Social Service Review
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
Load more
Recommended publications
-
Blog: Language Deprivation and Deaf Mental Health: Introduction to a Webinar Neil Glickman, Ph.D., University of Massachusetts Medical School
Blog: Language Deprivation and Deaf Mental Health: Introduction to a Webinar Neil Glickman, Ph.D., University of Massachusetts Medical School In many respects, the processes of providing mental health dysfluencies range from mild and barely noticeable to profound care to native users of sign and providing mental health care and complex, but they are often clinically significant in mental to users of spoken languages are comparable. This is health contexts. especially the case when working with deaf people who are At the extreme end of the language deprivation continuum are a- native users of either spoken or sign languages. In those lingual deaf people—people with no or minimal formal language cases, a cultural perspective on Deaf people is often much skills. Hearing people have usually never met such people and more useful than a disability perspective in planning services. may find it hard to believe that human beings with normal Mental health care of culturally Deaf people has many parallels intelligence can be, essentially, language-less. Inside the Deaf with mental health care of other linguistic, social, or cultural Community, however, the problem of language deprivation is well- minorities (Glickman, 2013; Glickman & Gulati, 2003; Glickman known. Programs and specialists that serve D/deaf people usually & Harvey, 1996; Leigh, 2010). Cultural self-awareness, a know some a-lingual or semi-lingual deaf people. respectful, affirmative attitude, a body of specialized In the United States, we are most likely to find a-lingual deaf knowledge about the target community, specialized language, people among immigrants from third world countries where and communication and intervention skills are all essential, as they received minimal education, but you can also find them in they are when working with other minority populations rural, isolated American communities or other places where (Glickman, 1996; Sue, Arredondo, & McDavis, 1992). -
Age of Language Acquisition and Prevalence of Suicidal Behavior in a Deaf Population with Co-Occurring Substance Use Disorder
JADARA Volume 51 Number 3 Article 1 2017 Age of Language Acquisition and Prevalence of Suicidal Behavior in a Deaf Population with Co-occurring Substance Use Disorder Jared A. Embree Wright State University Nicole R. Kinzeler Wright State University Susan Fraker Wright State University Sandra Castle Wright State University Josephine F. Wilson Wright State University Follow this and additional works at: https://repository.wcsu.edu/jadara Part of the Social and Behavioral Sciences Commons Recommended Citation Embree, J. A., Kinzeler, N. R., Fraker, S., Castle, S., & Wilson, J. F. (2017). Age of Language Acquisition and Prevalence of Suicidal Behavior in a Deaf Population with Co-occurring Substance Use Disorder. JADARA, 51(3), 1-24. Retrieved from https://repository.wcsu.edu/jadara/vol51/iss3/1 Embree et al.: Age of Language Acquisition and Suicidal Behavior in a Deaf Population Introduction People who are deaf encounter multiple obstacles in gaining knowledge of the world around them, including communication barriers between a deaf person and their parents and teachers, public misconceptions and stigma about deafness, and a scarcity of accessible information during early development (Guthmann & Moore, 2007). These issues are sometimes compounded by delayed exposure to language and cultural misunderstandings in part due to language differences. If language and cultural acquisition are delayed through major developmental milestones of childhood, deaf individuals may have less access to the tools necessary to build social support and a positive social identity. Thus, a person who is first exposed to language at a later point in life will have to confront the emotional ramifications of their childhood isolation (Pettis, 2014; Schaller & Sacks, 1991), and this delay may be tied to mental illness (Flouri, 2005). -
From Languageless Interaction to Enlanguaged Interaction: a Philosophical Study of Ildefonso
Copyright is owned by the Author of the thesis. Permission is given for a copy to be downloaded by an individual for the purpose of research and private study only. The thesis may not be reproduced elsewhere without the permission of the Author. FROM LANGUAGELESS INTERACTION TO ENLANGUAGED INTERACTION: A PHILOSOPHICAL STUDY OF ILDEFONSO A thesis presented in partial fulfilment of the requirements fo r the degree of Doctor of Philosophy In Philosophy At Massey University, Palmerston North, New Zealand Anne Jennifer Mackenzie 1999 11 ABSTRACT This investigation centres on a man, Ildefonso, learning his first language (American Sign Language) at twenty-seven. The prime source of material is Susan Schaller's account of teaching Ildefonso, A Man Without Words. Certain claims about language and education from the Deaf community, and writings of the Deaf are a second important source of material. The deaf individual, not the theory of language as such, is the focus of the issue of languagelessness in these materials and in this dissertation . • Schaller's account throws doubt on some conventional notions of language, and an alternative view of language emerges. Her early lessons are based on conventional ideas: that language is fu ndamentally a symbol system; that language is a tool for transferring information; that vocabulary and grammar are of prime importance. Lessons based on these ideas fail with Ildefonso. Progress occurs when Schaller stops trying to show Ildefonso ASL signs and begins trying to confer with him. Her description of his progress suggests languaging is fu ndamentally a particular kind of interaction. I characterise this as reciprocal back and fo rth interaction, and trace its development in three dialogues between Schaller and Ildefonso. -
Effects of Early Language Deprivation: Mapping Between Brain and Behavioral Outcomes
Effects of Early Language Deprivation: Mapping between Brain and Behavioral Outcomes Qi Cheng, Eric Halgren, and Rachel Mayberry 1. Introduction One core issue regarding language development is the role of early language experience. The literature on child language development has found that the quality and quantity of early language input is associated with children’s language performance, but the exact nature of the relation between early language experience and language development is less clear. Because language is ubiquitous in the environment, almost all children are immersed into a linguistically rich environment from birth. This makes it difficult to directly test the effects of a lack of language experience on subsequent language development. Here we present a study of individuals born deaf who experienced sparse language throughout childhood that will help illuminate the question. Postnatal brain development follows genetically predetermined growth patterns, but is also strongly influenced by learning and environmental factors (Huttenlocher 2002). Nonetheless, little is known about how early linguistic experience may affect the establishment of the brain’s dynamic language network. Studies on brain development suggest that the language network matures relatively late (Sowell et al. 2004; Lebel et al. 2012; Pujol et al. 2006), and that its degree of maturation correlates with language performance (Pujol et al. 2006; Mills, Coffe-Corina, and Neville 1997). However, there are multiple possible explanations for the correlation between late establishment of the neural system and accompanying language development. One possibility is that this protracted development is solely due to late maturation alone, and, as a result, that this constrains language development in early years. -
Motives of Pinker's Criticism of Whorfian
Sławomir Wacewicz LANGUAGE AND THINKING: MOTIVES OF PINKER’S CRITICISM OF WHORFIAN LINGUISTIC RELATIVISM In The Language Instinct (1995 [1994], henceforth: TLI), a book that despite its popular character has become virtually a classic in discussions concerning the innateness of language, Steven Pinker attacks the broadly understood “Whorfian” standpoint according to which human thinking is influenced in fundamental ways by one’s native language. Due to the status of the author and popularity of the book, it is an influential voice in the ongoing debate on the mutual relation between language and “thought.” Rather than joining this debate, in the present text I would like to examine the construction and integrity of Pinker’s argumentation in TLI. I suggest that this author’s attack on broadly understood interdependence of language and thought is motivated by his general theoretical commitments, rather than by independent evidence. 1. Introduction and the reconstruction of Pinker’s initial position The best way of reconstructing Steven Pinker’s initial position is by situating his views in the general spectrum of approaches present in contemporary Cognitive Science. Pinker’s principal commitments appear to be evolutionary, which is visible in his popular reception, where he is recognised as an evolutionary psychologist much more readily than as a linguist or psycholinguist. Such a theoretical inclination often goes together with two general views: nativism and modularism; views that are separable, but related and mutually supporting. Steven Pinker is an advocate for both nativism and modularism with respect to language, as well as to cognition in general. In crudest terms, nativism is a view according to which human knowledge (or, more generally, the content of the human mind) is in some vital part innate. -
Language and Mind Prof
LING 1010 Language and Mind Prof. Jon Sprouse 03.15.21: The critical period for language acquisition What is the timeline of typical language acquisition? Now that we’ve reviewed examples of acquisition in phonology, morphology, and syntax, we can put it all together to look at the timeline of acquisition: Birth 6 mo 12 mo 2 yr 3 yr 4 yr 5 yr Age 6 babbling begins at 6mo, becomes variable and language-specific by 12 mo. First words are produced between 10-15 mo. For many children, word learning accelerates dramatically around 18 mo. This is called the vocabulary explosion. Complex morphology appears on words. Two word utterances Function words and longer utterances More complex syntax (transformations) Stages, not ages People really like to talk about milestones for babies (child development). There are entire websites dedicated to this. Milestones serve an important role in helping parents to identify potential problems early (if the baby doesn’t reach a milestone, it could be evidence of a problem). The problem with milestones is that people tend to link them to ages. This is the wrong way to do it. There is quite a bit of variation in the ages that children hit milestones, especially for language. Instead, for language we should look at the stages that children go through. What we find is that all children go through the same stages in the same order. All children move from babbling, to first words, to two-word utterances, to longer utterances, in that order. But they may do it at slightly different speeds. -
Language Deprivation Syndrome: a Possible Neurodevelopmental Disorder with Sociocultural Origins
Soc Psychiatry Psychiatr Epidemiol DOI 10.1007/s00127-017-1351-7 ORIGINAL PAPER Language deprivation syndrome: a possible neurodevelopmental disorder with sociocultural origins Wyatte C. Hall1,3 · Leonard L. Levin2 · Melissa L. Anderson3 Received: 7 September 2016 / Accepted: 22 January 2017 © Springer-Verlag Berlin Heidelberg 2017 Abstract Language development experiences have an epidemiologi- Purpose There is a need to better understand the epide- cal relationship with psychiatric outcomes in deaf people. miological relationship between language development and This requires more empirical attention and has implications psychiatric symptomatology. Language development can for other populations with behavioral health disparities as be particularly impacted by social factors—as seen in the well. developmental choices made for deaf children, which can create language deprivation. A possible mental health syn- Keywords Behavioral health · Language deprivation · drome may be present in deaf patients with severe language Sign language · Hearing loss · Social psychiatry deprivation. Methods Electronic databases were searched to identify publications focusing on language development and mental Introduction health in the deaf population. Screening of relevant publi- cations narrowed the search results to 35 publications. Psychiatric health is often epidemiologically affected by Results Although there is very limited empirical evi- social factors, such as poverty, social distress, and preju- dence, there appears to be suggestions of a mental health dice [1–5]. In this review, we argue that language develop- syndrome by clinicians working with deaf patients. Possi- ment, or the disruption of language development, is another ble features include language dysfluency, fund of knowl- social factor that contributes to the epidemiology of mental edge deficits, and disruptions in thinking, mood, and/or illness—as observed in the deaf population. -
An Illusion of Inclusion That Perpetuates Language Deprivation
Running Head: THE ILLUSION OF INCLUSION 1 American Sign Language Interpreters in Public Schools: An Illusion of Inclusion that Perpetuates Language Deprivation Naomi K. Caselli1, Wyatte C. Hall2, Jonathan Henner3 Boston University1 University of Rochester Medical Center2 University of North Carolina Greensboro3 Author Note Research reported in this publication was supported by the National Institute On Deafness And Other Communication Disorders of the National Institutes of Health under Award Number R21DC016104. Wyatte Hall was supported by the University of Rochester CTSA award number 3 UL1 TR002001-03S2 from the National Center for Advancing Translational Sciences of the National Institutes of Health. The content is solely the responsibility of the authors and does not necessarily represent the official views of the National Institutes of Health. This work is also supported by National Science Foundation grants BCS 1625793 and 1918252 to Naomi Caselli. Correspondence concerning this article should be addressed to Naomi K. Caselli, Wheelock College of Education and Human Development, Boston University, 2 Silber Way, 3rd Floor, Boston, MA, 02215 ([email protected]). Running Head: THE ILLUSION OF INCLUSION 2 Abstract Purpose: Many deaf children have limited access to language, spoken or signed, during early childhood – which has damaging effects on many aspects of development. There has been a recent shift to consider deafness and language deprivation as separate but related conditions. As such, educational plans should differentiate between services related to deafness and services related to language deprivation. Description: Many deaf children attend mainstream public schools, and the primary service offered to students who use American Sign Language (ASL) is generally a sign language interpreter. -
Leap of Faith, the Story of a Contemporary Miracle
Leap of Faith, the Story of a Contemporary Miracle A Conversation with Susan Schaller Are there adults living today who have not learned any language, who cannot even conceive of language? They do exist, although, according to Susan Schaller, there's almost nothing written about them. Perhaps that's because, according to the prevailing views of experts, adults who have not acquired language will never be able to do so. This was not an area to which I'd given a single thought until my unexpected meeting with a stranger one day in a Berkeley restaurant. It's a nice coincidence that the unlikely meeting took place in an unlikely setting, on the one afternoon a week that "A Taste of the Himalayas" is taken over by a group of grassroots philanthropists who serve a four-course meal for the price of $0.00. It's Karma Kitchen, where all customers are treated to a pay-it-forward dining experience. The atmosphere that inevitably seems to develop releases some of the reticence of strangers meeting strangers. People are invited to share tables with people they haven't met before. Besides the radical practice of not charging for the meal, it's another piece of the gentle iconoclasm of the place. Without fail, each week lots of stories are generated about unexpected connections, and my meeting Susan Schaller is one of them. Susan was seated directly across the table from me. I'm less anxious than I used to be about meeting strangers, but it's still an awkward process. We were both feeling our way along. -
Language As a Source of Inner Grounding and Flexibility in Abstract Concepts
Words have a weight: language as a source of inner grounding and flexibility in abstract concepts Guy Dove University of Louisville ORCID: 0000-0003-0470-7006 Corresponding author: [email protected] Laura Barca Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies of the Italian National Research Council (ISTC-CNR) ORCID: 0000-0002-8553-7717 Luca Tummolini Institute of Cognitive Sciences and Technologies of the Italian National Research Council (ISTC-CNR) ORCID: 0000-0001-7791-0457 Anna M. Borghi Sapienza University of Rome ORCID: 0000-0001-9455-8408 Words have a weight Words have a weight: language as a source of inner grounding and flexibility in abstract concepts Abstract: The role played by language in our cognitive lives is a topic at the centre of contemporary debates in cognitive (neuro)science. In this paper we illustrate and compare two theories that offer embodied explanations of this role: the WAT (Words As social Tools) and the LENS (Language is an Embodied Neuroenhancement and Scaffold) theories. WAT and LENS differ from other current proposals because they connect the impact of the neurologically realized language system on our cognition to the ways in which language shapes our interaction with the physical and social environment. Examining these theories together, their tenets and supporting evidence, sharpens our understanding of each, but also contributes to a better understanding of the contribution that language might make to the acquisition, representation and use of abstract concepts. Here we focus on how language provides a source of inner grounding, especially metacognition and inner speech, and supports the flexibility of our thought. -
Deaf Children As' English Learners': the Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf
education sciences Review Deaf Children as ‘English Learners’: The Psycholinguistic Turn in Deaf Education Amanda Howerton-Fox 1,* and Jodi L. Falk 2 1 Education Department, Iona College, New Rochelle, NY 10801, USA 2 St. Joseph’s School for the Deaf, Bronx, NY 10465, USA; [email protected] * Correspondence: [email protected]; Tel.: +1-914-633-2680 Received: 9 May 2019; Accepted: 11 June 2019; Published: 14 June 2019 Abstract: The purpose of this literature review is to present the arguments in support of conceptualizing deaf children as ‘English Learners’, to explore the educational implications of such conceptualizations, and to suggest directions for future inquiry. Three ways of interpreting the label ‘English Learner’ in relationship to deaf children are explored: (1) as applied to deaf children whose native language is American Sign Language; (2) as applied to deaf children whose parents speak a language other than English; and (3) as applied to deaf children who have limited access to the spoken English used by their parents. Recent research from the fields of linguistics and neuroscience on the effects of language deprivation is presented and conceptualized within a framework that we refer to as the psycholinguistic turn in deaf education. The implications for developing the literacy skills of signing deaf children are explored, particularly around the theoretical construct of a ‘bridge’ between sign language proficiency and print-based literacy. Finally, promising directions for future inquiry are presented. Keywords: deaf education; critical period for language; sign bilingualism; deaf multilingual learner (DML); english learner (EL); age of acquisition; literacy; cognition; ableism 1. Introduction The purpose of this literature review is to present the arguments in support of conceptualizing deaf children as ‘English Learners’, to explore the educational implications of such conceptualizations, and to suggest directions for future inquiry. -
Lessons from the LEAD-K Campaign for Language Equality for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children
Lessons from the LEAD-K Campaign for Language Equality for Deaf and Hard of Hearing Children Christina Payne-Tsoupros* ABSTRACT This Article asserts that early intervention under the Individuals with Disabilities Education Improvement Act of 2004 (IDEIA) should be amended to recognize the needs of the young child with the disability as primary over the needs of the child’s family. This Article contends that certain requirements of the IDEIA cause early intervention professionals to view and treat the child’s family, rather than the child herself, as the ultimate recipient of support. In many situations, the needs of the family and the needs of the child may wholly align, but that is an assumption that bears questioning. This Article analyzes groundbreaking legislation efforts in the field of deaf education, the Language Equality & Acquisition for Deaf Kids (LEAD-K) campaign, to illustrate a framework that maintains the focus on the primacy of the child’s needs, with support to the family in service of those needs. The LEAD-K campaign has developed model legislation for adoption by states. Since 2016, twelve states have adopted a form of the LEAD-K model bill. There are approximately fifteen more states with LEAD-K teams in various stages of development. This Article highlights LEAD-K for its potential to transform early intervention for deaf and hard of hearing children and uses the LEAD-K model bill to illustrate a flaw in the IDEIA. The IDEIA subsumes the needs of the child within the consideration of the needs of the family, when the child and family should instead be considered as separate (but related) stakeholders.