Chapter Four: Feature Line Textures

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Chapter Four: Feature Line Textures Chapter Four: Feature Line Textures Preceding chapters described the motivation for adding a sparse, opaque texture to an overlaid transparent surface, to help better communicate its shape and relative depth distance from underlying opaque structures while preserving its overall transparent character. The goal of this chapter is to describe the perceptual motivation for selectively opacifying valley and ridge regions and to present an implementation that I developed, independently and concurrently with similar efforts elsewhere, to do this. Inspired by the ability of gifted artists to define a figure with just a few strokes, I would like to define a technique for illustrating layered transparent surfaces, in three dimensions, so that they can be both clearly seen and easily seen through at the same time. My aim is to efficiently and effectively communicate the essential features of the superimposed surface in an intuitively meaningful way, using a clear and simple representation that is appropriate for dynamic viewing conditions, minimizes extraneous detail and allows a largely unobstructed and undistorted view of underlying objects. Research in pictorial representation and image understanding indicates that line drawings, and outline in particular, are a natural and universally understood means for communicating information about objects. A wide range of experiments (cited in [Kennedy 1974]) with animals, children and individuals from a variety of culturally-diverse populations provide evidence that the ability to recognize objects from line drawings is inborn (as opposed to a learned technique). The ease with which we interpret line drawings may suggest an intrinsic relationship between this type of representation and the way our visual system processes and stores visual information. David Marr’s “primal sketch” theory of visual information processing [Marr 1976], for example, is founded on the observation that many of the essential elements of image understanding can be derived from the type of information that is encoded in a line drawing representation of a scene; he postulates that the extraction of this “primal sketch” is a first step in visual information processing, at least for two-dimensional intensity images. Experiments by Biederman and Ju [1988] showed that familiar objects could be identified slightly more quickly and accurately in simple line drawings than in color slides when the images were presented, with masking, for extremely brief (50ms) exposure durations. (The accuracy and speed of object identification was equivalent for the two presentation methods when exposure durations were longer, and the objects could be correctly identified in nearly all of the images when viewing times were extended to 100ms.) These results provide further evidence both for the importance of the information that a line representation can carry and also for the merits of a display mode in which important information is highlighted while less essential detail is removed to improve clarity. Figure 4.1 illustrates some of the sample stimuli used in these object recognition experiments. Although the essential elements of many objects can be completely captured in a line drawing, it’s important to recognize that simple lines (particularly contours or outlines) don’t always provide sufficient information for object representation in general. Biederman and Ju [1988] noted that objects whose projections were structurally very similar (for example a peach and a plum) could not be easily differentiated on the basis of outline or contour information alone. They also observed that certain types of objects — such as hairbrushes — were poorly characterized by their outline. 77 Figure 4.1: Examples of sample stimuli used by Biederman and Ju [1988] in experiments showing slightly lower error rates and faster reaction times for naming (or verifying the identity of) objects represented by line drawings as opposed to color photographs, for very brief, masked exposure durations. Perceptual studies in facial recognition have consistently shown that observers perform poorly on tasks measuring the ability to identify familiar people from line drawings (and particularly poorly when only “outlines” of depth and intensity discontinuity are provided), even though the same individuals can be easily identified in the photographs upon which the line drawings are based. In experiments by Davies et al. [1978], individuals were correctly identified 90% of the time in photographs but only 23% of the time in outline (depth discontinuity and feature boundary) representations and 47% of the time in more detailed line drawings (in which intensity discontinuities were represented in addition to the outline). Subsequent experiments by Bruce et al. [1992] showed that inclusion of even the most basic form of bi-level shading (see figure 4.21-right for an example) could dramatically improve performance on facial recognition Figure 4.2: Examples of photographic vs. line drawing facial recognition task stimuli, generated by [Rhodes et al. 1987]. Left: a black & white photograph. Center: a line tracing of the photograph. Right: a computer-generated caricature derived from the line drawing. 78 tasks. The images in figure 4.2, from [Rhodes et al. 1987], are indicative of the type of sample stimuli used in these kinds of facial recognition experiments. Interestingly, the studies by Rhodes et al. [1987] showed that familiar individuals could be more easily recognized in line drawings when their most atypical facial features were selectively exaggerated, in the style of a caricature. It appears that simple outline drawings — in which lines are used only to mark intensity and depth discontinuities — lack certain essential information required for some higher-level object identification tasks. Although we can often easily categorize an object based on a simple line representation, experiments such as those by Price and Humphreys [1989] indicate that we do use other surface information, particularly shape, texture, or characteristic color information, to make finer distinctions between similar members of the same class. Price and Humphreys [1989] suggest that edge-based and surface-based recognition processes might operate in a cascaded manner, with the effects of the latter being most significant when the information encoded in the former is inadequate for the task. Despite the insufficiency of line drawings for some higher-level object differentiation tasks, it remains evident that line representations can convey a great deal of information about the objects in a scene. For a number of reasons, which will be discussed in greater detail in chapter five, texturing methods in which the surface opacity is explicitly varied according to the surface shading parameters do not appear particular promising; preliminary experiments — described in the appendix — appear to confirm this view. However, a sparse, opaque texture that approximates a “three-dimensional line drawing” might be useful for emphasizing the important features of a transparent surface without unduly occluding underlying objects. Recognizing the roles of various types of lines in visual perception, the question then becomes: how can we best define a set of descriptive lines to clearly and efficiently communicate the essential shape features of a transparent surface in a perceptually intuitive way? 4.1: Silhouette and contour curves Silhouette and contour curves are the two-dimensional projection of points on a surface in 3-space whose surface normal is orthogonal to the line of sight. Silhouette curves form a closed outline around the projected form, while contour curves may appear within the projected form and may be discontinuous. Figure 4.3 illustrates the silhouette and contour lines in a projected image of a simple object, from [Koenderink 1990]. Figure 4.3: Examples of silhouette and contour curves in the projection of a simple object. Left: detailed image of a banana-shaped surface, from [Koenderink 1990]. Center: the silhouette curve. Right: the contour curve. 79 Silhouette and contour curves are ubiquitous in two-dimensional line art and illustration; it’s difficult to imagine a successful line drawing that isn’t based in large part upon these curves. Silhouette and contour lines possess a number of features that make them useful for image understanding and object recognition. Of particular importance, contour lines mark the depth discontinuities in a two-dimensional image, and silhouette lines separate the figure from the ground. One popular method for automatically generating line drawings of three-dimensional models, proposed by Saito and Takahashi [1990], operates directly from this observation, defining the contour lines in a two-dimensional projection by using a gradient operator to locate discontinuities in the depths of the surfaces that map to adjacent pixels in the projected image. Figure 4.4, from [Saito and Takahashi 1990], shows examples of some “perceptually-enhanced” images produced by this method. Figure 4.4: Enhanced images, generated by Saito and Takahashi [1990], in which various types of depth discontinuities, found using an edge operator on a two-dimensional depth map, are highlighted with black or white lines. Left: Upper left quadrant: the original “nut” image. Lower left quadrant: black lines represent the detected locations of zero-order depth discontinuities; white lines represent the detected locations of first-order (slope) discontinuities. Upper right quadrant: lines of zero-
Recommended publications
  • Download Complete Issue
    VOLUME 1, No.2 30 June 2017 ISSN 2514-3174 bsdj.org.uk Advanced Necrotising Enterocolitis How well are we managing them? The British Student Doctor is an open access journal, which means that all content is available without charge to the user or his/her institution. You are allowed to read, download, copy, distribute, print, search, or link to the full texts of the articles in this journal without asking prior permission from either the publisher or the author. bsdj.org.uk Journal DOI 10.18573/issn.2514-3174 /thebsdj Issue DOI @thebsdj 10.18573/bsdj.v2i1 @thebsdj This journal is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution- NonCommercial-NoDerivatives 4.0 International License. The copyright of all articles belongs to The British Student Doctor, and a citation should be made when any article is quoted, used or referred to in another work. The British Student Doctor is an imprint of Cardiff University Press, an innovative open-access publisher of academic research, where ‘open-access’ means free for both readers and writers. cardiffuniversitypress.org Contents EDITORIAL 1 Dr James M Kilgour and Dr Shivali Fulchand Editorial: Mental health and medical students Natalie Ellis, Munzir Quraisy, Dr Matthew Hoskins, ORIGINAL RESEARCH 4 Dr James Walters, Dr Steve Riley and Dr Liz Forty Medical student attitudes to mental health and psychiatry: the use of a patient-experience short film DISCUSSION 12 Alexander J Martin Medicalisation: the definition of disease and the role of tomorrow’s doctors 18 Michael Houssemayne du Boulay An inconsistency
    [Show full text]
  • Attention Deficit Hyperactivity Disorder Symptoms
    SCHRES-07994; No of Pages 6 Schizophrenia Research xxx (2018) xxx–xxx Contents lists available at ScienceDirect Schizophrenia Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/schres Attention deficit hyperactivity disorder symptoms as antecedents of later psychotic outcomes in 22q11.2 deletion syndrome Maria Niarchou a,b,⁎, Samuel J.R.A. Chawner a, Ania Fiksinski c,d,e, Jacob A.S. Vorstman d,e,f, Johanna Maeder g, Maude Schneider g, Stephan Eliez g, Marco Armando g, Maria Pontillo h, Stefano Vicari h, Donna M. McDonald-McGinn i, Beverly S. Emanuel i, Elaine H. Zackai i, Carrie E. Bearden j, Vandana Shashi k, Stephen R. Hooper l, Michael J. Owen a, Raquel E. Gur m,n,o, Naomi R. Wray b, Marianne B.M. van den Bree a,1, Anita Thapar a,1, on behalf of the International 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome Brain and Behavior Consortium a Medical Research Council Centre for Neuropsychiatric Genetics and Genomics, Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, Cardiff, United Kingdom b Institute for Molecular Bioscience, University of Queensland, Brisbane, Australia c Department of Psychiatry, Rudolf Magnus Institute of Neuroscience, University Medical Center Utrecht, Utrecht, the Netherlands d Clinical Genetics Research Program, Centre for Addiction and Mental Health, Toronto, Ontario, Canada e The Dalglish Family 22q Clinic for 22q11.2 Deletion Syndrome, Toronto General Hospital, University Health Network, Toronto, Ontario, Canada f The Hospital for Sick Children, Toronto, Canada g Department of Psychiatry, University
    [Show full text]
  • Title of the Trial
    RAPIDTFCBT – Draft Protocol V1 - 19 August 2016 TITLE OF THE TRIAL: Pragmatic Randomised controlled trial of a trauma-focused guided self help Programme versus Individual Trauma- Focused Cognitive Behavioural Therapy for post traumatic stress disorder (RAPIDTFCBT) PROTOCOL VERSION NUMBER AND DATE: Version 1.1 – 8 August 2016 SPONSOR: Cardiff University IRAS NUMBER: ISRCTN NUMBER: SPONSOR’S NUMBER: FUNDER’S NUMBER: HTA 14/192/97 This protocol has regard for the HRA guidance and order of content 1 RAPIDTFCBT – Draft Protocol V1 - 19 August 2016 SIGNATURE PAGE The undersigned confirm that the following protocol has been agreed and accepted and that the Chief Investigator agrees to conduct the trial in compliance with the approved protocol and will adhere to the GCP guidelines, the Sponsor’s SOPs, and other regulatory requirements as amended. I agree to ensure that the confidential information contained in this document will not be used for any other purpose other than the evaluation or conduct of the clinical investigation without the prior written consent of the Sponsor I also confirm that I will make the findings of the study publically available through publication or other dissemination tools without any unnecessary delay and that an honest accurate and transparent account of the study will be given; and that any discrepancies from the study as planned in this protocol will be explained. For and on behalf of the Study Sponsor: Signature: ………………………………………………………. Date: ....../....../...... Name (please print): ………………………………………….. Position:
    [Show full text]
  • Predicting the Incidence of Early-Onset MDD: a Prospective Familial High-Risk Study
    Antecedents of new-onset major depressive disorder in adolescence: a longitudinal familial high-risk study Article (Accepted Version) Rice, Frances, Sellers, Ruth, Hammerton, Gemma, Eyre, Olga, Bevan-Jones, Rhys, Thapar, Ajay K, Collishaw, Stephan, Harold, Gordon T and Thapar, Anita (2017) Antecedents of new-onset major depressive disorder in adolescence: a longitudinal familial high-risk study. JAMA Psychiatry, 74 (2). pp. 153-160. ISSN 2168-622X This version is available from Sussex Research Online: http://sro.sussex.ac.uk/id/eprint/63980/ This document is made available in accordance with publisher policies and may differ from the published version or from the version of record. If you wish to cite this item you are advised to consult the publisher’s version. Please see the URL above for details on accessing the published version. Copyright and reuse: Sussex Research Online is a digital repository of the research output of the University. Copyright and all moral rights to the version of the paper presented here belong to the individual author(s) and/or other copyright owners. To the extent reasonable and practicable, the material made available in SRO has been checked for eligibility before being made available. Copies of full text items generally can be reproduced, displayed or performed and given to third parties in any format or medium for personal research or study, educational, or not-for-profit purposes without prior permission or charge, provided that the authors, title and full bibliographic details are credited, a hyperlink and/or URL is given for the original metadata page and the content is not changed in any way.
    [Show full text]
  • Clinical Research
    14_CH_8004_BA_INTERIEUR.qxd:DCNS#55 3/03/14 18:05 Page 103 Clinical research Neurofeedback and networks of depression David E. J. Linden, Dr med, Dr phil, DPhil (Oxon) Self-regulation through neurofeedback: technique and rationale Since its invention 20 years ago, functional mag- netic resonance imaging (fMRI) has become a central technique of cognitive and clinical neuroscience. The par- ticular strengths of this noninvasive technique are its spa- tial resolution, fidelity, and ability to reach deep subcorti- cal structures. Its whole-brain coverage enables the Recent advances in imaging technology and in the under- mapping of functionally connected networks and the standing of neural circuits relevant to emotion, motivation, extraction of information from distributed activation pat- and depression have boosted interest and experimental terns. These features make fMRI particularly suitable for work in neuromodulation for affective disorders. Real-time applications to mental disorders, where the pathology is functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) can be used generally assumed to reside in faulty network activity, to train patients in the self-regulation of these circuits, and rather than focal lesions, and where deep structures play thus complement existing neurofeedback technologies a major role. The fMRI technique is particularly powerful based on electroencephalography (EEG). EEG neurofeed- in mapping correlates of mental states, another very back for depression has mainly been based on models of attractive feature for psychiatry, which deals predomi- altered hemispheric asymmetry. fMRI-based neurofeed- nantly with altered states of thought, emotion, and behav- back (fMRI-NF) can utilize functional localizer scans that ior. For example, fMRI scans acquired from patients with allow the dynamic adjustment of the target areas or net- chronic schizophrenia during the experience of auditory works for self-regulation training to individual patterns of verbal hallucinations have revealed activation in the audi- emotion processing.
    [Show full text]
  • Gossip and Reputation in Childhood
    Gossip and Reputation in Childhood Gordon P. D. Ingram Department of Psychology, Universidad de los Andes, Colombia Email: [email protected] Full reference: Ingram, G. P. D. (2019). Gossip and reputation in childhood. In F. Giardini & R. Wittek (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of gossip and reputation. Oxford, England: Oxford University Press, pp. 132–151. ISBN: 9780190494087. https://global.oup.com/academic/product/the-oxford-handbook-of- gossip-and-reputation-9780190494087?cc=us&lang=en& Abstract Analysis of the development of gossip and reputation during childhood can help with understanding these processes in adulthood, as well as with understanding children’s own social worlds. Five stages of gossip-related behavior and reputation-related cognition are considered. Infants seem to be prepared for a reputational world in that they are sensitive to social stimuli; approach or avoid social agents who act positively or negatively to others, respectively; and point interaction partners toward relevant information. Young children engage in verbal signaling (normative protests and tattling) about individuals who violate social norms. In middle childhood, the development of higher-order theory of mind leads to a fully explicit awareness of reputation as something that can be linguistically transmitted. Because of this, preadolescents start to engage in increased conflict regarding others’ verbal evaluations. Finally, during adolescence and adulthood, gossip becomes more covert, more ambiguous, and less openly negative. The driving force behind all these changes is seen as children’s progressive independence from adults and dependence on peer relationships. Keywords child development, evolutionary developmental psychology, indirect aggression, ontogeny, Piaget, social selection, tattling, Tomasello Introduction This chapter attempts to show how an understanding of gossip and reputation in adults can be informed by an analysis of the growth of simpler forms of behavioral reporting and character evaluation in children.
    [Show full text]
  • Abstract Book 2016 Abstracts Begin on Page
    JOURNAL OF PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY SUPPLEMENT TO VOLUME 30, ISSUE 8, AUGUST 2016 These papers were presented at the Summer Meeting of the BRITISH ASSOCIATION FOR PSYCHOPHARMACOLOGY 17 – 20 July, Brighton, UK Indemnity The scientific material presented at this meeting reflects the opinions of the contributing authors and speakers. The British Association for Psychopharmacology accepts no responsibility for the contents of the verbal or any published proceedings of this meeting. All contributors completed a Declaration of Interests form when submitting their abstract BAP Office 36 Cambridge Place Hills Road Cambridge CB2 1NS www.bap.org.uk Aii CONTENTS Abstract Book 2016 Abstracts begin on page: SYMPOSIUM 1 New paradigms and treatment approaches in schizophrenia (S01–S04) A1 SYMPOSIUM 2 New perspectives on functions of 5-HT sub-systems (S05–S08) A3 SYMPOSIUM 3 Towards a gender specific psychopharmacology: The impact of neurosteroids, gonadal steroids and oxytocin (S09–S12) A5 SYMPOSIUM 4 Bipolar disorder: Progress on many fronts (S13–S16) A7 SYMPOSIUM 5 Ketamine and addiction: Promising treatment or probable cause? (S17–S20) A9 SYMPOSIUM 6 Neuronal stem cells in mental health: Will neurogenesis and iPS cells give us the antidepressants of the future? (S21–S24) A11 SYMPOSIUM 7 Anhedonia, apathy, amotivation, anergia? Disrupted reward processing as a trans-diagnostic construct in mental illness (S25–S28) A13 SYMPOSIUM 8 Dysfunctional neuro-immune system interactions in psychiatric disorders and their relevance for novel treatment strategies (S29–S32)
    [Show full text]
  • The Nature of Delusion and Delusion-Like Belief
    THE NATURE OF DELUSION AND DELUSION-LIKE BELIEF Rachel Pechey Thesis submitted to Cardiff University in fulfilment of the requirements for the degree of Doctor of Philosophy March 2010 UMI Number: U584420 All rights reserved INFORMATION TO ALL USERS The quality of this reproduction is dependent upon the quality of the copy submitted. In the unlikely event that the author did not send a complete manuscript and there are missing pages, these will be noted. Also, if material had to be removed, a note will indicate the deletion. Dissertation Publishing UMI U584420 Published by ProQuest LLC 2013. Copyright in the Dissertation held by the Author. Microform Edition © ProQuest LLC. All rights reserved. This work is protected against unauthorized copying under Title 17, United States Code. ProQuest LLC 789 East Eisenhower Parkway P.O. Box 1346 Ann Arbor, Ml 48106-1346 ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS Firstly, 1 would like to particularly thank Professor Peter Halligan for all his help, enthusiasm and encouragement throughout this project. I would also like to take this opportunity to thank Dr. Gordon Harold for his helpful advice on a number of issues, and Dr. Sirwan Abdul and Dr. Kate Pietura, who offered advice and support for my work with patients. I am also very grateful to Tom Hamilton-James and the rest of the team at mruk, who carried out the research interviews. In addition, I am indebted to Sue Dentten for all her help with administrative issues. Special thanks are also due to Rhys Morgan, Harriet Over, Alan Pechey, and Hannah Togneri, who read and made constructive criticisms on earlier drafts.
    [Show full text]
  • Understanding Postpartum Psychosis
    CHALLENGETHE RESEARCH MAGAZINE FOR CARDIFF UNIVERSITYCARDIFFAUTUMN 2019 Understanding postpartum psychosis Sally Wilson finds out how the work of Professor Ian Jones is improving our understanding of this illness 2 In This Issue The research magazine for Cardiff University cardiff.ac.uk/research 31 08 16 11 IN THIS ISSUE 04 22 A welcome from Wales’ first multi-disease Challenge Cardiff Autumn 18 03 the Vice-Chancellor 22 biobank opens in Cardiff Managing Editor – Claire Sanders Editor – Alison Tobin Research round up What made me curious Contributors – Katie Bodinger, Paul Gauci, 04 24 Kevin Leonard and Julia Short Photography – Paul Gauci, Getty Images, Improving relationship and GW4 celebrates five years of Michael Hall, Matthew Horwood 08 sexuality education 26 innovation and collaboration and Wales News Service Design – ElevatorDesign.co.uk, front cover The enormous social illustration by Zoe Phare Research helps race to go challenge of achieving the Proofing – WeltchMedia.com 11 28 green UK’s carbon target Print – Harlequin Printing and Packaging Challenge Cardiff is produced by the Undergraduate research Communications and Marketing department, Images of research 14 scheme celebrates 10 years 31 Cardiff University, Friary House, Greyfriars Road, Cardiff, CF10 3AE. Understanding postpartum Challenge Cardiff is published in English and Research round up 16 psychosis 34 Welsh. To ensure we deliver the right medium to you please email [email protected] In conversation with to let us know which language you would prefer Research Institute focus to receive your edition in. 19 Dr Timothy Easun 38 Challenge Cardiff | Autumn 2019 Introduction 3 Welcome to Challenge Cardiff ARLIER THIS YEAR, Universities UK launched its #MadeAtUni campaign which highlights the “100+ ways that universities are saving lives and keeping us healthy”.
    [Show full text]
  • Impulsive Choice in Mice Lacking Paternal Expression of Grb10 Suggests Intra-Genomic
    Genetics: Early Online, published on March 21, 2018 as 10.1534/genetics.118.300898 1 Impulsive choice in mice lacking paternal expression of Grb10 suggests intra-genomic 2 conflict in behavior 3 Claire L. Dent*, Trevor Humby†, Katie Lewis*, Andrew Ward‡, Reiner Fischer-Colbrie§, 4 LawrenCe S. Wilkinson*,†, Jon F Wilkins** & Anthony R. Isles*, †† 5 *Behavioural GenetiCs GrouP, MRC Centre for NeuroPsyChiatriC GenetiCs and GenomiCs, 6 NeurosCienCe and Mental Health ResearCh Institute, Cardiff University, Hadyn Ellis Building, 7 Maindy Road, Cardiff, United Kingdom 8 †Behavioural GenetiCs GrouP, SChool of PsyChology, Cardiff University, Tower Building, 9 Cardiff, United Kingdom 10 ‡DePartment of Biology and BioChemistry, University of Bath, Bath, United Kingdom 11 §DePartment of PharmaCology, InnsbruCk MediCal University, InnsbruCk, Austria 12 **Ronin Institute, Montclair, NJ 07043, USA 13 ††Author for CorresPondenCe: Anthony R. Isles, E-mail [email protected] 14 15 Running title: Intragenomic conflict and impulsivity 16 Keywords: genomiC imPrinting; NesP55; delayed reinforCement; intra-genomiC ConfliCt; 17 Grb10 18 19 20 21 1 Copyright 2018. 1 Abstract 2 ImPrinted genes are exPressed from one Parental allele only as a ConsequenCe of ePigenetiC 3 events that take Place in the mammalian germ line and are thought to have evolved through 4 intra-genomiC ConfliCt between Parental alleles. We demonstrate, for the first time, 5 oPPositional effeCts of imPrinted genes on brain and behavior. SPeCifiCally, here we show 6 that miCe lacking Paternal Grb10 make fewer impulsive choices, with no dissoCiable effeCts 7 on a seParate measure of imPulsive action. Taken together with Previous work showing that 8 miCe lacking maternal NesP55 make more imPulsive ChoiCes this suggests that imPulsive 9 ChoiCe behavior is a substrate for the action of genomiC imPrinting.
    [Show full text]
  • The Timing and Impact of Psychiatric, Cognitive and Motor Abnormalities in 2 Huntington’S Disease 3 4 Branduff Mcallister, Bsc, Phd1, James F
    bioRxiv preprint doi: https://doi.org/10.1101/2020.05.26.116798; this version posted January 14, 2021. The copyright holder for this preprint (which was not certified by peer review) is the author/funder. All rights reserved. No reuse allowed without permission. 1 The timing and impact of psychiatric, cognitive and motor abnormalities in 2 Huntington’s disease 3 4 Branduff McAllister, BSc, PhD1, James F. Gusella, PhD2, G. Bernhard 5 Landwehrmeyer, MD PhD3, Jong-Min Lee, PhD2, Marcy E. MacDonald, PhD2, 6 Michael Orth, MD PhD4, Anne E. Rosser, MB BChir, FRCP, PhD5,6, Nigel M. 7 Williams, BSc, PhD1, Peter Holmans, BA, PhD1, Lesley Jones, BSc, PhD1* & 8 Thomas H. Massey, MA, BM BCh, DPhil1* on behalf of the REGISTRY Investigators 9 of the European Huntington’s disease network7 10 11 12 1 Division of Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences, Cardiff University, 13 Cardiff, United Kingdom 14 2 Molecular Neurogenetic Unit, Center for Genomic Medicine, Massachusetts 15 General Hospital and Department of Genetics, Harvard Medical School, Boston MA 16 02114 USA 17 3 University of Ulm, Department of Neurology, Ulm, Germany 18 4 Swiss Huntington’s disease Centre, Siloah, Bern, Switzerland 19 5 Brain Repair Group, Schools of Medicine and Biosciences, Cardiff University, 20 Cardiff, United Kingdom 21 6 Neuroscience and Mental Health Research Institute, Cardiff University, Cardiff, 22 United Kingdom 23 7 See Appendix 2 24 25 * Corresponding authors 26 Lesley Jones, Hadyn Ellis Building, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United 27 Kingdom. Phone: 00442920688469. Email: [email protected] 28 Thomas Massey, Hadyn Ellis Building, Cardiff University, Cardiff CF24 4HQ, United 29 Kingdom.
    [Show full text]
  • HHS Public Access Author Manuscript
    HHS Public Access Author manuscript Author Manuscript Author ManuscriptJ Neurol Author Manuscript. Author manuscript; Author Manuscript available in PMC 2016 December 01. Published in final edited form as: J Neurol. 2015 December ; 262(12): 2691–2698. doi:10.1007/s00415-015-7900-7. Motor onset and diagnosis in Huntington disease using the diagnostic confidence level Dawei Liu, PhD1,2, Jeffrey D. Long, PhD1,3, Ying Zhang, PhD4, Lynn A. Raymond, PhD, MD5, Karen Marder, MD6, Anne Rosser, PhD, FRCP7,8, Elizabeth A. McCusker, MB, BS (Hons), FRACP9, James A. Mills, MS1, and Jane S. Paulsen, PhD1,10,11,* the PREDICT-HD Investigators and Coordinators of the Huntington Study Group 1Department of Psychiatry, Carver College of Medicine, The University of Iowa, 200 Hawkins Drive, Iowa City, IA, 52242, USA 2Biogen, 300 Binney Street, Cambridge, MA, 02142, USA (current affiliation) 3Department of Biostatistics, College of Public Health, The University of Iowa, S160 CPHB, 145 N. Riverside Drive, Iowa City, IA, 52242, USA 4Department of Biostatistics, Indiana University Fairbanks School of Public Health, 410 W. Tenth Street, Suite 3000, Indianapolis, IN, 46202, USA 5Department of Psychiatry and Brain Research Centre, University of British Columbia, Detwiller Pavilion, 2255 Wesbrook Mall, Vancouver, BC, V6T 2A1, Canada 6Departments of Neurology and Psychiatry, Sergievsky Center and Taub Institute, Columbia University College of Physicians and Surgeons, 710 West 168th Street, New York, NY, 10032, USA 7Institute for Psychological Medicine and Clinical Neurosciences,
    [Show full text]