Impulsive Choice in Mice Lacking Paternal Expression of Grb10 Suggests Intra-Genomic

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Impulsive Choice in Mice Lacking Paternal Expression of Grb10 Suggests Intra-Genomic Genetics: Early Online, published on March 21, 2018 as 10.1534/genetics.118.300898 1 Impulsive choice in mice lacking paternal expression of Grb10 suggests intra-genomic 2 conflict in behavior 3 Claire L. Dent*, Trevor Humby†, Katie Lewis*, Andrew Ward‡, Reiner Fischer-Colbrie§, 4 LawrenCe S. Wilkinson*,†, Jon F Wilkins** & Anthony R. Isles*, †† 5 *Behavioural GenetiCs GrouP, MRC Centre for NeuroPsyChiatriC GenetiCs and GenomiCs, 6 NeurosCienCe and Mental Health ResearCh Institute, Cardiff University, Hadyn Ellis Building, 7 Maindy Road, Cardiff, United Kingdom 8 †Behavioural GenetiCs GrouP, SChool of PsyChology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, 9 Cardiff, United Kingdom 10 ‡DePartment of Biology and BioChemistry, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom 11 §DePartment of PharmaCology, InnsbruCk MediCal University, InnsbruCk, Austria 12 **Ronin Institute, Montclair, NJ 07043, USA 13 ††Author for CorresPondenCe: Anthony R. Isles, E-mail [email protected] 14 15 Running title: Intragenomic conflict and impulsivity 16 Keywords: genomiC imPrinting; NesP55; delayed reinforCement; intra-genomiC ConfliCt; 17 Grb10 18 19 20 21 1 Copyright 2018. 1 Abstract 2 ImPrinted genes are exPressed from one Parental allele only as a ConsequenCe of ePigenetiC 3 events that take Place in the mammalian germ line and are thought to have evolved through 4 intra-genomiC ConfliCt between Parental alleles. We demonstrate, for the first time, 5 oPPositional effeCts of imPrinted genes on brain and behavior. SPeCifiCally, here we show 6 that miCe lacking Paternal Grb10 make fewer impulsive choices, with no dissoCiable effeCts 7 on a seParate measure of imPulsive action. Taken together with Previous work showing that 8 miCe lacking maternal NesP55 make more imPulsive ChoiCes this suggests that imPulsive 9 ChoiCe behavior is a substrate for the action of genomiC imPrinting. Moreover, the 10 Contrasting effeCt of these two genes suggests imPulsive ChoiCes are subjeCt to intra- 11 genomiC ConfliCt and that maternal and Paternal interests Pull this behavior in oPPosite 12 direCtions. Finally, these data may also indiCate that an imbalanCe in exPression of imPrinted 13 genes Contributes to PathologiCal Conditions suCh as gambling and drug addiCtion, where 14 impulsive behavior beComes maladaptive. 2 1 INTRODUCTION 2 ImPrinted genes are exPressed from one Parental allele only as a ConsequenCe of ePigenetiC 3 events that take Place in the mammalian germ line (FERGUSON-SMITH 2011). FunCtionally, 4 imPrinted genes Converge on sPeCifiC biologiCal ProCesses that have Prominent imPortanCe 5 in mammals, suCh as in utero growth, metabolism and behavior (PETERS 2014; WILKINS et al. 6 2016). Many of the effeCts that maternally- and Paternally-exPressed imPrinted genes have 7 on in utero growth and during the early Post-natal Period are oPPositional in direCtion (HAIG 8 AND GRAHAM 1991; MADON-SIMON et al. 2014), lending suPPort to the theory that imPrinting 9 has evolved as a ConsequenCe of intra-genomiC ConfliCt (MOORE AND HAIG 1991; WILKINS AND 10 HAIG 2003). 11 Grb10 is Currently a unique examPle of an imPrinted gene that is exPressed from the 12 maternal allele only in some tissues, and the Paternal allele only in others (ARNAUD et al. 13 2003; HIKICHI et al. 2003; MONK et al. 2009). Consequently, different Parental alleles of Grb10 14 mediate distinCt PhysiologiCal funCtions (GARFIELD et al. 2011). Maternal Grb10 is expressed 15 in many PeriPheral body tissues, whilst being exCluded from the Central nervous system 16 (GARFIELD et al. 2011), and Plays a Prominent role in Controlling Placental funCtion 17 (CHARALAMBOUS et al. 2003; CHARALAMBOUS et al. 2010) and metabolism (SMITH et al. 2007). In 18 Contrast, Paternal Grb10 is essentially restriCted to the brain alone and imPacts on 19 behavioral PhenotyPes, including soCial dominanCe (GARFIELD et al. 2011). Grb10 exPression 20 in the brain is neuronal, showing a Pattern of exPression in disCrete brain regions that is 21 shared with the maternally exPressed imPrinted gene Nesp (PLAGGE et al. 2005). This overlap 22 in brain exPression of maternal Nesp (encoding Nesp55) and Paternal Grb10 has led to 23 sPeCulation that these two genes may influenCe common adult behaviors, Possibly in an 3 1 oPPosite manner (GARFIELD et al. 2011; DENT AND ISLES 2014), whiCh would be Consistent with 2 the PrediCtion from the intra-genomiC ConfliCt theory of genomiC imPrinting evolution (HAIG 3 2000). 4 Here, we address this question direCtly. We have shown that miCe lacking maternal NesP55 m/+ 5 (Nesp ) make more imPulsive ChoiCes (DENT et al. 2016). In Contrast, here we show that 6 Grb10+/P mice make less impulsive choiCes relative to their littermate Controls. Furthermore, 7 there were no dissoCiable effeCts on a seParate measure of imPulsive action, ComPletely m/+ 8 matching the sPeCifiCity of effeCts seen Previously in Nesp mice (DENT et al. 2016). Our 9 data Provide new findings on Grb10 funCtion in brain and reveal oPPosite effeCts of 10 maniPulating Grb10 and Nesp exPression on disCrete asPeCts of imPulsive resPonding. Taken 11 together these data are Consistent with the ConfliCt theory of the evolution of imPrinted 12 genes and as suCh Provide the first direCt evidenCe for intra-genomiC ConfliCt imPacting 13 adult behavior. 14 15 RESULTS AND DISCUSSION 16 Grb10+/p mice make less impulsive choices 17 We used the delayed-reinforCement task to examine imPulsive ChoiCe behavior (ISLES et al. 18 2003), where subjeCts made a ChoiCe between reCeiving a small food reward after a short 19 (1s) delay, or a larger reward after a longer delay (1, 8 or 16s). The extent to whiCh subjeCts 20 make imPulsive ChoiCes is indexed by a PreferenCe for Choosing the immediate but smaller 21 reward, versus a larger delayed reward (delayed gratifiCation). InCreasing the delay to the 22 larger reward within session deCreased the likelihood of Choosing that reward across all 2 23 subjeCts (Figure 1A; main effeCt of DELAY, F2,42=9.20, p=0.001, Partial η =0.37). However, 4 1 Grb10+/P miCe were signifiCantly more tolerant of an increased delay to the larger reward 2 than wild-tyPe (WT) littermate miCe (Figure 1A; main effeCt of GENOTYPE, F1,21=6.45, 3 p=0.019, Partial η2=0.25). However, whilst the data indiCate that the exPerimental grouP 4 diverges from wild-tyPe inCreasingly as the delay inCreases, this effeCt failed to result in a 5 signifiCant interaction between GENOTYPE and DELAY (Figure 1A; F2,42=0.56, p=0.946, Partial 6 η2=0.003), due largely to a Pre-existing grouP differenCe in the first 1s delay bloCk. 7 A standard Control measure to assess the extent to whiCh behavior is sensitive to delay is a 8 task maniPulation where any differential delay assoCiated with the larger and smaller 9 reward was equalized (1s) across all three bloCks. Here, all subjeCts now demonstrated the 10 exPeCted PreferenCe for the larger reward throughout the session (Figure 1B; no main effeCt 2 11 of DELAY, F1.7,35.1=1.88, p=0.17, Partial η =0.08); that is under these task Conditions there 12 were no differences in Choice-bias between Grb10+/P and WT miCe (no main effeCt of 2 13 GENOTYPE, F1,21=0.63, p=0.44, Partial η =0.03). Furthermore, there was no differenCe in the 14 sessions taken to achieve a stable baseline in these Conditions (WT 9 ±0.8; Grb10+/P 8 ±0.5; 15 t21=1.51, P=0.15). 16 The Contrasting Patterns of ChoiCe behavior between the Grb10+/P and WT miCe was also not 17 due to any differenCes between the grouPs in terms of basiC motivation to Carry out the task 18 as Grb10+/P animals acquired the task at the same rate as WT littermates (sessions to last +/P 19 day of baseline; WT 50.4 ±1.1 SEM, Grb10 47.7 ±1.2 SEM t21=1.73, p=0.10). Additionally, 20 variability between Grb10+/P and WT miCe was not related to differenCes in exPerienCing the 21 information trial ContingenCies. In the ‘forCed’ trials (where no ChoiCe was available), both 22 Grb10+/P and their WT littermates made equal resPonses to the large and small reward- 23 related stimuli at all delays (no interaction GENOTYPE x DELAY x CHOICE, F2,42=0.44, 5 1 p=0.649; data not shown). Finally, there was no differenCe between Grb10+/P and WT miCe 2 on general measures of task PerformanCe (see SuPPlemental Information). 3 Figure 1 Grb10+/p mice make less impulsive choices on a delayed-reinforcement task. Behavior of both Grb10+/P and WT miCe Changed across session bloCks (blk) with inCreasing delay, suCh that ChoiCe bias moved away from the resPonse leading to the large reward towards the small reward, with inCreasing delay (A). However, there were systematiC differenCes between the grouPs in their behavior, suCh that Grb10+/P animals switched their ChoiCe to the small, less delayed reward less quiCkly than WT miCe. When the delay assoCiated with the large and small rewards was equal (1s) throughout the session (B), ChoiCe bias was Consistently high (large reward Chosen apProximately 80% of the time). Under these task Conditions there were no differenCes in ChoiCe bias between Grb10+/P and WT miCe. Data shows mean ±SEM of three ConseCutive stable sessions; * rePresents P<0.05 main effeCt of GENOTYPE; ## rePresents P<0.01 main effeCt of DELAY. 4 6 1 In large Part Choice bias behavior in delayed reinforCement tasks is governed by three 2 Potentially interacting PsyChologiCal ProCesses; the PerCeived value of the rewards, and the 3 perCeived length and aversive nature of the delay (HO et al. 1999). The difference in choice 4 between Grb10+/P and WT miCe in the first bloCk of the task (where delay for the larger and 5 smaller rewards are both 1s) may suggest a Contribution to behavior in the Grb10+/P from a 6 relatively more less PronounCed generalized Conditioned Place aversion to the delayed 7 larger reward resPonse (see ISLES et al.
Recommended publications
  • Download Complete Issue
    VOLUME 1, No.2 30 June 2017 ISSN 2514-3174 bsdj.org.uk Advanced Necrotising Enterocolitis How well are we managing them? The British Student Doctor is an open access journal, which means that all content is available without charge to the user or his/her institution. You are allowed to read, download, copy, distribute, print, search, or link to the full texts of the articles in this journal without asking prior permission from either the publisher or the author. bsdj.org.uk Journal DOI 10.18573/issn.2514-3174 /thebsdj Issue DOI @thebsdj 10.18573/bsdj.v2i1 @thebsdj This journal is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution- NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. The copyright of all articles belongs to The British Student Doctor, and a citation should be made when any article is quoted, used or referred to in another work. The British Student Doctor is an imprint of Cardiff University Press, an innovative open-access publisher of academic research, where ‘open-access’ means free for both readers and writers. cardiffuniversitypress.org Contents EDITORIAL 1 Dr James M Kilgour and Dr Shivali Fulchand Editorial: Mental health and medical students Natalie Ellis, Munzir Quraisy, Dr Matthew Hoskins, ORIGINAL RESEARCH 4 Dr James Walters, Dr Steve Riley and Dr Liz Forty Medical student attitudes to mental health and psychiatry: the use of a patient-experience short film DISCUSSION 12 Alexander J Martin Medicalisation: the definition of disease and the role of tomorrow’s doctors 18 Michael Houssemayne du Boulay An inconsistency
    [Show full text]
  • Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms
    SCHRES-07994; No of Pages 6 Schizophrenia Research xxx (2018) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Schizophrenia Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms as antecedents of later psychotic outcomes in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome Maria Niarchou a,b,⁎, Samuel J.R.A. Chawner a, Ania Fiksinski c,d,e, Jacob A.S. Vorstman d,e,f, Johanna Maeder g, Maude Schneider g, Stephan Eliez g, Marco Armando g, Maria Pontillo h, Stefano Vicari h, Donna M. McDonald-McGinn i, Beverly S. Emanuel i, Elaine H. Zackai i, Carrie E. Bearden j, Vandana Shashi k, Stephen R. Hooper l, Michael J. Owen a, Raquel E. Gur m,n,o, Naomi R. Wray b, Marianne B.M. van den Bree a,1, Anita Thapar a,1, on behalf of the International 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Brain and Behavior Consortium a Medical Research Council Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom b Institute for Molecular Bioscience, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia c Department of Psychiatry, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands d Clinical Genetics Research Program, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada e The Dalglish Family 22q Clinic for 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome, Toronto General Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada f The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada g Department of Psychiatry, University
    [Show full text]
  • Title of the Trial
    RAPIDTFCBT – Draft Protocol V1 - 19 August 2016 TITLE OF THE TRIAL: Pragmatic Randomised controlled trial of a trauma-focused guided self help Programme versus Individual Trauma- Focused Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for post traumatic stress disorder (RAPIDTFCBT) PROTOCOL VERSION NUMBER AND DATE: Version 1.1 – 8 August 2016 SPONSOR: Cardiff University IRAS NUMBER: ISRCTN NUMBER: SPONSOR’S NUMBER: FUNDER’S NUMBER: HTA 14/192/97 This protocol has regard for the HRA guidance and order of content 1 RAPIDTFCBT – Draft Protocol V1 - 19 August 2016 SIGNATURE PAGE The undersigned confirm that the following protocol has been agreed and accepted and that the Chief Investigator agrees to conduct the trial in compliance with the approved protocol and will adhere to the GCP guidelines, the Sponsor’s SOPs, and other regulatory requirements as amended. I agree to ensure that the confidential information contained in this document will not be used for any other purpose other than the evaluation or conduct of the clinical investigation without the prior written consent of the Sponsor I also confirm that I will make the findings of the study publically available through publication or other dissemination tools without any unnecessary delay and that an honest accurate and transparent account of the study will be given; and that any discrepancies from the study as planned in this protocol will be explained. For and on behalf of the Study Sponsor: Signature: ………………………………………………………. Date: ....../....../...... Name (please print): ………………………………………….. Position:
    [Show full text]
  • Predicting the Incidence of Early-Onset MDD: a Prospective Familial High-Risk Study
    Antecedents of new-onset major depressive disorder in adolescence: a longitudinal familial high-risk study Article (Accepted Version) Rice, Frances, Sellers, Ruth, Hammerton, Gemma, Eyre, Olga, Bevan-Jones, Rhys, Thapar, Ajay K, Collishaw, Stephan, Harold, Gordon T and Thapar, Anita (2017) Antecedents of new-onset major depressive disorder in adolescence: a longitudinal familial high-risk study. JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (2). pp. 153-160. ISSN 2168-622X This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/63980/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way.
    [Show full text]
  • Clinical Research
    14_CH_8004_BA_INTERIEUR.qxd:DCNS#55 3/03/14 18:05 Page 103 Clinical research Neurofeedback and networks of depression David E. J. Linden, Dr med, Dr phil, DPhil (Oxon) Self-regulation through neurofeedback: technique and rationale Since its invention 20 years ago, functional mag- netic resonance imaging (fMRI) has become a central technique of cognitive and clinical neuroscience. The par- ticular strengths of this noninvasive technique are its spa- tial resolution, fidelity, and ability to reach deep subcorti- cal structures. Its whole-brain coverage enables the Recent advances in imaging technology and in the under- mapping of functionally connected networks and the standing of neural circuits relevant to emotion, motivation, extraction of information from distributed activation pat- and depression have boosted interest and experimental terns. These features make fMRI particularly suitable for work in neuromodulation for affective disorders. Real-time applications to mental disorders, where the pathology is functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can be used generally assumed to reside in faulty network activity, to train patients in the self-regulation of these circuits, and rather than focal lesions, and where deep structures play thus complement existing neurofeedback technologies a major role. The fMRI technique is particularly powerful based on electroencephalography (EEG). EEG neurofeed- in mapping correlates of mental states, another very back for depression has mainly been based on models of attractive feature for psychiatry, which deals predomi- altered hemispheric asymmetry. fMRI-based neurofeed- nantly with altered states of thought, emotion, and behav- back (fMRI-NF) can utilize functional localizer scans that ior. For example, fMRI scans acquired from patients with allow the dynamic adjustment of the target areas or net- chronic schizophrenia during the experience of auditory works for self-regulation training to individual patterns of verbal hallucinations have revealed activation in the audi- emotion processing.
    [Show full text]
  • Chapter Four: Feature Line Textures
    Chapter Four: Feature Line Textures Preceding chapters described the motivation for adding a sparse, opaque texture to an overlaid transparent surface, to help better communicate its shape and relative depth distance from underlying opaque structures while preserving its overall transparent character. The goal of this chapter is to describe the perceptual motivation for selectively opacifying valley and ridge regions and to present an implementation that I developed, independently and concurrently with similar efforts elsewhere, to do this. Inspired by the ability of gifted artists to define a figure with just a few strokes, I would like to define a technique for illustrating layered transparent surfaces, in three dimensions, so that they can be both clearly seen and easily seen through at the same time. My aim is to efficiently and effectively communicate the essential features of the superimposed surface in an intuitively meaningful way, using a clear and simple representation that is appropriate for dynamic viewing conditions, minimizes extraneous detail and allows a largely unobstructed and undistorted view of underlying objects. Research in pictorial representation and image understanding indicates that line drawings, and outline in particular, are a natural and universally understood means for communicating information about objects. A wide range of experiments (cited in [Kennedy 1974]) with animals, children and individuals from a variety of culturally-diverse populations provide evidence that the ability to recognize objects from line drawings is inborn (as opposed to a learned technique). The ease with which we interpret line drawings may suggest an intrinsic relationship between this type of representation and the way our visual system processes and stores visual information.
    [Show full text]
  • Gossip and Reputation in Childhood
    Gossip and Reputation in Childhood Gordon P. D. Ingram Department of Psychology, Universidad de los Andes, Colombia Email: [email protected] Full reference: Ingram, G. P. D. (2019). Gossip and reputation in childhood. In F. Giardini & R. Wittek (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of gossip and reputation. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 132–151. ISBN: 9780190494087. https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-oxford-handbook-of- gossip-and-reputation-9780190494087?cc=us&lang=en& Abstract Analysis of the development of gossip and reputation during childhood can help with understanding these processes in adulthood, as well as with understanding children’s own social worlds. Five stages of gossip-related behavior and reputation-related cognition are considered. Infants seem to be prepared for a reputational world in that they are sensitive to social stimuli; approach or avoid social agents who act positively or negatively to others, respectively; and point interaction partners toward relevant information. Young children engage in verbal signaling (normative protests and tattling) about individuals who violate social norms. In middle childhood, the development of higher-order theory of mind leads to a fully explicit awareness of reputation as something that can be linguistically transmitted. Because of this, preadolescents start to engage in increased conflict regarding others’ verbal evaluations. Finally, during adolescence and adulthood, gossip becomes more covert, more ambiguous, and less openly negative. The driving force behind all these changes is seen as children’s progressive independence from adults and dependence on peer relationships. Keywords child development, evolutionary developmental psychology, indirect aggression, ontogeny, Piaget, social selection, tattling, Tomasello Introduction This chapter attempts to show how an understanding of gossip and reputation in adults can be informed by an analysis of the growth of simpler forms of behavioral reporting and character evaluation in children.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract Book 2016 Abstracts Begin on Page
    JOURNAL OF PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY SUPPLEMENT TO VOLUME 30, ISSUE 8, AUGUST 2016 These papers were presented at the Summer Meeting of the BRITISH ASSOCIATION FOR PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 17 – 20 July, Brighton, UK Indemnity The scientific material presented at this meeting reflects the opinions of the contributing authors and speakers. The British Association for Psychopharmacology accepts no responsibility for the contents of the verbal or any published proceedings of this meeting. All contributors completed a Declaration of Interests form when submitting their abstract BAP Office 36 Cambridge Place Hills Road Cambridge CB2 1NS www.bap.org.uk Aii CONTENTS Abstract Book 2016 Abstracts begin on page: SYMPOSIUM 1 New paradigms and treatment approaches in schizophrenia (S01–S04) A1 SYMPOSIUM 2 New perspectives on functions of 5-HT sub-systems (S05–S08) A3 SYMPOSIUM 3 Towards a gender specific psychopharmacology: The impact of neurosteroids, gonadal steroids and oxytocin (S09–S12) A5 SYMPOSIUM 4 Bipolar disorder: Progress on many fronts (S13–S16) A7 SYMPOSIUM 5 Ketamine and addiction: Promising treatment or probable cause? (S17–S20) A9 SYMPOSIUM 6 Neuronal stem cells in mental health: Will neurogenesis and iPS cells give us the antidepressants of the future? (S21–S24) A11 SYMPOSIUM 7 Anhedonia, apathy, amotivation, anergia? Disrupted reward processing as a trans-diagnostic construct in mental illness (S25–S28) A13 SYMPOSIUM 8 Dysfunctional neuro-immune system interactions in psychiatric disorders and their relevance for novel treatment strategies (S29–S32)
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature of Delusion and Delusion-Like Belief
    THE NATURE OF DELUSION AND DELUSION-LIKE BELIEF Rachel Pechey Thesis submitted to Cardiff University in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy March 2010 UMI Number: U584420 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U584420 Published by ProQuest LLC 2013. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Firstly, 1 would like to particularly thank Professor Peter Halligan for all his help, enthusiasm and encouragement throughout this project. I would also like to take this opportunity to thank Dr. Gordon Harold for his helpful advice on a number of issues, and Dr. Sirwan Abdul and Dr. Kate Pietura, who offered advice and support for my work with patients. I am also very grateful to Tom Hamilton-James and the rest of the team at mruk, who carried out the research interviews. In addition, I am indebted to Sue Dentten for all her help with administrative issues. Special thanks are also due to Rhys Morgan, Harriet Over, Alan Pechey, and Hannah Togneri, who read and made constructive criticisms on earlier drafts.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Postpartum Psychosis
    CHALLENGETHE RESEARCH MAGAZINE FOR CARDIFF UNIVERSITYCARDIFFAUTUMN 2019 Understanding postpartum psychosis Sally Wilson finds out how the work of Professor Ian Jones is improving our understanding of this illness 2 In This Issue The research magazine for Cardiff University cardiff.ac.uk/research 31 08 16 11 IN THIS ISSUE 04 22 A welcome from Wales’ first multi-disease Challenge Cardiff Autumn 18 03 the Vice-Chancellor 22 biobank opens in Cardiff Managing Editor – Claire Sanders Editor – Alison Tobin Research round up What made me curious Contributors – Katie Bodinger, Paul Gauci, 04 24 Kevin Leonard and Julia Short Photography – Paul Gauci, Getty Images, Improving relationship and GW4 celebrates five years of Michael Hall, Matthew Horwood 08 sexuality education 26 innovation and collaboration and Wales News Service Design – ElevatorDesign.co.uk, front cover The enormous social illustration by Zoe Phare Research helps race to go challenge of achieving the Proofing – WeltchMedia.com 11 28 green UK’s carbon target Print – Harlequin Printing and Packaging Challenge Cardiff is produced by the Undergraduate research Communications and Marketing department, Images of research 14 scheme celebrates 10 years 31 Cardiff University, Friary House, Greyfriars Road, Cardiff, CF10 3AE. Understanding postpartum Challenge Cardiff is published in English and Research round up 16 psychosis 34 Welsh. To ensure we deliver the right medium to you please email [email protected] In conversation with to let us know which language you would prefer Research Institute focus to receive your edition in. 19 Dr Timothy Easun 38 Challenge Cardiff | Autumn 2019 Introduction 3 Welcome to Challenge Cardiff ARLIER THIS YEAR, Universities UK launched its #MadeAtUni campaign which highlights the “100+ ways that universities are saving lives and keeping us healthy”.
    [Show full text]
  • The Timing and Impact of Psychiatric, Cognitive and Motor Abnormalities in 2 Huntington’S Disease 3 4 Branduff Mcallister, Bsc, Phd1, James F
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.05.26.116798; this version posted January 14, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 The timing and impact of psychiatric, cognitive and motor abnormalities in 2 Huntington’s disease 3 4 Branduff McAllister, BSc, PhD1, James F. Gusella, PhD2, G. Bernhard 5 Landwehrmeyer, MD PhD3, Jong-Min Lee, PhD2, Marcy E. MacDonald, PhD2, 6 Michael Orth, MD PhD4, Anne E. Rosser, MB BChir, FRCP, PhD5,6, Nigel M. 7 Williams, BSc, PhD1, Peter Holmans, BA, PhD1, Lesley Jones, BSc, PhD1* & 8 Thomas H. Massey, MA, BM BCh, DPhil1* on behalf of the REGISTRY Investigators 9 of the European Huntington’s disease network7 10 11 12 1 Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, 13 Cardiff, United Kingdom 14 2 Molecular Neurogenetic Unit, Center for Genomic Medicine, Massachusetts 15 General Hospital and Department of Genetics, Harvard Medical School, Boston MA 16 02114 USA 17 3 University of Ulm, Department of Neurology, Ulm, Germany 18 4 Swiss Huntington’s disease Centre, Siloah, Bern, Switzerland 19 5 Brain Repair Group, Schools of Medicine and Biosciences, Cardiff University, 20 Cardiff, United Kingdom 21 6 Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute, Cardiff University, Cardiff, 22 United Kingdom 23 7 See Appendix 2 24 25 * Corresponding authors 26 Lesley Jones, Hadyn Ellis Building, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United 27 Kingdom. Phone: 00442920688469. Email: [email protected] 28 Thomas Massey, Hadyn Ellis Building, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United 29 Kingdom.
    [Show full text]
  • HHS Public Access Author Manuscript
    HHS Public Access Author manuscript Author Manuscript Author ManuscriptJ Neurol Author Manuscript. Author manuscript; Author Manuscript available in PMC 2016 December 01. Published in final edited form as: J Neurol. 2015 December ; 262(12): 2691–2698. doi:10.1007/s00415-015-7900-7. Motor onset and diagnosis in Huntington disease using the diagnostic confidence level Dawei Liu, PhD1,2, Jeffrey D. Long, PhD1,3, Ying Zhang, PhD4, Lynn A. Raymond, PhD, MD5, Karen Marder, MD6, Anne Rosser, PhD, FRCP7,8, Elizabeth A. McCusker, MB, BS (Hons), FRACP9, James A. Mills, MS1, and Jane S. Paulsen, PhD1,10,11,* the PREDICT-HD Investigators and Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group 1Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, The University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA, 52242, USA 2Biogen, 300 Binney Street, Cambridge, MA, 02142, USA (current affiliation) 3Department of Biostatistics, College of Public Health, The University of Iowa, S160 CPHB, 145 N. Riverside Drive, Iowa City, IA, 52242, USA 4Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University Fairbanks School of Public Health, 410 W. Tenth Street, Suite 3000, Indianapolis, IN, 46202, USA 5Department of Psychiatry and Brain Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Detwiller Pavilion, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC, V6T 2A1, Canada 6Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sergievsky Center and Taub Institute, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, 710 West 168th Street, New York, NY, 10032, USA 7Institute for Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences,
    [Show full text]