PROPER and DARK HEROES AS DADS and CADS Alternative Mating Strategies in British Romantic Literature

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

PROPER and DARK HEROES AS DADS and CADS Alternative Mating Strategies in British Romantic Literature 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 305 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 PROPER AND DARK HEROES AS DADS AND CADS Alternative Mating Strategies in British Romantic Literature Daniel J. Kruger Maryanne Fisher University of Michigan York University Ian Jobling Buffalo, New York Empirical tests described in this article support hypotheses derived from evolutionary theory on the perceptions of literary characters. The proper and dark heroes in British Romantic literature of the late eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries respectively represent long-term and short- term mating strategies. Recent studies indicate that for long-term rela- tionships, women seek partners with the ability and willingness to sustain paternal investment in extended relationships. For short-term re- lationships, women choose partners whose features indicate high genetic quality. In hypothetical scenarios, females preferred proper heroes for long-term relationships. The shorter the relationship under consideration, the more likely women were to choose dark heroes as partners. KEY WORDS: Darwinian literary studies; Life history; Mating strategy; Sexual selection It is difficult to make progress in literary studies because, unlike scientists, literary scholars do not base their findings on theories that are subject to empirical tests. The imaginations of literary researchers are allowed to run Received February 4, 2003; accepted May 22, 2003; revised version received June 15, 2003. Address all correspondence to Daniel J. Kruger, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, P.O. Box 1248, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106-1248. E-mail address: kruger@ umich.edu Copyright 2003 by Walter de Gruyter, Inc., New York Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 305–317. 1045-6767/03/$1.00+.10 305 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 306 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 306 Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, 2003 wild, and theories like deconstruction and Lacanian psychoanalysis are se- lected not because of their effectiveness in generating empirically valid hypotheses, but because people just happen to like them. Also, many hu- manists have an anti-scientific mythology that perpetuates this situation. This weakness in the field has drawn extensive criticism (e.g., Carroll 1995; Pinker 1997:521–565, 2002:400–420). This paper attempts to further the project of Darwinian literary studies, which seeks to promote consilience, or integration, between literary stud- ies and the sciences (Carroll 1995; Wilson 1998). We show here how liter- ary studies can be grounded in both the ideas and methods of the sciences by providing an empirical test of a literary interpretation that is derived from evolutionary theory on human sexuality. Our project uses psycho- logical research techniques to generate modern reactions to character types in late eighteenth and early nineteenth century British Romantic lit- erature. We hope to encourage literary researchers to develop their theo- ries of human cognition and behavior out of the best contemporary research on these subjects and to form partnerships with scientists to test their interpretations of literary texts empirically. We also hope to demon- strate to social and biological scientists that literature affords rich possibil- ities for testing and developing psychological theories. Literary historians have drawn a distinction between two basic heroic types in the British Romantic period: the proper hero and the dark hero (Railo 1927; Thorslev 1962; Welsh 1992). The proper hero is law-abiding, compassionate, kind, and monogamous. The dark hero is dominant, re- bellious, frequently a criminal, and often promiscuous. There is a close cor- respondence between the personality traits of proper and dark heroes and those of two personality types described by evolutionary scientists: cads and dads (Jobling 2002a, 2002b, 2002c). Cads are designed to maximize their reproductive success by having many short-term relationships with- out parental investment. Dads, on the other hand, maximize their repro- ductive success through long-term, monogamous relationships with women and parenting (Dawkins 1976). Women, on their part, possess psy- chological adaptations that cause them to be attracted to cads for short- term relationships and dads for long-term ones. Naturally occurring variances among cads and dads are the reason for the emergence of these Romantic character types (Jobling 2002a, 2002b, 2002c). CAD AND DAD MATING STRATEGIES Recent research on human sexuality suggests that humans evolved to pur- sue both short-term and long-term, or cad and dad, mating strategies. Con- siderable evidence suggests that, unlike most mammals, humans are 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 307 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 Heroic Dads and Cads 307 designed for long-term sexual relationships with substantial male parental investment in children. Human infants require considerable parental care, and children reared in father-absent households suffer much higher mor- tality rates than those reared in father-present households, especially in preindustrial societies (Hill and Hurtado 1996:434–439; Geary 1998:110– 115). The sexual psychology of women also indicates that human sexuality has been shaped by long-term sexual relationships. A number of studies both in the United States and cross-culturally have shown that women reg- ularly report being attracted to men who are socially respected, financially well-off, ambitious, industrious, dependable, emotionally stable, and ro- mantic, all qualities that indicate the ability and willingness to sustain long- term, parentally investing relationships with women (Buss 1994:32–34). However, aspects of both men’s and women’s sexuality also show that humans did not evolve exclusively to pursue long-term mating. Men con- sistently show a marked desire for sexual variety (Buss 1994:76–83). The fitness advantage of short-term sexual relationships to men is obvious: the number of potential offspring men can have increases linearly with the number of sexual partners. This advantage is the reason why short-term mating without parental investment is the strategy that most male mam- mals pursue (Trivers 1985:207–209). A successful womanizer in the envi- ronment in which humans evolved would have been able to sire a large number of children, many of whom would likely have survived despite higher mortality risks, especially in a resource-rich environment. The cad mating strategy has been shown to be successful in some circumstances, such as when there is a small effect of male parental investment on female reproductive success due to the local ecology (Barash and Lipton 2001; Draper and Harpending 1982, 1988; Gangestad and Simpson 2000; Lan- caster and Kaplan 1992). Why would cads have been attractive to ancestral women who would have had to provide on their own the immense parental investment nec- essary to bear and raise a child? The most common answer to this question is the “sexy son hypothesis.” When a woman mates with a cad, the genes that made the father successful as a cad will be passed on to their son. This sexy son would increase his mother’s fitness by giving her numerous grandchildren. Many theorists have hypothesized that women may have evolved in some circumstances to prefer cues in men that show high sex- ual success, or “good genes,” to cues of potential paternal investment (Barash and Lipton 2001; Cashdan 1996; Gangestad and Simpson 2000). It is also possible that women with a parentally investing mate would have extra-pair copulations with a cad in order to benefit from his genetic contribution. Draper, Harpending, and Belsky have argued that men evolved to specialize in one of the two mating strategies and have found that, 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 308 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 308 Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, 2003 cross-culturally, cads and dads show distinct clusters of personality traits. They believe that cads and dads are different human morphs, just as work- ers and queens are different morphs of ants, and that whether a man be- comes one or the other depends on an environmental trigger: the presence or absence of the father in the household where the son grows up. The sons of father-absent households will become cads, and those of father-present households will become dads (Draper and Harpending 1982, 1988; Draper and Belsky 1990). One of the most reliable findings in the biological study of sexual be- havior is that male sexual competitiveness is proportional to the degree of polygyny in a species (Alexander et al. 1979). The more females one male impregnates, the fewer the opportunities for other males. Consequently, the higher the degree of polygyny, the more intense the selection pressure on males to develop effective means of sexual competition. Consequently, Draper, Harpending, and Belsky reason that, because cads are more polyg- ynous than dads, they ought to have developed to be more highly sexually competitive. Their review of the literature on the subject supports this view. Cross-culturally, men from father-absent households favor a sexu- ally promiscuous mating strategy and are misogynistic and reluctant to engage in parental investment; they are also violent, aggressive, rebel- lious, high in risk-taking, and at high risk of incarceration. Boys from father-present households are more likely than boys from father-absent households to delay sexual experience, have positive attitudes towards and develop stable pair-bonds with women, have good relations with male peers, and accept authority. Draper, Harpending, and Belsky’s account of cad and dad mating strate- gies meshes well with other research. If women had evolved exclusively to pursue long-term mateships, their sexual attraction would be uniform in all contexts: they would always look for men who showed the ability and willingness to parentally invest in children. However, some studies show that women have different criteria for choosing long- and short-term mates, and the traits that they look for in short-term mates correspond quite well to the cad traits that Draper and Harpending have described.
Recommended publications
  • How Evolution Can Help Us Understand Child Development and Behaviour Annie Swanepoel, Daniela F
    BJPsych Advances (2016), vol. 22, 36–43 doi: 10.1192/apt.bp.114.014043 ARTICLE How evolution can help us understand child development and behaviour Annie Swanepoel, Daniela F. Sieff, Graham Music, John Launer, Michael Reiss & Bernadette Wren Annie Swanepoel is a consultant us how, in response to environmental influences, SUMMARY child and adolescent psychiatrist genes are switched on or off. It is also showing in the Hertfordshire Partnership The traditional disease model, still dominant in us that these effects can be transmitted down University Foundation Trust. She psychiatry, is less than ideal for making sense of is interested in the integration of generations. Yet surprisingly few people have psychological issues such as the effects of early body and mind, nature and nurture, childhood experiences on development. We argue asked wider questions such as ‘Why does psychoneuroimmunology, as well as this sensitivity exist?’ and ‘Why does this evolutionary psychology. Daniela F. that a model based on evolutionary thinking can Sieff has a doctorate in biological deepen understanding and aid clinical practice sensitivity sometimes lead to apparently negative anthropology. She is interested by showing how behaviours, bodily responses developmental patterns?’. However, these in the ways that interdisciplinary and psychological beliefs tend to develop for questions can be fruitfully addressed if we turn to perspectives can foster the ‘adaptive’ reasons, even when these ways of being understanding of emotional trauma. an evolutionary perspective. Graham Music is a consultant might on first appearance seem pathological. Such Organisms that are better fitted to their child and adolescent psychotherapist understanding has implications for treatment. It environment (which means better adapted to their at the Tavistock Clinic and an adult also challenges the genetic determinist model, environment) have a greater chance of surviving psychotherapist in private practice.
    [Show full text]
  • King's Research Portal
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by King's Research Portal King’s Research Portal Document Version Peer reviewed version Link to publication record in King's Research Portal Citation for published version (APA): Wertz, J., Belsky, J., Moffitt, T. E., Belsky, D. W., Harrington, H., Avinun, R., ... Caspi, A. (Accepted/In press). Genetics of nurture: A test of the hypothesis that parents’ genetics predict their observed caregiving . Developmental Psychology. Citing this paper Please note that where the full-text provided on King's Research Portal is the Author Accepted Manuscript or Post-Print version this may differ from the final Published version. If citing, it is advised that you check and use the publisher's definitive version for pagination, volume/issue, and date of publication details. And where the final published version is provided on the Research Portal, if citing you are again advised to check the publisher's website for any subsequent corrections. General rights Copyright and moral rights for the publications made accessible in the Research Portal are retained by the authors and/or other copyright owners and it is a condition of accessing publications that users recognize and abide by the legal requirements associated with these rights. •Users may download and print one copy of any publication from the Research Portal for the purpose of private study or research. •You may not further distribute the material or use it for any profit-making activity or commercial gain •You may freely distribute the URL identifying the publication in the Research Portal Take down policy If you believe that this document breaches copyright please contact [email protected] providing details, and we will remove access to the work immediately and investigate your claim.
    [Show full text]
  • Same-Sex Marriage, Lesbigay Parenting, and the Psychology of Disgust
    Working Paper Series Villanova University Charles Widger School of Law Year 2007 It's Really About Sex: Same-Sex Marriage, Lesbigay Parenting, and the Psychology of Disgust Richard E. Redding Villanova University School of Law, [email protected] This paper is posted at Villanova University Charles Widger School of Law Digital Repository. http://digitalcommons.law.villanova.edu/wps/art92 113495-TEXT .NATIVE .1205175861. DOC 3/10/2008 12:05:16 PM IT’S REALLY ABOUT SEX : SAME -SEX MARRIAGE , LESBIGAY PARENTING , AND THE PSYCHOLOGY OF DISGUST RICHARD E. REDDING * TABLE OF CONTENTS INTRODUCTION .............................................................................................................. 102 I. THE STATE OF SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH ON LESBIGAY PARENTING .................... 108 A. Early Research Returns: “No Differences” Between Children Raised by Lesbigay Versus Heterosexual Parents...................... 109 B. Critics Take a Fresh Look at the Research: Fatally Flawed or Flawed But Informative?................................................................ 110 C. The Importance of “Getting It Right” ................................................... 116 D. Three Recent Studies............................................................................... 119 II. DOES SOCIAL SCIENCE RESEARCH ON LESBIGAY PARENTING PROVIDE A BASIS FOR PROHIBITING LESBIGAY MARRIAGE OR ADOPTION ? .................... 120 A. Are Children Raised by Lesbigay Parents More Likely to Be Homosexual? ..................................................................................
    [Show full text]
  • 1 December 19, 2019 the Psychology of Online Political Hostility
    The Psychology of Online Political Hostility: A Comprehensive, Cross-National Test of the Mismatch Hypothesis Alexander Bor* & Michael Bang Petersen Department of Political Science Aarhus University August 30, 2021 Please cite the final version of this paper published in the American Political Science Review at https://doi.org/10.1017/S0003055421000885. Abstract Why are online discussions about politics more hostile than offline discussions? A popular answer argues that human psychology is tailored for face-to-face interaction and people’s behavior therefore changes for the worse in impersonal online discussions. We provide a theoretical formalization and empirical test of this explanation: the mismatch hypothesis. We argue that mismatches between human psychology and novel features of online environments could (a) change people’s behavior, (b) create adverse selection effects and (c) bias people’s perceptions. Across eight studies, leveraging cross-national surveys and behavioral experiments (total N=8,434), we test the mismatch hypothesis but only find evidence for limited selection effects. Instead, hostile political discussions are the result of status-driven individuals who are drawn to politics and are equally hostile both online and offline. Finally, we offer initial evidence that online discussions feel more hostile, in part, because the behavior of such individuals is more visible than offline. Acknowledgements This research has benefitted from discussions with Vin Arceneaux, Matt Levendusky, Mark Van Vugt, John Tooby, and members of the Research on Online Political Hostility (ROHP) group, among many others. We are grateful for constructive comments to workshop attendees at the Political Behavior Section of Aarhus University, at the NYU-SMAPP Lab, at the NYU Social Justice Lab, at the Hertie School, and to conference audiences at APSA 2019, HBES 2019, and ROPH 2020.
    [Show full text]
  • Download the Language Instinct 1St Edition Free Ebook
    THE LANGUAGE INSTINCT 1ST EDITION DOWNLOAD FREE BOOK Steven Pinker | --- | --- | --- | 9780688121419 | --- | --- The language instinct Simon Baron-Cohen Justin L. Download for print-disabled. Pinker's assumptions about the innateness of language have been challenged by some; English linguist Geoffrey Sampson has contested some of the claims made in the book about this. Categories : non-fiction books Cognitive science literature English-language books Evolution of language Linguistics books Works by Steven Pinker. You're rating the book as a worknot the seller or the specific copy you purchased! The book has been awarded with William James Book Awardand many others. Includes bibliographical references p. Paperback in English - 1 edition. Loved each and every part of this book. I will definitely recommend this book to non fiction, science lovers. Check nearby libraries Library. He deals sympathetically with Noam Chomsky 's claim that all human language The Language Instinct 1st edition evidence of a universal grammarbut dissents from Chomsky's skepticism that evolutionary theory can explain the human language instinct. Add a review Your Rating: Your Comment:. Fine less some stray publisher's glue on the front and rear gutters in fine dustwrapper with a tiny tear on the front flap fold. We routinely issue extensively illustrated color catalogs, available by subscription. First edition. The main characters of this non fiction, science story are. The The Language Instinct 1st edition Object Pagination p. This edition published in by HarperPerennial in New York. Adaptation Altruism Coevolution Cultural group selection Kin selection Sexual selection Evolutionarily stable strategy Social selection. If your book order is heavy or oversized, we may contact you to let you know extra shipping is required.
    [Show full text]
  • Evolutionary Theory and Reproductive Responses to Father Absence: Implications of Kin Selection and the Reproductive Returns to Mating and Parenting Effort
    In Handbook of Father Involvement: Multidisciplinary Perspectives, eds. Catherine S. Tamis -LeMonda and Natasha Cabrera. Lawrence Earlbaum Associates. To be published 2001 EVOLUTIONARY THEORY AND REPRODUCTIVE RESPONSES TO FATHER ABSENCE: IMPLICATIONS OF KIN SELECTION AND THE REPRODUCTIVE RETURNS TO MATING AND PARENTING EFFORT David Waynforth _________________________________________ Visiting Professor 2000-2001 International Research Center for Japanese Studies 3-2 Oeyama-cho, Goryo Nishikyo-ku Kyoto 610-1192 Japan E-mail: [email protected] Office Tel. (075)335-2119 Home Tel. (075)335-2540 ext 6849 & Department of Anthropology University of New Mexico Albuquerque, New Mexico 87131-1086 U.S.A. E-mail: [email protected] Evolutionary theory and father absence David Waynforth INTRODUCTION Evolutionary theorists are in the business of seeking ultimate explanations for behavior. Ultimate explanations concern why a behavior or trait would have evolved by Darwinian natural or sexual selection and would therefore be adaptive in particular environments. This is in contrast to what is termed proximate explanation, which is usually about more immediate types of motivation, an example being understanding the hormonal underpinnings of a behavior. The topic of father absence due to divorce has not escaped the attention of evolutionary theorists seeking ultimate explanation for its patterns and effects, and the first part of this chapter focuses on a particularly influential ultimate explanation for the pattern of events that appear to be typically associated with father absent household structure. After tracing the history of this evolutionary approach to father absence and showing some empirical evidence for and against it, I move on to how father absence can fit into evolutionary ecological approaches to mating strategies more generally, with a view to improving our understanding of its effects.
    [Show full text]
  • Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter, 2003. INSTITUTION Society for Research in Child Development
    DOCUMENT RESUME ED 480 656 PS 031 539 AUTHOR Reid, Pamela Trotman, Ed.; Ehart, Bridget, Ed. TITLE Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter, 2003. INSTITUTION Society for Research in Child Development. PUB DATE 2003-00-00 NOTE 62p.; For the 2002 issues, see PS 031 538. AVAILABLE FROM Society for Research in Child Development, University of Michigan, 3131 South State Street, Suite 302, Ann Arbor, MI 48108-1623. Tel: 734-998-6574; Fax: 734-998-6569; e-mail: [email protected]; Web site: http://www.srcd.org. PUB TYPE Collected Works Serials (022) JOURNAL CIT Society for Research in Child Development Newsletter; v46 nl- 4 2003 EDRS PRICE EDRS Price MF01/PC03 Plus Postage. DESCRIPTORS *Child Development; Coping; *Developmental Psychology; Emotional Adjustment; Financial Support; Information Dissemination; Newsletters; **Organizations (Groups); Program Descriptions; Psychological Studies; Public Policy; School Desegregation; Terrorism IDENTIFIERS Project Head Start; *Society for Research in Child Development ABSTRACT This document consists of the four 2003 issues of a newsletter disseminating information on the Society for Research in Child Development (SRCD) and providing a forum for important news, research, and information concerning advancement in child growth and development research. Each issue of the newsletter includes announcements and notices of conferences, workshops, position openings, fellowship, and member obituaries. The January issue summarizes meetings of the Consortium for Social Science Associations and the Human Development
    [Show full text]
  • A Mathematical Model of the Evolution of Individual Differences in Developmental Plasticity Arising Through Parental Bet-Hedging
    UC Davis UC Davis Previously Published Works Title A mathematical model of the evolution of individual differences in developmental plasticity arising through parental bet-hedging. Permalink https://escholarship.org/uc/item/4bh108gv Journal Developmental science, 19(2) ISSN 1363-755X Authors Frankenhuis, Willem E Panchanathan, Karthik Belsky, Jay Publication Date 2016-03-01 DOI 10.1111/desc.12309 Peer reviewed eScholarship.org Powered by the California Digital Library University of California Developmental Science 19:2 (2016), pp 251–274 DOI: 10.1111/desc.12309 PAPER A mathematical model of the evolution of individual differences in developmental plasticity arising through parental bet-hedging Willem E. Frankenhuis,1 Karthik Panchanathan2 and Jay Belsky3 1. Behavioural Science Institute, Radboud University Nijmegen, The Netherlands 2. Department of Anthropology, University of Missouri, USA 3. Human Ecology, University of California, Davis, USA Abstract Children vary in the extent to which their development is shaped by particular experiences (e.g. maltreatment, social support). This variation raises a question: Is there no single level of plasticity that maximizes biological fitness? One influential hypothesis states that when different levels of plasticity are optimal in different environmental states and the environment fluctuates unpredictably, natural selection may favor parents producing offspring with varyinglevels of plasticity. The current article presents a mathematical model assessing the logic of this hypothesis – specifically, it examines what conditions are required for natural selection to favor parents to bet-hedge by varying their offspring’s plasticity. Consistent with existing theory from biology, results show that between- individual variation in plasticity cannot evolve when the environment only varies across space.
    [Show full text]
  • Phenotypic Plasticity and Human Development
    Development and Psychopathology 25 (2013), 1243–1261 # Cambridge University Press 2013 doi:10.1017/S095457941300059X Beyond risk, resilience, and dysregulation: Phenotypic plasticity and human development a,b,c d JAY BELSKY AND MICHAEL PLUESS aUniversity of California, Davis; bKing Abdulaziz University; cBirkbeck University of London; and dQueen Mary University of London Abstract We provide a theoretical and empirical basis for the claim that individual differences exist in developmental plasticity and that phenotypic plasticity should be a subject of study in its own right. To advance this argument, we begin by highlighting challenges that evolutionary thinking poses for a science of development and psychopathology, including for the diathesis–stress framework that has (fruitfully) guided so much empirical inquiry on developmental risk, resilience, and dysregulation. With this foundation laid, we raise a series of issues that the differential-susceptibility hypothesis calls attention to, while highlighting findings that have emerged over just the past several years and are pertinent to some of the questions posed. Even though it is clear that this new perspective on PersonÂEnvironment interaction is stimulating research and influencing how hypotheses are framed and data interpreted, a great many topics remain that need empirical attention. Our intention is to encourage students of development and psychopathology to treat phenotypic plasticity as an individual-difference construct while exploring unknowns in the differential-susceptibility equation. Empirical research on risk and resilience first emerged in the more than in the study of GeneÂEnvironment (GÂE) inter- 1970s (e.g., Garmezy, 1974; Rutter, 1979; Werner & Smith, action (e.g., Caspi et al., 2002). 1977) and has come to exert a major influence on the study of One of the challenging issues that arises with respect to di- development and psychopathology (for reviews, see Luthar, athesis–stress motivated GÂE research has to do with under- Cicchetti, & Becker, 2000; Masten, 2007; Rutter, 2012).
    [Show full text]
  • Social Science and Parenting Plans for Young Children: a Consensus Report
    Psychology, Public Policy, and Law © 2014 American Psychological Association 2014, Vol. 20, No. 1, 46–67 1076-8971/14/$12.00 DOI: 10.1037/law0000005 Social Science and Parenting Plans for Young Children: A Consensus Report Richard A. Warshak University of Texas Southwestern Medical Center, with the endorsement of the researchers and practitioners listed in the Appendix Two central issues addressed in this article are the extent to which young children’s time should be spent predominantly in the care of the same parent or divided more evenly between both parents, and whether children under the age of 4 should sleep in the same home every night or spend overnights in both parents’ homes. A broad consensus of accomplished researchers and practitioners agree that, in normal circumstances, the evidence supports shared residential arrangements for children under 4 years of age whose parents live apart from each other. Because of the well-documented vulnerability of father–child relationships among never-married and divorced parents, the studies that identify overnights as a protective factor associated with increased father commitment to child rearing and reduced incidence of father drop-out, and the absence of studies that demonstrate any net risk of overnights, policymakers and decision makers should recognize that depriving young children of overnights with their fathers could compromise the quality of developing father-child relationships. Sufficient evidence does not exist to support postponing the introduction of regular and frequent involvement, including overnights, of both parents with their babies and toddlers. The theoretical and practical considerations favoring overnights for most young children are more compelling than concerns that overnights might jeopardize children’s development.
    [Show full text]
  • Opportunity, Responsibility, and Security a Consensus Plan for Reducing Poverty and Restoring the American Dream
    OPPORTUNITY, RESPONSIBILITY, AND SECURITY A CONSENSUS PLAN FOR REDUCING POVERTY AND RESTORING THE AMERICAN DREAM AEI/Brookings Working Group on Poverty and Opportunity Members of the AEI/Brookings Working Group on Poverty and Opportunity LAWRENCE ABER, Willner Family Professor of Psychology and Public Policy, New York University STUART BUTLER, Senior Fellow in Economic Studies, Brookings Institution SHELDON DANZIGER, President, Russell Sage Foundation ROBERT DOAR, Morgridge Fellow in Poverty Studies, American Enterprise Institute DAVID T. ELLWOOD, Scott M. Black Professor of Political Economy, Harvard University JUDITH M. GUERON, President Emerita, MDRC JONATHAN HAIDT, Thomas Cooley Professor of Ethical Leadership, New York University RON HASKINS, Cabot Family Chair and Senior Fellow, Economic Studies, Brookings Institution HARRY J. HOLZER, Professor of Public Policy, Georgetown University KAY HYMOWITZ, William E. Simon Fellow, Manhattan Institute for Policy Research LAWRENCE MEAD, Professor of Politics and Public Policy, New York University RONALD MINCY, Maurice V. Russell Professor of Social Policy and Social Work Practice, Columbia University RICHARD V. REEVES, Senior Fellow in Economic Studies, Brookings Institution MICHAEL R. STRAIN, Deputy Director of Economic Policy Studies and Resident Scholar, American Enterprise Institute JANE WALDFOGEL, Compton Foundation Centennial Professor for the Prevention of Children and Youth Problems, Columbia University OPPORTUNITY, RESPONSIBILITY, AND SECURITY A CONSENSUS PLAN FOR REDUCING POVERTY AND RESTORING THE AMERICAN DREAM AEI/Brookings Working Group on Poverty and Opportunity © 2015 by the American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research and the Brookings Institution. All rights reserved. The American Enterprise Institute for Public Policy Research (AEI) and the Brookings Institution are nonpartisan, not-for-profit, 501(c)(3) educational organizations.
    [Show full text]
  • Marian Allsopp Dissertation
    View metadata, citation and similar papers at core.ac.uk brought to you by CORE provided by LSE Theses Online INVISIBLE WOUNDS: A GENEALOGY OF EMOTIONAL ABUSE AND OTHER PSYCHIC HARMS INTRODUCTORY CHAPTER This dissertation is about how the concept of harm, damage or wound is applied as a metaphor to a site often called the self or the soul. This is the social space of the individual subject, which is, paradoxically, placed by our language and culture in a person’s interior – a place where we are all said to be vulnerable and endangered by a potentially hostile environment. The thesis consists of a series of studies which are designed to show how the concept of harm to an inner life emerges from different discursive contexts, and how it does so in distinctly variable versions: psychological, emotional, neurological or social, in more or less stable hybrid forms. Using primary sources which are mostly documentary, supported by some interviews, the studies range from a look at the psychiatric history of post traumatic stress disorder (PTSD) and at the story of its rewriting in English tort law; the recent reprised popularity of attachment theory and its marriage to neurology and a look at the career of the concept of the emotional abuse of children as a social problem category in the legal/administrative processes of Child Protection. These are introduced by a first chapter which concentrates on the metaphoric content of invisible wounds or psychic trauma 11 and the way it produces particular forms of the self. The studies which follow this are clustered around the literature and practices of the psychiatric, psychological, psycho-analytic, social work and legal professions, in order to show how the work of these professionals makes the concept of a psychic injury visible, discussible, treatable, administrable and justiciable.
    [Show full text]