PROPER and DARK HEROES AS DADS and CADS Alternative Mating Strategies in British Romantic Literature
Total Page:16
File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb
05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 305 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 PROPER AND DARK HEROES AS DADS AND CADS Alternative Mating Strategies in British Romantic Literature Daniel J. Kruger Maryanne Fisher University of Michigan York University Ian Jobling Buffalo, New York Empirical tests described in this article support hypotheses derived from evolutionary theory on the perceptions of literary characters. The proper and dark heroes in British Romantic literature of the late eighteenth and early nineteenth centuries respectively represent long-term and short- term mating strategies. Recent studies indicate that for long-term rela- tionships, women seek partners with the ability and willingness to sustain paternal investment in extended relationships. For short-term re- lationships, women choose partners whose features indicate high genetic quality. In hypothetical scenarios, females preferred proper heroes for long-term relationships. The shorter the relationship under consideration, the more likely women were to choose dark heroes as partners. KEY WORDS: Darwinian literary studies; Life history; Mating strategy; Sexual selection It is difficult to make progress in literary studies because, unlike scientists, literary scholars do not base their findings on theories that are subject to empirical tests. The imaginations of literary researchers are allowed to run Received February 4, 2003; accepted May 22, 2003; revised version received June 15, 2003. Address all correspondence to Daniel J. Kruger, Institute for Social Research, University of Michigan, P.O. Box 1248, Ann Arbor, Michigan 48106-1248. E-mail address: kruger@ umich.edu Copyright 2003 by Walter de Gruyter, Inc., New York Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, pp. 305–317. 1045-6767/03/$1.00+.10 305 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 306 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 306 Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, 2003 wild, and theories like deconstruction and Lacanian psychoanalysis are se- lected not because of their effectiveness in generating empirically valid hypotheses, but because people just happen to like them. Also, many hu- manists have an anti-scientific mythology that perpetuates this situation. This weakness in the field has drawn extensive criticism (e.g., Carroll 1995; Pinker 1997:521–565, 2002:400–420). This paper attempts to further the project of Darwinian literary studies, which seeks to promote consilience, or integration, between literary stud- ies and the sciences (Carroll 1995; Wilson 1998). We show here how liter- ary studies can be grounded in both the ideas and methods of the sciences by providing an empirical test of a literary interpretation that is derived from evolutionary theory on human sexuality. Our project uses psycho- logical research techniques to generate modern reactions to character types in late eighteenth and early nineteenth century British Romantic lit- erature. We hope to encourage literary researchers to develop their theo- ries of human cognition and behavior out of the best contemporary research on these subjects and to form partnerships with scientists to test their interpretations of literary texts empirically. We also hope to demon- strate to social and biological scientists that literature affords rich possibil- ities for testing and developing psychological theories. Literary historians have drawn a distinction between two basic heroic types in the British Romantic period: the proper hero and the dark hero (Railo 1927; Thorslev 1962; Welsh 1992). The proper hero is law-abiding, compassionate, kind, and monogamous. The dark hero is dominant, re- bellious, frequently a criminal, and often promiscuous. There is a close cor- respondence between the personality traits of proper and dark heroes and those of two personality types described by evolutionary scientists: cads and dads (Jobling 2002a, 2002b, 2002c). Cads are designed to maximize their reproductive success by having many short-term relationships with- out parental investment. Dads, on the other hand, maximize their repro- ductive success through long-term, monogamous relationships with women and parenting (Dawkins 1976). Women, on their part, possess psy- chological adaptations that cause them to be attracted to cads for short- term relationships and dads for long-term ones. Naturally occurring variances among cads and dads are the reason for the emergence of these Romantic character types (Jobling 2002a, 2002b, 2002c). CAD AND DAD MATING STRATEGIES Recent research on human sexuality suggests that humans evolved to pur- sue both short-term and long-term, or cad and dad, mating strategies. Con- siderable evidence suggests that, unlike most mammals, humans are 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 307 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 Heroic Dads and Cads 307 designed for long-term sexual relationships with substantial male parental investment in children. Human infants require considerable parental care, and children reared in father-absent households suffer much higher mor- tality rates than those reared in father-present households, especially in preindustrial societies (Hill and Hurtado 1996:434–439; Geary 1998:110– 115). The sexual psychology of women also indicates that human sexuality has been shaped by long-term sexual relationships. A number of studies both in the United States and cross-culturally have shown that women reg- ularly report being attracted to men who are socially respected, financially well-off, ambitious, industrious, dependable, emotionally stable, and ro- mantic, all qualities that indicate the ability and willingness to sustain long- term, parentally investing relationships with women (Buss 1994:32–34). However, aspects of both men’s and women’s sexuality also show that humans did not evolve exclusively to pursue long-term mating. Men con- sistently show a marked desire for sexual variety (Buss 1994:76–83). The fitness advantage of short-term sexual relationships to men is obvious: the number of potential offspring men can have increases linearly with the number of sexual partners. This advantage is the reason why short-term mating without parental investment is the strategy that most male mam- mals pursue (Trivers 1985:207–209). A successful womanizer in the envi- ronment in which humans evolved would have been able to sire a large number of children, many of whom would likely have survived despite higher mortality risks, especially in a resource-rich environment. The cad mating strategy has been shown to be successful in some circumstances, such as when there is a small effect of male parental investment on female reproductive success due to the local ecology (Barash and Lipton 2001; Draper and Harpending 1982, 1988; Gangestad and Simpson 2000; Lan- caster and Kaplan 1992). Why would cads have been attractive to ancestral women who would have had to provide on their own the immense parental investment nec- essary to bear and raise a child? The most common answer to this question is the “sexy son hypothesis.” When a woman mates with a cad, the genes that made the father successful as a cad will be passed on to their son. This sexy son would increase his mother’s fitness by giving her numerous grandchildren. Many theorists have hypothesized that women may have evolved in some circumstances to prefer cues in men that show high sex- ual success, or “good genes,” to cues of potential paternal investment (Barash and Lipton 2001; Cashdan 1996; Gangestad and Simpson 2000). It is also possible that women with a parentally investing mate would have extra-pair copulations with a cad in order to benefit from his genetic contribution. Draper, Harpending, and Belsky have argued that men evolved to specialize in one of the two mating strategies and have found that, 05ch/Kruger 9/12/03 13:39 Page 308 Kitty’s SD Aldine Human Nature Vol. 14#3 168020 308 Human Nature, Vol. 14, No. 3, 2003 cross-culturally, cads and dads show distinct clusters of personality traits. They believe that cads and dads are different human morphs, just as work- ers and queens are different morphs of ants, and that whether a man be- comes one or the other depends on an environmental trigger: the presence or absence of the father in the household where the son grows up. The sons of father-absent households will become cads, and those of father-present households will become dads (Draper and Harpending 1982, 1988; Draper and Belsky 1990). One of the most reliable findings in the biological study of sexual be- havior is that male sexual competitiveness is proportional to the degree of polygyny in a species (Alexander et al. 1979). The more females one male impregnates, the fewer the opportunities for other males. Consequently, the higher the degree of polygyny, the more intense the selection pressure on males to develop effective means of sexual competition. Consequently, Draper, Harpending, and Belsky reason that, because cads are more polyg- ynous than dads, they ought to have developed to be more highly sexually competitive. Their review of the literature on the subject supports this view. Cross-culturally, men from father-absent households favor a sexu- ally promiscuous mating strategy and are misogynistic and reluctant to engage in parental investment; they are also violent, aggressive, rebel- lious, high in risk-taking, and at high risk of incarceration. Boys from father-present households are more likely than boys from father-absent households to delay sexual experience, have positive attitudes towards and develop stable pair-bonds with women, have good relations with male peers, and accept authority. Draper, Harpending, and Belsky’s account of cad and dad mating strate- gies meshes well with other research. If women had evolved exclusively to pursue long-term mateships, their sexual attraction would be uniform in all contexts: they would always look for men who showed the ability and willingness to parentally invest in children. However, some studies show that women have different criteria for choosing long- and short-term mates, and the traits that they look for in short-term mates correspond quite well to the cad traits that Draper and Harpending have described.