Reflections of Verbal Syntax in Nominalization and Adjectivization

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Reflections of Verbal Syntax in Nominalization and Adjectivization Poznań Studies in Contemporary Linguistics 44(3), 2008, pp. 284–301 © School of English, Adam Mickiewicz University, Poznań, Poland doi:10.2478/v10010-008-0014-y REFLECTIONS OF VERBAL SYNTAX IN NOMINALIZATION AND ADJECTIVIZATION ADAM BIAŁY University of Wrocław [email protected] ABSTRACT In Distributed Morphology (DM), morphology is seen as a reflection of syntactic processes and requirements. Since DM is an essentially syntax-based approach to word formation processes, and derivation in particular, it assumes the retention of original structure in a given process of derivation where a new form is created. For example, the structure for the verb integrate has to be contained within the structure for the derived noun integration . The question is what consti- tutes the structure of integrate , and to what extent it is preserved in the structure of the deverbal noun. Following Borer (2003) and Harley (in press), the analysis of the relationships between such related forms reveals important rules governing phrase structure in general. The paper focuses mainly on Polish nominalization and adjectivization data and sets it against similar investigations conducted on English (e.g., Harley in press). The aim of the analy- sis is to investigate whether the accounts proposed for languages such as English, in the spirit of DM, can be maintained for Polish. The results also add to the general discussion of what part of meaning, or interpretation, should be associated with the lexical root (i.e. lexicon), and what part is inherently structural or functional. KEYWORDS : derivation; categorization; participials; scalar structure. 1. The background Approaches to syntax, such as Distributed Morphology (DM) (Halle and Marantz 1993, 1994), reject the multi-layer view on language representation in favour of a monolithic one. As a result, the “older” generative assumption that morphology and syntax are two separate components of grammar is rejected. In DM, natural language is argued to pos- sess a repertoire of syntactic or functional projections which are mapped onto seman- tics/interpretation. Languages may differ in three respects. Firstly, there is a fixed reper- toire of structural/functional projections from which a subset is assigned to a particular language; secondlly, the positions for these projections in the overall structure of a sen- tence may be different; thirdly, they may lack overt morphological items to check a 284 A. Biały given position. DM provides a linguist with two kinds of tools that can be used to check the structural build-up of a given language: morphology and meaning. This results from the assumption that syntactic structure is interpretable (a common denominator of all constructionist approaches). 1 The aim of the paper is to investigate Polish categorization data and compare it with similar investigations conducted on English (e.g. Harley in press). The analysis focuses on derived constructs which arise as a result of verbalization, nominalization, and adjec- tivization. The discussion is strongly based on the account of Jabłońska (2007) and can be seen as a minor refinement of that approach in matters relevant for the subject matter of this paper. The results contribute to the general discussion of which part of meaning, or interpretation, should be associated with the lexical root (i.e. lexicon), and which part is inherently structural or functional. 2. Categorization in DM DM makes use of two classes of terminal nodes: a-categorial roots (√s) and grammati- cal elements of various kinds ( f-morphemes , which correspond to functional projections in many other approaches). Roots acquire a category when they merge with a given f- morpheme in the course of the syntactic derivation. The category creating f-morphemes are: v° (verbalizer), n° (nominalizer), a° (adjectivizer). 2.1. Nominalizations in DM In DM derivation is seen as a cyclical process and it is directly correlated with mor- phology. Once a given structure is derived, it is retained in the course of further deriva- tion. The relevant example is the nominalization of a verbal base where the noun retains some characteristics of the underlying verb, like Case assignment and adverbial modifi- cation, in (1c), but not in (1b, d). (1a) John unwillingly sold the car. (1b) John’s unwilling sale of the car led to his nervous breakdown. (1c) John(’s) unwillingly selling the car led to his nervous breakdown. (1d) John’s unwilling selling of the car led to his nervous breakdown. On the basis of such bahaviour, Harley (in press) distinguishes two types of nominaliza- tions: “ACC-ing nominalizations” (1c) and “OF-ing nominalizations” (1b, d), and lists the following properties of the two. 1 This approach is succintly described in Harley (2006): “wherever you see a morpheme, there must be a corresponding terminal node in the structural analysis of the sentence. Where you don’t see a morpheme, there may well be a terminal node filled by a ∅ element; this happens all the time in English.” Verbal syntax in nominalization and adjectivization 285 (2) ACC-ing properties: Verbal characteristics: Accusative Case assignment, adverbial modification OF-ing properties: Nominal characteristics: of -Case assignment, adjectival modification Harley, following Kratzer (1996), postulates that the difference between the two types of -ing nominals results from the position in which the nominalizing suffix is attached in the derivation. The low position (above a subject-introducing projection) results in the OF-ing type (4). The high position, the position at which external argument and ACC Case are assigned, results in the ACC-ing structure (3). In Marantz (1997), the verbalizing vº head is associated with the external argument- introducing vP shell. The projection becomes verbal when the root gets affixed to vº (the verbalizer) via head-movement. OF-ing nominals lack the verbalizing vP above the root, so they fail to reveal verbal characteristics. (3) presents the structure for John sell- ing the car , while (4) is the structure for an OF-ing nominalization, nominalization of verbs . (3) John selling the car 286 A. Biały (4) ominalization of verbs (Marantz 1997) (5) Full verbal structure including agent-introducing head (Harley in press) Harley (in press) argues, contrary to Marantz, that many deverbal nominals (OF-ing type) contain the verbalizing morpheme -ize , so they should contain the verbalizing pro- jection (v°) in line with DM criteria. As indicated by the example in (3), the v° projec- tion is associated with the FP, which in turn gives rise to the Accusative Case assigning ability. This ability is clearly absent in the nominalization of verbs , where the object ar- gument has to be realized in a PP. The way out of this dilemma is to dissociate the ver- balizing projection from the Accusative Case checking and external argument assigning projections (5), as it is done in Pylkkänen (2002). Verbal syntax in nominalization and adjectivization 287 The analysis of nominals indicates that derivation proceeds in a clearly defined fashion with given parts of the projection being associated with particular interpretation. The lower part of the projection is associated with nominal characteristics, whereas the upper part is “verbal”. Such analysis meets the assumption adopted in this paper that categorization is an interpretative notion and is associated with sequential derivation. 2.1.1. Nominalization in Polish The division into ACC-ing and OF-ing nominals (cf. (1b, c, d)) is not accounted for in Polish. (6a) sprzedaż samochodu przez Janka sale car-Gen by John (6b) sprzedanie samochodu przez Janka selling car-Gen by John (6c) *Janek sprzedanie samochód John-om selling car-Acc (6d) *sprzedaż przez Janka ‘sales by Janek’ The distinction that is observable in Polish is between result (6a, d) and process nomi- nals (6b). Grzegorczykowa and Puzynina (1999) distinguish “regular” nominals in (7a), and “irregular” nominals in (7b). These types of nominals also differ with respect to the type and number of categorial characteristics they share. The irregular nominals (7b), for example, exhibit restrictions with respect to the arguments they license. (7a) pocałowanie Zosi (przez Janka) kissing Zosia-Gen (by John) (7b) *pocałunek Zosi (przez Janka) kiss Zosia-Gen (przez Janka) Pocałowanie can license the direct argument, but pocałunek cannot. The nominal po- całunek Zosi in (7b) becomes acceptable when the interpretation is changed and Zosia is interpreted as the Agent. This is illustrated with additional context in (8). (8) Tomek pragnął zachować każdy pocałunek Zosi w pamięci. ‘Tom wished to keep Zosia’s every kiss in his memory.’ 288 A. Biały Polish nominals do not allow adverbial modification but they can be modified by adjec- tival modifiers. 2 (9a) nagła/*nagle sprzedaż samochodu ‘sudden/suddenly sale of the car’ (9b) nagłe/*nagle sprzedanie samochodu ‘sudden/suddenly selling of the car’ The fact that there exist constructs which seem to behave inaccordingly with their tradi- tional categorial labelling is addressed by Jabłońska (2007), who postulates that nomi- nal and verbal projections should be differentiated on a functional sequence. The func- tional sequence is responsible for the aspectual and thematic meaning that differentiates Agent and Patient arguments. 3 In this system, all functional meaning is interpretative and it is related to specific projections in the functional sequence. The projection of relevant layers of the functional sequence is reflected by morphological markers. Nominals in Polish do not exhibit verbal characteristics, such as Case assignment and adverbial modification, because they do not reach the layer of derivation in which Case and agentive characteristics are assigned. Categorisation is seen as an interpretative no- tion relating to the hierarchical development of the derivation. Jabłońska (2007) argues that Polish nominals cannot “take over” verbal morphol- ogy – they can only realise the bottom or right-side part of the structure. Thus, they are not structural Case assigners and they do not allow adverbial modification associated with higher functional projections.
Recommended publications
  • Syntactic Aspects of Nominalization in Five Tibeto-Burman Languages of the Himalayan Area1
    Linguistics of the Tibeto-Burman Area Volume 31.2 — October 2008 SYNTACTIC ASPECTS OF NOMINALIZATION IN FIVE TIBETO-BURMAN LANGUAGES OF THE HIMALAYAN AREA1 Carol Genetti University of California, Santa Barbara Research Centre for Linguistic Typology A.R. Coupe Ellen Bartee Kristine Hildebrandt You-Jing Lin La Trobe University SIL SIUE UC Santa Barbara The goal of this paper is to describe some of the syntactic structures that are created through nominalization processes in Himalayan Tibeto-Burman languages and the relationships between those structures. These include both structures involving the nominalization of clauses (e.g. complement clauses, relative clauses) and structures involving the nominalization of verbs and predicates (e.g. the derivation of nouns and adjectives). We will argue that, synchronically, clausal nominalization, structurally represented as [clause]NP, is the basic structure underlying many of the nominalizing constructions in these languages, even though individual constructions embed and alter this structure in interesting ways. In addition to clausal nominalization, we will illustrate the presence of derivational nominalization, represented as [V-NOM]N and [V-NOM]ADJ, although some nominal derivations target the predicate, not the verb root as their domain. We will also demonstrate that derivational nominalization can be seen as having developed from clausal nominalization, at least for some forms in some languages, and that the opposite direction of development, from derivational to clausal structures, is also attested. We will conclude with some syntactic observations pertinent to recent claims made on the historical relationship between nominalization and relativization, demonstrating that there are various ways that these structures can be related.
    [Show full text]
  • Nominalization – Lexical and Syntactic Aspects
    Manfred Bierwisch Nominalization – Lexical and Syntactic Aspects 1. The Issue The main tenet of the present paper is the thesis that nominalization – like other cases of derivational morphology – is an essentially lexical phenomenon with well defined syntactic (and semantic) conditions and consequences. More specifically, it will be argued that the relation between a verb and the noun derived from it is subject to both systematic and idiosyncratic conditions with respect to lexical as well as syntactic aspects. Familiar cases like (1) and (2) illustrate the point: (1)(a) Daß Eva nach Bern fuhr, überraschte uns. (That Eva drove to Bern, surprised us.) (b) Evas Fahrt nach Bern überraschte uns. (Eva's drive to Bern surprised us.) (2)(a) Daß Peter die Regeln erklärte, war nützlich. (That Peter explained the rules, was useful.) (b) Peters Erklärung der Regeln war nützlich. (Peter's explanation of the rules was useful.) While the choice of morphological markers is lexically determined and highly idiosyncratic – fahren/Fahrt (drive/drive) vs. erklären/Erklärung (explain/explanation)-, syntactic combination follows general rules: the verb assigns nominative and accusative case to the subject and direct object, the derived noun systematically requires genitive case for both complements. These observations exhaust neither the idiosyncratic nor the systematic properties of nominalizations, but they highlight the central issue of the present paper: The relation between lexical entries that enter into verbal and nominal constructions, respectively, can neither be captured in purely syntactic terms, nor can it be reduced to lexical properties, ignoring the systematic syntactic (and semantic) conditions and consequences. This raises the question how the systematic and idiosyncratic conditions of nominal and verbal constructions are represented, and how they interact.
    [Show full text]
  • Nominalization and Its Various Uses in Thulung Aimée Lahaussois
    Nominalization and its various uses in Thulung Aimée Lahaussois To cite this version: Aimée Lahaussois. Nominalization and its various uses in Thulung. Languages of the Tibeto-Burman Area, 2003, 26 (1), pp.33-57. halshs-00004758 HAL Id: halshs-00004758 https://halshs.archives-ouvertes.fr/halshs-00004758 Submitted on 30 Sep 2005 HAL is a multi-disciplinary open access L’archive ouverte pluridisciplinaire HAL, est archive for the deposit and dissemination of sci- destinée au dépôt et à la diffusion de documents entific research documents, whether they are pub- scientifiques de niveau recherche, publiés ou non, lished or not. The documents may come from émanant des établissements d’enseignement et de teaching and research institutions in France or recherche français ou étrangers, des laboratoires abroad, or from public or private research centers. publics ou privés. Nominalization and its various uses in Thulung Rai1 Aimée Lahaussois LACITO-CNRS Villejuif, France The phenomenon of extensive nominalization in Sino-Tibetan languages is a pattern whose extent was first described by Matisoff for Lahu (Matisoff 1972), and has been described as typical for Sino-Tibetan nominalization (Bickel 1999), with the same morpheme found in languages throughout Tibeto-Burman for nominalization, relativization, and also sometimes to express a genitive relationship. For Lahu, this pattern is seen in the various uses of ve in the following examples (all from Matisoff 1972). Genitive 1. ŋà ve mí-ch I shoulder-bag ‘my shoulder-bag’ Relativization 2. và qhe chu ve Pîch-pā ô tê γâ pig as fat Shan that one person ‘That Shan over there who’s fat as a pig’ Nominalization 3.
    [Show full text]
  • Verb Nominalization of Manggarai Language: the Case of Central Manggarai Dialect in West Flores Indonesia
    International Journal of Language and Literature June 2016, Vol. 4, No. 1, pp. 122-136 ISSN: 2334-234X (Print), 2334-2358 (Online) Copyright © The Author(s). 2015. All Rights Reserved. Published by American Research Institute for Policy Development DOI: 10.15640/ijll.v4n1a13 URL: https://doi.org/10.15640/ijll.v4n1a13 Verb Nominalization of Manggarai Language: The Case of Central Manggarai Dialect in West Flores Indonesia Agustinus Semiun1 & Kosmas Jeladu2 Abstract This paper presents nominalization process but concentrating on nominalizing verbs of central Manggarai dialect of Manggarai language in West Flores island Indonesia. The aim is to explain how verbs of the dialect are nominalized. By applying closed interview, observation and documentary techniques of qualitative approach, this paper present very valuable findings. Firstly, the Central Manggarai Dialect, has its own way to nominalize verbs but not by changing lexical verbs into lexical nouns. Secondly as shown by the data, the Central Manggarai Dialect performs seven types of verb nominalization like those presented by Comrie and Thompson in Shopen (2007) that is action nominalization, agentive nominalization, instrumental nominalization, manner nominalization, locative nominalization, objective nominalization, and reason nominalization. As seen in the content pages of this paper, Central Manggarai Dialect uses third singular possessive enclitics -n and third plural possessive –d to denote action nominalization, the free word ata is used to denote agentive nominalization, a bound marker or prefix -ter and free word palang denoting place nominalization, the free words le or ali, or wajole, or wajoali denoting reason nominalization. Interestingly to denote objective nominalization the verb it- self, with no certain marker or free word, is used.
    [Show full text]
  • 1 on Agent Nominalizations and Why They Are Not Like Event
    On agent nominalizations and why they are not like event nominalizations1 Mark C. Baker and Nadya Vinokurova Rutgers University and Research Institute of Humanities -Yakutsk Abstract: This paper focuses on agent-denoting nominalizations in various languages (e.g. the finder of the wallet), contrasting them with the much better studied action/event- denoting nominalizations. In particular, we show that in Sakha, Mapudungun, and English, agent-denoting nominalizations have none of the verbal features that event- denoting nominalizations sometimes have: they cannot contain adverbs, voice markers, expressions of aspect or mood, or verbal negation. An apparent exception to this generalization is that Sakha allows accusative-case marked objects in agentive nominalizations. We show that in fact the structure of agentive nominalizations in Sakha is as purely nominal as in other languages, and the difference is attributable to the rule of accusative case assignment. We explain these restrictions by arguing that agentive nominalizers have a semantics very much like the one proposed by Kratzer (1996) for Voice heads. Given this, the natural order of semantic composition implies that agentive nominalizers must combine directly with VP, just as Voice heads must. As a preliminary to testing this idea typologically, we show how a true agentive nominalization can be distinguished from a headless subject relative clause, illustrating with data from Mapudungun. We then present the results of a 34-language survey, showing that indeed none of these languages allow clause-like syntax inside a true agentive nominalization. We conclude that a generative-style investigation into the details of particular languages can be a productive source of things to look for in typological surveys.
    [Show full text]
  • Nominalizations, Agentless Passives, and Social Actor Mystification
    NOMINALIZATIONS, AGENTLESS PASSIVES, AND SOCIAL ACTOR MYSTIFICATION: NEWSPAPER EDITORIALS ON THE GREEK FINANCIAL CRISIS by WILLIAM ALAN LINGLE A thesis submitted to the University of Birmingham for the degree of DOCTOR OF PHILOSOPHY Department of English Language and Applied Linguistics School of English, Drama and American & Canadian Studies College of Arts and Law University of Birmingham April 2017 University of Birmingham Research Archive e-theses repository This unpublished thesis/dissertation is copyright of the author and/or third parties. The intellectual property rights of the author or third parties in respect of this work are as defined by The Copyright Designs and Patents Act 1988 or as modified by any successor legislation. Any use made of information contained in this thesis/dissertation must be in accordance with that legislation and must be properly acknowledged. Further distribution or reproduction in any format is prohibited without the permission of the copyright holder. Abstract Nominalization and agentless passives have attracted sustained attention in critical linguistics and Critical Discourse Analysis (CDA), where, it is argued, they ‘mystify’, i.e., reduce reader comprehension of, the role of social actors in depictions of events, particularly in news media discourse. Yet the capacity of readers to generate inferences automatically from textual cues and background information has not been adequately reflected in CDA accounts of reader cognition. The question of whether particular instances of nominalization or agentless passives actually reduce reader comprehension of social actors’ agentive roles was put to an empirical test by asking volunteer readers to identify social actors deleted from newspaper editorials by the addition of nominalization and agentless passives.
    [Show full text]
  • Noun Generation for Nominalization in Academic Writing
    Noun Generation for Nominalization in Academic Writing Dariush Saberi, John Lee Department of Linguistics and Translation City University of Hong Kong [email protected], [email protected] Abstract Previous studies on automatic manipulation of clausal structure have mostly concentrated on Nominalization is a common technique in aca- syntactic simplification, typically by splitting a demic writing for producing abstract and for- mal text. Since it often involves paraphras- complex sentence into two or more simple sen- ing a clause with a verb or adjectival phrase tences (Siddharthan, 2002; Alu´ısio et al., 2008; into a noun phrase, an important task is to Narayan and Gardent, 2014). More recent re- generate the noun to replace the original verb search has also attempted semi-automatic nomi- or adjective. Given that a verb or adjective nalization (Lee et al., 2018), which aims to para- may have multiple nominalized forms with phrase a complex clause into a simplex clause by similar meaning, the system needs to be able transforming verb or adjectival phrases into noun to automatically select the most appropriate one. We propose an unsupervised algorithm phrases. that makes the selection with BERT, a state- Noun generation is a core task in the nom- of-the-art neural language model. Experimen- inalization pipeline (Table2). Resources such tal results show that it significantly outper- as NOMLEX (Meyers et al., 1998) and CAT- forms baselines based on word frequencies, VAR (Habash and Dorr, 2003) have greatly facili- word2vec and doc2vec. tated this task by providing lists of related nouns, 1 Introduction verbs and adjectives.
    [Show full text]
  • Handout 1 (Writing): Nouns, Verbs, and Adjectives
    Communication in the Mathematical Sciences, Spring 2020 Handout 1 (Writing): Nouns, Verbs, and Adjectives Definitions Noun A person, place, or thing. The kinds of words that refer to things, people, and other nameable or conceivable entities. Examples: lily, hoax, telephone, bargain, idea, proof, theorem, terror, George Washington, conciousness. Verb An action word. The kinds of words which are inflected for tense and which often refer to an action or a state. Examples: He kicked the football. I thought I saw a pussycat. I am strong. Adjective A descriptive word. Typically refers to a property or state. Often it is a word you can put “very” in front of (but not always; exceptions include “major”, “additional”, etc.) Examples: big, round, red, afraid, gratuitous, hesitant, occupational. Nominalization A noun derived from a verb or an adjective. Examples: nominalize (verb) → nominalization Verb → Nominalization: discover → discovery resist → resistance react → reaction Adjective → Nominalization: careless → carelessness different → difference proficient → proficiency Exercises Exercise 1: Underline the nouns. 1) I love watching my cat play with the pink yarn. 2) It is raining! Everyone, grab your umbrella and rain hat and watch out for the puddles! 2) Our lack of data prevented evaluation of UN actions in targeting funds to areas most in need of assistance. Communication in the Mathematical Sciences, Spring 2020 Exercise 2: Underline the verbs. 1a) Once upon a time, as a walk through the woods was taking place on the part of Little Red Riding Hood, the Wolf's jump out from behind a tree occurred, causing her fright. 1b) Once upon a time, Little Red Riding Hood was walking through the woods, when the Wolf jumped out from behind a tree and frightened her.
    [Show full text]
  • Particle Shift in Gerundive Nominalizations Bachelor ’SDiplomaThesis Supervisor:Mgr.JanChovanec,Ph.D. 2008
    Masaryk University Faculty of Arts Department of English and American Studies English Language and Literature Helena Jezdinská Nominalizations in English: Particle Shift in Gerundive Nominalizations Bachelor ’s Diploma Thesis Supervisor: Mgr. Jan Chovanec, Ph.D. 2008 Ideclare that Ihaveworkedonthisthesisindependently, usingonly the primary andsecondary sourceslistedinthebibliography. ........................................................... HelenaJezdinská Acknowledgement Aboveall,I wouldlike tothankprofessorPetrKarlíkforall hisadvice,including recommendationofthe sources,valuableandinterestingdiscussionsand moralsupport. Iwouldalsolike tothankdr.JanChovanecfor beingmysupervisorandhelpingmeto copewiththeinadequaciesofthethesis.Last,butnotleast,I thankdr.Jarmila Fictumová,HonzaandJanafortheirhelpingmetoworkwiththeBNC. Table of Contents LISTOF ABBREVIATIONS ...........................................................................................1 INTRODUCTION ............................................................................................................2 1.NOMINALIZATION INCONTEMPORARY ENGLISH GRAMMARS ..................4 1.1. ENGLISH GRAMMAR BOOKS ..........................................................................4 1.1.1.QUIRK ............................................................................................................5 1.1.1.1.NOMINALIZATION ...............................................................................5 1.1.1.2.–ing CLAUSES ........................................................................................7
    [Show full text]
  • Is Conversion a Syntactic Or a Lexical Process of Word Formation?
    Is conversion a syntactic or a lexical process of word formation? Alexandra Soares Rodrigues [email protected] Escola Superior de Educação de Bragança – Instituto Politécnico de Bragança CELGA – Universidade de Coimbra (Portugal) ABSTRACT. Conversion is sometimes described as a syntactic phenomenon by which a lexical item changes its lexical category according to the syntactic environment where it is inserted. This syntactic-ordered approach comes from theoretical fields that conceive the lexicon as the domain of irregularity, whilst regular patterns are treated in syntax (Chomsky 1995). However, Portuguese converted deverbal nouns (remendo ‘event of mending’, curte ‘event of having fun’, trinca ‘event of biting’) manifest a structural behaviour that permits us to situate their formation in the lexicon instead of in the syntax. According to the theoretical allusion we made above, this would characterise converted deverbal nouns as lacking a regular pattern. However, what we mean is that the lexicon is not the field of irregularity. Apart from the irregular material that must be stored in long term memory as to be used by speakers, such as inherited lexemes (e.g. rato ‘mouse’, cão ‘dog’, rir ‘to laugh’, verde ‘green’, etc.), the lexicon is the domain of word formation, which is constraint-based (Rodrigues 2008, 2009). This means the word formation part of the lexicon is constrained by regular patterns that are neither directional in principle, nor syntactic in nature. We follow Jackendoff (2002) conception on the lexicon, conceiving it an interface of syntax, phonology and semantics. Converted deverbal nouns formation seems to agree with this conception, since it depends on phonological, semantic and syntactical constraints (Rodrigues 2004, 2009).
    [Show full text]
  • Nominalizations in Naskapi: Production and Inflection
    Nominalizations in Naskapi: Production and Inflection BILL JANCEWICZ Summer Institute of Linguistics and Naskapi Development Corporation The language described in this paper has been referred to as "Western Naskapi". It is a palatalized ^-dialect spoken in the northeast of the Cree- Montagnais-Naskapi continuum. The data are from speakers resident at the Naskapi community of Kawawachikamach, near Schefferville, Quebec. PRODUCTION PROCESS Like other languages in the Cree-Montagnais-Naskapi continuum, one very productive means employed to form new words in Naskapi is the process referred to as nominalization. Essentially, this is accomplished by adding a kaa- prefix to a verb inflected for conjunct indicative neutral, third person singular. Although kaa- is homophonous with the prefix that marks a conjunct verb as past reference, Lynn Drapeau (1978:214) points out that in Montagnais this kaa- functions as a relative particle referring to an antecedent, meaning roughly 'the one who...' or 'the thing that...', forming a relative clause. As in Montagnais, it is from these relative conjunct verbs that Naskapi nominalizations arise, when they are lexicalized as nouns by the speech community. Once thus lexicalized, nominalizations function syntactically as nouns and are (for the most part) inflected as nouns:1 (1) a. chiskutimaachaa-w Al 's/he teaches' b. kaa-chiskutimaachaa-t NomA 'teacher' (2) a. nituutiihkwaa-w Al 's/he is hunting caribou' b. kaa-nituutiihkwaa-t NomA 'caribou hunter' 1 As in other Algonquian languages, the citation form for a verb (as listed in a lexicon or dictionary, for example) is the third person singular of the independent indicative neutral.
    [Show full text]
  • Noun Phrases and Nominalization
    Secret agents: Teaching complex noun phrases & nominalisation to developing academic writers Stephen Bolton English Language Centre City University of Hong Kong [email protected] 1 Why focus on noun phrases/nominalisation? 1. There’s no escape • “Nominalizations of verbs and adjectives are ubiquitous in academic and professional discourse in English.” (Hinkel, 2002) • Biber & Gray (2010): “Instead of dependent clauses, academic writing relies heavily on phrases to add information.” (p. 7) 2 Sample Abstracts [1] • The Asian economic crisis of 1997-1998 unleashed unprecedented policy and political changes in Asia. With many governments’ capacity to manage their economies discredited and the pernicious effects of the lack of protection system exposed, governments adopted transformative policy changes. • One of the great fundamental problems facing modern medicine is the seemingly inevitable emergence of resistance to drugs such as antibiotics by bacteria, or the resistance to chemotherapy by cancer cells. 3 Sample Abstracts [2] • The Asian economic crisis of 1997-1998 unleashed unprecedented policy and political changes in Asia. With many governments’ capacity to manage their economies discredited and the pernicious effects of the lack of protection system exposed, governments adopted transformative policy changes. • One of the great fundamental problems facing modern medicine is the seemingly inevitable emergence of resistance to drugs such as antibiotics by bacteria, or the resistance to chemotherapy by cancer cells. 4 Why focus
    [Show full text]