagnostic test accuracy Overview of reviews A systematic review starts with a clearly formulated question and uses systematic and explicit methods to identify, select, and critically appraise relevant research, and to collect and analyse data from the studies that are included in the review Egg slide adapted from Madhu Pai Why systematic reviews? • Scientific summary of all available evidence • Transparent and reproducible • Minimize bias • Studies can be formally compared to establish generalizability and consistency • Heterogeneity can be identified and investigated • Meta-analyses may increase the precision of the overall results J Reitsma, Cochrane DTA Workshop, Amsterdam, Sept 2011 Why systematic reviews? Standards for systematic reviews - 21 standards (~ 80 elements of performance) US Institute of Medicine - 80 standards for conducting - 108 standards for reporting The Cochrane Collaboration Selected Elements, Protocols and Reviews of Diagnostic Test Accuracy, Chapter 4, Handbook Systematic review author Diagnostic
    [Show full text]
  • Can Psychology Become a Science?
    Personality and Individual Differences 49 (2010) 281–288 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Can psychology become a science? Scott O. Lilienfeld Department of Psychology, Emory University, Room 473, 36 Eagle Row, Atlanta 30322, Georgia article info abstract Article history: I am profoundly grateful to Tom Bouchard for helping me learn to think scientifically. Scientific thinking, Received 3 December 2009 which is characterized by a set of safeguards against confirmation bias, does not come naturally to the Received in revised form 16 January 2010 human species, as the relatively recent appearance of science in history attests. Even today, scientific Accepted 25 January 2010 thinking is in woefully short supply in many domains of psychology, including clinical psychology and Available online 23 February 2010 cognate disciplines. I survey five key threats to scientific psychology – (a) political correctness, (b) radical environmentalism, (c) the resurrection of ‘‘common sense” and intuition as arbiters of scientific truth, Keywords: (d) postmodernism, and (e) pseudoscience – and conclude that these threats must be confronted directly Psychology by psychological science. I propose a set of educational and institutional reforms that should place Science Pseudoscience psychology on firmer scientific footing. Postmodernism Ó 2010 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved. Psychotherapy 1. Can psychology become a science? groups, they would need to possess above zero validity for at least one subgroup. When I entered graduate school in psychology at the University Of course, a naïve graduate student can perhaps be forgiven for of Minnesota in the Fall of 1982, I was a bright-eyed, bushy-tailed such logical errors, especially one embarking on his training nearly 21 year-old eager to learn about the mysteries of the mind.
    [Show full text]
  • Preregistration: a Pragmatic Tool to Reduce Bias and Calibrate Confidence in Scientific Research
    PERSPECTIVE Preregistration: A pragmatic tool to reduce bias and calibrate confidence in scientific research Tom E. Hardwicke* & Eric-Jan Wagenmakers# Department of Psychology, University of Amsterdam Scientific research is performed by fallible humans. Degrees of freedom in the construction and selection of evidence and hypotheses grant scientists considerable latitude to obtain study outcomes that align more with their preferences than is warranted. This creates a risk of bias and can lead to scientists fooling themselves and fooling others. Preregistration involves archiving study information (e.g., hypotheses, methods, and analyses) in a public registry before data are inspected. This offers two potential benefits: (1) reduce bias by ensuring that research decisions are made independently of study outcomes; and (2) calibrate confidence in research by transparently communicating information about a study’s risk of bias. In this article, we briefly review the historical evolution of preregistration in medicine, psychology, and other domains, clarify its pragmatic functions, discuss relevant meta-research, and provide recommendations for scientists and journal editors. * [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0001-9485-4952 # [email protected] ORCID: 0000-0003-1596-1034 “The mind lingers with pleasure upon the facts that fall happily into the embrace of the theory...There springs up...an unconscious pressing of the theory to make it fit the facts, and a pressing of the facts to make them fit the theory.” — Chamberlin (1890/1965)1 “We are human after all.” — Daft Punk (2005)2 1. Introduction Science is a human endeavour and humans are fallible. Scientists’ cognitive limitations and self-interested motivations can infuse bias into the research process, undermining the production of reliable knowledge (Box 1)3–6.
    [Show full text]
  • Episode 62 – Diagnostic Decision Making Part 1: EBM, Risk Tolerance
    Chris Hicks and Doug Sinclair), teamwork failure, systems or process failure, and wider community issues such as access to care. Five Strategies to master evidence-based Episode 62 – Diagnostic Decision Making diagnostic decision making Part 1: EBM, Risk Tolerance, Over-testing 1. Evidence Based Medicine incorporates & Shared Decision Making patient values and clinical expertise - not only the evidence With Dr. David Dushenski, Dr. Chris Hicks & Dr. Walter Himmel Prepared by Dr. Anton Helman, April 2015 Are doctors effective diagnosticians? Diagnostic errors accounted for 17% of preventable errors in medical practice in the Harvard Medical Practice Study and a systematic review of autopsy studies conducted over four decades found that nearly 1 in 10 patients suffered a major ante-mortem diagnostic error; a figure that has fallen by only approximately 5% For an explanation of the interaction of the 3 spheres of EBM see despite all of todays’ advanced imaging technology and increased Episode 49: Walter Himmel on Evidence Based Medicine from the testing utilization. NYGH EMU Conference 2014 The factors that contribute to diagnostic error (adapted from the http://emergencymedicinecases.com/episode-47-walter-himmel- Ottawa M&M model) include patient factors such as a language evidence-based-medicine-nygh-emu-conference-2014/ barrier, clinician factors such as knowledge base, fatigue or emotional distress; cognitive biases (reviewed in Episode 11 with The intent of EBM is to… 7. Was the reference standard appropriate? • Make the ethical care of the patient its top priority 8. Was there good follow up? • Demand individualised evidence in a format that clinicians and patients can understand 9.
    [Show full text]
  • A Systematic Review and Quantitative Meta-Analysis of the Accuracy Of
    A SYSTEMATIC REVIEW AND QUANTITATIVE META-ANALYSIS OF THE ACCURACY OF VISUAL INSPECTION FOR CERVICAL CANCER SCREENING: DOES PROVIDER TYPE OR TRAINING MATTER? by Susan D. Driscoll A Dissertation Submitted to the Faculty of the Christine E. Lynn College of Nursing In Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy Florida Atlantic University Boca Raton, FL December 2016 Copyright 2016 by Susan D. Driscoll ii ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS My dear friend and fellow PhD student referred to her doctoral dissertation as an “excrementitious” process and I couldn’t agree more. I would like to acknowledge a few of the many people who helped me navigate through the winding road of life, career, and academia. Dr. Tappen always had faith in my abilities and her easy going nature made the climb a little easier than expected. Dr. Goodman’s undying commitment to nursing and education is contagious and inspired me to continue the long hard slog. Dr. David Newman can always make me laugh and guided me through complicated statistics. Dr. Drowos’ medical perspectives were welcomed and needed. My sincerest thanks to all the wonderful teachers and mentors in my academic and professional career. Dr. Moore gave me the best career advice ever, “don’t get into medicine unless you can’t see yourself doing anything else”…so I didn’t. Teri Richards gave me my first glimpse at nursing. Dick Keyser introduced me to the business of healthcare, Dr. Grinspoon and Dr. Dolan-Looby sparked my interest in clinical research, Dr. Andrist and Dr. Steele sharpened my clinical skills, Dr.
    [Show full text]
  • Evidence to Inform the Development of QUADAS-2
    Updating QUADAS: Evidence to inform the development of QUADAS-2 Penny Whiting, Anne Rutjes, Marie Westwood, Susan Mallett, Mariska Leeflang, Hans Reitsma, Jon Deeks, Jonathan Sterne, Patrick Bossuyt 1 QUADAS Steering Group members: Penny Whiting, Department of Social Medicine, University of Bristol Anne Rutjes, Departments of Social and Preventive Medicine and Rheumatology, University of Berne Jonathan Sterne, Department of Social Medicine, University of Bristol Jon Deeks, Unit of Public Health, Epidemiology & Biostatistics, University of Birmingham Mariska Leeflang, Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Informatics, AMC, University of Amsterdam Patrick Bossuyt, Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Informatics, AMC, University of Amsterdam Hans Reitsma, Department of Clinical Epidemiology, Biostatistics and Informatics, AMC, University of Amsterdam Marie Westwood, Kleijnen Systematic Reviews, York Susan Mallett, Centre for Statistics in Medicine, University of Oxford 2 Contents Chapter 1: Background .......................................................................................................................... 5 Items in italics are those removed from the Cochrane version of QUADAS.Chapter 2: Approach and Scope of QUADAS-2 ............................................................................................................................... 6 Chapter 2: Approach and Scope of QUADAS-2 ...................................................................................... 7 2.1 Rationale
    [Show full text]
  • Bias and Confounding
    Bias and Confounding [email protected] Objectives By the end of this session you should: – Understand the concept of bias – Be able to give examples of differing types of bias that may be introduced into a study – Understand the concept of confounding – Be able to give examples of confounding within studies – Understand how bias and confounding may affect study results – Be able to identify ways to control for confounding Good research All epidemiological studies should: − Obtain a sample population that is as representative of the true population of interest as possible − Aim to acquire the best information possible on parameters of interest amongst their sample population Causality • Causality is the central concern of epidemiology Obesity CHD OR, RR, IRR Bias “Systematic, non-random deviation of results and inferences from the truth, or processes leading to such deviation.” (Dictionary of Epidemiology, 3rd ed.) Bias (including confounding) must be avoided or reduced as much as is possible – Help to produce accurate, precise, valid and reliable measurements – Ensure inferences made are true and generalisable Selection bias Systematic differences exist between comparison groups in a sample population Or, when the sample population does not represent the target population Examples of selection bias Respondent bias those who agree to be in a study are in some way different from those who refuse so the sample population is not truly representative of the target population Healthy worker effect employed population have lower mortality and less disease when compared with the general population Ascertainment bias the cases gathered are not representative of cases in the target population e.g.
    [Show full text]