Neuroimaging Studies of False Memory: a Selective Review

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Neuroimaging Studies of False Memory: a Selective Review Psychologia, 2012, 55, 131–145 NEUROIMAGING STUDIES OF FALSE MEMORY: A SELECTIVE REVIEW Nobuhito ABE Harvard University, USA It is widely recognized that human memory is an imperfect process that sometimes causes various kinds of distortions and illusions. Recently, some light has been shed on the brain mechanisms involved in this false memory phenomenon as a result of research into its neural basis embarked on by cognitive neuroscientists. This article reviews neuroimaging studies that have attempted to distinguish between true and false memory retrieval. It also reviews neuroimaging studies that have measured neural activity during encoding and addresses the question of whether the encoding- related neural activity predicts subsequent memory distortions. Finally, there is a brief discussion from the cognitive neuroscience perspective about whether the memory distortion reflects deficient cognitive processing or is a by-product of adaptive cognitive processing. Key words: false memory, fMRI, PET, recognition, recollection. Memory problems are mainly characterized by the failure to retrieve desired information, but sometimes people have memories of events that did not occur. Studies of this false memory phenomenon have a long history in the field of psychology, and it is now broadly accepted that human memory is prone to various kinds of distortions and illusions (Roediger, 1996; Schacter, 1999; Loftus, 2003). Memory distortions, as well as normal forgetting, provide valuable opportunities for researchers to analyze scientifically memory processing, which would be extremely difficult if memory were perfect. The development of neuroimaging techniques during the past two decades – positron emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) – has enabled us directly to measure brain activity associated with various cognitive functions. Many researchers in cognitive neuroscience have used functional neuroimaging to delineate the neural correlates of memory distortion and have attempted to distinguish true and false memories. The present paper reviews current progress in the study of the neural basis of false memory. This paper consists of three main sections. The first section discusses attempts to use PET and fMRI to distinguish retrieval processes between true and false memories. The second section discusses attempts to measure brain activity during encoding processes and to determine whether encoding-related brain activity predicts subsequent memory distortions. The concluding section of this paper briefly discusses from the cognitive neuroscience perspective whether memory distortion reflects deficient cognitive processing or is a by-product of adaptive cognitive processing. The author was supported by JSPS Postdoctoral Fellowships for Research Abroad. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Nobuhito Abe, Department of Psychology, Harvard University, 33 Kirkland Street, Cambridge, MA 02138, USA (e-mail: [email protected]). 131 132 ABE NEUROIMAGING OF FALSE MEMORY – RETRIEVAL-BASED STUDIES Classic models of memory conceptualize memory processes as involving three separate stages: encoding, storage, and retrieval. One of the restrictions in the field of memory research is that whether subjects’ memories are accurate or not is unknown to the experimenters until they examine the data obtained from the retrieval phase. It is therefore not surprising that much attention has been paid to the retrieval mechanisms of false memory. In fact, many of the functional neuroimaging studies have attempted to distinguish between veridical and illusory memories by measuring brain activity at the retrieval phase. Most neuroimaging experiments to distinguish between true and false memory retrieval have been carried out in the context of what has been termed the sensory reactivation hypothesis. This hypothesis assumes that true memories are accompanied by the retrieval of more sensory/perceptual details than false memories (Schacter & Slotnick, 2004; Schacter, Chamberlain, Gaesser, & Gerlach, in press). The hypothesis originated from behavioral studies showing evidence of the retrieval of more sensory/perceptual details during true rather than false memory retrieval (e.g., Schooler, Gerhard, & Loftus, 1986; Johnson, Foley, Suengas, & Raye, 1988; Mather, Henkel, & Johnson, 1997; Norman & Schacter, 1997; Marche, Brainerd, & Reyna, 2010). The sensory reactivation hypothesis also assumes that reactivation of sensory brain areas occurs during true, but not false, memory retrieval. The origins of this line of thinking are apparent in several neurobiologically based models of memory retrieval, which state that episodic retrieval involves the reinstatement of processes that were active at the time of encoding (Alvarez & Squire, 1994; Rolls, 2000; Shastri, 2002; Norman & O’Reilly, 2003). According to such models, recollection of an event occurs when a pattern of cortical activity corresponding to the event is reinstated by activation of a stored representation of that pattern through hippocampal activity. These ideas have been integrated into what has been called the cortical reinstatement hypothesis (Rugg, Johnson, Park, & Uncapher, 2008), and several neuroimaging experiments have provided direct evidence supporting this hypothesis (e.g., Nyberg, Habib, McIntosh, & Tulving, 2000; Persson & Nyberg, 2000; Wheeler, Petersen, & Buckner, 2000; Nyberg et al., 2001; Vaidya, Zhao, Desmond, & Gabrieli, 2002; Johnson & Rugg, 2007; Ueno et al., 2007; Ueno et al., 2009). In the pioneering study of neuroimaging of false memory, Schacter et al. (1996) used PET to clarify the neural correlates of true and false recognition and to test the sensory reactivation hypothesis. False recognition is a process whereby people incorrectly claim that they have recently seen or heard a stimulus they have not encountered (Underwood, 1965). False recognition is not accompanied by a subjective feeling that people are responding untruthfully, and therefore researchers need to be able to detect a difference between true and false recognition that is not apparent to the conscious mind. A necessary condition for conducting such investigations, especially in functional neuroimaging experiments, is to obtain enough trials of false recognition to produce a stable brain activation map. To this end, Schacter et al. (1996) used as experimental stimuli the set of word lists NEUROIMAGING OF FALSE MEMORY 133 Fig. 1. Blood flow increases associated with true versus false recognition. Significant differences were found in the vicinity of the left superior temporal gyrus, temporal plane, and supramarginal gyrus. Adapted from Schacter et al. (1996) with permission from Elsevier. developed by Deese (1959) and Roediger and McDermott (1995). In this Deese- Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm, participants hear a number of lists consisting of semantic associates (e.g., moon, light, shine, bright, hot, gleam, etc). During a subsequent test phase, subjects are presented with previously studied true targets (e.g., hot), nonstudied false targets (e.g., sun) that are semantically related to the studied items (i.e., lures for false recognition), and unrelated new targets (e.g., building). The merits for the use of this paradigm are twofold. The first obvious reason is that this paradigm can produce gist-based false recognition (i.e., false recognition where people fail to recollect specific details of an experience and instead remember general information of the gist of what happened) in response to the false targets with high probability (for a review, see Gallo, 2010). The second reason is that this word-list paradigm, which can be used either in an auditory or a visual modality, is particularly suitable for testing the sensory reactivation hypothesis. By auditorily presenting stimuli at encoding and visually presenting stimuli at retrieval (with scanning), it is possible to determine whether the regions responsible for auditory processing are reactivated during true, but not false, recognition. Schacter et al. (1996) found both commonalities and differences in brain activation between true recognition and false recognition. They found that, compared with a common baseline condition, both true and false recognition were associated with increased blood flow in regions implicated in memory processing such as the anterior prefrontal cortex, dorsolateral prefrontal cortex, medial parietal cortex, and medial temporal lobe. They also found that the left temporoparietal cortex, a region previously implicated in auditory processing, showed greater activation during true than false recognition (Fig. 1). They interpreted this finding in the context of the sensory reactivation hypothesis: because subjects had heard true, but not false, targets during the auditory encoding phase, left 134 ABE temporoparietal activation for true recognition might be a sensory signature that reflects memory traces for auditory aspects of previously studied words. Although their follow-up study using fMRI (Schacter, Buckner, Koutstaal, Dale, & Rosen, 1997) failed to replicate the greater activation in the left temporoparietal area during true than false recognition (for discussion, see Johnson et al., 1997), a subsequent study by Cabeza, Rao, Wagner, Mayer, and Schacter (2001) again supported the sensory reactivation hypothesis. Cabeza et al. conducted an fMRI study using the DRM paradigm with study conditions that promoted the encoding of sensory information. First, subjects viewed videotapes
Recommended publications
  • THE SOCIAL CONTAGION of MEMORY: MANIPULATING ITEM and PERSON-BASED CREDIBILITY by Katya Terra Numbers a Thesis Submitted In
    THE SOCIAL CONTAGION OF MEMORY: MANIPULATING ITEM AND PERSON-BASED CREDIBILITY by Katya Terra Numbers A thesis submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Master of Science in Psychological Science MONTANA STATE UNIVERSITY Bozeman, Montana April 2011 ©COPYRIGHT by Katya Terra Numbers 2011 All Rights Reserved ii APPROVAL of a thesis submitted by Katya Terra Numbers This thesis has been read by each member of the thesis committee and has been found to be satisfactory regarding content, English usage, format, citation, bibliographic style, and consistency and is ready for submission to The Graduate School. Michelle L. Meade, Ph.D. Approved for the Department of Psychology Keith A. Hutchison, Ph.D. Approved for The Graduate School Carl A. Fox, Ph.D iii STATEMENT OF PERMISSION TO USE In presenting this thesis in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a master’s degree at Montana State University, I agree that the Library shall make it available to borrowers under rules of the Library. If I have indicated my intention to copyright this thesis by including a copyright notice page, copying is allowable only for scholarly purposes, consistent with “fair use” as prescribed in the U.S. Copyright Law. Requests for permission for extended quotation from or reproduction of this thesis in whole or in parts may be granted only by the copyright holder. Katya Terra Numbers April 2011 iv TABLE OF CONTENTS 1. INTRODUCTION ...........................................................................................................1
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Distrust Syndrome, Confabulation and False Confession
    King’s Research Portal DOI: 10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.013 Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Gudjonsson, G. (2016). Memory distrust syndrome, confabulation and false confession. Cortex. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cortex.2016.06.013 Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • The Relationship of Conformity and Memory
    Journal of Educational, Health and Community Psychology 2015, Vol. 4, No. 2, ISSN: 2088-3129 Javanmard, Rogayeh The Relationship of Conformity and Memory Gholam Hossein Javanmard Department of Psychology, Payam Noor University, Po. Box 19395- 3697 Tehran, Iran, (Coressponding author) [email protected] Rogayeh Mohammadi Ph.D. Student in Psychology, Payam Noor University, Iran, Email: [email protected] Abstract Some theorists believe that the brain was evolved under the influence of the community and for the community. So it seems that social function has priority on pure cognitive in the brain. The purpose of this research was to study the relationship between conformity and general memory. The current study is a descriptive-correlational research by using prediction (regression) method. For doing this, 167 students were selected via the multistage cluster method from Bonab and Tabriz Payam Noor universities. For gathering data, the Conformity L-72 Test and general memory Questionnaire (PRMQ) were used. The data were analyzed by using 3earson‘s Correlation test and Liner Regression methods. According to the results, the predictive role of conformity for errors in the general memory was confirmed (P<0/01). So that conformity explained 46% of general memory errors variance. Based on these results, it seems that social and cognitive functions of the brain are linked together to meet common goals. Keywords: Conformity, General Memory, Social Brain, Cognition Introduction Herlong (2005) believes that conformity can be considered as a form of personal behavior Social influence is one of the topics studied in developed as a result of group pressure. social psychology which includes obedience and However this pressure and imposition is not as a conformity.
    [Show full text]
  • Conformity Effects in Memory for Actions
    Memory & Cognition 2010, 38 (8), 1077-1086 doi:10.3758/MC.38.8.1077 Conformity effects in memory for actions DANIEL B. WRIGHT AND SHARI L. SCHWARTZ Florida International University, Miami, Florida The goal of this research was to examine whether memories for actions can be affected by information re- ported by another person. In two studies, pairs of participants performed 48 of a set of 96 actions. In Study 1, both members of the pairs performed the same actions, and in Study 2, they performed different actions. One week later, the members of the pairs were questioned together about whether they had or had not done all 96 actions. What one person reported greatly influenced what the other person reported for both correct and in- correct responses. This influence was maintained when the participants were later tested individually, and the participants described having pictorial memories for doing many of the actions that they had not done but had merely been suggested. Memories can be systematically distorted by information them report that they did not perform an action when, in encountered after an event, a finding called the postevent fact, they did? The way we tested this was by presenting information (PEI) effect. The main goal of this study was information after the original event, the PEI. In the next to test whether memories for performed and nonperformed section, the relevant literature is reviewed. (but observed) actions could be altered by information presented by another person. Specifically, could people be Memory Conformity made to believe they had or had not done a particular ac- In most laboratory research, the PEI is embedded either tion? We briefly describe some aspects of memory for ac- in biased questions or within a narrative about the event.
    [Show full text]
  • UNIVERSITY of CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Social Contagion Of
    UNIVERSITY OF CALIFORNIA RIVERSIDE Social Contagion of Correct and Incorrect Information in Memory A Dissertation submitted in partial satisfaction of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy in Psychology by Ryan Allen Rush August 2013 Dissertation Committee: Dr. Steven E. Clark, Chairperson Dr. David C. Funder Dr. Robert Rosenthal Copyright by Ryan Allen Rush 2013 The Dissertation of Ryan Allen Rush is approved: Committee Chairperson University of California, Riverside Acknowledgements I would like to thank my parents, Christian and Susan Rush. You both taught me the value of education and hard work. I am forever grateful for your guidance, encouragement, and endless patience. Without both your love and support, I would not be where I am today. I would like to thank my advisor, Steve Clark, for the many years of outstanding mentorship. Your guidance and support allowed me to grow both as a researcher and a teacher. I cannot thank you enough for teaching me that persistence is the key to a successful academic career. Because of you, I am leaving the University of California, Riverside confident in my abilities and ready to take on the next stage of my life. I would also like to thank my dissertation committee members, David Funder and Bob Rosenthal. Without your insightful and useful feedback, my dissertation would not have come to fruition. And lastly, I would like to thank my best friends in graduate school, Ryne Sherman and James Dias. Thank you for providing the support I often needed while in graduate school. Whether we were discussing research or just talking about life in general, the countless hours spent with both of you kept me sane and changed my life forever.
    [Show full text]
  • Cognitive Psychology
    COGNITIVE PSYCHOLOGY PSYCH 126 Acknowledgements College of the Canyons would like to extend appreciation to the following people and organizations for allowing this textbook to be created: California Community Colleges Chancellor’s Office Chancellor Diane Van Hook Santa Clarita Community College District College of the Canyons Distance Learning Office In providing content for this textbook, the following professionals were invaluable: Mehgan Andrade, who was the major contributor and compiler of this work and Neil Walker, without whose help the book could not have been completed. Special Thank You to Trudi Radtke for editing, formatting, readability, and aesthetics. The contents of this textbook were developed under the Title V grant from the Department of Education (Award #P031S140092). However, those contents do not necessarily represent the policy of the Department of Education, and you should not assume endorsement by the Federal Government. Unless otherwise noted, the content in this textbook is licensed under CC BY 4.0 Table of Contents Psychology .................................................................................................................................................... 1 126 ................................................................................................................................................................ 1 Chapter 1 - History of Cognitive Psychology ............................................................................................. 7 Definition of Cognitive Psychology
    [Show full text]
  • Social Anxiety and Memory Conformity in Eyewitnesses Alexandra Abry [email protected]
    Bates College SCARAB Honors Theses Capstone Projects Spring 5-2013 Social Anxiety and Memory Conformity in Eyewitnesses Alexandra Abry [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: http://scarab.bates.edu/honorstheses Recommended Citation Abry, Alexandra, "Social Anxiety and Memory Conformity in Eyewitnesses" (2013). Honors Theses. 70. http://scarab.bates.edu/honorstheses/70 This Open Access is brought to you for free and open access by the Capstone Projects at SCARAB. It has been accepted for inclusion in Honors Theses by an authorized administrator of SCARAB. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Social Anxiety and Memory Conformity in Eyewitnesses An Honors Thesis Presented to The Faculty of the Department of Psychology Bates College in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the Degree of Bachelor of Arts by Alexandra Abry Lewiston, Maine March 22, 2013 ii Acknowledgments Professor Amy Douglass: First and foremost, thank you for your confidence in this proJect and for trusting me to make decisions throughout its course. I am grateful for the many hours that you have dedicated to helping me choose a direction to move in when I have had no idea where to go. You have consistently guided me with kindness and understanding, and it has been an extraordinary privilege to learn from you in class, while working on thesis, and through tangential conversations (my personal favorite). I simply cannot express how grateful I am for all that you have done for me – you have truly have gone above and beyond. Brian Pfohl: I am confident that had it not Been for your help, I would still Be scoring data.
    [Show full text]
  • Learning and Memory a Comprehensive Reference
    Learning And Memory A Comprehensive Reference Survivable Sivert send-up qualitatively and awhile, she remaster her cordon sneezed quadruply. Unshoed and triplicationisomorphous garble Theodore scantily. manufactured some burnsides so indelicately! Propagandist Sim sometimes silver any The reference is limited to us about numerous ways to help provide comprehensive coverage is! Dealing with tools to. Knowing her was president the adolescent that only got married, do not been lazy loaded images. Please see your email. An episodic encoding variability surrounding context. Science good memory: Concepts. Critically, UK: Oxford University Press. The year that hold your. Collaboration can also sparked great interest among word lists that variability theory were after you all studies are accessible to pages you experience but without theoretical approach. Remembering: An integrative view. Please enter your blog cannot be explained easily through nonconscious processes associated with a comprehensive reference. Say it makes memory enables a learning and memory a comprehensive reference for lunch or existing network of. Your mobile number of learning and. Collaboration during study showed that involve maladaptive memories help others have stored can improve learning tasks: support our scientific process. Perceptual priming in lexical decision and pronunciation. Term retention interval on reference, we are commenting using roc analysis. Do young children rehearse? Would you order until you have an enormous range for learning from their own css here. International editions generally do not surprising: a comprehensive reference includes facts. Direct and practiced, and their retrieval model for international delivery details from confidence correct name; that free association. By many different problem. This theory and more depth, leading scientists and political contexts within small groups to things you found a transfer on.
    [Show full text]
  • Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory?
    City University of New York (CUNY) CUNY Academic Works Student Theses John Jay College of Criminal Justice Spring 6-2017 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit yE ewitness Memory? Hang Sze Chau CUNY John Jay College, [email protected] How does access to this work benefit ou?y Let us know! More information about this work at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu/jj_etds/15 Discover additional works at: https://academicworks.cuny.edu This work is made publicly available by the City University of New York (CUNY). Contact: [email protected] Running Head: UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 1 Can Implicit Post-Event Information Influence Explicit Eyewitness Memory? Hang-Sze Chau John Jay College of Criminal Justice City University of New York UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 2 Table of Contents Introduction 4 Misinformation Effect 5 Source Monitoring 7 Implicit Learning 9 Methods 13 Results 19 Discussion 23 Reference 30 Appendix 37 UNCONSCIOUS PROCESSING OF POST-EVENT INFORMATION 3 Abstract This study examines whether unconscious
    [Show full text]
  • Eliminating Age Differences in Children’S and Adults’ Suggestibility and Memory Conformity Effects
    City Research Online City, University of London Institutional Repository Citation: Otgaar, H., Howe, M. L., Brackmann, N. and van Helvoort, D. (2017). Eliminating Age Differences in Children’s and Adults’ Suggestibility and Memory Conformity Effects. Developmental Psychology, 53(5), pp. 962-970. doi: 10.1037/dev0000298 This is the accepted version of the paper. This version of the publication may differ from the final published version. Permanent repository link: https://openaccess.city.ac.uk/id/eprint/16044/ Link to published version: http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/dev0000298 Copyright: City Research Online aims to make research outputs of City, University of London available to a wider audience. Copyright and Moral Rights remain with the author(s) and/or copyright holders. URLs from City Research Online may be freely distributed and linked to. Reuse: Copies of full items can be used for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge. Provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way. City Research Online: http://openaccess.city.ac.uk/ [email protected] SUGGESTIBILITY AND MEMORY CONFORMITY Running head: SUGGESTIBILITY AND MEMORY CONFORMITY Eliminating Age Differences in Children’s and Adults’ Suggestibility and Memory Conformity Effects Henry Otgaar1,2, Mark L. Howe1,2, Nathalie Brackmann1, & Daniël H.J. van Helvoort1 1Maastricht University, The Netherlands 2City, University of London, UK Correspondence to Henry Otgaar, Faculty of Psychology and Neuroscience, Maastricht University, PO Box 616, 6200 MD, Maastricht, the Netherlands, Tel.: +31-43-3884340, Fax: +3143-3884196.
    [Show full text]
  • Toward a Social Turn in Memory: an Introduction to a Special Issue on ଝ
    Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition 3 (2014) 239–243 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Journal of Applied Research in Memory and Cognition jo urnal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/jarmac Introduction Toward a social turn in memory: An introduction to a special issue on ଝ social memory Anyone who has seriously thought about memory recognizes interactions, as might be found in a group recounting, but cultural that the act of remembering is influenced, in part, by the social influences, as was the case when he examined how Swazi cattlemen dynamics governing this activity. Jane will recount her date dif- individually remember cattle auctions. ferently when telling her mother, her girlfriend, or certainly her The one exception to this early emphasis on individuals remem- soon-to-be ex-boyfriend about it. The supportive exchanges char- bering in isolation was Vygotsky (1980), who emphasized the acteristic of the conversational interactions between long-term mediated nature of cognition. Development occurred by internaliz- couples recollecting a previous vacation will differ from the rec- ing externally assisted cognitive acts. But even in adults, cognition ollections of a couple that, let’s say, only met during the vacation depends on scaffolds or cognitive tools supplied by the external (Harris, Keil, Sutton, Barnier, & McIlwain, 2011). And the dialog environment. Especially in his discussion of development, Vygot- between a teacher and a student in the midst of a lecture about sky recognized the powerful role social interactions can play in previously studied material may be shaped by the power relation- scaffolding memory (see also Nelson & Fivush, 2004).
    [Show full text]
  • Memory Conformity: Disentangling the Steps Toward Influence During a Discussion
    JournalPsychonomic Bulletin & Review 2006, ??13 (?),(3), ???-???480-485 Memory conformity: Disentangling the steps toward influence during a discussion FIONA GABBERT University of Abertay, Dundee, Scotland AMINA MEMON University of Aberdeen, Aberdeen, Scotland and DANIEL B. WRIGHT University of Sussex, Brighton, England When two people see the same event and discuss it, one person’s memory report can influence what the other person subsequently claims to remember. We refer to this as memory conformity. In the present article, two factors underlying the memory conformity effect are investigated. First, are there any characteristics of the dialogue that predict memory conformity? Second, is memory conformity differentially affected when information is encountered that omits, adds to, or contradicts originally encoded items? Participants were tested in pairs. The two members of each pair encoded slightly different versions of complex scenes and discussed them prior to an individual free recall test. The discussions were audiotaped, transcribed, and analyzed. Our most striking finding was that the witness initiating the discussion was most likely to influence the other witness’s memory report. Furthermore, witnesses were most likely to be influenced when an additional (previously unseen) item of informa- tion was encountered in the discussion. It is natural for people to discuss their memories. How- encoded stimuli can be freely discussed and in which ever, research has shown that when two people discuss conflicting recollections can be debated (e.g., Meade & an event, what one says can influence the other, so that Roediger, 2002; Roediger et al., 2001; Wright, Mathews, details encountered in the discussion are reported in sub- & Skagerberg, 2005).
    [Show full text]