The Age-Graded Consequences of Victimization

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

The Age-Graded Consequences of Victimization The author(s) shown below used Federal funds provided by the U.S. Department of Justice and prepared the following final report: Document Title: The Age-Graded Consequences of Victimization Author(s): Jillian Juliet Turanovic Document No.: 249103 Date Received: August 2015 Award Number: 2014-IJ-CX-0006 This report has not been published by the U.S. Department of Justice. To provide better customer service, NCJRS has made this federally funded grant report available electronically. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. The Age-Graded Consequences of Victimization by Jillian Juliet Turanovic A Dissertation Presented in Partial Fulfillment of the Requirements for the Degree Doctor of Philosophy Approved April 2015 by the Graduate Supervisory Committee: Michael D. Reisig, Chair Kevin A. Wright Kristy Holtfreter ARIZONA STATE UNIVERSITY May 2015 This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ABSTRACT A large body of research links victimization to various harms. Yet it remains unclear how the effects of victimization vary over the life course, or why some victims are more likely to experience negative outcomes than others. Accordingly, this study seeks to advance the literature and inform victim service interventions by examining the effects of violent victimization and social ties on multiple behavioral, psychological, and health-related outcomes across three distinct stages of the life course: adolescence, early adulthood, and adulthood. Specifically, I ask two primary questions: 1) are the consequences of victimization age-graded? And 2) are the effects of social ties in mitigating the consequences of victimization age-graded? Existing data from Waves I (1994-1995), III (2001-2002), and IV (2008-2009) of the National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health (Add Health) are used. The Add Health is a nationally-representative sample of over 20,000 American adolescents enrolled in middle and high school during the 1994-1995 school year. On average, respondents are 15 years of age at Wave I (11-18 years), 22 years of age at Wave III (ranging from 18 to 26 years), and 29 years of age at Wave IV (ranging from 24 to 32 years). Multivariate regression models (e.g., ordinary least-squares, logistic, and negative binomial models) are used to assess the effects of violent victimization on the various behavioral, social, psychological, and health-related outcomes at each wave of data. Two- stage sample selection models are estimated to examine whether social ties explain variation in these outcomes among a subsample of victims at each stage of the life course. i This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. The results indicate that the negative consequences of victimization vary considerably across different stages of the life course, and that the spectrum of negative outcomes linked to victimization narrows into adulthood. The effects of social ties appear to be age-graded as well, where ties are more protective for victims of violence in adolescence and adulthood than they are in early adulthood. These patterns of findings are discussed in light of their implications for continued theoretical development, future empirical research, and the creation of public policy concerning victimization. ii This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. ACKNOWLEDGMENTS This project would not have been possible without the support of many kind and talented people. First, thank you to my dissertation chair, Mike Reisig, for your time, comments, and attention to detail. I am fortunate to have benefited from your guidance throughout graduate school. To my committee members—Kevin Wright and Kristy Holtfreter—your support means so much. Kevin, thank you for all that you do for your students, and for always making time for me. I give special thanks to my mentor, Travis Pratt, who has given me tremendous support and encouragement throughout my graduate career. Thank you for all of your advice and guidance, for teaching me how to write articles, and for giving me the tools I need to ask important questions. I hope to one day be able to teach my own students as much as you have taught me over the years. Thank you for always being a phone call away. I am also grateful to Nancy Rodriguez for her remarkable energy, commitment to students, and for the invaluable advice and support she has given me. I appreciate of all the research opportunities you provided me with throughout graduate school. Lastly, I wish to thank my wonderful colleagues and fellow doctoral students— especially Melinda Tasca, Andrea Borrego, Laura Beckman, and Mario Cano—for their support, feedback, and friendship. This research uses data from Add Health, a program project directed by Kathleen Mullan Harris and designed by J. Richard Udry, Peter S. Bearman, and Kathleen Mullan Harris at the University of North Carolina at Chapel Hill, and funded by grant P01- HD31921 from the Eunice Kennedy Shriver National Institute of Child Health and Human Development, with cooperative funding from 23 other federal agencies and iii This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. foundations. Special acknowledgment is due Ronald R. Rindfuss and Barbara Entwisle for assistance in the original design. Information on how to obtain the Add Health data files is available on the Add Health Web site (http://www.cpc.unc.edu/addhealth). No direct support was received from grant P01-HD31921 for this analysis. This dissertation research was supported by an award from the U.S. Department of Justice, National Institute of Justice Graduate Research Fellowship Program (Award No. 2014-IJ-CX-0006). Points of view expressed in this document do not necessarily express the views of the Department of Justice. iv This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. TABLE OF CONTENTS Page LIST OF TABLES ............................................................................................................... x CHAPTER 1 INTRODUCTION ................................................................................................... 1 Things We Know about Victimization.............................................................5 Remaining Questions in the Victimization Literature ....................................10 Victimization over the Life Course ....................................................11 Aging and the Consequences of Victimization ..................................12 Victims’ Lives and Social Ties ..........................................................14 Research Purpose ...........................................................................................16 National Longitudinal Study of Adolescent Health........................................17 Organization of the Dissertation ....................................................................21 2 VICTIMIZATION IN ADOLESCENCE ............................................................. 24 Social Ties in Adolescence ............................................................................27 Sample ...........................................................................................................30 Empirical Measures .......................................................................................31 Adolescent Violent Victimization ......................................................31 Adolescent Social Ties.......................................................................32 Adolescent Psychological Outcomes .................................................34 Adolescent Behavioral Outcomes ......................................................35 Adolescent Health Outcomes .............................................................37 Control Variables ...............................................................................38 v This document is a research report submitted to the U.S. Department of Justice. This report has not been published by the Department. Opinions or points of view expressed are those of the author(s) and do not necessarily reflect the official position or policies of the U.S. Department of Justice. CHAPTER Page Effects of Victimization on Adolescent Outcomes.........................................41 Models of Victimization and Adolescent Outcomes
Recommended publications
  • Rule-Based Reinforcement Learning Augmented by External Knowledge
    Rule-based Reinforcement Learning augmented by External Knowledge Nicolas Bougie12, Ryutaro Ichise1 1 National Institute of Informatics 2 The Graduate University for Advanced Studies Sokendai [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Learning from scratch and lack of interpretability impose some problems on deep reinforcement learning methods. Reinforcement learning has achieved several suc- Randomly initializing the weights of a neural network is in- cesses in sequential decision problems. However, efficient. Furthermore, this is likely intractable to train the these methods require a large number of training model in many domains due to a large amount of required iterations in complex environments. A standard data. Additionally, most RL algorithms cannot introduce ex- paradigm to tackle this challenge is to extend rein- ternal knowledge limiting their performance. Moreover, the forcement learning to handle function approxima- impossibility to explain and understand the reason for a de- tion with deep learning. Lack of interpretability cision restricts their use to non-safety critical domains, ex- and impossibility to introduce background knowl- cluding for example medicine or law. An approach to tackle edge limits their usability in many safety-critical these problems is to combine simple reinforcement learning real-world scenarios. In this paper, we study how techniques and external knowledge. to combine reinforcement learning and external A powerful recent idea to address the problem of computa- knowledge. We derive a rule-based variant ver- tional expenses is to modularize the model into an ensemble sion of the Sarsa(λ) algorithm, which we call Sarsa- of experts [Lample and Chaplot, 2017], [Bougie and Ichise, rb(λ), that augments data with complex knowledge 2017].
    [Show full text]
  • Personality and Individual Differences 128 (2018) 162–169
    Personality and Individual Differences 128 (2018) 162–169 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Risk as reward: Reinforcement sensitivity theory and psychopathic T personality perspectives on everyday risk-taking ⁎ Liam P. Satchella, , Alison M. Baconb, Jennifer L. Firthc, Philip J. Corrd a School of Law and Criminology, University of West London, United Kingdom b School of Psychology, Plymouth University, United Kingdom c Department of Psychology, Nottingham Trent University, United Kingdom d Department of Psychology, City, University of London, United Kingdom ARTICLE INFO ABSTRACT Keywords: This study updates and synthesises research on the extent to which impulsive and antisocial disposition predicts Personality everyday pro- and antisocial risk-taking behaviour. We use the Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory (RST) of Reinforcement Sensitivity Theory personality to measure approach, avoidance, and inhibition dispositions, as well as measures of Callous- Psychopathy Unemotional and psychopathic personalities. In an international sample of 454 respondents, results showed that Callous-unemotional traits RST, psychopathic personality, and callous-unemotional measures accounted for different aspects of risk-taking Risk-taking behaviour. Specifically, traits associated with ‘fearlessness’ related more to ‘prosocial’ (recreational and social) risk-taking, whilst traits associated with ‘impulsivity’ related more to ‘antisocial’ (ethical and health) risk-taking. Further, we demonstrate that psychopathic personality may be demonstrated by combining the RST and callous- unemotional traits (high impulsivity, callousness, and low fear). Overall this study showed how impulsive, fearless and antisocial traits can be used in combination to identify pro- and anti-social risk-taking behaviours; suggestions for future research are indicated. 1.
    [Show full text]
  • 2. Contemporary Criminological Approaches to Crime
    LJMU Research Online Kewley, S Strength based approaches and protective factors from a criminological perspective http://researchonline.ljmu.ac.uk/id/eprint/7023/ Article Citation (please note it is advisable to refer to the publisher’s version if you intend to cite from this work) Kewley, S (2017) Strength based approaches and protective factors from a criminological perspective. Aggression and Violent Behavior, 32. pp. 11-18. ISSN 1359-1789 LJMU has developed LJMU Research Online for users to access the research output of the University more effectively. Copyright © and Moral Rights for the papers on this site are retained by the individual authors and/or other copyright owners. Users may download and/or print one copy of any article(s) in LJMU Research Online to facilitate their private study or for non-commercial research. You may not engage in further distribution of the material or use it for any profit-making activities or any commercial gain. The version presented here may differ from the published version or from the version of the record. Please see the repository URL above for details on accessing the published version and note that access may require a subscription. For more information please contact [email protected] http://researchonline.ljmu.ac.uk/ A criminological perspective Strength based approaches and protective factors from a criminological perspective 1 A criminological perspective Abstract This paper provides a review of the current criminological landscape in relation to positive criminology and strengths based responses to crime. It does this by drawing on four forms of ‘offender’ rehabilitation as detailed in Fergus McNeill’s (2012) seminal work.
    [Show full text]
  • Action to End Child Sexual Abuse and Exploitation
    ACTION TO END CHILD SEXUAL ABUSE AND EXPLOITATION Published by UNICEF Child Protection Section Programme Division 3 United Nations Plaza New York, NY 10017 Email: [email protected] Website: www.unicef.org © United Nations Children’s Fund (UNICEF) December 2020. Permission is required to reproduce any part of this publication. Permission will be freely granted to educational or non-profit organizations. For more information on usage rights, please contact: [email protected] Cover photo: © UNICEF/UNI303881/Zaidi Design and layout by Big Yellow Taxi, Inc. Suggested citation: United Nations Children’s Fund (2020) Action to end child sexual abuse and exploitation, UNICEF, New York This publication has been produced with financial support from the End Violence Fund. However, the opinions, findings, conclusions, and recommendations expressed herein do not necessarily reflect those of the End Violence Fund. Click on section bars to navigate publication CONTENTS 1. Introduction ............................................3 6. Service delivery ...................................21 2. A Global Problem...................................5 7. Social & behavioural change ................27 3. Building on the evidence .................... 11 8. Gaps & challenges ...............................31 4. A Theory of Change ............................13 Endnotes .................................................32 5. Enabling National Environments ..........15 1 Ending Child Sexual Abuse and Exploitation: A Review of the Evidence ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS
    [Show full text]
  • Blame Attribution in Sexual Victimization ⇑ Carin Perilloux , Joshua D
    Personality and Individual Differences 63 (2014) 81–86 Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Personality and Individual Differences journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/paid Blame attribution in sexual victimization ⇑ Carin Perilloux , Joshua D. Duntley 1, David M. Buss University of Texas at Austin, Austin, TX 78712, United States article info abstract Article history: The current study explored how victims and third-parties attribute blame and perpetrator motivation for Received 13 October 2013 actual sexual victimization experiences. Although we do not assert that victims are responsible for per- Received in revised form 24 January 2014 petrators’ behavior, we found that some victims do not allocate all blame to their perpetrator. We sought Accepted 25 January 2014 to examine how victims and third-parties allocate blame in instances of actual completed and attempted sexual victimization and how they perceived perpetrator motivations. Victims of completed rape (n = 49) and attempted sexual assault (n = 91), and third-parties who knew a victim of sexual assault (n = 152) Keywords: allocated blame across multiple targets: perpetrator, self/victim, friends, family, and the situation. Partic- Rape ipants also described their perceptions of perpetrator’s motivation for the sexual assault. Victims tended Blame Perpetrator to assign more blame to themselves than third-parties assigned to victims. Furthermore, victims per- Victim ceived perpetrators as being more sexually-motivated than third-parties did, who viewed perpetrators Sexual violence as more power-motivated. Results suggest that perceptions of rape and sexual assault significantly differ between victims and third-party individuals who have never directly experienced such a trauma. Ó 2014 Elsevier Ltd.
    [Show full text]
  • Fact Sheet - Reinforcement
    FACT SHEET - REINFORCEMENT Positive Reinforcement Because many children with autism have difficulty with communication, play skills, and socialization, it is often difficult to motivate them to engage in activities that incorporate these skills. Positive reinforcement can provide additional motivation to help shape and increase developmentally approriate behaviors. A positive reinforcer is anything that is added following a behavior that increases the likelihood of the behavior occuring again in the future. Rewards are often given to children when they engage in desirable behaviors, but if the reward does Contact Information not cause those behaviors to increase in the future, then the reward is not actually a Website: positive reinforcer. www.coe.fau.edu/card/ Various Forms of Reinforcement Boca Raton Campus 777 Glades Road Natural Reinforcement: A child’s positive behaviors and social interactions Boca Raton, FL. 33431 are reinforced naturally. The natural consequences of positive behaviors become reinforcing themselves. Successful interactions become motivating to the child. Main Line: 561/ 297-2023 Toll Free: 1-888-632-6395 Examples: Fax: 561/297-2507 ♦ There is a ball out of reach for a child. The child says, “Ball,” and an adult Port St Lucie Campus hands the ball to the child. Access to the ball is reinforcing and increases the likelihood of the child requesting “ball” in the future. 500 NW California Blvd. ♦ A child is struggling with a difficult puzzle. The child says, “Help,” and an Port St Lucie, FL. 34986 adult helps the child. Completion of the puzzle is reinforcing. This successful interaction increases the likelihood of the child attempting puzzles Main Line: 561/ 297-2023 in the future and requesting help when needed.
    [Show full text]
  • What's Wrong with the Sociology of Punishment?1
    1 What’s Wrong with the Sociology of Punishment?1 John Braithwaite Australian National University Abstract The sociology of punishment is seen through the work of David Garland, as contributing useful insights, but less than it might because of its focus on societal choices of whether and how to punish instead of on choices of whether to regulate by punishment or by a range of other important strategies. A problem in Garland’s genealogical method is that branches of the genealogy are sawn off – the branches where the chosen instruments of regulation decentre punishment. This blinds us to the hybridity of predominantly punitive regulation of crime in the streets that is reshaped by more risk- preventive and restorative technologies of regulation for crime in the suites, and vice versa. Such hybridity is illustrated by contrasting the regulation of pharmaceuticals with that of “narcotics” and by juxtaposing the approaches to a variety of business regulatory challenges. A four-act drama of Rudoph Guiliani’s career as a law enforcer - Wall Street prosecutor (I), “zero-tolerance” mayor (II), Mafia enforcement in New York (III), and Rudi the Rock of America’s stand against terrorism (IV) - is used to illustrate the significance of the hybridity we could be more open to seeing. Guiliani takes criminal enforcement models into business regulation and business regulatory models to street crime. The weakness of a genealogical approach to punishment can be covered by the strengths of a history of regulation that is methodologically committed to synoptically surveying all the important techniques of regulation deployed to control a specific problem (like drug abuse).
    [Show full text]
  • Punishment on Trial √ Feel Guilty When You Punish Your Child for Some Misbehavior, but Have Ennio Been Told That Such Is Bad Parenting?
    PunishmentPunishment onon TrialTrial Cipani PunishmentPunishment onon TrialTrial Do you: √ believe that extreme child misbehaviors necessitate physical punishment? √ equate spanking with punishment? √ believe punishment does not work for your child? √ hear from professionals that punishing children for misbehavior is abusive and doesn’t even work? Punishment on Trial Punishment on √ feel guilty when you punish your child for some misbehavior, but have Ennio been told that such is bad parenting? If you answered “yes” to one or more of the above questions, this book may Cipani be just the definitive resource you need. Punishment is a controversial topic that parents face daily: To use or not to use? Professionals, parents, and teachers need answers that are based on factual information. This book, Punishment on Trial, provides that source. Effective punishment can take many forms, most of which do not involve physical punishment. This book brings a blend of science, clinical experience, and logic to a discussion of the efficacy of punishment for child behavior problems. Dr. Cipani is a licensed psychologist with over 25 years of experience working with children and adults. He is the author of numerous books on child behavior, and is a full professor in clinical psychology at Alliant International University in Fresno, California. 52495 Context Press $24.95 9 781878 978516 1-878978-51-9 A Resource Guide to Child Discipline i Punishment on Trial ii iii Punishment on Trial Ennio Cipani Alliant International University CONTEXT PRESS Reno, Nevada iv ________________________________________________________________________ Punishment on Trial Paperback pp. 137 Distributed by New Harbinger Publications, Inc. ________________________________________________________________________ Library of Congress Cataloging-in-Publication Data Cipani, Ennio.
    [Show full text]
  • What Bullying Is What Bullying Can Look Like in Elementary School What Bullying Can Look Like in Junior High School
    FS-570 BulliesBullies When I was a young boy, What bullying is What bullying the bully called me names, stole my bicycle, forced With all the focus that has surrounded can look like in me off the playground. teenage gangs and gun violence, it may elementary school be easy to forget that the teenage years He made fun of me in are not the only times that children face Being a victim is the most common front of other children, violent behavior. In fact, aggressive in second grade, and the likelihood of forced me to turn over behavior and bullying are even more being bullied decreases each year after my lunch money each day, common in elementary school than in that (see Figure 1). Bullies in elementary threatened to give me junior and senior high! Some studies school are more likely to pick on children a black eye if I told suggest that around 20 percent of all younger than themselves. Bullying is adult authority figures. American children have been the victim often very physical in nature, with open At different times I was of bullying at some point in elementary attacks of aggression being the most subject to a wide range of school, and about the same number common. Boys are more likely to be degradation and abuse – have described themselves as engaging doing the bullying, but girls and boys de-pantsing, spit in my face, in some form of bullying behavior. are equally likely to be victims. forced to eat the playground Bullying can range from teasing, to dirt....To this day, their stealing lunch money, to a group of handprints, like a slap students physically abusing a classmate.
    [Show full text]
  • Disclosing the Identities of Juvenile Felons: Introducing Accountability to Juvenile Justice Arthur R
    Loyola University Chicago Law Journal Volume 27 Article 7 Issue 2 Winter 1996 1996 Disclosing the Identities of Juvenile Felons: Introducing Accountability to Juvenile Justice Arthur R. Blum Follow this and additional works at: http://lawecommons.luc.edu/luclj Part of the Criminal Law Commons, and the Juvenile Law Commons Recommended Citation Arthur R. Blum, Disclosing the Identities of Juvenile Felons: Introducing Accountability to Juvenile Justice, 27 Loy. U. Chi. L. J. 349 (1996). Available at: http://lawecommons.luc.edu/luclj/vol27/iss2/7 This Comment is brought to you for free and open access by LAW eCommons. It has been accepted for inclusion in Loyola University Chicago Law Journal by an authorized administrator of LAW eCommons. For more information, please contact [email protected]. Comments Disclosing the Identities of Juvenile Felons: Introducing Accountability to Juvenile Justice When he wasn't stealing cars, he was throwing things at them or setting them on fire. "What could you do? . Tell his grandmother? She'd yell at him, and he'd be right back on the street. If the police picked him up, they'd just bring him back home because he was too young to lock up. He was untouchable, and he knew that."' I. INTRODUCTION The above passage articulates the exasperation felt by many who knew and feared eleven-year-old Robert Sandifer.2 In just a year and a half, he compiled a rap sheet that contained twenty-three felonies and five misdemeanors. 3 On August 28, 1994, apparently acting upon in- structions from older members of his gang, 4 he fired a semi-automatic weapon into a group of kids playing football and killed fourteen-year- old Shavon White, who happened to be in the area.5 Shortly after the shooting, according to the Chicago Police, fourteen-year-old Derrick Hardaway and his sixteen-year-old brother Cragg, both members of 1.
    [Show full text]
  • The Port Securityscape an Ethnography
    The port securityscape an ethnography by Yarin Eski Thesis submitted in fulfilment for the Degree of Doctor of Philosophy School of Social and Political Sciences University of Glasgow Scottish Centre for Crime and Justice Research 63 Gibson Street Glasgow G12 8LR Scotland Prof Fergus McNeill (first supervisor) Prof Simon Mackenzie (second supervisor) Dr Nicole Bourque (internal examiner) Prof Ian Loader (external examiner) Abstract 9/11 changed the face of maritime transport that is responsible for moving 80% of everything we consume. Ports are vital hubs in that maritime transport and any disruption there instantly affects global trade. To protect the global supply chain from crime and terrorism, both must be disrupted locally in the port by port police and security officers that are responsible for port security at operational level. Public and critical criminological attention to these key security actors, however, is virtually non-existent. This thesis therefore explores how their occupational realities and identities are (re)established in two major European ports, by providing an ethnographic account. To do so, the thesis builds on multi-sited ethnographic fieldwork in the ports of Rotterdam and Hamburg between 2011 and 2012, during which everyday policing and security work has been documented, followed by a thematic analysis. The key argument runs thus: the port is a local space for the global trade, which is underappreciated and underestimated by the public, and has its police and security professionals in place both aboard and on shore who protect and defend that vital trade site. The aggressive commercialist governmentality that goes on behind that vital global trade is unwillingly yielded to by these guardians but not without any bottom-up resistance.
    [Show full text]
  • Development and Persuasion Processing: an Investigation of Children's Advertising Susceptibility and Understanding
    University of Pennsylvania ScholarlyCommons Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations 2013 Development and Persuasion Processing: An Investigation of Children's Advertising Susceptibility and Understanding Matthew Allen Lapierre University of Pennsylvania, [email protected] Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations Part of the Communication Commons Recommended Citation Lapierre, Matthew Allen, "Development and Persuasion Processing: An Investigation of Children's Advertising Susceptibility and Understanding" (2013). Publicly Accessible Penn Dissertations. 770. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/770 This paper is posted at ScholarlyCommons. https://repository.upenn.edu/edissertations/770 For more information, please contact [email protected]. Development and Persuasion Processing: An Investigation of Children's Advertising Susceptibility and Understanding Abstract Over the past 40 years, research on children's understanding of commercial messages and how they respond to these messages has tried to explain why younger children are less likely to understand these messages and are more likely to respond favorably to them with varying success (Kunkel et al., 2004; Ward, Wackman, & Wartella, 1977), however this line of research has been criticized for not adequately engaging developmental research or theorizing to explain why/how children responde to persuasive messages (Moses & Baldwin, 2005; Rozendaal, Lapierre, Buijzen, van Reijmersdal, 2011). The current study attempts to change this by empirically testing whether children's developing theory of mind, executive function, and emotion regulation helps to bolster their reaction to advertisements and their understanding of commercial messages. With a sample of 79 children between the ages of 6 to 9 and their parents, this study sought to determine if these developmental mechanisms were linked to processing of advertisements and understanding of commercial intent.
    [Show full text]