DOCUMENT RESUME

ED 068 914 CS 000 265 AUTHOR Dawson, Mildred A., Comp. TITLE Teaching Word Recognition Skills. INSTITUTION International Reading Association, Newark, Del. PUB DATE 71 NOTE 308p. AVAILABLE FROMInternational Reading Association, 6 lyre Avenue, Newark, Del. 19711 ($4.00 non-member, $3.00 member)

EDRS PRICE MF-$0.65 HC-$13.16 DESCRIPTORS *Beginning Reading; College Students; Context Clues; Elementary School Students; Instructional Materials; Linguistics; *Phonics; Reading Comprehension; *Reading Instruction; *Reading Research; Secondary School Students; Structural Analysis; Visual Discrimination; *Word Recognition

ABSTRACT A series of articles with the chief emphasis on phonics as a means of analyzing words is presented. Various articles pertain to elementary, secondary, and college level instruction. The first of the five parts into which the volume is divided is comprised of a single article which gives an excellent overview of the field of word recognition. Part 2 includes a dozen recent articles that present the overall general program of word analysis and the policies that underlie it. In Part 3 are found articles which evaluate certain phonic elements and the utility of generalizations concerning them. For instance, two articles discuss rules that deal with accents on syllables and their effect on the pronunciation of words. Part 4 is concerned with such aspects of word recognition as sight vocabulary, sensory cues, visual discrimination, contextual clues, and phonics. The articles differ from those in Part 3 in that they more narrowly deal with particular aspects of word recognition and are more concerned with methodology. Certain articles were found to be only partially pertinent to a volume dealing with word recognition andare included in a general section in Part 5. Tables and referencesare included. (This document previously announced as ED 047 924.)(DH) U.S. DEPARTMENT OF HEALTH. EDUCATION & WELFARE OFFICE OF EDUCATION 4- THIS DOCUMENT HAS BEEN REPRO. DUCED EXACTLY AS RECEIVED FROM -""t THE PERSON OR ORGANIZATION ORIG. INATING IT POINTS OF VIEW OR OPIN. Cr. IONS STATE() DO NOT NECESSARILY REPRESENT OFFICIAL OFFICE OF EDU CO CATION POSITION OR POLICY w TEACHING WORDRECOGNITION SKILLS

compiled by Mildred A. Dawson

Ira

Reprinted from the Publications of the !nternational Reading Association Newark, Delaware 19711

FILMED FROM BEST AVAILABLE COPY INTERNATIONAL READING ASSOCIATION OFFICERS 1970-1971

President: DONALD L.CI.EI.AND, University of Pittsburgh, Pittsburgh, Pev.nsylvania President-elect: TnEonouE L. HARRIS, University of Puget Sound, Tacoma, Washington PastPresident: HELENHuus, University of Missouri, Kansas City, Missouri

DIRECTORS

Term expiring Spring1971 1%rillimnK. Durr, Michigan State University, East Lansing, Michigan Mildred It Freeman, Urban Laboratory in Education, Atlanta, Georgia Ethel M. King, University of Calgary, Calgary, Alberta

Term expiring Spring1972 Thomas C. Barrett, University of Wisconsin, Nladison, Wisconsin Constance M. McCullough, San Francisco State College, San Francisco, California Eileen E. Sargent, Nicola Union High School, Milwaukee, Wisconsin

Term expiring Spring1973 Marjorie S. Johnson, Temple University, Philadelphia. Pennsylvania Robert Karlin, Queens College, City University of New York, Flushing, New York Olive S. Niles, State Department of Education, Hartford. Connecticut

a a *

Executive Secretary-Treasurer:RAITH C. STAIGER, University of Delaware, Newark, Delaware AssistantExecutive Secretary: RONAI.D W. MITCHELL,International Read- ing Association, Newark, Delaware Publications Coordinator: FAYE R. BRANcA, International Reading Asso- ciation, Newark, Delaware a a * Copyright 1971 by the International Reading Associati nn Inc.

"PERMISSION TO REPRODUCE THIS COPY All Rights reserved RIGHTED MATERIAL HAS BEEN GRANTED Printed in the United States of America BYInternational Reading Association TO ERIC AND ORGANIZATIONS OPERATING UNDER AGREEMENTS WITH THE U S OFFICE OF EDUCATION FURTHER REPRODUCTION OUTSIDE THE ERIC SYSTEM REQUIRES PER MISSION OF THE COPYRIGHT OWNER CONTENTS*

Introductionvii

OVERVIEW 2 Teaching Word Recognition Skills limy SingerUniversity of California GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES 16 Why Not an IntensiveGradual PhonicApproach Carol K. ll'inkleyNorthern Illinois University 2.1 The Role of Phonics in Teaching Reading Jack BagfordUniversity of Iowa 30 A Critical Look at Instruction in Word Recognitionat the Elementary Level Juanita LewisColorado State College 35 Word Recognition for the JuniorHigh School Kathleen ClaytonManilas, New York, Public Schools 39 Word Attack Skills for the Retarded Reader inCollege Mildred B. FordVorhees College 43Applying Research Findings in Word Perceptionto Classroom Practice Mildred 1VittickPaterson State College .19Translating Research into Practice: Reading Readiness, Visual Perception, and Auditory Perception Ira E. AaronUniversity of Georgia 56Phonics InstructionWhen? What? For Whom? H. Alan RobinsonHofstra University 60 When Should Phonics Instruction Begin? Joseph BrzeinskiDenver Public Schools 65 Common Sense about Phonics Josephine PiekarzNew York University 69Phonics Materials: A Big Seller Dolores DurkinUniversity of Illinois 71 The Differences between Linguistics and Phonics Dorothy Z. Seymour Ginn and Company THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION 80Fundamental Principles Underlying Phonics Instruction Dolores DurkinUniversity of Illinois 83 The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in the Primary Grades Theodore ClymerUniversity of Minnesota 90 The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in Grades Onethrough Six Mildred Hart BaileyNorthwestern Louisiana State College

The articles in this volume have been reprinted from the following InternationalRead. ing Association publications:Proceedings of the Conventions, The Reading Teacher, Handbook for the Volunteer Tutor, and Frst Grade Reading Programs. Originalsource is cited at the beginning of each article. 96 The Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations above the Primary Grades Robert ErnanS Ohio State University 103Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations Lou E. BurmeisterUniversity of Texai 112Utility of Vowel Digraph Generalizations in Grades One through Six Mildred Hart BaileyNorthwestern Louisiana State College 116 When Two Vowels Go Walking Alvina Trott Burrows and Zyra Lourie New York University 120 When Two Vowels Go Walking and Other Such Things Robert Emans Ohio State University 129 Vowel Pairs Lou E. BurmeisterUniversity of Texas 138 A SoundSymbol Frequency Count David R. StoneUtah State University 145 The Effect of Syllabic Position and Accent on the Phonemic Behavior of Single Vowel Graphemes Lou E. BurmeisterUniversity of Texas 150 Which Accent Generalizations Are Worth Teaching? Carol WhalleyNorthern Illinois University

INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION 158Extending the Sight Vocabulary J. Richard ChambersBoston University 160Different Sensory Cues as Aids in Beginning Reading Ethel M. King and Sicgmar Much! University of Alberta and University of Iowa 166Recent Research in Visual Discrimination: Significance for Beginning Reading Siegmar Much! and Ethel M. King University of Iowa and University of Alberta 173Consonant Substitution in Word Attack Charles E. RailsbachLa Grange Park, Illinois, Schools 177First Grade Children Work with Variant Word Endings Irene IV. Hanson Augsburg College 181 Use of Context Clues Robert Emans Ohio State University 188 New Approaches to Easing Word Attack at die Beginning Reader Levels Dorothy DietrichUniondale, New York, Schools 19.1 Phonic Emphasis Approaches Arthur W. HeilmanPennsylvania State University 210 New Alphabet Approaches Edward Fly RutgersThe State University 223Strategies for Teaching SoundLetter Relationships Morton BotelUniversity of Pennsylvania 227 The Teaching of Word Analysis through Perceptual Conditioning Gerald G. GlassAde 1phi University 231Teaching Structural Analysis Leo M. SchellKansas State University

iv

4 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL 238 Word Analysis and Comprehension Donald A. Benz and Robert A. Rosemier Wisconsin State University and Northern Illinois University 2.1.1 Linguistics and the Teaching of Reading Dolores DurkinUniversity of Illinois 219 The Effectiveness of Code- and Meaning-Emphasis Beginning Reading Programs Robert DykstraUniversity of Minnesota 257Linguistics and Reading Problems at the junior High School Level Charles C. FriesUniversity of Michigan 261Vowel Sounds in VCC Words Paula FuldCenter for Urban Education Beginning Reading Project 205 Reading Achievement and Word Recognition Skills Ben H. Hackney, Jr.University of North Carolina 269 PhonemeGrapheme Correspondences in Monosyllabic Words Richard E. HodgesUniversity of Chicago 276 Conference on Perceptual and Linguistic Aspects of Reading Isvan KeislerLos Angeles, California 283 Word Recognition Practice: Basal vs. Phonics Programs George E. MasonUniversity of Georgia 285 A Study of the Techniques of Word Identification H. Alan RobinsonHofstra University 290 Visual Discrimination of Words and Reading Readiness Carol Ann SheaSouthern Connecticut State College 297 What Do Teachers Know about Phonics and Syllabication? George D. Spache and Maly E. BaggeltUniversity of Florida 301Developing Advanced %'ord Perception Skills Diana (blatant,Saginaw, Michigan, Public Schools `..- -.... I .1

The International Reading As- sociation attempts, throughits publications, to provide a forum for a wide spectrum of opinion on reading.This policy permits divergent viewpoints without as- suming the endorsement of the Association.

vi INTRODUCTION

PDF:ACM:RS are perennially interested in articles concerned with the word- 1. attack skills.In recent years, this interest seems to have increased as certain linguists have applied their knowledge of the nature of language to the phonology of words. While sounding out words is only one of several aspects of word recognition, almost all of the pertinent articles published by the International Reading Association have dealt directly with phonics, to the neglect of structural analysis and contextual clues. This volume, therefore, gives chief entphasis to phonics as a means of analyzing words. There are five parts in this volume. The first is comprised of a single article that gives an excellent overview of the field of word recognition. Part 11 includes a dozen recent articles that present the overall general program of word analysis and the policies that underlie it.Part III con- tains articles dating back to 1963 when The Reading Teacher published the pioneer article by Clymer in which he reported on the comparative utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. Nine of the remaining articles similarly evaluate certain phonic elements and the generalizations concerning them. Two discuss rules that deal with accent on syllables and their effect on the pronunciation of words. Part IV is concerned with such special aspects of word recognition as sight vocabulary, sensory cues, visual discrimination, contextual clues, and of coursephonics. The articles differ from those in Part Ill in that they more narrowly deal with particular aspects of word recognition and are more concerned with methodology in certain instances. Three surveys of innovative approaches to reading, such at i.t.a. and "words in color," are included. Certain articles were found to be only partially pertinent to a volume dealing with word recognition. These are listed in a general bibliography in Part V. The reader is advised to look up those articles that promise additional and desired information. Mn.mtEn A.DAWSON Compiler

vii

7 PART I: Overview Dr. Singer has written an overview with excellentcoverage, great clarity, and creditable organization. While his article is intendedfor the para- professional who is tutoring children or adults, it is fullyas valuable for a teacher or supervisor who wishes a clear-cut overview of the entirefield of word recognition. Teaching Word Recognition Skills

HARRY SINGER*

TO TEACH worm recognition, the tutor should know some principles of teaching word recognition, have an idea of a sequence of it, and learn some basic techniques for teaching it.In each of the remaining sections of this chapter are practical suggestions to use in teaching word recognition. Principles of Teaching Word Recognition I.Proceed from the familiar to the unfamiliar a step at a time. Although there may be some disagreement, the following is an ex- ample of a sequence going from the familiar to the unfamiliar: learning names for actual objects, associating a name with a picture of an object, recognizing a name in print in conjunction with a pictured object, and finally recognizing the printed word alone.In this sequence, one goes from a sensors- motor - perceptual type of ex- perience to symbols to represent the experience. For example, from a name of a live dog, to the name for a picture of a dog, to recognition of the printed word "dog" alone. 2. From dependence on the tutor to independence. To follow this principle, first tell the student the whole word, then give hints, and finally let the student figure out the word from the variety of tech- niques he may have learned. Thus, at first the tutor tells the student that the word is "dog." Next time, the tutor gives the individual clues to help the student figure out the word clues, such as, inferring from the context (a picture context or a sentence context or both), "The said, "Bow wow... Or the tutor gives the direction to sound out the word, a task which implies, of course, that there is for the particular word a one-to-one sound-symbol correspondence. But, this approach does not work for all words in the English lan- guage. For example, it works for "dog" but not for "right... 3. Teach the individual a variety of ways of recognizing words. Indeed, each technique of word recognition breaks down or is in- adequate because no one technique applies to all words in the English language. The skilled reader must learn to use a variety of techniques, and he must also learn to shift appropriately from one to another. For example, he might use phonics to sound out "cat."But, in addition to sounding out c-a-t, he must also synthesize or put the sounds together to get the whole sound of "cat" which he then Handbook for the Veltshleer Tutor,1969, 46.59.

9 No, I

SINGER 3 recognizes as a familiar word. In contrast, individuals, except in a classroom, never hear anyone point to the actual oiject and say that's a "c-a-t" (usually pronounced "cult-ah-tuh"). Phonics or one-for-one sound symbol correspondence might work for "cat" but not for "right." If the individual tried to use phonics on "right," it would come out "r-i-g-h-t." No matter how many times the individual letters were sounded out, the word wouldn't come out "right." To make the word come cnt "right," the in- dividual would have to shift to another one of several techniques; could use context plus the initial consonant to infer the word, as in -.he following sentence: After the boy answered the question, the teacher smiled and said, "That's r Or he could have been taught the initial consonant (through such words as red, row, run) and the phonogram, iglo through such words as light, fight, night. By substituting the initial consonant, "r," he would get the word "right." Another way of recognizing the word is by using a combination of phonics and knowledge of silent letters, ri(gh)t. The student who has learned a variety of approaches can then be flexible in recognizing words. If one approach doesn't work, he could switch to another. He is likely to do so not only if he knows a variety of approaches but if he is also continuously testing his so- lution against the criterion of meaning or asking himself. "Does this word pronounced this way make sense ?" 4. Introduce new words and new techniques gradually and with adequate repetition so that tile learner has a growing feeling of mastery. The basal readers or textbooks used in the primary grades for developing word-recognition skills provide for about ten repeti- tions of a newly introduced word and live repetitions of previously introduced words.Itis estimated that the average individual re- quires about 38 repetitions to recognize a new word quickly and accurately. 5. Use an interesting way of having individuals practice recog- nizing new words. The most interesting way is to have the individual do a lot of reading in which he is likely to use the new words. The basal readers follow the introduction of new words with a story in which, by design, the new words appear several or more times. High interest, but low-level difficulty reading material is likely to contain words that the tutor is trying to get the student to learn. A list of words that most pupils should know by the third grade has been constructed by DtIch who discovered that these words accounted for about 75 percelic of all primary words and 50 percent of all adult words. The Dolch Readers and other materials published by the Garrard Press systematically use this vocabulary.* Basic Sight Words, Garrard Press, Champaign, Illinois. This word list can also be found in M. A. Tinker and C. M. McCullough,Teaching Elementary Reading(2nd Ed.). New York: Apple- tonCentury Crofts, 1962, 550-551. 10 sir 4 OVERVIEW The Reader's Digest Reading Skil !builder** is anotherset of high- interest materials which start at a low level of difficulty and increase in difficulty.Together these two sets of materials would be very useful in teaching older individuals whose interestsare more mature but who for one reason or another have not learned how to read or how to read as well as they should. 6. If the individual needs drill on recognition of words, try to provide it in a variety of ways. One technique that works well is to have the individual make up sentences using the word. Copy down the sentences and have the individual read them. Then cut up the sentences into words and have him recognize the individual words. Then have him group common initial consonants or common sounds in these words. Or he might group common syllablesor prefixes and suffixes. He might also search through magazines for illustrations for his words. 7. A student's knowledge of progress is extremely important. Two techniques that can be utilized for concretely showing progressare 1) making a file of new words and 2) constructinga cumulative chart. The card file might show the word in a sentence on the front with the word in the corner divided into syllables and perhaps even dia- critically marked. On the back of the card, the word wouldappear in isolation. The individual could test himself by looking at the back of the card to see if he knows the word in isolation and checking himself to see if he was right by reading thesentence and using con- text clues to check himself. For young children, pictures might also be used to identify the words.t The cumulative chart could be kept daily or weekly. Below are examples of these charts: Dailyto indicate immediate consequence of practice in recognizing new words.

6 5 No. 4 Correct 3 2

1. 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 Trials

Educational Division, Reader's Digest Service, Pleasantville, N.Y. IThc Garrard Press's "Popper Words" is based on this principle. SINGER 5 Weekly or monthlyto indicate cumulative numberof new words learned.

60 50 Cumulative 40 No. of New Words 30 20 10 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 Weeks

8. In each lesson, try to maximize the probability ofsuccess. Individuals who have experienced failureare extremely sensitive to further failure. One strategyto attain success is to start by having the student tell a story. (See Chapter V fora detailed explanation of the language-experience approach.) Or, if the individualprefers, have him relate the activities for the dayor tell an anecdote. Copy down the story and teach the individualto read the story.First, go over the story and have the individual identify words he doesn't recognize. Then, teach these words, which willbe put into the word file and plotted on the chart. Then, havethe individual read the story. Next, cut up a copy of thestory into sentences and have the individual reassemble thesentences. Then, cut the sentences into words. Again, have the story reassembled. Then,have the individual use the words to make up new sentences. 9. From the very beginning, consult with the student'sclassroom teacher, provided that the individual is still attendingschool. The tutor might get from the classroom teacher valuable suggestions and even materials for helping the individual. In any case, thetutor and the classroom teacher should be working togetherto help the student. If the teacher or the studentwants the tutor to help with the daily assigned lesson, the tutor coulduse the above principles with the assigned materials. Helping with the daily lessonis a short -range strategy that might pay off equally as well as a developmental,sys- tematic program, provided the discrepancy betweenthe student's assignment and his level of reading ability isnot so great that he is overwhelmed by too many new wordsto learn. If so, the tutor might have to concentrate his efforts upon the long-rangestrategy of taking the individual back to a level where hecan be successful and starting a developmental reading program at that level. 12 6 OVERVIEW 10. For developmental or corrective reading instruction, itis necessary to followa sequence for teaching word recognition.There are several logical sequences that could be used, but no one of them can be said to be the sequence to follow. The following is one sequence:

A Developmental Sequence of Word Recognition (A list of materials for teaching each of these aspects of word recognition can be found at the end of this section.) Approximate Grade Level

1 Sight words 1-2 Initial consonants Final consonants Consonant digraphs (ch, th, .vh, sh) 1-2 Consonant blends (fr, sl, cl, pl - - and those needed) Advanced consonant digraphs (qu, nk, ng); variants (s, es, ed, ing) Silent consonants Contractions Simple structure of sentences and punctua- tion markers for speech patterns, such as use of commas, periods, question marks. Approximate Grade Level 2 Vowels Long and short sounds Vowels in phonogram ( "right, niake, etc.) Simple suffix (farmer) Rhyming endings More complex sentence structure and punc- tuation markers 3 Syllabication 4 Prefixes, suffixes, roots 5 Accent and dictionary work Types of Word-Recognition Techniques Thetypes of word recognition techniques can be groupedinto three somewhat-overlapping categories: predominantly visual clues, emphasis on meaning, and mainly analytical procedures.

Predominantly Visual Clues 1. Picture aids.Printed words are often learned by their association with pictures.For identifying nouns, pictures can readily be 13 SINGER 7 found in magazines. For other categories of speech, suchas verbs and adjectives, cartoon stripscan be used. A variety of ways of illustrating various parts of speechmay be found in a small pocket- book, English Through Pictures.*(See list of references.)This pocketbook may also be used for teaching Englishto bilingual children by having the childrenact out the sentences as they read them. 2. Sight words or "instant" recognition of words. Through recogni- tion of the same words in a variety of situations, individuals build up a sight-word vocabulary. This vocabulary increasesas the individual matures in reading.Eventually the skilled reader recognizes all words at sight or recognizes themso rapidly that he becomes almost oblivious to theprocess because it occurs so quickly and so effortlessly. Two procedurescan be used for de- veloping sight words: (a) Flash cards. This procedure has already been discussedin the section on principles of teaching word recognition.It's a useful technique but should only be used fora short period of time because boredom sets in rapidly. However, thereare a variety of techniques for making such exercisesinteresting, such as having the studentuse the words to construct sentences. (b) Easy reading. Seeing thesame words in a variety of stories is the most interesting and best way of teaching sight-word recognition. A list of interesting, yet easy-to-read books for older children and adults are listed in Chapters VIII andIX. Whether a book is easy depends on how well theperson can read. The rule of thumb is that if the individual haidifficulty with more than one or two wordsper hundred, it's not an easy book for him. Whether a book is interesting, ofcourse, de- pends upon the interests of the reader.For determining whether any book is easy, but interesting, the best procedure is to have she individual select the book himself andtry it out. Emphasis on Meaning 1. Context clues. This technique consists of usinga sentence to help the individual infer the meaning of the unknown word andthen to think of the word itself. By looking at the initialconsonant and other parts of thesentence, the individual is employing a very powerful combination of word-recognition techniq The teaching procedure calls for constructinga sentence in which the unknown word is omitted or all but the initialconsonant is omitted. Then the individual reads thesentence and tries to infer the unknown word. Thesesentences may be placed on cards with

'I. A. Richards and C. M. Gibson. English Through Picture,. Pocketbooks,1953. OVERVIEW the missing word on the back so that the flash-card technique can also be used and the cards then filed as tangible evidence of achieve- ment. The tutor can teach the individual to recognize and use these clues whenever they appear in stories, and the tutor can also use this approach in giving hints for recognizing new words. Examples* of various types of context clues follow: (a) Definition. The unknown word is defined. "Tom liked to ride on the last car on the train. The last car on the train is the c99 (caboose) (b) Experience. The individual can infer the word from his exper- ience."Betty was going to grow her own flowers.In each row, she placed s (seeds) (c) Contrast. The unknown word is opposite in meaning to known words or phrases. "She missed the noises of the big city. On the farm it was very q (quiet) (d) Familiar expression. The unknown word is part of an idiom or everyday expression.To his surprise, the big man was as g as a lamb." (gentle) (e) Summary. The unknown word sums up the ideas in the pre- ceding sentences."First, Tom saw the riders rope the steers. Then he laughed at the cowboy clown on his horse. Then he watched the cowboys race around the ring. Tom had a lot of fun at the r (rodeo) (f) Reflection of a mood or situation. The unknown word explains the mood of the story. "After a few days away from home at camp, she began to miss her family, her dog, and her friends. She was h 99 (homesick) 2. Compound words. Words which represent the combination of two words may be recognized when separated, and the meaning of the two words gives the meaning of the combined word. At the pri- mary grade level, such words as "summertime" and "fireman" can be recognized this way. One technique is to have each part of the compound word on a separate card and bring the two cards together so that they appear to make one word. Or, the two parts of the word can be underlined separately, e.g. fire man.

'C. M. McCullough. "The Recognition of Context Clues in Reading," Elementary English Review, 22 (1945), 1-15. Adapted with permission of Dr. McCullough. 15 SINGER 9 Analytical Techniques Structural and phonetic analysis are the two major types of analytical techniques. 1. Included in structural analysis are the following: a. Compound and hyphenated words (by-product). b. Root words and their variants which include the various end- ings to indicate tense and number, such as "rush'. and "rushed," "boy" and "boys." c.Contractions, such as "didn't." d. Syllabication, the parts of a word whichmay or may not represent meaning components, such as prefixes or suffixes, but do represent boundaries between sequences of sounds ina word. Structural analysis in this section will emphasize syllabication. In general, a word has as many syllablesas heard vowel sounds. To teach syllabication, do the following: a. Pupil and tutor should first pronounce the word carefully. b. Identify the number of heard vowel sounds. c. Group about five words together that fit a principle of syllab- ication. Teach the pupil to divide each word into its syllables. Have the pupil then try to formulate the rule. Then have him supply or search for words that fit the rule.After he has learned the following rules, he can then classifynew words according to the rules. Remember, this technique requiresa thorough understanding of vowel sounds andconsonants if it is to be successful.

Rules for Syllabication (Many students have difficulty with this. Use only when applicable.) 1. If there is only one heard vowel sound ina word, the word is monosyllabic and, therefore, cannot be further divided. Examples: late, night, trees. 2. In words containing two heard vowel sounds, whentwo conso- nants are together in the word with vowels on either side, the word is usually divided between the consonants.Examples: of ter, but ter, bar gain. 3. Affixes (prefixes and suffixes) usually forma separate syllable. Examples: unhappy, lively. (Note: "ed" is usually onlya sep- arate syllable when preceded by "t" or "d" as in "wanted" and "needed." When preceded by other consonants, "ed" doesnot form a separate syllable as in "called," "rushed," "liked," etc.) 4. If there is one consonant between two vowels ina two-syllable word and the first vowel sound is long, the long vowel usually 10 OVERVIEW ends the first syllable and theconsonant begins the second syllable. Examples: la dies, po lite. (The firstsyllable is called an "open" syllable.) 5. If there is one consonant betweentwo vowels in a two syllable word and the first vowel is short,the consonant usually ends the first syllable and the second vowelsound begins the second syl- lable. Example:cam el, nag ic. (First syllable is called a "closed" syllable.) 6. Compound words are divided into theircomponent parts.Ex- amples: high mut,rail road, snow flake. 7. "Le" endings are special cases.If the word ends in "ck" when le is taken off, then "le isa separate syllable.Examples: crackle, pickle. In all other words, "le" takesthe preceding con- sonant. Examples: eagle, candle. 8. For words which containmore than two syllables, follow the former principles, proceeding from the largestto the smallest division of the word : a. Inspect for compound word. Example: steel worker, kinder garten. b. Take off affix: worker, kindergarten. c.If one or more of the parts havemore than one heard vowel sound, determine whether vowel soundsare long or short. Then divide into open and closed syllables,as in kind er E ten. d. Inspect for special "le" endings. Phonics "Sounding out" wordsor the application of phonetic principles to the recognition of new words is useful for about 80to 85 percent of words. One procedure for applyingphonics or relating sounds to letters is the following: a. Group known sightwords with a commonconsonant or vowel. For example: can cut car b. Have individual read each wordto make sure each word is a known sight word. c. Have pupil notice the initial letters of the wordsare alike and The initial sounds are alike.Tell the pupil the name of the letter (small and capital letter). d. To test whether pupil has learnedthe sound for the initial consonant, have the pupil give additional words with thesame initial sound. If he gives the following words,form them into two columns: cold kite city cup kit cent cap kind e. In the preceding examples, point out 17 SINGER 11

1. some words have the same sound-letter correspondence. 2. some words have the same sound but a different letter, and 3. some words have the same letter and a different sound. This discovery will help the pupil limit his generalization and perhaps will help him become flexible in shifting fromone sound-letter correspondence to another in recognizing words. f.Pupils can also learn phonograms the same way: came night game right tame light After reading through the list of words with a common phono- gram, have pupils supply or search for additional words with the same phonogram. (See list of references for sets of phono- grams.) g. With knowledge of consonants and phonograms, pupils can figure out new words by means of consonant substitution. For example, the pupil who knows "tell" and sell and the initial consonant **V* can then figure out the new word "bell." h. Although pupils can and do learn to read by implicitly using rules, it may help to know the rules: To teach them, proceed in the following way. Teach inductively by presenting words that illustrate a vowel principle, and then teach deductively by stating a vowel principle and having words categorized under that principle. One can teach the following vowel and consonant rules: 1. Long vowel sdounds are usually the names of the vowel letters, as in ate, eat, ice, open, use, or as in the following sentence: A sweet, nice, old Unicorn. 2. When two vowels are together in a word, the first is usually long and the second is usually silent, as in Tad, east, tried, bat, rueful. 3. However, there are many exceptions to the preceding rule, particularly when the first vowel is followed by u," "o," and "i."Examples: haul, caught, good, spoon, broom, said, oil. 4. In words containing two vowels, one of which is a final "e," the final "e is usually silent and the preceding vowel is usually long. Examples: ate, see, ice, note, use. 5. Single vowels followed by "1, "r," or "w" usually have a blend sound: Examples: talk, ball; car, far; saw, raw. 6. When "y" ends a word that has no other vowel, then "y" has the sound of long "i."Examples: my, try, sky.If "y" ends a two-syllable word, it frequently has the sound of long "e."Examples: merry, scary, surely.If "y" is

18 12 OVERVIEW preceded by another vowel, there isa different sound for both. Examples: they, boy. 7. Consonants are sometimes silentor not sounded. Examples: lamb, night, ghost, hour, know, 2neumonia,island, buy, wrote. These silent consonants must be learnedas sight words. That is, an individual hasto learn that in certain words, there areone or more consonants that are silent. This condition is frequeltlytrue for words beginning with "kn.' (know, knife), words that have "gh"in them (light, night, right), and words ending in "mb" (lamb,thumb, limb). Although silentconsonants make phonics inappli- cable to many words, silentconsonants are important for word recognition,particularly for discriminating hom- onyms (words that sound alike but are spelled differently), such as "buy "and "by," "two and "to,""know" and "no." If words did not have silent letters inthem, one would have to use some other clues, suchas diacritical markings for determining pronunciation. Forexaitipl?try to read this sentence in which the silent letters havebeen omitted: "We taut eit fish at eit this morning andat them at nit." This sentence with the silent letters in reads:"We caught eight fish at eight this morning andate them at night." Cautions in Teaching Word Recognition 1. Proceed only as rapidlyas the student can be cumulatively successful.Whether teachingnew sight words, a new structural analysis technique, or anew phonic principle, provide sufficient prac- tice and use of thenew instruction in various reading situationsso that the pupil doesnot get overloaded with too much too quickly. Some students needa slow rate of learning, and others can havea faster rate of learning. Sometimesthe same student can learnsome things slowly and other things quickly.One has to judge what is the best pace or rate of learning forthe student. Begin each session with a review of the instruction ofthe previous session; then after the pupil demonstrates he remembers what hewas taught, go on to new instruction. If the student doesn't remember, then havea quick review of the previous instruction beforegoing on to new instruction. But, be sure to evaluateyour rate of instruction to determine whether it fits your student'srate of learning.If you can, keep a graph or chart of the rate of instruction. 2. Plan each lesson carefully. Makesure you know what you want your student to accomplish at each session. A briefoutline of the lesson will help. A typical lessonmight include the following. a. Review words. SINGER 13 b. Introduce new words (print word in sentence) and discuss them to get pronunciation, ways of recognizing word, word meaning, and similarity to previous words and experiences associated with the word. c. Have the student recognize new words in a variety of sentences. d. Make a card (for card file) for each new word. e. Make an experience chart (see Chapter IV) using the new words or read a story containing the new words. f.Try to arouse curiosity for reading the story by having the student formulate questions about the story from knowing its title or from looking at the illustrations.If the story has questions at the end, at times let the student read the questions first. Then read the story.Later, let him predict in advance what the questions are when he reads the title of the story. The eventual objective is to get him to formulate questions as he reads and thus read to answer his own questions. This process will make him anactive reader. g.Have the student reread story into a tape recorder and then listen to the playback.Let him evaluate his own per- formance. h. Teach a new word-recognition skill and have the student practice the skill in an interesting way. 3. Try to work with the student at least twice a week. At first, do not require him to do anything outside of the tutoring session. As he becomes successful, he will ask to take home books or he will begin to bring books, new words, or materials to the session. Grad- ually you might begin to assign homework. Be sure, though, that di :student knows how to do the homework. If he is to read a book a -home, he should know practicallyallthe words in the book. As e learns ways to recognize new words, he can be given books with few words which he can attack using skills 116 has already learned in the tutoring session. 4. As much as possible, plan and evaluate with your student. Find out what he thinks his difficulty is, how he is tryingto solve it, how he tries to read, and what he would liketo learn. Make the process of teaching and learning a mutual situation in which both you and your student are working together to achieve a common goal.

'41

20 14 OVERVIEW Selected References to Teaching Materialsand Teaching Aids Word Recognition Materials (For a more complete listing,see Chapters VIII and IX) Basic Sight Words. Champaign, Illinois: GarrardPress. Breaking the Sound Barrier. New York:Macmillan. English Through Pictures, I. A. Richards and C.M. Gibson. New York: Pocketbooks. Eye and Ear Fun, Book 4, Word Independencein the Middle Grades, Clarence R. Stone. St. Louis: Webster. Acomplete sequence of word-recognition techniques and exercises fromletter names and sounds to word meaning analysis, usingprefixes, suffixes, and MOM Phonics in Proper Perspective, Arthur W.Heilman. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill, 1964. Generalinstructions on teaching phon- ics,includes techniques and exercises forteaching consonant sounds, vowel sounds, syllabication, andaccent. Phonics Skilltexts. Columbus, Ohio:Merrill. Phonics We Use. Chicago: Lyons& Carnahan. Phonics and Word Power, My Weekly ReaderPractice Books. Colum- bus, Ohio 43216: Education Center.Workbook-type materials designed for grades 1-3. Ranges frominstruction on initial con- sonants to syllabication. Reading with Phonics. Philadelphia: Lippincott. Speech-to-Print Phonics, Donald D. Durrell andHelen A. Murphy. New York: Harcourt, Brace & World. PART II:General Program and Policies This part of the reprint volume affords a general view of a program in word recognition, extending from the early elementary school years through college. Expert analysis of research findings to date is provided by Wittick and Aaron as a means of validating desirable policies and of deciding which policies to avoid or eliminate.Robinson and Brzeinski give good advice on the desirable time to initiate instruction in word rec- ognition skills, while Pickarz and Durkin give knowledgeable warnings based on common sense as well as their thorough acquaintance with re- search findings. Seymour makes clear the difference between phonics and the linguistic approach to beginning reading in terms of phonology.

22 Why Not an Intensive-Gradual Phonic Approach

CAROL K. W1NKLEY*

AS ONE attempts to discuss the familiar, age-oldtopic of phonics, there is a temptation to presume that thereis, or ought to be, some sane solution to the controversy that has raged in recentyears and reached new heights with the publication ofChall's (1967), Learn- ing to Read: The Great Debate. AsArt ley (1967), Strang (1968), and others have professed, it is doubtfulthat a topic so basic to "breaking the code" should becomea subject for any more than thoughtful, well-designed, and impartial researchas well as careful and discerning analysis of theprocess involved.

EXAMINATION OF THE PROBLEM Evidence from research certainlysupports the practice of teaching sound-symbol or phoneme-graphemerelationships. How- ever, an examination of researchwhether it be thatsummarized by Chall (1967), Gurren and Hughes(1965), Weintraub (1966), or othersseems to raise more questions than itanswers. It has not shown why somany pupils fail to acquire a work- able technique for decoding written languagewhen Austin and Morrison (1963) found that 95 to 99per cent of the elementary schools participating in theirsurvey reported that phonetic analysis was taught in grades one through six, andwhen such leaders in the reading field as Bond, Fay (1968),and Durrell (1969) believe that phonics is actually over-taught. Inaddition, research has not provided conclusive evidence concerningthe specific phonic gener- alizations that actually prove to bemost helpful to pupils faced with the task of unlocking unknown words.Word counts have been made by Clymer (1963), Emans (.1967),and others to determine the "utility" of certain principlesbut it is not known whether their findings bear any relationshipto children's actual utilization of the principles when they are engaged in theact of decoding unfamiliar words. It appears, though, that themost important question that remains unanswered today pertains tomethodologywhat is the best, most efficient procedure for teaching pupilsthe relationship that exists between the sounds of the spokenlanguage, with which they are familiar, and the symbols used torepresent that language when it is recorded or written? At thesame time teaching "reading The Reading Teacher,23 (April 1970) , 611.617, 620.

rtfll.t., WINKLEY 17

for meaning," an issue about which there shouldbe no debate, has become hopelessly entwined in thecontroversy. In an attempt to shed some lighton the problems involved in teaching pupils the phoneme-graphemecorrespondence character- istic of the English language, three questionswill be considered. First, why is phonic instructionso ineffective in our schools; second, how can phonics be taught efficiently;and third, what method of instruction appears to be dictatedby a careful analysis of the process involved.

REASONS FOR INEFFECTIVE PHONICS INSTRUCTION Several years of in-service training of readingteachers, first as a reading consultant and now as a college instructor of graduate students, have convinced the author that theaverage teacher of reading does not know as much phonicsas the average third-grade pupil. On a test of phonic knowledge, administeredto forty teachers enrolled in a graduate level course at Northern IllinoisUniversity, the mean score was ninety out of 110 possible points,or about 82 per cent mastery. Reviewing several similar studies of teachers' knowledge of phonic skills, Spache (1964) concludedthat "the average teacher is not thoroughly trained in phonics in college reading courses."Itis even questionable whether the college instructors themselves know the phonic principlesone might expect to be taught in elementary school classrooms. Thisfact seems to support Austin's ( 1961 ) contention, following hersurveys reported in The Torch lighters and The First R( 1963), that the place to begin, if we are to improve the readingskills of children, is with thorough and intensive training of collegeinstructors of teachers of reading. Since college professors frequently offertime limitations as their excuse for not teaching phonic generalizationsmore thor- oughly, Marion Hull, who is involved inpre-service as well as in-service training of reading teachers at NorthernIllinois Univer- sity, has prepared a program for self-instructionof phonic prin- ciples. Entitled Phonics for the Teacher of Reading,this 116-page booklet, providing immediate feedbackregarding the accuracy of responses, can be completed by a student or teacher inapproxi- mately five hours of independent work. Amongthe phonic prin- ciples taught arc11 the twenty-five consonant phonemes(voiced, unvoiced, and nasal) witha discussion of other graphemes repre- senting each sound and other phonemes representedby each graph- eme; 2] the nineteen vowel phonemes including the longsounds, short sounds, other single vowel sounds,diphthongs, and digraphs; 24 18 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

3J vowel clues;4] syllabication principles; and 5] accentgener- alizations. It can be argued that in order to read the literature withany degree of understanding teachers need to know the definitions, concepts, and principles presented. However, it is extremely doubt- ful that they should all be taught to children. For example, isit really essential that children know the difference between voiced and unvoiced consonants? Must they learn the definitions of pho- neme, grapherhe, digraph, diphthong, etc.? Should they be expected to recall when the inflectional ending "ed" forms a separate syllable and when itis pronounced with the preceding syllable? These concepts are of interest to teachers studying the characteristics and consistencies of the English language but they probably would be of little or no value to a pupil wishing to use his phonic knowledge to figure out an unfamiliar word. There is a desperate need for well-controlled studies which will provide evidence regardingthe specific phonic concepts actually helpful to pupilsattempting to identify words. In addition to deficiencies in teachers' knowledge of phonics, another possible explanation for the ineffective teaching of phonics in many classrooms is that it is not being taught efficiently. Lacking complete understanding of what they are attempting to teach, teachers are "belaboring phonics to death" by teachingconcepts and understandings that the children have already acquired. This leads to the second question, "How can phonics be taught effi- ciently?"

PRETESTING TO INCREASE EFFICIENCY OF TEACHING It appears reasonable to say that knowledge ofa phonic principle cannot be considered acquired byany pupil until he can use it to identify unknown words to which it is applicable. This is the true purpose of teaching phonics. Itcan, therefore, be assumed that children go through various stagesor reach various levels of understanding concerning any phonic generalization. The job of the teacher then becomesone of finding out where the "breakdown" is for each and every child who is unableto apply a particular generalization. A pre-test consisting often steps is recommended as a useful procedure for identifying the starting point for teaching any generalization. Although itis commonly done, teachers should not go back to the auditory discrimination step, for example, and use games and various auditory exercises to help pupils discriminate one sound from another unless the

25 \VIM:LEY 19

child's knowledge has not progressed beyond that stage. The following steps are proposed for pre-testing of children's knowledge of any phonic principle. The steps are continued until the teacher locates the level at which each child demonstrates successful achievement. Because pre-testing begins at the highest level and procedes to successively lower levels of mastery, the first pre-test step is numbered Step 10 (which coincides with the num- ber of the related teaching step.) Step10: Present unknown words, to which the principle is applicable, and ask the pupil to pronounce each word. When testing a pupil's knowledge of consonant and vowel sounds, phoneme substitution in known words can be utilized. Step 9 :Ask the chili'. what phoneme the grapheme represents in isolation. Step 8 :Have the pupils write the letter that stands for a specified sound heard in words as they are pronounced by the teacher. Step 7 :Ask pupils to select the letter, for which they see a visual representation, which stands for a particular sound heard in the name of a pictured object. Step 6 :Several words are presented from which the pupils are asked to select the one(s) in which a particular sound appears. Step5:Several letters art. presented from which the pupils select the one that represents a particular sound in a word pronounced by the teacher. Step4: The pupil is asked to name the letter(s). Step3 :As the teacher pronounces several words, the child is asked to select those which contain a particular sound heard in a word. Step2 : The child is asked to isolate a particular sound in a spoken word. Step 1: The ch.ldren are asked to locate a certain symbol appearing in several written words. There appears to be a logical progression of skill development illustrated in these steps that are necessarily pre-tested in reverse order. No **diagnostic" test, presently on the market, is constructed in such a way that it identifies the level at which a child's skill breaks down in relation to any phonic generalization nor even whether a usable knowledge of the principle has been acquired by a child. Thus, responsibility for pretesting and interpreting the results falls squarely on the classroom teacher. Certain adaptations of these steps will be necessary to pre-test knowledge of vowel principles, syllabication, and accent clues. It is possible to simplify the pre-testing process by testing at alternate steps or even every third step. Nevertheless, the judicious use of this procedure of pinpointing children's starting levels in learning phonic generalizations should go a long way toward obviating the practice of overteaching phonics.

AN INTENSIVE - GRADUAL PHONIC APPROACH If the hypothesis is accepted that the use of pre-testing as a

26 20 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

diagnostic procedure will, in turn, improve the efficiency of phonic teaching, a method of instruction can be examined which is based on the assumption that the teacher has knowledge of each pupil's starting level for the generalizations she plans to teach. Because children's levels of progress will differ, individualized or small-group instruction will be required. Each child will need to be "plugged in" at the appropriate level ( the lowest level at which the child makes any errors or where he is unsure of his responses) and be taught by successive steps until he reaches the level at which he can identify unknown words either in isolation or in context. In addi- tion, he must learn a "plan of attack" for words that turn out to be exceptions to the principle being taught. Using an inductive approach, leading from the known to the unknown, children can be tauE.it any phonic principle as soon as they have in their sight vocabularies three of four words to which that principle applies.If one endorses a synthetic method of phonic analysis, no knowledge of sight words is needed anda deductive approach is dictated whereby the teacher presents the "rule" which children are expected to apply in their attackon unknown words. An analytic method of phonic analysis, termed "gradual" by Gurren and Hughes and "intrinsic" by Chall,- is recommended in this article for the following reasons: 1. It permits reading to be taught from the very beginningas a thought-getting process. The child's first encounter with the "code' is for the purpose of expressing and/or securing ideas or meanings. 2. The content of reading materials need not include nonsense words or nonsense stories necessitated when phonic principles are taught first and then used as limiting factors in the choice of vocabulary. 3. Unknown words can be identified quickly and efficiently since letter-by-letter sounding is not required. The pronunciation of consonants in isolation is unnecessary, thereby eliminating the unavoidable addition of an "uh" or schwa sound to each consonant phoneme as it is sounded when a synthetic method is employed. 4. Phonic principles are applicable to multisyllabic words, making it possible for children to readily identify the longer words met at the intermediate grade levels. 5. It lends itself to an intensive treatment to thesame degree as a synthetic method. An analytic method is characterized by a gradual introduction of phonic principlesas readiness is WINKLEY 21

established and a need exists for pupils to learn them,but this does not prevent an intensive teaching of the principlesonce they are introduced. How does the analytic method work? In orderto ensure com- plete mastery of a phonic generalization, twelvesteps, the first ten of which correspond to the pre-testing steps described earlier, are recommended: 1. Teach the children to recognize the grapheme, representingthe phoneme. as it appears in words and other printed material. 2. Help the child to hear and isolate the individual soundas it is heard in a word pronounced by the teacher and repeated by the child. 3. Teach the children to recognize the sound each time itis heard in a word pronounced by the teacher. Have the children indicate insome way the words in which the sound is heard. 4. Teach the letter name. Durrell's continual insistence of the value of teaching letter names is based on the fact that the sound represented by the letter is heard when the names of twenty-two of the twenty-six letters of the alphabet are pronounced. In addition to the five vowels that stand for their letter names when they represent their long sounds. Durrell has identified eight long "e"con- sonants (b, c. d, g, p. t, v, and z) six short "e" consonants (f, 1, m, n, s. and x), two long "a" consonants (j and k), as well as "r", whosenames, when pro- nounced, contain one phoneme that each graphemerepresents. 5. Teach the children to recognize the grapheme that stands fora particular sound heard in spoken words. 6. Have the children identify words containing the grapheme representingthe spe- cific sound heard in a spoken word. 7. Show pictures of objects whose names begin with the particularphoneme(s) being learned. Teach children to select the grapheme representing each particu- lar phoneme. Now the child does not hear the sound butmust "think" it in order to select the correct visual symbol. 8. Have the children write the graphemes representing particularphonemes heard in words pronounced by the teacher. Now, the visual symbol isnot seen and the children must recall it in order to match it with the auditory stimulus. The children should, at this point, be led to makea generalization regarding the phonic principle. Based on the examples presented, what "clue" would help them with the sound if they came to a word they didn't know? 9. Have the children recall words which contain the sound represented by the grapheme. List them on the board so that the sound-symbol relationshipcan be reinforced. W. Provide practice in phoneme substitution or applying the clue to unknown words in isolation. Start with known words if using phoneme substitution. Have children "think off" a particular phoneme and "think on" the sound represented by the letter being substituted. If a vowel principle, syllabication clue, or accent generalization is being taught. the children should be helped to apply the clue to unknown words. 11. Since context reading provides an additional help and is our ultimate goal for using word recognition skills, practice should be provided in identifyingun- known words appearing in sentences and paragraphs. 12. Teach children a procedure for identifying words that are exceptions to the clue taught. Since the reader does not know a word is an exception when he meets it in his reading, he will first apply the clue he has learned. If he doesn't recognize the word as one in his speaking vocabulary, he should be taught to try other sounds. It is hopec that he will ultimately derive the correct 22 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

pronunciation. If not, he should be taught to use the dictionary. Vhenever a word pronunciation does not "trigger- meaning. the dictionary becomes the last resort. It is important that any principles or clues taught for use in single syllable words will also be applicable in multisyllabic words. When unknown words of more than one syllable are met, the chil- dren must be taught clues for dividing words into syllables as well as generalizations for determining which syllableis accented. There is a definite need for teaching accent generalizations since the vowel principles are applicable in accented syllables only. The vowel sound ift the unaccented syllable can be expected to be that represented by the schwa symbol (a) or a short "u", and occasion- ally by a short "i" sound.

COMPARISON OF ANALYTIC WITH SYNTHETIC APPROACH Although the research reviewed by Chall (1967) and earlier by Curren and Hughes (1965) was admitted to be "shockingly inconclusive** (Chall ), the conclusion was reached that a "code- emphasis," "systematic," or Intensive approach, which was really a synthetic method, produced results superior to those of methods comparable to the one described previously. What is a synthetic method? It is an approach whe'reby children are taught to sound letters and letter combinations singly and then blend these individ- ual sounds to form a word. For example, children would sound the word "tent" as "tuh", "e", "n", "tuh", since they had previously learned the sound represented by each letter ("t" says "tuh", for example.) By an analytic method, the generalization is made that the letter "t" usually represents the sound heard at the beginning of the key word -top." In attacking the word "tent" the child would be taught to recall a known word resembling this word"went," perhapsthen "think off" the "w" and "think on" the "t" of "top" making "tent". There is no logical reason that an analytic method, which has been termeda "meaning-emphasis," "intrinsic,"or"gradual" approach, can not and should not also be an intensive, systematic code-emphasis approach. Although it must necessarily be gradual in its introduction of phonic principles, this fact does not preclude an intensive attack on teaching the clues, once they are introduced.

CONCLUSION It is recommended that teachers, thoroughly knowledgeable W1NKLEY 23

regarding clues to phoneme-grapheme relationships, first pre-test to determine their pupils' levels of understanding of phonic gener- alizations, and then begin at the point of breakdown to teach bya systematic inductive approach those generalizations for which the children already know a few sight words. This analytic method has undoubtedly failed to demonstrate superiority over synthetic methods in some studies chiefly because it has been poorly taught. This proposed code-meaning emphasis approach, introduced gradu- ally and intrinsically as a part of the overall reading program, should be taught systematically and intensively. It is hypothesized that such an approach would go a long way toward solving the decoding problems of readers of all ages.

REFERENCES Art ley, A. Sterl. Significant issues in the teaching of reading. In J. E. Frost (Ed.) 15588e5 and innovations in the teaching of reading. Glenview, Ill.:Scott, Foresman, 1967. Pp. ,192.194. Austin, Mary C. The torch lighters: tomorrow's teachers of reading. Cambridge, Mass.: Harvard University Press. 1961. Austin, Mary C.. and Morrison. C. The first r: the Harvard report on reading in elementary schools. New York: Macmillan, 1963. Chall. Jeanne. Learning to rend: the great debate. New York: McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1967. Clymer, T. The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 1963, 16, 252.258. Durrell, D. Assuring success in beginning reading. Paper presented at the First Conference of the Illinois Reading Council, Normal. Illinois, March 22, 1969. Emans, R. The usefulness of phonic generalizations above the primary level. The Reading Teacher, 1967, 20, 419-425. Emans, R. When two vowels go walking and other such things. The Reading Teacher, 1967. 21. 262.269. Fay, L. The classroom teacher as an innovator; experimenter in reading. Paper presented at meeting of Northern Illinois Reading Council, De Kalb. Illinois. Octo- ber 15, 1968. Curren, Louise. and Hughes, Ann. Intensive phonics vs. gradual phonics in beginning reading: a review. The Journal of Echacational Research, 1965, 58, 339 346. Hull, Marion A. Phonics for the teacher of reading. De Kalb, Illinois: Northern Illinois University. (Unpublished programmed WAN Spachc, G. D. Reading in the elementary school. Boston: Allyn and Bacon. Inc., 1964. P. 283. Strang. Ruth: flow successful readers learned. Paper presented at Second World Congress on Reading, Copenhagen, Deark. August 2. 1968. Weintraub. S. A critique of a review of phonic studies. The Elementary School Journal, 1966. 67. 3440.

30 The Role of Phonics in Teaching Reading

JACK BAGFORD'

IN THE PAST,there has been much main types,. analytic approaches and controversy among teachers concern- synthetic approaches. ing the value of phonics to the teaching Theanalytic approachesto teaching of reading.Some have argued that phonics are those approaches in which phonics has limited usefulness because the teacher first teaches a limited num- of the relatively unphonetic character ber of sight words, possibly 75 to 100, of the English language; others have and then teaches the reader to utilize felt that such knowledge is not only a these known words toinferletter- useful but necessary part of the read- sound associations for unknown words. ing program.Fortunately. there are In presenting phonics analytically, a now some limited agreements about teacher might teach a number of sight the use of phonics in the teaching of words. including,for example,bat. reading. bill.and brig. Then byanalycingthe There is no longer any serious doubt words and noting that they all begin aboutwhetherphonics content should with the same sound, thestudents be included in the reading program : learn the letter-sound association forb. teachers and reading specialists almost Subsequently. when unknown words universally accept it as an indispensa- suchasbasket.bitter,andbundle ble tool for teaching children to read. occur in his reading. the student will Disagreements concerning phonics are know the1,sound and will thus have a still very much in evidence, but they clue to hell: him identify the words. have now centered largely on questions Thesynthetic- approachesto teach- of 1) how phonics should be presented, ing plionicare those approaches in 2) what content should be included, which theteacherfirstteaches the and 3) when should it he emphasized. sounds which certain letters represent Though space will not permit a pene- and then teaches the pupil to combine trating analysis of these questions, an ( or synthesize) the sounds into words. attempt will be made to raise some Following one of the syntheticap- basic issues regarding the manner in proaches. a teacher would first present whichthese questions may be an- the sounds represented by the printed swered and to provide reading teachers form of the letters: for example, p usu- with guidelines for action until results ally sounds likepith; asounds likea; of research and practice answer them andtsounds likehuh. When the more adequately. sounds are blended, the word is pat. Later on, when thestudentmeets How should phonics be taught words like pen and pig. he will know Historically, there have been several that they begin with the p sound and different approaches to the teaching of thus he will have a clue to their identi- phonics.In recent decades it has been fication. customary to categorize them into two Since the early 1930's, those who fa- Reading and Realism, IRAProccedinrs Vol, 13, Part 1, 1969, 82.87. BAGFORD 25

vored analytic approaches have been in entirelycontrary tofact. Children the majority, but there has been con- taughtbysyntheticmethods may tinuous support for the synthetic ap- over-learn some word-analysis habits proaches. Recently,sincelinguistic whichlater militate against reading scholarshavefocused attentionon growth: they may learn to concentrate "breaking the code" as the prime em- so intently on word analysis that atten- phasis for early reading instruction. tion to meaning is impeded; they may the synthetic approaches have gained acquirehabitsthatslow down the remarkably in their popularity. reading rate and thus make it difficult Beginning with the Boston studies in to comprehend rapidly; they may grow the mid-fifties (7) and continuing with to believe that reading is a process of the Sparks-Fay study (11), the Bear drill on seemingly meaningless sounds study(3), theBliesmer-Yarborough and thus grow to dislike reading.If study (4), and the usoi First and in their zeal for phonics mastery, pri- Second Grades Studies (8), evidence mary grade teachers have overemphas- has been presented to support the con- ized habits that will need to he un- tention that synthetic approaches pro- learned at a later date, thenit does vide a more rapid start in reading than seem probable that children taught by analytic approaches do. the more moderate or the more ana- Chall (5) recently presented a cal- lytic approach would become the better villein case forthose reading. pro- readers. grams which make use of the synthetic With present knowledge teachers approaches. Under a grant from the still must rely somewhat on their own Carnegie Foundation, she has made a judgment about what is best.Itis searching analysis of the major re- comforting to note that children do search findings related to problems of learn to read by any of several meth- beginning reading instruction.One of ods. At this point in time a reasona- her major conclusions was that "code ble course seems to he 1) teach letter- emphasis" approaches (synthetic ap- sound associations relatively early in proaches) proved superior, at least in the reading progran, with a synthetic the primary grades, to "meaning em- emphasis while at the same time con- phasis" approaches (analytic ap- sidering interest and comprehension as proaches). primegoals and prime guidesfor There does appear to be some ques- teaching procedures. and 2) after the tion about whether early gains made child has progressed sufficiently in his by synthetic approaches can be main- word recognition ability, shift the em- tained as the children progress through phasis rather rapidly to comprehension the reading program (Ii).Further while at the same time trying to foster longitudinal research is needed on this high interest in reading. very important point, but one would think that intermediate grade teachers What phonic content and curriculumworkers couldfind should be taught ways of maintainingreactinggains Through the years much informa- achieved by primary grade teachers, tionhas beencompiled concerning almost regardless of the manner in speech sounds and their written repre- which the gains were achieved. sentation&It is a generally accepted This assumption, however, may be fact that some of the informationis 32 26 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

helpful in teaching reading and some to the teaching of reading.It seems of it is not.In fact, this matter is im- plausible to assume that some children plied by the way phonics is defined. learn better from a method which em- Phoneticsis generally defined as the phasizes a whole-word approachto science of speech sounds, whilephonics word recognition while others probably is defined as that portion of phonetics learn better from a method which em- which is applicable in teaching children phasizes sound-symbol correspondence. to read.For the purpose of teaching To put it another way, some children reading, it is neither feasible nor desir- probably learn better through visual able to try to teach all that is known means whileotherslearn better about phonetics. through auditory means. Generally One of the basic reasons for includ- speaking, teaching materials are de- ing any phonetic knowledge in a read- signed with the underlying assumption ingprogram istoimprovethe that all children learn equally well with efficiency of the teaching process. To all modalities.This assumption may accomplish this good, programs should or may not be correct.Thus it seems concentrate on content which occurs logical to advise that whenever a child frequently in reading, is easy to teach, is experiencing difficulty with learning and is relatively regular in its applica- to read, the teacher should investigate tion. the possibility that he may be empha Studies by Clymer (6), Fry (10), sizing the least effective modality for Bailey (2), and Emans(9)have in- the child in question. vestigated the question of "what con- Research studies that arrive at gen- tent" by making use of one or more of eralizations about which method works the preceding criteria in judging the best for large groups of children miss a value of selectedphoniccontent. very basic point :i.e.. methods which They have found that at least some of producesignificantlyhigher mean the phonic content that is usually in- scores for the total group do not neces- cluded in reading programs is not ade- sarily work best for each individual quatelyjustifiedbythesecriteria. student in the group.Certain individ- These studies need to be expanded and uals may profit more from a method amplified into other pertinent areas, which has been shown to produce sig- but they do provide some substantial nificantly lower mean scores than an- data which'should prove extremely other.Teachers should recognize this helpful as teachers concern themselves possibility and adjust their teaching with problems of what phonic content accordingly. shouldandshouldnot be included in Itseems likely that some words are the reading program. moreeasilylearnedbyaphonic Some basic considerations method than by a sight ;Ittihad. while In determining the proper role of others are more easily learned by the phonicsina reading program one sightmethod. High frequency, but ir- needs to consider underlying factors regularly sounded, words probably are which relate to this role.Some of the more efficientlytaught bya sight basic considerations follow. methodwhilephoneticallyregular Children differintheir abilityto words and words which contain easily Following one of the synthetic ap- learned sounds probably arebetter

33 BAGFORD 27 taught by a phonic method.Learning do a good jo of teaching readingre- the word recognition skills is a step in gardless of how good or how bad the a developmental process, one of the method might be.When selecting a goals of which is to know a large num- particular phonics progcam or deter- ber of words by sight.Accomplishing mining degree of emphasison content this goal by the most efficient method or methodology, one of the key factors is important. Sometimes the most ef- to be considered should be what the ficient method is determined by thena- teachers think about it. ture of the word itself. !lamest may not be directly related A given child may be able to utilize to method.Itis doubtful that one a .sound- oriented approach betterat method is inherently more interesting one age than another.The concept of than ;mother.Enthusiastic teachers reading readiness suggests that there is can take very dull content and make an an optimum time in the developmental interesting lesson out of it.Others process for a child to learn any given can take what seems to be very inter- skill.Presumably, attempts to mach a esting material and createpure drudg- skill prior to this optimum time will ery for children. Whether a method is prove unsuccessful and may even cause interesting is probably less relatedto emotional or psychological prithlems method than it is to other factorsre- which seriously retard normal growth. lated to the teaching-learning situation. Also, itis assumed that if instruction Two factors which influence pupil is postponed until later than this opti- interest are variety of presentationand mu ;line, the skill involved is not as appropriateness of teaching level.If readily learned as it would have been presentationsarevariedwithina at the optimum time. method, interestis not likely to be In a like manner, each child may lacking.Likewise, if a child is givena have an optimum time in his total de- learning challenge, but ata level where velopment for learning phonics con- he has a relatively good chancefor tent.For some, phonic readiness may success, he will seldom lose interest. be achieved relatively early in school The important point related to phonics while others may take considerably is that approaches probably shouldnot longer.In presenting phonics content, be accepted or rejected because ofin- teachers should consider the natural terest or lack of it.Rather, effective growth patterns of the pupils. approaches should he selected foruse Flow the teacher feels aboutthe and then adjustments made inthe teaching procedure which he is follow- teaching situation to maintaina high ing seems to make a difference in the interest level. effectivene.is of the teaching method. I f children can learn to read by any of Guidelines for the reading teacher several approaches, which apparently In teaching phonics, the major task they can, then how the teacher feels which confronts today's reading about the method may well be one of teacher is how to maintaina proper the most important factors in deter- balance between attentionto phonics mining its success.If the teacher is and attention to other important read- philosophicallycommitted to the ing goals.The myriad of research re- method he is using, then he is likely to sults and the verbal wranglings of

34 "Mr 7

28 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES reading "experts" are likely to confuse that emphasis can be adjusted to best the average teacher about the proper serve the total reading program. course of action as he performs the The teacher is the key person in de- daily tasks of teaching reading.The termining the success of a reading pro- followingare suggestedas broad gram.Whether children learn better guidelinestofollow as teachersat- by one method than another is largely tempt to determine the role of phonics determined by the skill and enthusiasm in the teaching of reading. of the teacher.In recent years, re- Phonics contentis taught so that search has consistently shown that the children have a tool to identify words quality of the teacher in the classroom which are known in the spoken form is the most iniportant variable relating but not in the printed form.All deci- to how well the pupils in a class learn sions concerning the use of phonics to read.Effective functioning in such should reflect this purpose.Teachers a key role requires that a teacher know should regularly ask themselves as much as possible about 1) phonics whetherthephoniccontentbeing and research related to phonics, 2) the taught and the methods being em- total reading process. and 3) the pu- ployed in teaching it contribute to the pils' reading abilities and needs. accomplishment of this major purpose. Acting in terms of the preceding If not, the teacher should adjust ac- guidelines leads one directly to the cordingly. next. Teachers should take an active Phonics is but one aspect of word part in determining the role of phonics recognition: word recognitionis but inthereading program. On the one goalofthereading program. whole, modern-day teachers are well- Phonics isbest used in conjunction trained, competent people who are ca- with other word recognition skills.As pable of determining the reading needs a child learns to read, he gradually of pupils and adjusting the program to learns several ways to identify words. nueet these needs.Caring for individ- Ideally, he learns them in such a man- ual differences is a constant job, and ner so that he can coordinate and com- only teachers are in a position to know bine their use as he attacks unknown these needs well enough to adjust in- words. The ability to use sound-sym- structional procedures to meet them : bol relationships is one of the more im- teachers should be encouraged to do portant reading skills, but it is just one so. and should be so considered. This guideline means, for example. The second aspect of this guideline that teachers should adjust content and has to do with the relationship of word methodforchildren who are slow recognition skills to the total reading learners or fast learners; for children program. Word identificationtech- who have speech and hearing prob- niques should be taught in a manner lems ; and for those who learn better that facilitates, not hampers, the attain- through visual means than through au- ment of other important reading goals. ditory means.It means that teachers Intensive attention to phonics can seri- need to recognize and adjust for the ously impair progress toward goals of fact thatsome !Atoniccontent is speed, interest, and meaning; teachers learned by all pupils without any direct need to recognize this possibility so teaching. 35 BAGFORD 29

itis recognized that adjusting for jug to methods and materials and to individualdifferencesisan age-old utilizetheir knowledge inadjusting problem that has no easy solutions. theirprocedurestotheindividual Nevertheless, with the wide variety of needs in their own classrooms. high quality materials available to to- Guidelines for making these adjust- day's teachers, intensive efforts toward ments are provided. recognizing differences and providing for them can produce rich benefits for REFERENCES the pupils. I.Bagford, Jack. Phonics:Its Rote in Teaching Reading.Iowa City :Sernoll, Relatively speaking. phonics should 1967. be taught fairly early in the reading 2.Bailey, Mildred Hart."The Utility of program. Basically, the two major Phonic Generalizations in Grades One Through Six."ReadingTeacher, 20 goals of a reading program are word (February 1967), 413-418. recognition and comprehension. These 3.Bear, David. "PhonicsFor First Grade: A Comparison of Two Meth- goals can hardly he separated. but for ods,"Elementary School Journal. 59 instructional purposesitis probably (April 19591, 394-402. better to place the heavy emphasis 4.Blies:net% Emery P., and Betty H. Yar- on borough. "A Comparison of Ten Dif- one and then the other. Early in the ferent Beginning Reading Programs in process of learning to read', word rec- First Grade," Phi Delta Kappan. 46 ognition (June 1965), 500-504. (including phonics)should 5.Chill, Jratute.Learning to Read: The receive major attention: and as prog- Great !)chafe.New York :McGraw- ress is made. the emphasis should be 11111, 1967. Clymer, Theodore. "TheUtilityof shifted to comprehension. Phonics Generalizations in the Primary Grades," Reading Teacher, 16 (Febru- Summary ary 19631.252 -258. 7.Durrell. Donald D., (Ed.)."Success in Phonics has an extremely important First Grade Reading," Journal of Edu- role to play in the teaching of reading. cation.BostonUniversity,(February In this paper it is assumed that phonic 19581. 1-48. 8.Dykstra, Robert.Continuation ofthe analysisisbest used in conjunction CoordinatingCenterforFirst-Grade withotherword-identificationtech- Reading lnstructi Pray raMS. USOE. Project Number 6-1651.Minneapolis: niques for the purpose of unlocking University of Minnesota. 1967. words which are known in their spo- 9.Ematts.Robert. "The Usefulness of ken forum but unknown in their written Phonic Generalizations Above the Pri- tttaryLevel,"Reading Teacher,20 forum.it is known that the pupils can February 19671, 419-425. learn to read by am' of a number of 10.Fry, Edward. "A Frequency Approach methods. toPhonics," Elementary English. 41 Thus teachers, rather than (November 1964), 759-765. method, are the most important varia- 11.Sparks, Paul E., and Leo C. Fay. "An ble in the teaching process.Teachers Evaluation of Two Methods of Teach- ing Reading." Elementary .School Jour- are encouraged to know research relat- nal, 57 (April 1957), 386-390.

36 A Critical Look at Instruction in Word Recognition at the Elementary Level

JUANITA LEWIS.

WORD RECOGNITION, the same as any this activity is the give the child prac- other term in the field of reading, can tice in using picture clues to identify become ambiguous when one examines words.Most readiness programs and it in the many different pieces of litera- preprimers offer opportunities for the ture of a reading program. For the child to develop this skill.It, too, is purpose of this paper togrd recognition frequently supplemented with oral and refers to the child's ability to recognize printed context. a word and its meaning in context. Printed Context.The purpose of Before a child can recognize a word, printed context is two-fold : he must first have the opportunity and 1. To give the child practice in using skills necessary to identify that word, printedcontexttoidentifynew or someone must tellit to him. Ac- words. and tually, one could hardly talk about To give the child practice in com- word recognition without getting into prehension. word identification skills.Therein lies Oral and picture context used along the ambiguity of word recognition; it with printed context seems to be a is a result of word identification. means to an endwhat end? Reading ! Instruction in word recognition Phonetic Analysis Consider the ways a child learns to Varying degrees of phonetic analysis recognize or identify a word.There appear in reading programs. A con- are four major areas to be considered sensus seems to be that children need as far as instruction in word recogni- help with at least three skills : tion is concerned.They are 1) Con- Identifyinglikenessesanddiffer - text, 2) Phonetic Analysis, 3) Struc- ences in beginning sounds.Attention tural Analysis, and 4) Comprehension. is devoted here to giving children skill Context in sounding out the beginning of a Anyone who has examined one or word to enable them to identify words. more of the available reading programs Identifyinglikenessesanddiffer- being used by children today is aware ences in sounds in the medial posi- of the fact that attention is given to tion.Attention is devoted here to giv- helping children through three diffe- ing children skill in sounding out the rent kinds of context. "middle" of a word of syllable to help Oral Context.The purpose of oral them identify words. context is to give the child practice in Identifyinglikenessesanddiffer- using verbal clues to identify words. ences in ending sounds.Attention is Frequently,itissupplemented with devoted here to giving children skill in picture and/or printed context, struc- sounding out the ending of a word to tural and/or phonetic analysis, or in help them identify words. some cases any miracle that comes to Structural analysis the teacher's mind. Again, almost any method of teach- Picture Context.The purpose ofing reading that could be mentioned Forging Ahead in Reading,IRA Proceedings,vol. 12, Part 1, 1968, 55.59.

t) LEWIS 31 gives attention to structural analysis. to helping children develop comprehen- This skill enables the child to take a sion through listening to oral context. word apart and sound out the parts to Printed Context. No method ig- help him identifyit.Children are nores printed context and the role it helped withfive different kinds of playsincomprehension.The most structural analysis : common use made of printed context Root 2vords. It is believed that to is. find a root word in a strange word will I. Silent Re-Iding. The child is given help a child identify the strange word. a purpose ;he examines the printed Prefixes. After the child has learned to identify the root word, he. context silently and through compre- identifies the prefix to further aid him hension fulfills his purpose. in identifying the strange word. 2. Oral Reading. Again the child is Suffixes.Again the child is encour- given a purpose for reading;*hereads aged to find the root word plus the orally and fulfills his purpose. suffix to help him identify the strange 3. Skimming.Sometimes the child word. is given a purpose and is called upon Compound words. Childrenare to skim the material with comprehen- also given practice in discovering that sion to fulfill his purpose. the strange word may consist of two 4.Drill.The many different types words that they already know. of drill employed in helping children Syllabication. Attention is devoted with comprehension would be too nu- to helping children develop skill in di- merous to list at this time.Suffice it viding words into syllables as a means to say, the end result should be com- of identifying strange words. prehension of the material which the children have handled. Comprehension A critical look at instruction The advocatesof any method of in word recognition teaching reading would strongly agree that their approach to reading leads to Now for a critical look at instruction comprehension ; otherwise, what is the in word recognition.As was pointed purpose of context, phonetic analysis. out earlier in this paper, word recogni- orstructuralanalysis? Some are tion is the ability to recognize a word happy if the child understands a word. and its meaning in a particular con- Others are not happy unless the child text. It was also pointed out that understands the whole sentence. there are four major areas in instruc- Many are not satisfied until the child tion in word recognition: 1) Context, comprehends awhole paragraph, a 2) PhoneticAnalysis,3) Structural whole story, or the main idea of a se- Analysis, and 4) Comprehension. lection.Literature shows that author- Take a critical look at these four areas ities expect the child to understand which have become sacred to people in what he reads.Most authorities agree the tick! of reading. that it is from the printed context that Context. comprehension conies. Picture Context.Let's look first at Picture Context. Programs devote picture context.Couldn't we devote a sizable amount of time to helping more time to printed context rather children develop comprehension with than picture context?When I exa- picture symbols. mine a basal series, I never fail to ask Oral Context. Programs devote time myself this question.I will agree that

38 32 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES inthebeginning children do enjoy when he is reading independently.In SON( E picture reading, but through summary, I suppose my major criti- observing children (hiringmy class- cism is the fact that teachers using de- room teaching,experiencesIfound tailed oral context (luring a silent or time after time that children became oral reading lesson can be interfering !Hired and restless when asked to read with comprehension rather than help- a picturetodeath. Agreed,s(nne ing the child. method needs to be used to "put words Solution?Tell the child the word into the mouths of poor readers." but now ; help him later with his problem! even they rebel after a while.I have Printed Context.Couldn't we have nothing butsympathy forthesix- more words in beginning reading, and year-old child nho has to go througha couldn't we include more than just reading readiness program where he is one-syllable words? asked to read pictures andmore pic- Sometimes I wonder what children tures and more pictures. think of our schools when we take Those who arc first grade teachers them to a reading circle, build up their have probably had the same experience interestinreading, and thenhand I had in first grade reading: The child them a story with inch limited vocabu- who can go through the whole story lary that it couldn't possibly compete reading every detail in the picture plus with library books they are reading or adding numerous ones ofhis own, are having read to them or with their when asked to read the selection, looks conversations duringrecess. Please at you with a question in his mind and don't misunderstand me.I am not ad- asks, "What selection?" vocating a recording of the conversa- Solution?Publishing companies and tionatrecess timefollowed by a teachers must concentrate less on pic- printed chart from which the child ture context. reads and rereads and rereads what a Oral Context.I-low about oral con- group had to say during a particular text ?Does it help the child or not ? recess in a particular situation.I am Have yon ever sat in the back of the not advocating this approach at all.I room and listenedto afirstgrade have tried that approach and it didn't teacher teach reading?If you have, I work forme. believe you will agree with me that Solution?I would like to see more much of the oral context given by the reading readiness programs devote at- teacher is not what we are after at all. tention to the skills basic to heginning For example: when a child is reading reading and less attention to picture a sentence and stops in the middle of reading of everyday situations or nur- the sentence because he can't identify a sery rhymes. word, it would seem to me that he As for two-or more-syllable words, I needs to know that word immediately recall from my own classroom experi- for comprehension, otherwise, he loses ence that Washington was a word that his train of thought and begins to word all of my first grade children could re- call. In my opinion, teachers don't cognize when they saw itin printed help by such oral context as "It begins formyes, even the children in the with the same sound as ball, bird" or low group. "We had that word yesterday, remem- Solution?Put more words in pri- ber ?" or "That's one of our new mary reading material, and let's not be words."I'm concerned, too, when the afraid to try the big ones. teacher gives the child oral context Phonetic Analysis that he will not have access to later I have several questions concerning 39 LEWIS 33

phonetic analysis: vowel.I remember hearing a saying Beginning Sounds. The reading one time "get what you need bnt need readiness program? what you get.""Needing what you Why spend timeinour reading get" brings me to my next comment. readiness books with activitiescom- I am a little disturbed when '1 go into pletely unrelated to beginning reading? classrooms and see teachers using pho- It seems to me that by the timea netic programs that are not in any way child completes his reading readiness tied to the immediate needs of the program he should be on the road to child. reading.If this is true, it would seem I'm sure that teachers are using these highly advertised, colorful, pho- to me thatour readingreadiness should get him ready ! netic programs with good intentions. How about teaching beginning I would just like to mention thatac- sounds? cording to the laws of learning, chil- This makes sense to me, and since I dren tend to forget what they don't use have worked with kindergarten and and, secondly, that what they learn first grade children, I know thatmany should be used immediately in a mean- of them can successfully work with be- ingful situation. ginning sounds. Not only from obser- Iwould be happy to see a structural vation do I know this, but I also know program in phonics if it is a program it from examining and conductingre- that goes along with the reading pro- search.I have already heard the ar- gram the teacher is ttsing at that time gument that all children cannot do this and if it concentrates on the high fre- work at kindergarten level.Is this quency needs of the children.Why any reason for not letting those who should a child have to drill on a spe- can move forward?If we hold back cific sound if he is not having trouble for this reason, we can't have reading . with it? at first grade level or geometry at high Solution?Give children practice in school, can we? hearing likenesses and differences in Solution? Givethechildmore words they are using now. Don't wait work with letter forms and sounds. untilthat magic period, the second Medial Sounds. Why can't children grade. be exposed to them earlier? Structural analysis At first grade levelit bothers me I have three criticisms concerning that we can'tstart working earlier structural analysis : with certain phonetic analysis Why have children find little words skills--to be specific, with vowels.I in big words?I am concerned about don't recall too many words the child programs that encourage children to meets in his recreational reading where find little words in big words.This he wouldn't he confronted with vowels. procedure forms a BAD habit that I believe that if we would work with children must break sooner than one the vowels soon enough at early first expects. 7 can see no advantage in grade level, we could help children be- this acti,ity whatsoever because the come more independent in their read- English language is not that consistent. ing habits.After all, aren't we sup- The minute the childstarts having posed to give the child what he can put two-syllable words he is going to run to use immediately? And surely one into trouble. can't deny that even in the first word What about configuration?I am in the preprimer the child meets a equally concerned about the series that 40 34 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES wastes the child's time by having him ask for such specific details that chil- look at a printed word and visualize a dren lose the meaning of the whole picture of that word that, probably. is story.Some teachers fail to help chil- completelyunrelatedtothatword. dren see that the material they are When you have a child look at the reading silently or orally must make word been, think of it with an outline sense. Some of our teachers have re- around it, add wheels to it, and make signed themselves to the fact tbat chil- smoke come nut of the top, you're tak- drensimply can'tuse expression ing his mind off the word. Again. very therefore. "I'm not going to push the rarely haveIseen childrens books issue." Some teachers have become where,firstofalltheword was obsessed with rate at the expense of framed: and I never recall seeing one comprehension.And some teachers where the word's had wheels on them. have not themselves fully understood 117hy (lo we ask children to divide what reading actually is. words in syllables?Itis well and .Solution?Provide practice for chil- good to have children divide words drenthatwillhelp them critically intosyllables.butmany programs think and critically read so they will never go beyond this step.Children learn to demand meaning from printed should then be taught 'to look at each material. syllable as an independent word that would be identified the same as any Summary small word. As I stated at the beginning of this Solution?Have the child take the paper. word recognition refers to the word apart in a logical manner and use child's ability to recognize a word and these parts to identify the word. its meaning in a particular context. Comprehension We must realize that recognizing a word is only part of the job.Unless a Some of our programs make little, ifchild understands what he reads. we any,provision forcomprehension. Some of our teacher's manuals ask are not preparing him to cope with the printed material he will be meeting as such regurgitative-type questions that he continues his education.Let's not the only thing a child has to do is to be so involved in instruction in word repeat a sentence whetherit makes recognition that we ourselves forget to sense. Some of the drills provided by take a critical look at it and the role it publishing companies and by teachers plays in helping children learn to read!

41 Word Recognition for the Junior High School

KATHLEEN CLAYTON WHEN considering the topic of word Let us consider the various word recognition skills for. the junior high recognitiontechniques already men- school, one has to think about what tioned that have been taught in the ele- word recognition techniques these stu- mentary school and that may need re- dents have been taught in the elemen- viewing orreteaching in the junior tary grades.Next, some type of eval- high school. nation or diagnosis, should be planned SightWords. Mostprofessional to determine if these junior high stu- books on reading and basalreader dents have these skills.After adminis- manuals have descriptive material on tering a group or an individual instru- sight words in the discussion of teach- ment, the teacher has to plan the pro- ing word recognition.Sight words gram around the needs of the students. are those words that are recognized as Some groups of students may have lit- a whole.The sight word is the word tle need for extensive work in wordre- that is told to the child and is the basis cognition techniques at the junior high for teaching phonetic and structural level because their skills are already analysis.The child becomes familiar welldeveloped. Some groupswill with words already in his listening and need the kind of program that involves speaking vocabularies by having the a heavy concentration on word recog- printed forms of the words presented nition techniques. to him as wholes in meaningful con- What is word recognition?It has text.The sight word is used in a meaningfulsituationanddiscussed. been defined as the "identification ofa word by means of a context clueor Then there is the need for this word to skillin analysis of the word form" be used frequently in different settings so thatthe word isrecognizedin- (2). Wordrecognitioninvolves a number of techniques: the use of sight stantly and spontaneously. words, the use of phonetic analysis, the Phonetic analysis.Phonetic analy- use of structural analysis, the use of sis is a term that is a source of confu- context, and the use of the dictionary. sion in discussions on the teaching of . Wordrecognitionskillsarethose reading. We talk about phonetics, methods or techniques used by readers phonics, and phonetic analysis. to identify, to pronounce, or to recall Phoneticsisthescienceofspeech words.Before the reader can attach sounds. Phonicsisthe scienceof meaning to a word, he must be able to speech sounds as applied to reading. identifyit. Learning to read and Phonetic analysis is the analysis of a being able to read requires wordrec- word intoits phonetic elementsfor ognition skills.Johnson has said: pronunciation purposes (2).Bamman says, "The recognition of symbols and The ultimate aim of the reading pro- is grain in relationto word recognition the sounds which they represent ability, should he the spontaneous recog- known as phonetic analysis (1)." nition of virtually every word. Accom- What we are to present or teach at a panying this ability to react effortlessly grade levelwill be dependent upon to the majority of words should be a reservoir of word analysis skills to un- what the student already knows about lock the few unfamiliar words which phonics. Phonicsispartof every are met from time to time (5). sound reading program.The junior Forging Ahead in Reading, IRA Proceedings, Vol. 12, Part 1, 1968,59-62.

42 36 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES high school program needs to be con- should be acquainted.These are sin- cerned with phonics and phonetic anal- gle-vowel letters as a as in cat or o as ysisto the extentthatthe student in go.The student needs to under- should be able to use phonics as a stand that these single-vowel symbols word recognition technique. can be used to represent variant single The teaching of phonetic analysis vowel sounds. He needs to know that proceeds from the known to the un- two-vowel letters may be used to rep- known or from the simple to the com- resentvariantsingle vowel sounds; plex.Teach those phonetic principles e.g., oo as in book or food:.ea as in that are more generally and universally tea. tear. or read.The third general appliedbefore movingtolessfre- type is the knowledge of the dipthongs quently used analytic skills.The de- or two-vowel letters that are used to velopment of skills in phonetic analysis represent two-vowel sounds blended to is based on phonetic understandings. form one speech sound ;e.g.,oitin The student must have a knowledge of mouse. the sounds that are used in our Eng- Theunderstandingsofsilentness lish language and of the symbols that and variability of consonants and vow- stand for those sounds.It is generally els are basic ().Double consonants acceptedthat we have about forty- at the end of a word represent one threeseparate sound units or pho- sound ;e.g.,sell.tall. Words with nemes in our language. Each of these double consonants followed by a vowel phonemes is either a vowel or a conso- are likely to have two syllables, e.g., nant sound. We have only twenty-six middle.happen. Silent consonant written symbols in traditional ortho- symbols in a word may have a mean- graphy to represent these sounds. We ing function. The student should know have more sounds than symbols, and or be taught the hard and soft sounds some symbols must be used to repre- of c and q and how he can determine sent more than one sound.This situa- these for pronunciation and meaning tionnaturally makesitdifficultto purposes. teach phonetic analysis. lie should know that vowel letters The student should have learned to are sometimes silent andthatthey associate sounds with various types of have afunction. One shimldalso consonant and vowel symbols. He know that each vowel letter represents should know the single consonant let- several vowel sounds. Final c on a ters that are used to represent a single word is usually silent and usually pro- consonant sound. He should know or longsthesoundofthepreceding be taught consonant blends.Where vowel. If there are two vowels to- two or three consonantletters are gether in a word or a syllable, the first blended so closely that they are pro- vowel is usually long and the second duced almost as one sound; e.g., br, st, vowel is usuallysilent. Ifthere is scr. The special two-letter. consonant only one vowel in the syllable or word, symbols that are used to represent a the vowel is usually short unless it is singleconsonantsoundshould be at the end of the word.If the only known. The letters sit as in shirt or vowel letter in a word or syllable is shape,forexample,represent:me followed by r, the sound of the vowel sound that is different from that of ei- isusually affected by ther. The ther of the single letters in the symbol teacher must he carefulin teaching sh. these principles as rules that are al- There are three generaltypes of ways true.They must be taught as vowel symbols with which the student principles that generally apply to word 43 CLAYTON 37

analysis. but there are exceptions to Syllabication. Syllabicationisdi- the rules or principles.The student viding words into syllables for thepur- needs practice and opportunities for pose of identifying unknown words. applying these principles in his daily It functions as an aid in word recogni- reading. tionbyhelpingthestudentbreak Many good sources list the phonetic words into smaller units,pronounce principles that students should know these, blend, and thus recognize words and be able to use.Materials by Gray, in his speaking and listening vocabu- Betts, Thunman, Karlin, Spache, and laries.Syllabication 'helps students in others are most helpful in the teaching their spelling and writing.As most of phonetic analysis. students matureinreadinginthe Structural aalysis. Strnctnral upper elementary grades, they become analysis involves knowledge of com- increasinglydependent upontheir pound words: inflected forms: derived knowledge of syllables and lessupon forms including prefixes, suffixes, and letter phonics.The student needs to roots: syllabication: and contractions. understand that everysyllable must A compound word is a combination of contain a vowel sound. With this two distinct known words that are knowledge, he has a method of break- joined to form one word.Students ing words into divisions for prommcia- who do not recognize compounds are tion purposes. weak in word attack skills and have Principles of syllabicationcan be not learned to look at words for famil- foundintheprofessionalbooks of iar parts.Students who are troubled Spache, Batman, Karlin, Gray, and by compound words need specific in- Betts.Be careful of having students struction if they are to learn to work memorize syllabication principles. with compounds which are a part of Generalizations which are formulated our everyday reading materials in the by the students will bemore meaning- classroom. ful than memorized principles.These Inflected forms of words have to do generalizations should be kept as few withperson,case,gender, number, and as simple as possible.Emphasize tense, and comparison.Common in- thosegeneralizationswhich clearly flectional endings are -s. -es. -er, -est. help in pronouncing a word. and nsnally, thebasic Context clues.Contextual analysis meaning of an inflected form is con- or the use of context clues is identi- tained within the root word.Derived fying a new word by anticipation of the forms are those words which havepre- meaning or through the words and fixes or suffixes.These are both re- ideas adjacent to the new word (2). ferred to as affixes.This form differs Readers frequently rely upon thecon- from the inflected form in thatthe text to help them with unknown or affixes have meanings of theirown. new reading words.Students should Then these affixes are added toa root be encouraged to use context clues form the meaning of the total wordor as an identification technique. We do form may be entirely different. The need to teach students how to use con- meaning of the root form does not textual dues.Contextual clues should change. The addition of the affix is not be used alone to identify unknown what changes the meaning of the total words.Phonics and structural clues word or form.Developing skill in the need to be 'combined with the contex- recognition of derived forms serves tualclues wherepossible. Spache two purposes: expanding meaning vo- states that most context clues demand cabulary and gaining an understanding some degree ofinferentialthinking. of the origin, or history, of words (1). Such inferential thinking isan essen-

44 38 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

tial part of the reading process at all his ovm? maturity levels and should be strongly Does he use his oral language back- encouraged (7). ground as checkpoint for his anal- Jan-Tausch has indicated that words ysis of a printed form? are learned best when introduced in conjunction with meaningful context. Does he skip over unfamiliar words This remark applies to the teaching of or use aslurredpronunciation all word recognition skills. rather than attempttoanalyze them ? The dictionary.The dictionaryis used as an aid in identifying unknown Does he drop word endings (5) ? words met in reading and as a check Reading is a series of many skills in- on a pronunciation or a meaning tenta-termeshed with one another. It is a tively arrived at by the reader throughthinking process and not subject mat- the use of context clues, word-formter to be taught.Word recognition clues,andsystematicanalysis(4).techniques must be taught, evaluated, The dictionary is a storehouse of theand retaught where needed for effec- meanings of words but is a word recog-tive use and readingskilldevelop- nition helper. ment. Skills needed in using the dictionary as a word recognition technique are REFERENCES 1) being able to locate the word by I.Baconian, Henry A., Ursala Hogan, and opening the dictionary to the section in C. E. Greene.Reading Instruction in the which the word is contained and being Secondary School.New York :Long- mans, Green, 1961. able to apply alphabetical knowledge to 2.Betts, Emmett A.Foundations of Read- find the word ;2) being able to use ing Instruction.New York :American guide words as aids in the location of Book Co., 1957. words; 3) being able to identify the3. Caroline, Sister M., IHM. "Word Rec- ognition and Vocabulary Development," root form from alternate forms of the in J. Allen Figurel( Ed.). Reading and word : and 4) being able to pronounce Inquiry. Proceedings of the International the word (6). Reading Association, 10 (1965), 227-229. Evaluation of needs.As stated ear-4. Gray, WilliamS.On Their Own in Reading.Chicago: Scott, Foresman lier,in order to plan a program the Co., 1960. teacher must know what are the stu- 5.Johnson, Marjorie S."Evaluating the dent's needs.Different types of test- SecondarySchoolReadingProgram," ing or evaluative instruments can be Reading Instruction in Secondary Schools, Perspectives in Reading No. 2. used to determine these needs. Newark, Delaware :International Read- Standardized tests can be administered ing Association, 1964. 117-128. and compared with national norms. 6.Karlin,Robert. TeachingReadingin Individual infomial reading invento- Iligh School.Boston: Allyn & Bacon, ries can be administered where word Inc., 1964. 7.Spache, George D. Reading in the Ele- recognition skills of the student are mentarySchool. Boston :Allyn & carefully studied.Johnson has sug- Bacon, Inc.. 1964. gested pertinent questions on word rec- ognition skills as listed below : Does he see base words in affixed forms? Does he apply the final- e principle? Does he use the dictionary sponta- neously to get the pronunciation of a word he cannot analyze on 45 Word Attack Skills for the RetardedReader in College

MILDRED B. FORD

GOOD READERS not only must be able to 5.Learning recognize quickly and accurately skills in auditory the blendingand visually words they have met before but they synthesizing word parts torapidly pronounce or must also have at hand tools with recognize the word as a whole which they can determine the ; and mean- 6. Forming the habit of usingthe ings of new words.One of the most more analytical and the pronunciation important of these tools is the ability techniques when andonly when to pronounce the words because some- needed. times one knows a word when he Wordrecognition problemsare hears it spoken and yet does notrec- often found to be at the ognize it when he sees it root of the dif- in print. ficulty of disabled readers who fall into Principles of word recognition the descriptive categories of limiting disability and complex disability. The terms word recognition and College readers(retarded)fallinto word attack are used somewhat in- these areas ; some can be helped with terchangeably as appliedtoreading individualizedinstruction and some skills (5).Word recognitionis an have problems that are too difficultto important basic reading skill.It is in cope with in a classroom situation. word recognition skills that retarded readers most commonlyare deficient. Effective teaching techniques Even though comprehension is the pri- of word attack skills mary goal of reading instruction, word To determine the degree of difficulty recognition is prerequisite. the college reader has in being ableto We are primarily concerned with attackwords,systematicevaluation finding the method ofteaching that should be administered.The evalua- best advances the retarded pupil inau- tion techniques must be informalor ditory and visual discrimination. standardized. Reader's Digestself- Bond and Tinker (1) identify wond testing section is suggestedas an eclec- recognition with the following basic tic procedure. Teacher observations skills: should be usedtodetermine what 1.Associating the approrpriate methods are used by the readerto at- meanings with the printed symbol; tack new words. Does the studentuse 2. Usingthecontextcluesand context clues? Or phonetics?Or other meaning aids to anticipatethe structural analysis ? Does he know wordstoberecognizedandthen and use the rules syllabication? checking the accuracy of the recogni- Knowledge of word attack skills is tion: :mportant,butskillinusing them 3. Becoming flexible and efficientin while reading makes the knowledge of visually analyzing the word into usable value (6). recognition elements; Scholastic Scope (Scholastic Maga- 4.Developing knowledges of visual, zine),a weeklyperiodicalfor high structural and phonetic elements. school students, is one reading media knowledge of consonant andvowel which has been used successfully in sounds. blends and diagraphs, prefixes teaching the use of word meaning and and suffixes, etc.; context clues to retarded college read- Forging Ahead in Reading, IRAProceedings,Vol.12. Part 1, 1968, 66-70.

46 40 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

ers. have notprepared himforcollege Training in the utilization of context reading. More than one half of the clues5110111(1 be stressedin teaching students entering the small colleges are word attack skills. The student at this taking remedial courses. High school level should be encouraged to employ preparationhasnotbeen efficient; shrewd guesses in an attempt to deter- therefore, the studentisin "trouble" mine the meaning of a word from the with reading. way it is used in the text (3).:\ stu- dent In structural analysis, words should proficientinutilizingcontext be taught clues can usually derive enough sense interms of their compo- from a word to satisfy his need at the nents. prefixes, snffixes, and roots so monient.One method of increasing that students can see the effects of pre- his abilityisto present the student fixes and suffixes upon the meaning of with a list of sentences in which the the root, learn the meaning of common critical word is omitted and have the Latin and Greek roots and affixes, and students fill in the blanks or have them get the meaning and pronunication of a readshortinterestingarticleswith word when the surrounding context is words they should know. may not, but not enough (2).The students can can determine the meaning of because build their own derivations, place them of the context. in original context, and exchange them The problem a College student faces with one another to try out their skills. when he meets a new word is likely to In order to use roots, prefixes, and be different from that of a child seeing suffixes most effectivelythestudent a word for the firsttime.When a must be proficient in syllabication.If childfirststarts to read, the words he possesses scanty knowledge in this that he encounters are, for the most part.strange to himin form only. area, syllabication as means of word That is. most of the words that a child attack should be demonstrated and the finds in his beginning reading assign- rules for dividing words into syllables ments are already in his speaking vo- should be presented. cabulary.The words that are new to Roberts (4) has outlined the princi- a college student, however, are usually ples of syllabication with examples and strange in both meaning and form (6). e x erci s es onthecollegelevel. When the college freshman enters his Sometimes the student is so deficient history class, the vocabulary is as for- that an individual approach should be eign as German. He has had no pre- adopted. He also suggests exercises vious experience which would give him using Greek and Latin roots so that familiarity to the concept or the ab- students who have problems unlocking straction for which the word stands. new words may review the rules. His success in the course is partially If the college student cannot recog- dependent upon his ability to under- nize words quickly and accurately and stand the technical terminology. cannot begin the attack of new words In attempting to develop word rec- by dividing them intosyllables and ognition for college students, the req- pronouncing them, it is very probable uisite is to stimulate interest in learn- he has never become a mature reader, ing new words and to make the stu- with reading skills developed beyond dent aware of the words he does not those of the elementary school child. know. The student will not gain much film)) training in vocabulary, rate, and com- The inherent factors which the stu- phrehension unless he can recognize a dent possesses when he enters college key word by its context. by structural

47 FORD 41 or phonetic analysis, or by syllabica- most common exceptions."The stu- tion, the next thing to do is look it up dent is not to be made a specialist in inthedictionary and learn how to phonetics." states Triggs (6). pronounce the u n f amiliar word. Once the basic facts have been mas- Independent use of the dictionary re- tered. the lessons become a matter of quires skills in the use of guide words, practicing their application in forms as use of the pronunciation key, and the varied as the instructor's ingenuity can use of several definitions given for aid supply and for as long as the student's in word recognition.These four tech- disabilityrequires. This application niques should be stressed with the re- should include drill in discriminating tarded reader if the techniques have among words of similar configuration hem forgotten. and in retaining the visual image of the Inthereading laboratory several word once itis learned. This work levels of dictionaries should be avail- must be done first with words in isola- able so that students may refer to them tion. but it should be applied immedi- as they are needed. ately in sentences so as to approximate The retarded reader should have at the reading situation.Workbooks are hisdisposal many kinds of reading recommended for this practice. along materials of low levels but of high in- with teacher made exercises. terest. The Globe Book Company New York) has all of the classics on Materials for the retarded these levels.Careful selection of read- reader ing material is important so that the Word Attack (Harcourt, Brace and student will not feel that he is reading World, 1956) is acceptable to college "chill stuff." students because of its hard cover and One step of correcting a disability in It contains lists of words and word recognition is to make the stu- format. to develop dent sound conscious (3).He must permits the student learn to hear sounds and to think of cluesan aid to word identification. The idea of becoming a word detective letters in terms of sounds, and his les- sons may vary with a series of exer- appeals to students and helps them de- velop the important concept that figur- cises that teach him the various sounds ing out words is a responsibility each attached to the vowel and the conso- person must accept if he is to read in- nants.After he has learned to hear the sounds and to associate them with dependently. the proper letters, he must be taught to Mentionedearlier,Scholastic Scope blend them in combination.All this (Scholastic Magazine, Inc.,1966-67) activity can he achieved through the is desirable because context clues and use of simple words whose pronuncia- word meaning skills are presented in tionhe already knows; he simply such a way that students are enthusias- learns to break their sounds down into tic about performing those tasks. their component parts until he can BasicReadingSA-illsforJunior hear the value of each letter and com- High School Use (Scott, Foresman, bination of letters. 1957) contains exercises which may be Next, the student should be given used in reviewing and developing word the basic rules for building these into attack skills with older pupils. words, that is, the rules of syllabica- Oral reading is necessary to assure tion, accent, and pronunciation. These the application of word attacks skills. rules should be simple and general as Preparing a newspaper report, short possible, taking account of only the book review, excerpts from magazine 42 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

articles to read before a group helps to inventorieswould giveevidenceof give the studentthe confidence heprogress in word attack skills. needs to read aloud inhis regular classroom situation. Concluding statement Reading Laboratory II lb (Science Developing word attack skillsfor Research Associates)has beenac- the retarded college reader is a slow cepted by retarded college readers as and difficult process.Rapport should an aid to word attack skills because of be established among the teachers and its multilevel activities.The exercises studentto acquire the bestresults. are designedforculturallydeficient The physical condition of the readers.After completion of Labora- classroomshouldbeconduciveto tory IIM the student is moved to Lab- learning.A soundproof room is rec- oratory IVa, a more difficult reading ommended. Instructionshouldin- aid, to promote learning of higher lev- clude the utilization of context clues, els of prefixes, suffixes, context clues, structural and phonetic analysis and and word meaning skills. the teachings of suffixes, roots, pre- The use of Flash X's and a Tachis- fixes, and dictionary skills; and the de- toscope(EducationalDevelopmental vising of supplementary exercises by Laboratories) with some of the college the instructor.Reliable measurement students for improving concentration of growth should be provided for at in- and quickness in perceiving words and associating themwith meaning has tervals of nine-week periods. been used.Students' like the feel of a machine; they think that this device is something new and scientific that will help them tolearnto read better. Students at this age need motivation if REFERENCES results are to be expected. 1.Bond, Guy L., and Miles A. Tinker, Phonics We UseBook F (Lyons ReadingDifficulties:TheirDiagnosis and Correction.New York :Appleton- and Carnaham-1964) is an excellent Century, Croft, Inc., 1957. refresher course for students who are 2.Fitzgerald, Frances."In Corrective and in need of independent attack on words. RemedialClasses," Reading Instruction Analytical inspectionof words plus in Various Patterns of Grouping. Supple- context clues are used in attacking new mentary Educational Monographs, Vol. XXI, No. 89, The University of Chicago words in a modern program of phon- Press, 1959, 134-137. ics. 3. Reed, James C. "In College," Corrective The college retarded reader is often Reading in Classroom and Clinic, Supple- culturallyinefficientbecause he has mentary Educational Monographs, Vol. IV, No. 79, The University of Chicago been carelessly taught by teachers who Press, 1953, 140-144. are not proficient in the teaching of 4. Roberts, Clyde.Word Attack: A Way reading. To Better Reading.New York :Har- Evaluation of progress court, Brace and World, Inc., 1956. 5.Smith, Henry P., and Emerald V. De- The appraisalprocedures areas chant.Psychology in Teaching Reading. broad as the concept of reading and Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey:Pren- the program planned to achieve the tice-Hall, Inc., 1961. broad objectives. 6. Triggs, FrancesOralind. Remedial Standardized text Reading: The Diagnosis and Correction should not be used alone.Observation of ReadingDifficulties atthe College isofgreatestimportance; informal Level. The UniversityofMinnesota tests, practice exercizes, and interest Press, Minneapolis, 1943.

49 Applying Research Findings in Word Perception to Classroom Practice

MILDRED WITTICK

RESEARCH INthe area of word recog- Montessori's methods, popular about nition generally uses subjects from the 1915 and now enjoying a revival of in- primary grades. Where older pupils terest,includedteachingthe young are involved in word recognition inves- child to recognize, name, sound, and tigations, the research tends to be con- even write the letters of the alphabet. cerned with methods and/or materials 0. K. Moore of Harvard has been fur use with remedial readers. interestedinthe pastten yearsin How one teaches reading depends teaching three- and four-year-olds to upon how one defines reading.Word read by the use of automated typewrit- recognition is the process of producing ers.His hypothesis is that early read- the sounds represented by the printed ing experiences are beneficial for chil- symbols and putting themtogether dren'sgeneralintellectualdevelop- into words. Sometimesitiscalled ment. In what Moore calls a "Re- "identification," "decoding," or "break- sponsive Environment," the child first ing the code."However, word recog- explores the typewriter and learns let- nitionis only the initial step if one ters. When he strikes a typewriter considers reading to be the process of key, the automated machine calls out gettingmeaning andunderstanding the name of the letter or the symbol from printed symbols.In completing that was struck. Inthisway, he the reading act the individual reacts to learns the names of theletters and the material and integrates what he what they look like.later a teacher has read with what he already knows. guides him in learning to reproduce on In this way, his ideas and attitudes the electric typewriter individuallet- change and his intellectual growth con- tersthat appear on atelevision-like tinues. screen before him.Finally, he types words and sentences. The idea of Preschool reading experiences using typewriters with young children related to word perception is not new, as experiments using regu- A few years ago, a popular women's lar typewriters infirst grade classes magazinepublishedan article on were conducted at the University of teaching one's baby to read and sug- Missouri in the early 1930's. gested this procedure might be started A studyinitiatedinthe Denver withtwo-year-olds;educators were Public Schools was designed to show alternatelyannoyed,a ppa lied, and parents how to provide reading readi- amused. This "system" began with ness activities for preschool children. teaching recognition of large letters in Prcharing Your Child forReading isolation.The idea was not really new was the guidebook used in conjunction as many generations of children have with sixteen TV.programs which elab- played with blocks that had letters, orated on specific techniques that par- words, and pictures ontheirsides. ents might use.These children were Others have had cutout letters of wood studied over a five-year period ;the or plastic with which to experiment early reading experience appeared to and perhaps build words. have a positive and continuing effect. Perception and Reading, IRA Proceedings, Vol. 12, Part 4, 1968, 125-130. (It 1

44 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

Developing word perception skills the child had made inkindergarten in the kindergarten (3). The study also showed that the early reading instruction created no Since about 1955 there have been special problemsrelatedtovision, severalstudiesconcerned withthe hearing, or social or academic adjust- readingofkindergarten children. ment. 1 Investigatorsfoundthat,coinpared How do such pre-first grade reading with the preceding generation,five- experiences affect classroom practice in year-olds today 1) have larger vocabu- Grade One? The teacher' findsa laries; 2) do more reading of signs wider range of individual differences; and labels; 3) use longer sentences; some children do not need the conven- 4) use more complex sentence struc- tionalreadiness program ;grouping tures; 5) have traveled more; 6) have may be more difficult, requiring special had more experience with hooks and flexibility ; and placing each child in records; 7) have more often had nur- the most appropriate reading material sery school experience ; 8) have substi- may create problems because the tuted TV viewing for the radio listen- teacher must evaluate the child's read- ing of their parents as Children ; and ing background with greataccuracy 9) have vocabularies that are different and locate the appropriate readingma in content. terials.These changes must be re- Durkin (6) studied the progress of flectedin the reading curriculum of 49 Oakland, California, children who all of the primary and even the later learned to read at home before enter- grades. ing the first grade of a public school. At the end of five years she reported Word perception in grade one that the majority of these preschool TheChallResearch. The most readers continued to be superior read- thorough recent study of the research ers as they advanced in school. How- on beginning reading instruction was ever, these pupils were described as 1) reported by Chall (4) as an outgrowth children who wanted to learn to read ; of the City College-Carnegie Reading 2) children who had many opportuni- Study (1962-1965).She considered ties to do so; and 3) children who had much of the research from 1910 to favorableattitudestowardreading. 1965 from several sources: the experi- Durkin pointed out that her findings mental research from classroom and do not necessarily support the proposal laboratory ;the findings from correla- for earlier school instruction in read- tional studies ; and the evidence from ing. several well-known clinical studies of Since 1960 the DenverPublic children who have failed to learn to Schools have been conducting a pilot read. Space here does not permit a study involving about 4,000 kindergar- discussion- of her methods, but her teners. The subjects received system- point of view should be noted.She atic instruction in beginning reading. suggested that the first step in learning Reading tests administered at the end to read one's native language is learn- of first grade showed that there wasa ing the printed code for speech.This significant difference in favor of the method agrees with Bloomfield'scon- experimental group over the control clusion which may be said to represent group of children who had participated the linguist's approach to initial read- in the regular kindergarten program. ing instruction.However, Chall re- Optimum readingachievementoc- ported her analysis did not prove or curred when the first grade program disprove that any one method of code was altered to take advantage of gains emphasis, i.e.,linguistic approach, 51 WITTICK 45 modifiedalphabets,orsystematic in-service program on teacher class- phonics, was significantly superior to room helm vior andpupil reading another.She commented that there is achievement ;the reading achievement some experimental evidence that chil- of first grade boys versus first grade dren of below-average and average in- : and a ciparisoncomparison of beginning telligence and children of lower socio- reading in three classroom organiza- economic background do better with an tional patterns. early code emphasis.Brighter chil- Stauffer(10) commented onthe dren and those with better socioeco- studies in the May 1966, issue of The nomicbackgrounds appearableto Reading Teacher that "No one method "break" much of the code independ- can be compared with another because ently. regardless of the methods used the methods were not sharply and in the school reading program. clearlydifferent. For example :all The U. S. Office of Education First taught the alphabet :all used writing Grade Reading Studies.In the school experiences : and so on.Methods that year 1964-65. 27 first grade reading were given the same label were often studies were begun, all sponsored by not the same. ... Reading instruction the U. S. Office of Education.All time could not be defined so as to be used some of the same pretests and acceptable to all twenty-seven studies. post-tests. All continued for approxi- Much effort was devoted to an attempt mately 140 school days.Bond, gen- to define reading instruction time at eral coordinator, met three times dur- the Coordinating Center meeting. but ing the year with all directors at the to no avail." Coordinating Center for the Coopera- Bond (1) made four tentative obser- tive Research Program in First Grade vations at the end of the first year of Reading Instruction at the University these studies : of Minnesota.The center served two 1.There is no one method that is functions :to maintain communication so outstanding that it should be used among project directors and to collect, to the exclusion of the others. organize,analyze. and interpretthe 2. The effectiveness of any one ap- common data from the projects. proach appears to be increased when The studies were made in 16 differ- it is broadened bythe additionof other ent states : five were done in Pennsyl- instructional components. For exam- vania; three, in California..; and three. ple, a basic program's effectiveness is in New York.Except for Florida and increased when writing experiences are North Carolina, no studies were done added; or a phonetic approach appears in the South. to profit from the addition of audio Twenty ofthestudies explored the and visual instructional aids. effectivenessof different methods of 3.Specific approaches to first grade teaching reading or developing readi- readinginstruction appeared toin- ness. Two ofthese used Spanish- crease children's achievement incer- speakingchildrenassubjects; one taininstructional outcomes butarc used the disadvantaged in a large city. weakinotheroutcomes. Another and one used the low reading group. method may develop different patterns Other research examined problems re- ofgrowth. This observationgives lated to an analysis of the interactions hope to the possibility that combina- of professed methods, teacher imple- tions of approaches that will encourage mentation, and child background;a . overall balanced reading growth will longitudinal readinessprogram; a be found. comparison of two methods of reading 4. There was greater variation be- supervision ; the effects of an intensive tween the teathers within the methods

52 46 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES than there was between the methods. Teaching Alphabet(i.t.a.),Unifcn, This finding again points up the im- and the Diacritical Marking System portance of the teacher's role in learn- (DMS).All of these "systems" are ing.This latter point raises the im- too new to have conclusive research portant issue as to whether the meth- about them.A special problem has ods debate is not an artifact and that been created by school systems that the teacher should receive major con- have rushed to' use them without de- sideration. veloping a thoughtful, careful experi- The U. S. Office of Education ex- mental design which will make the re- tended fourteen of the studies through stilts meaningful to other educators. the second and third grades. Such In four of the U. S. Office of Edu- continuedinvestigationsshould be cationStudieswhich evaluatethe especially valuable since one of the i.t.a. medium, conclusions ranged from great weaknesses of general research a)no difference fromother meth- in reading has been that too often it odstested;b)resultsnotalways was characterized by a proliferation of consistent ; c) inconclusive at the end bits and pieces looked at in isolation of grade one; and d) to use in com- rather than as a part of the sequential bination with other materials. development of the individual's reading Other studies ofi.t.a.,thoseof pattern. Bosnia and Farrow (2), Mazurkiewicz Phonics. Over the past thirty years (9),and Downing and Jones(5), probably more research in reading has generally favored the i.t.a. group. attackedtheproblem of phonics Experimentsusing Unifon have when ? and how much?than any been carried on in the Chicago city other plrie of the reading process.It schools, some of the Chicago suburban is almost impossible to isolate com- schools, in the St. Louis area, and in pletely the teaching of phonics from Detroit. other parts of the first grade program. Word PerceptionandLinguistic Approach. to Teaching Reading.The Even where the program begins with linguists believe that children should be formalphonicsmaterials,thechild taught the "decoding system" by first comes in contact with many books and learning those words that are spelled oral language experiences, so phonics regularly as can hardly be considered in isolation. fat, pat. mat, sat.Oral Teachers also know more about the reading should be stressed in the ini- various programs in phonics than they tial stages, and often materials at this level have no illustrations. When a do about any other area. . In the lx,st four years, two basal child does not recognize a word, he is reading programs with heavy phonics usually taught to spell it rather than to "sound it out." emphasis have been published. Children begin with letters and sounds, There are severallinguistic series and one series presents stories only now published, and research is just be- after a wealth of phonics materials has ginning on the effectiveness of this ap- been mastered.One of the results of proach. strong interest .n phonics has been the Other Methods of Teaching Word strengthening of this material in all of Perception.New materials for teach- the well-known basal readers. ingword perceptionare appearing The Modified Alphabets.There has with great rapidityat the moment. been much interest in the past seven Little or no research related to their years in the use of modified alphabets classroom use now exists.Programed to teach word recognition skills. The learning has been developed for begin- best known of these 'are theInitial ningreaders; computer-assistedin- 53 WITTICK 47

struction in initial reading is being ex- troitpublicschoolsinwhichfive perimented with in California ; a great widely differentmethods arebeing variety of new teaching machines are compared. These include McGraw- on the market ;audiovisual materials Hills' Programmed Reading, the Ginn are abundant and are specifically de- BasicReaders, Lippincott's Basic signed into several of the experimental Reading Program, Unifon, i.t.a., and programs. Harper and Row's Linguistic Read- Comparison of Methods of Teaching ders.One criticism of this study is Reading.In 1961 Van Allen (12) re- that Unifon was developed as a perma- ported a three-year study, "Three Ap- nent spelling reform, not as an alphabet proaches to the Teaching of Reading," for teaching beginning reading.For that had been carried on in the San thisreasonitisnot especially de- Diego area.The hypothesis was as signed, as is i.t.a., for making the tran- follows: There are numerous effective sition to the conventional English al- waysofteachingreadinginour phabet.The findings of the Detroit schools. Three methods were com- study, which was begun in 1965 and pared: the basic reading, the individ- will extend until 1968, should be espe- ualized, and the language-experience cially useful. approach.Each teacher who partici- Fry's (7) study compared his Dia- pated selected the approach he under- critical Marking System (DMS) with stoodone for which he had materials the Initial Teaching Alphabet and the to carry it outand received a suppor- Sheldon readers (Allyn and Bacon) Li tive in-service program. The hypothe- traditional orthography. Among Fry's sis, as judged by observations and test findings were the following:1) there results,was confirmed. The other was no statistically significant differ- findings includedthe following :1) ence between the mean scores of any of language-experience teachers who the subtests of the post-test (Stanford ruled out all other approaches found Achievement)for any of the three that their children made as much or groups; 2) there were no significant more progress in the skills (measured differences on the Gilmore Oral Read- on standardized reading tests) as did ing Test scores; 3) no methodwas the children who had direct teaching better for boys or girls or better for skills; 2) there is a closer relationship younger first graders or for older first between phonics and writing than be graders; 4)thevariationbetween tween phonics and reading ; 3) many classrooms was much greater than the teachers are now dealing with basic variation between methods; and 5) the sight vocabulary on an individual basis best predictor for reading achievement from oral language to written lan- was IQ raw score (M.A.). Fry also guage to recall of written language concluded that reading readiness mate- (this practice usually results inrecog- rials are not necessary inthefirst nition of high frequency words asa re- grade. This opinion was basedon the sult of repetition) ; and 4) each child finding that four traditional orthogra- gradually gains a personally tailored phy project classes that (lid notuse sight vocabulary which is functional reading readiness materials but began and which is in excess of words intro- reading instruction immediately after ducedinthecontrolledprograms. enteringfirst grade did significantly Control becomes an individual matter. better at December testing on the In- Ceilings are lifted for all children at all stant Word Recognition Test than .grade levels. four nonproject T.O. classes that bad An elaborate study of reading meth- someformalreadinessinstruction. ods is being carried on in the De- The value of readiness instruction can- 48 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES not be based on the results from such a own classrooms because any of several small sample :however, sonic of the methods willyield good results when used by a brilliant teacher devoted to her linguists agree that readiness materials work. It does not follow that the same are not essential. system should be universally adopted be- cause itis far from certain that itwill Summary and conclusions be equally effective with the average As a classroom teacher, how does teacher. one translateresearchfindingsinto REFERENCES classroom practice?I) Check the re- I.Bond, Guy 1.. "First-Grade Reading lationship of the research findings to Studies:Au Overview,"Mensentary one's own school and/or classroom ob- /English. 43, 5 aty 1966), 468 -4(9. Bosnia. Ruth and V.L.Farrow. jectives in the area of word perception. "Teaching Reading uith i.t.a.: a Re- 2) Be slow to abandon present meth- searchReport,"ReadingI lorizaus.6 ods if objective evidence exists that the t 1965), 6-19. program is successful in one's particu- 3.Brzeinski, Joseph E."Beginning Read- larsituationwithparticular ing in Denver,"The Reading Teacher. pupils. 18 (October 1964), 16-21. 3) Be slow to move into a word per- 4.Chan, Jeanne S.LearningiiiRead: ception program on which little or no TheGreat Debate. New York : research has been reported unless en- McGraw-I lill Book Company, 1967. gaged in evaluating the program one- 5.Downing. J., and Barbara Jones. self. 4) Reevaluate the readiness pro- "Some Problems of Evaluating i.t.a.: a Second Experiment,"EducationalRe- gram in terms of its content and its search,8 0966), 100 -114. flexibilityin providing for individual b.Durkin, Dolores. "A Fifth-Year Re- differences.5) Reconsider the phon- port on the Achievement of Early Read- ics program. (Decide how to handle ers,-F.lensentary SchoolJournal,65 the alphabet.Decide on the appropri- ( November 1964), 76.80. 7.Fry, Edward B."Comparing the Di-, ateness of the order of teaching decod- critical'Marking System. ITA. and a ing skills.Cheek methods of integrat- Basal Reading Series,"!Elementary !En- ing phonics skills with the rest of the glish,43, 6 (October 1966), 607-611. word perception program.) R.Loban, WalterI). TheLanguage of 6) Re- Elementay School Children.Research evaluate the materials in use.(Are ReportNo.I. Champaign,Illinois : these the best available for reaching National Council of Teachers of En- objectives?Are newer publications glish, 1963. superior ? Would certain audiovisual 9. Mazurkieiez,A. J."The Lehigh Uni- versity-Bethlehem Schools ITA Study : equipment be more efficient ?) and 7) Interim Report Nine,"Journal of the If a new program is to be instigated, Reading Specialist,5 (1965), 9-11. carefully study the theory upon which 10.Stauffer,RussellG. "TheVerdict : it is based as well as the materials to Speculative Controversy,"The Reading be used. Teacher,19 (May 1966), 563-564. The conscientious teacher of reading 11.Strickland, Ruth G.The Language of may find comfort in Woodring's com- Elementary School Children:Its Re- ments (13) on the proliferation of lationship to the Language of Reading methods for teaching reading. Textbooks and the Quality of Reading of SelectedChildren. Bulletin of the Thereisno one correct or sound School of Education, Vol. 38, No. -4, method of learning to read. Successful Bloomington, Indiana: Indiana Univer- teachers have always employed a variety sity, 1962. of methods, children have learnedto 12. Van Allen,R. "More Ways Than read in many different ways, and many One,"Childhood Education,38, No. 3 bright children can read before theyen- (November 1961), 108-111. ter school.Enthusiasts for any one of 13. Woodrum, Paul."Can Johnny Read?" the many systems which they themselves SaturdayReview. 45 (January 20, inventedcandemonstrateremarkable results with their own children in their 1962), 40. 55 Translating Research into Practice:Reading Readiness, Visual Perception, and AuditoryPerception

IRAE.AARON

TRANSLATING RESEARCH into practice in spite of the researcher. Thestudies is the final and often neglected phase fromwhich of research in reading. generalizationscan be Although nu- drawn usually are so statisticallyor- merous research studies dealing with iented that a second course in statistics various aspects of readingare pub- isa necessity for unraveling them; lished yearly, very few of thesemake a real difference in reading instruction. most teachers do not hav^afirst conrse. Teachersaremuch more The influence of most of these studies likely to be influenced by is restricted to the classrooms involved a noncon- in the investigations. trolled study in whichsome teacher tries out something in herown class- Dow are teachers' practices in the room. finds it successful with her chil- teaching of reading influenced byre- dren, and writes about it.Teachers search findings?What are the haz- can read these studies with under- ards Of interpreting accurately the re- standing; whereas researchersseem to sults of research in reading? Whatare write for other researchers. some of the recommended practices Someonercviewsthe growing from previous researchon literature, reading readiness, draws conclusions, andsuggests prac- visualperception. tical implications; teachers read and auditory perception? Before these and put recommendations intoprac- drawing implications front the studies tice. reported each of these three questions Research reviews are published will be discussed briefly. in a number of periodicalsthat are readily available to teachers.Most of Research findings and teacher change these reviews, however, donot include Change in practice as a result ofre- implications for practice, andmany of them review studies uncritically. search occurs only if teachers insonic The way become aware of the findings of teacher is more likely to be influenced pertinentinvestigations.Four ways by this type of article than bythe orig- inal research article itself. in which teachers may learn aboutre- Still, the search results arc discussed. amount of influence front such reviews Teachers read research reports in is likely to be small. professional journals and translate find- IFritersof professionaltextbooks ings into practice.This avenue is one review the literature andincorporate that might be usedto bring about findings in their recommendationsfor practice, changesas a resultofresearch. Those preservice andin- Despitethispossibility,verylittle service teachers who enrollin courses change occurs through this medium. using textbookson reading methodol- Most teachers are not well informed in ogy may be brought intocontact with the interpretation of research results, translations of research findingsin the and persons who prepare researchre- way of assigned or voluntary reading. ports are not always helpful in the way The quality of thesereviews varies greatly. they present the results.If a teacher sees a practical application from some Authors and editors of elementary published research reports, he does it and secondary school textbooksin- Perception and Reading,IRA Proceedings, Vol. 12, Part 4, 1968, 130.135.

56 Aro 50 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES corporate findings intheir materials. must be concerned with accuracy in Basal reading textbooks and their ac- reporting. A heavy Irirden of respon- companying guidebooks are the major sibility is placed upon persons who at- influences on elementary reading pro- tempt to draw practical implications grams in America.Some, but not all, from research investigations. If the of the recommendations in guidebooks interpreter, eitherinspeaking or in are based upon research. writing, attempts to draw implications The dozen or more series of basal for groups of people, then he is obli- readers now being used are notin gated to view the limitations as well as exact agreement on the elements of a the strengths of the studies.He must good reading program.If one were to act with a full knowledge of the gen- askthose who prepare materialsif eral limitations of educational research theirtexts and instructional sugges- in a school setting or in laborator - type situations. tionsarebased uponresearch.he would getanaffirmativeanswer. An educationalresearcherinthe That answer would be correct, to a de- area of reading must face these prob- lems: gree.Some aspects of these programs are based upon research; some, on in- illa»y perceptionstudies arcper- telligent hunches.It becomes difficult formedinlaboratory-type situations to unravel the parts based on solid re- that differ considerably from the class- search and those based on opinions. room setting.It is relatively easy to Those who prepare textbooks have a set up a one-time teaching situation responsibilityto study availablere- that may involve one teacher and one searchcarefully andtoincorporate pupil in an isolated setting outside the soundresearch-basedideasintheir classroom. Within the classroom au materials.The rush with which they unreal learning task may be assigned are faced in today's fast-moving world and monitored for a short period of forces them to act before all the evi- time.Such studies may provide clues denceisobtained. They may, from which explain some parts of what hap- economic necessity, be forced into an pens in the larger setting.In interpre- area before evidence to support the tation, the fact that the setting was an move is on hand.Some of the more unnatural one should be kept in mind. recent popular movements which have These are still useful studies if they vressed publishers to take action be- are continued long enough so that the fore they were researched carefully are little pieces can be put together into a thelinguisticapproach,programed larger explanation of how learning oc- materials, and programs for the cultur- curs. ally disadvantaged. In the regular classroom, the At this point the reader probably is learner, though he is unique in any disturbed over the futility of getting characteristic one may want to con- action from the findings of reading re- :icier, is usually surrounded by 25 to search. Improvement in dissemination 30 other unique beings, all of whom at is certainly needed.Persons in read- one time or another will influence his ing must find effective ways of dissem- learning. His teacher is an extremely inating research findings to teachers. influential factor in his learning. Be- If research does not influence practice. cause of what he means to the child in then it serves no real purpose. an emotionallypositiveornegative The hazards of research way, the teacher can accomplish more, sometimesless,than theresearcher The disseminator of research reports who makes an infrequent or one-stop

57 AARON 51

appearance in the classroom. Even the The interpreter of research mustre- routine interruptions or interferences frain from indulging in overgenerali- in the school day influence learning. zation.Often he may have to settle If these are removed in a research set- for a partial answer before actionmust ting, the situation is unlike the regular be taken. When the foregoing happens, classroom. Interpretationsmustbe he should not attempt tosupport his made with the foregoing in mind. action by research ;he should admit Research cannot always be in a nat- that he does not know nor doesanyone ural setting. The interpretation should else at this stage. recognize that other influencit7?; fac- The researcher may clothe an insig- tors were ruled out in the study and nificant or poorly designed study inan that the combination of the eliminated elaborate statistical dress.The inter- factors with whatever was tested might preter ofresearchoftenencounters have led to different results because studies that attempt to answerques- the global setting was changed. tions which are not very important, Brood generalizations may be dram, and yet the elaborateness of the statis- from a very limitedstudy.Often tical treatment makes the studyappear when a writer or a speaker states, to be a vital one. The commonness of "Research says...," he presents a the highspeed electronic computer in- small study performed with a limited fluences the number of such studies number of subjects in a particular type today.An assistant in a computer of setting.Sometimes he generalizes center, who has a good grasp of his own field but little knowledge of the tothe universe. Summaries ofre- search that one finds in college text- field in which the studywas under- taken, can lend to a statisticallyover- books and in professional periodicals dressed study. often fall victim to this hazard. The interpreterofresearch may Because of necessity and sometimes draw implications for practice when because of convenience, the researcher such implications are of doubtful value. uses a small sample when a large sam- If a study is presented orallyor in ple may be needed for what he is at- writing,theunsuspecting consumer tempting to do. His subjectsmay rep- may falsely assume that it has practical resent a very limited segment of the implications.This conclusion is not totalpopulationofschoolchildr-n. necessarily so. This procedure is certainly acceptable This hazard of expecting implica- if the researcher defines his population tions when they are not there is fos- clearly. The interpreter should keep tered by some professionaljournals the subjects in mind as he generalizes. and by some college people whomust What applies to culturally andeco- "publish or perish."The interpreter nomicallydeprivedchildreninone of research can at least refrain from place may or may not apply to those in being tripped by this hazard.Pub- another section. The populationsmay lishing exploratory studies is impor- be different. tant.However, they should be pub- Such studies are not useless. As lished as co/oratory research andnot particular studies are replicated, the as definitive studies.It is too easy to findings are put together.In almost mislead, even when seemingly adequate all situations the findings may be the precautions are taken. same or may apply only for certain The for,...going are Gusta few of the categories or types of children, as the hazards of research interpretation and intellectually superior or the culturally research implementation. Among the advantaged. others that could have been included

58 52 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES are the following: geared to the child's particular level of Tlw subjects may consciously or im- achievement in so far as this can be conscionsly respond in a certain way done. Further. the teacher 'mist ex- and thns negate the study. pect some childrento increase their The attritionrate may he exceed- perceptual skills at a more rapid rate ingly heavy in a longitudinal stmly. than others. Statisticaltechniques inappropriate The generalizationjeststatedis for the purpose may be used. widely accepted.However, hidden be- Assmitptisms mulerlying the use of neath the generalization are some nn- the statisticaltechnique may not be answered questions.To what extent met. are these individual differences the re- The level of significance may be in- sult of the child's preschool environ- appropriately selected. ment? To what extent can a modified The researcher himself may be so en vironmein help to overcome what dependent neon the computer that he some persons think are the results of a does not know the techniques tieing deprivedenvironment ? Whatrole used or how to interpret them. does the central nervous system play An usually large sample may lead to in individual differences in perceptual a statistical difference when the differ- abilities? What programs are best for ence is so small that itis of no practi- children who are weak in a given per- cal significance. ceptnalability? We havepartial answers to some of these questions. Generalizations from research studies lout inch rema.us to be learned. on auditory and visual perception Auditory mid visual perception are related to success in beginning reading. Many studies have been mulertaken This generalization is universally ac- to investigate auditory and visual per- cepted. Allbeginningreading and ception and their relation to reading, kindergarten programs are concerned especially beginning reading..\ few with assessing and developing auditory of the generally accepted findings that and visual perception.Reading read- are research-supported are presented iness tests attempt to assess the level here. of development of these two perceptnal Children show individual differences in perception. Any given gronp of The evidence from the studies which children will vary in their perceptnal have explored the differential learning development.The speed with which modalities of pupils is somewhat con- they can respond to varied auditory or flicting,Still other studies have inves- visual stimitli will differ, and their lev- tigated the effect of simultaneous audio els of development will vary.For ex- and visual presentations of stinin.h ample, some firstgraders will come Some ofthe materialsintypical into school knowing how to discrimi- reading readinessmaterials arenot nate anditorially between similar needed by most six-year-olds.Gross Words and to recognize identical words visual discrimination exercises are use- when spoken.Others will need inch less for many children.13y the time practice.This generalization is a part theygetintokindergarten orfirst of what the teacher means when she grade they can make these gross dis- says. "Some children are ready to read criminations. 1nfact. some studies when they enter school ; some children have indicated that most children en- are not." tering the first grade can discriminate The implication for practice is that Iwtween and match letters. perceptualskill teaching!mist be One of the best predictors of begin- 59 AARON 53

ning reading success is the extent to following limitations in mind: thetwo which the child can name the sets of materials used in the studyare Several studies have come to this con- not on opposite ends of the visual-au- clusion.Hidden beneath this generali- ditorycontinuum:childreninthe zation are some possible influencing study were c(onsiderahl aboveaverage factors that need investigation. Knowl- in intelligence. and the findingsmay edge of letter names may not be the not apply to other intellectual levels: major factor in success ;it may reflect no information was given abut how the level of intelligence of the child the teachers were selectedfor each and the attention he has received at class; except for the extremes in the home, both of which will influence his placement children. the pupilswere not learning to read. clearly shown to preferone learning The foregoing are a few of the gen- mode to another ; and. on the section eralizations from research on percep- of the study dealing with "good" and tion in the area of reading. They have "poor- readers. the lxor readerswere not been associated here with specific well above grade levelinsonicin- studies since they are generally ac- stanes. creted conclusions that aye beensup- The following implications forprac- ported by many iin,estigations. tice may be drawn from the study: I.All approach similar to that fol- Research studies reported and their lowed by the teachers who used the implicat ions "auditory- materials limy haveprom- The studies presented. though they ise for teaching intellectually superior fall tithin a general area. actuallyare children. separate studies and lutist he handled 2.Further studies should be under- as slick Some of them deal with kin- taken to assess the extent to which the dergartenandfirstgradechildren IiIferentmodes of learning are impor- while the remainder range from second tantinselectinginstructionaltech- grade through junior high school. niques for use both in deelipmental The reader may refer to certain in- and remedial reading. dividual research reports in thismoo- 3.Another study needs to he con- graph for the details of the studies. ducted in which a program designed to Following the Inoue of the researcher be primarily visual and another to he and the title of the study will he given primarily' auditory are used with vis- the study strengths and limitations and uallyandauditoriallyinclinedfirst Thentheimplicationsforpractice. graders.. The latter will include replication of the study where applicable. Fansiman. Marion Nea).I fleets of Perceplion Training in Kinder Arlen Raton:tn. Itarbara. The Compara- on First Grade Success in Reading. lhe lifficary of nd or y and a ris- Strengths ofthis study include the mil Method of First-Grade Reading careful selectionof members of tle /manic/ion.with;humor,. Learners ond control and experimental groups, the 1,isa/Learners. Aning the random assignment of teachers to the strengths of this study were the careful two groups. and the in-service training handling of the placement of children given to all participating teachers. intoresearch groups.thein-service As interpretations are made, the fol- sessions held for participating teachers, lowing limitations 5110111(1be kept in and the very adequate discussion of mind: the reader has no information study limitations. nn how the original 32 kindergarten Results must he interpreted with the classes were selected; the factors ac-

60 54 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES companying perceptual training (indi- milion of Visual Discrimination Train- vidualization of instruction,' diagnosis ingforBeginningReaders. The of learning needs, and better opportu- sample was selected carefully, and the nities for teachers to gain knowledge of study was presented in a clear manner. child growth and development) might Interpretation must conrider the fol- have favored the experimental group, lowing limitations : the teacher variable aside from the actual training was not discussed ; the class organiza- The followingimplications about tional patterns were not discussed; and practice can be drawn: degree of "highness- and "lowness" of I.Visualperceptioncanbeim- the socioeconomic extremes was not proved through training programs, and given. improvement ptobably operates favora- The following implications for prac- bly on learning to read. tice may be drawn : 2. The techniques used by experi- 1.The study soppf )rts the conten- mental teachers may be considered for tion that children can be taught visual incorporation into other kindergarten discrimination in the form of letter rec- programs. ognition during the readiness phase of 3.For well-organized kindergarten reading instruct ion. programs, a half-day program is not 2. The fact that low socioeconomic sufficient. level children profit more from train- ing suggests that these children should Rosen,CarlL.,andOlmmacht, be helped early. Fred.Sex Differences in the Factor 3. The children should be followed .51ructure of Selected Readiness Meas- to see what happens in initial reading ures and First Grade Reading instruction. Achievement. Among the strengths of 4. The study needs replication as a the study are the fact that the sample basis for broader generalizations. was selected with care and was of ade- quate size, and the related literature Gredler, G. R.Per on a was interwoven at appropriate points Perceptual Test with Children from a in the discussion. Culturally Disadmntaged Background A limitation of the study is that the The study was exploratory in nature. instructional program used was not de- The following limitations were scribed. noted: sample size was small; a more The following implications for prac- precise intelligencetest was needed tice can be drawn: andthediscussionofresultswas I.First grade teachers using stand- longer than the results justified. ardized reading tests similarto that The stink' needs replication with a used in this study can obtain little in- larger sample being used. formation of predictive value from sub- Color Cues as an tests of the Frostig and Metropolitan Otto, Wayne. idtoGood andPoorReaders' tests. Paired-Associate Learning. This study Teachers should be sensitive to 2. was neatly designed and clearly pre- perceptualdisabilitiesinfirstgrade sented.The researcher was especially children. cautions in drawing conclusions and 3. Teaches need to give attention generalizations. to the development of skills like those The study was of necessity limited measured by the Nletropolitan subsec- in scope. tions on word meaning, sentences, and The following implications can be information. drawn for further research: Wheelock, Warren H. An Investi- I. The same study should be under-

61 AARON 55

taken, this time with color cues being 3. A similar study involving a more pointed out to the subjects. realistic reading task, more like those 2.The study should be replicated encounteredinreadinginstruction, to determine ifthe results reflect a should be undertaken. chance happening.

62 Phonics InstructionWhen? What? For Whom?

H. ALAN ROBINSON-

A complexity involved in determining age child can articulate as many as twelve "When should phonicsinstruction be- consonant sounds. On entrance into first gin?" is in determining "What is phonics grade he can articulate all vowel sounds instruction?" Most of the studies do not andsixteentotwenty-oneconsonant describe programs of instruction in detail. sounds. However, it isn't until the end of They are vague about specific teaching grade three that most children are able to procedures and content. Terms are used discriminate among most sounds (12). It without suitable definition. Formal phon- appears that a great deal can be done with ics, analytical phonics, synthetic phonics, very young children in getting them to incidental phonics, and phonic readiness articulate soundsalthough more time are often used with the apparent expecta- seems needed in the development of audi- tion that the reader will understand with- tory discrimination. out explanation. Hence, phonics instruc- It also seems that much work needs to tion has become a term that often means be accomplished in helping children dis- different things to different people. criminate visuallynot for the so-called, Phonicsinstruction,inthiswriter's nonexistent look and say programbut opinion, is direct, planned teaching aimed forthe phonicsprogram. Successin at helping a pupil analyze printed or phonic analysis depends upon the achieve- written words to determine their pronun- ment of skill in each modalityvisual and ciations. As Gray has indicated, the child auditory. In addition, it would seem im- uses visual clues "...as aids in deter- perative that much emphasis be placed miningconsonant andvowelsounds, upon traininginsynthesis. Visual and syllabic divisions, and accented syllables." auditory discrimination must be synthe- Pupils must be helped to refine visual sized if a pupilis to achieve ultimate and auditory disgimination,". .. to success in phonic analysis. associateconsonant andvowel sounds with letters of the alphabet, and to blend When to Begin? these sounds into syllables and the sylla- bles into words with appropriate accent." "When should phonics instruction be- Phonics instruction, then, concentrates on gin?" is an ancient, confusing, and still helping pupils synthesize what they see essentiallyunanswered question.Only with what they hear in order to achieve seven reports spanning the years from accuracy in pronunciation. 1925 through 1958 (1, 3, 4, 5, 7, 10, 14) If this definition of phonics instruction represent the total range of "answers" is talid, phonic readiness must be consid- contributed by research. In these studies ered much more than the "ear training" a suggestions for when to begin phonics number of investigators suggest it may be. instruction are given in terms of mental From the investigations of people in the age or grade placement. speech field(2, 8),itis obvious that Reportinginterms of mentalage, articulation of soundsisan important Arthur (1) concluded that a mental age concept in phonic readiness. By the chron- of six and a half was the optimum time ological age of four and a half the aver- for introducing phonics instructional- 'William S. Gray, On Their Own in Reading. Chicago: Scott, Foresman and Co., 1960. Reading as an Intellectual Activity,IRA Proceedings, Vol. 8, 1963, 224.228.

63 ROBINSON 57

though some gains in phonic abilitywere England, also suggested delaying phonics made beginning at a mentalage of live instruction until grade two. The children and a half. She based her conclusionon in grade two of the English infants' the results of a study of 171 first graders school, however, were a yearyounger than grouped according to mentalage. Arthur's American children in grade two; hence, research design was well conceived,but Winch really concluded that phonics in- the teaching variable was not controlled. struction succeeds with most school chil- Dolch and Bloomster (3) also conducted dren who have reached the chronological a study of mental age and phonic achieve- age of five and a half. The studies con- ment. They found, on the basis of scatter. cerned with beginning phonics instruction grams made from 115 scores, that children and grade placement, although interest- of high mental age might sometimesfail ing in terms of group trends, suffered in acquiring phonic skills but children of from hazy control of significant variables. low mental age were certain to fail. A These few studies, with their flaws and mental age of seven seemed to be the limitations, appear to represent the only lowest at which a pupil could be expected available evidence of any substance relat- to use phonics. Dolch and Bloomster's ing mental age or grade placement to be- study, although it had a number of limi- ginning phonics instruction. Numerous tations recognized by the investigators, professionalwriters,apparently basing was carefully designed andreplicated. their conclusions on some of the available The word-attack tests used, however,were evidence, have generalized about the in- experimental and no informationwas fur- advisability of beginning phonics instruc- nished about their validity and reliability. tion before the mental age of six anda Durrell's recent report (4) basedon half or seven, or prior to a given time in four doctoral studies challenged the im- the chronology of instruction. Perhaps portance of mental age. Three of the in- such generalizations are necessary since no vestigators found rhat mentalage had a other conclusive proof is available and low relationship with the abilityto use teachers need the guidance in order to phonics and with reading achievementin develop an instructional program. On the general. Durrell recommended teaching other hand, such generalizations, repeated phonics, at varying rates, to all pupilsat over the years, eventually seem to become the very beginning of the first grade. accepted as factual information. Authors Helen Robinson, in a detailed criticLue of build them into reading series, curricula the Durrell report, concluded thatit did are based on them, and teachers accept. .. not supply dependable evidence to them. After a while, especially in instruc- determine... whether or not teaching tional situations, no one is sure of where letter names andlettersounds before a given generalization comes from, but teaching a sight vocabulary is essential."2 its very existence gives it some magical Reporting in terms of grade placement, importance. The doubter, the person with Garrison and Heard (5) concluded that another concept, will find it difficult in- most of the training in phonics should be deed to overcome the bias growing out of deferred until grades two and three. Mc- the "now-established-as-conclusive re- Dowell (7) and Sexton and Herron (10) search evidence." found that the teaching of phonicswas The professionalliterature does not of little or no value during the first half contain, by any standards, adequate re- of first grade. Sexton and Herron added ports by teachersdiscussing classroom that instruction in phonics was of greater experimentationindetail orreflecting value in grade two. Winch (14), ina about .possible changes in phonics instruc- study of teaching beginners to read in tion viewed by the teacher who has taught 'HelenM.Robinson, "News and Comment: first graders,for five or ten years. Since Methods of Teaching Beginning Readers." Eternal. torySchool Journal,LIX(May,1959), p. 426. there is elaaucity of controlled research

7-1 58 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES concerned with when to start phonics in- pupils make them then ready for phase struction, and since it is most difficult to one of phonics instruction. design research so that all variables are Can such evidence be obtained? Would controlled, an accumulation of accurate such research not be subject to the same reports by teachers would be of great basic flaws obvious in almost all of the value. Every effort must be made, how- research concerned with phonics instruc- ever, to report evidence based on logical tionlack of attention to the individual approaches tested and retested in many abilities and needs of pupils. Certainly situations. the research directed toward the appro- Even thenquestions about "Which priate time for instruction in phonics, re- came first, the chicken or the egg?" may portedhere,hasdealtwithgroup be raised, and with reason. For example, behavior, medians, and averages. the results of Russell's study(9) of A small body of research has been di- teachers' views on phonics indicated that rected toward certain kinds of individual the teachers favored emphasis on phonics needs. Investigators(11, 12, 13)inter- in grades two and three or in different ested in the auditory learning modality, grades dependent on need. What certainty have indicated that all children may not is there thatthis opinion is based on profit from an auditory-approach to read- years of experimental and creative teach- ing. Wepman (12) suggested that in- ing? Weren'ttheseteachersprobably struction, at least in the beginning stages, taught how to teach reading through the be individualized in terms of auditory use of professional materials and with and visuallearners. He indicated that the help of instructors dedicated to de- children "... who show inadequate dis- layed phonics instruction? Weren't they crimination of the discrete units of speech probably teaching reading with the help sounds usually have poor articulation in of manuals andinstructionalmaterials speech and equally poor ability to learn based on the concept of delayed phonics to read by phonicphonic approaches." j-le sug- instruction? Could they report without gested that . the teaching ofreading built-in bias? as well as therapy for the articulatory inaccuracy in speech production should first approach both tasks from other mo- Begin with Whom? dalities, e.g., visual or kinesthetic." If If phonics instruction incorporates the the question of when phonics instruction steps of visual discrimination, auditory should begin were now posed, the tenta- discrimination, and synthesis of the two, tive answer would have to be related to the research onwhen to teach phonicsis ability or needs in auditory discrimination. indeed inconclusive. Even those reading In the Garrison and Heard and Dolch programs labeled "traditional" or "look and Bloomstcr studies, there appeared to and say oriented" introduce this kind of be some evidence to support the conclu- phonics instruction in the kindergarten or sion that bright children in the first and very early in the first grade. Perhaps re- perhaps the second grades received more search directed toward when thevarious help from phonics instruction than the stepsof phonics instruction should take dull. One can assume, therefore, that an- place is needed. other answer might be to start teaching There is no reliable evidence that points phonics to the bright and delay instruc- to the latter part of kindergarten or to a tion for the dull. Grimes and Allinsmith mental age of five or to the first month of (6) on the basis of a study of third first grade as the best time for concentra- graders in two city schools, concluded tion on auditory or visual discrimination. that pupils who are highly compulsive, There is no reliable evidence that proves aJoseph M. Wepman, "The Inlerrelationahips of !fearing, Speech and Reading,"The Reading 50, 100, or three sight words learned by Teacher, XIV (March, 1961), p. 246.

fit5 ROBINSON 59

highly anxious, or both, will achieve bet- REFERENCES ter under a phonics program than under 1.Arthur, G., "A Quantitative Study of the the "look and say" approach. In this case Results of Grouping FirstGrade Classes According to Mental Age," Journal of the question of when seems related to EducationalResearch,XII (October, personality traits. 1925), pp. 173-185. It seems essential that thought and re- 2.Davis,I.P.,"The Speech Aspects of search related to phonics focus on the Reading Readiness," National Elementary Principal,17th Yearbook, XVII(July, individual needs of pupils. A great deal 1938), pp. 282-289. has been written and said about individ- 3. Dolch, E. W., and Bloomster, M., "Phonic uals and their needs, but research and Readiness," Elementary School Journal, instruction still seem pretty far removed XXXVIII (November, 1937), pp. 201- from practical utilization of the concept. 205. 4. Durrell, D. D. (ed.), "Success in First A number of questions related to the time Grade Reading," Journal of Education for beginning phonics instruction must CXL (February, 1958), pp 1-48. be answered. Do individual pupils learn 5.Garrison, S. C., and Heard, M. T., "Ar to attack words in very different ways and Experimental Study of the Value of Pho netics," Peabody Journal of Education, 11 at very different times? Are there enough (July, 1931), pp. 9-14. similarities about the way some children .6. Grimes, J. W., and Al tinsmith, W., "Corn learn so they can be grouped for instruc- pulsivity, Anxiety, and School Achieve tion in terms of certain modes of learn- ment," Merrill-Palmer Quarterly of Be havior and Development, VII (October ing? Would instruction based on specific 1961), pp. 247-269. learning needs accomplished in kinder- 7. McDowell, J. B., "A Report on the Pho garten or the beginning of grade one netic Meth 3d of Teaching Childrentc permit some childrentouse phonics Read," C 'shone Educational Review, L (October, 1953), pp. 506.519. earlier? Should some pupils never be 8. Poole, I., "Genetic Development of Articu- given instruction in phonics? If phonics lation of Consonant Sounds in Speech," instruction is begun very early, can there Elementary English Review, XI (June, be an adverse effect on pupils' school 1934), pp. 159-161. progress in later years? 9.Russell, D. H., "Teachers' Views on Phon- ics," Elementary English, XXXII (Octo- In a recent address concerned with the ber, 1955), pp. 371-375. future of beginning reading instruction, 10. Sexton, E. K., and Herron, J.S., The Gates stated that "whatever age is adopted Newark Phonics Experiment," Elementary as the 'normal' one for beginning reading, School Journal, XXVIII (May, 1928), pp. 691-701. some children can start earlier and others 11. Schmidt, B. G., "Teaching the Auditory should startlater for optimum results. Learner to Read," Chicago Schools Jour- The one certain outcome of research on nal, XIX (May-June, 1938), pp. 208-211. 12. Wepman, J. M., "Auditory Discrimina- the age of beginning reading is that an tion, Speech, and Reading," Elementary educational-psychological case or clinical School Journal, LX (March, 1960), pp study should be made of each child to 325-333. determine the best time and method of 13. Wepman, J. M., "The Interrelationship Hearing,Speech, andReading,"The introducing him to reading." Reading Teacher, XIV (March, 1961), pp. 245-247. 14. Winch, W. H., Teaching Beginners to 'Arthur T. Gates.A Look Ahead.Comments made Read in England: Its Methods, Results, at a Conference on Possibilities of Improving the and Psychological Bases, Journal of Edu- Teaching of Reading in the Primary Grades, under the auspices of the Department of Health, Education, cationalResearch Monographs No.8. and Welfare, Office of Education, Washington, D. Bloomington, Illinois: Publishing School C.. November 16, 1962, p. 2. Publishing Co., 1925, pp. 185. When Should Phonics Instruction Begin?

JOSEPHBRzEINSKI*

It is said, that in other times, a favorite problem. This needs to be kept in mind topic for discussion and debate was the when these research findings are cited. number of angels who could stand on the Then, too, the context of each research head of a pin. Currently, in its ability to study needs to be remembered. In decid- engender discussion, the_ subject of read- ing when beginning reading and phonics ing perhaps occupies a similar position. should be taught, itis necessary to con- Whenever topics such as the teaching of sider the individual learner, the kind and phonics or of beginning reading are men- quality of the materials and processes to tioned, quite strong views are expressed be taught. Obviously, the time at which a by parents and teachers alike. Professional highly complex system of phonics depend- journals, books, newspapers, and maga- ent upon memorization of rules and their zines have had numerous articles on these application could be taught would vary subjects. Many of the stories contain con- considerably from other less rigidly struc- flicting and quite contradictory claims and tured methods. counterclaims. In summary, available research evidence Research evidence is inconclusive. As appeared to establish that: Russell recently wrote concerning phonics, I. The mental age at which beginning "Unfortunately, no investigation in this readingand phonicscould be area can be labeled definitive. At least taught is quitedependentupon the thirty experimental or applied researches methods and materials of instruc have been carefully done but they all have tion. some limitations in score or technique."' 2.Auditory perception and visual That no definitive answer has been found discrimination are essential for suc- is also reflected by the question under cessful teaching of beginning read- discussion, "When should phonics in- ing and phonics. Some evidence also struction begin?" indicates skill in these areas can be To remove this problem from the realm developed by specific training. of fruitless discussion, the Denver Public 3.Success in beginning phonics and Schools decided to obtain objective data reading achievement is highly cor- concerning thesubject.Some national related to the child's opportunities publicity has been giventothis effort. for reading prior to entry into first The purpose of this report is to provide grade. Recentevidencesupports informationconcerning this research the position that early knowledge project which may be of some help in of letter names and sounds corre- answering the above question. lates substantiallywith reading A great deal of time during the first success in first grade. year was spent in reviewing pertinent re- 4.Children have been taught begin- search.Itsoon became apparentthat ning reading and phonics at ages studies concerning beginning reading and earlier than at which they presently phonics abound. However, because this are being taughtin most public is such a complex subject, most studies school systems. More detailed and tended to investigate a fragment of the comprehensiveinvestigations con- 'I)avid 11. Rtmell, "Reading Research That Makes caning introductoryreading and a thfierence," ifirmestary XXXVIII, No. 2 (February, 1961), pp. '4.78. phonics techniques inrelation to Reading as an Intellectual Activity, IRA Proceedings, 8, 1963, 228-232. BRZEINSKI 61

individual differences remain to be search design and the need for carefully carried out; for, in most kinder- planned instructional materials, it seemed gartens,quite a wide range of desirable to approach the project in a abilityexists. Some kindergarten cooperative manner. Such an approach children may be capable of learning secured the competencies of each of the beginning phonic and reading participants needed in a project of this skills. scope. Serving in an advisory capacity in theinitial formulation of the research Parenthetically, since the study began, design were Dr. Wilbur Schramm, Direc- there is a growing body of evidence to tor of the Stanford Institute for Commu- support the hypothesis that "The founda- nication Research, Dr. Howard E. Gruber tions of any subject may be taught to of the Behavioral Research Laboratory of anybody at any age in some form. "2 theUniversity of Colorado, and Mr. Reports emanating from Yale Univer- Theodore E. Albers, Director of Research sity, the University of Pittsburgh School and Statistics of the Colorado State De- of Education, and Teachers College, Co- partment of Education. Dr. John L. Hay- lumbiaUniversity,suggest that many man oftheStanford Institutefor three- and four-year-olds, if intellectually CommunicationResearch is statistical challenged and stimulated, can begin the consultant and analyst for the project. intellectual learning process. Working with the Denver Public Schools Perhaps, as was stated atthe 1959 as reading consultants are Professors Paul Woods HoleConference,"Readiness, McKee and M. Lucile Harrison of the thatis,is a function not so much of Colorado State College. Reading materials maturation as it is of our intentions and produced by them comprised the trial our skillattranslating ideasintothe reading program. language and concepts of the age level we are teaching."3 Reading Method With guidelines similar tothesein mind, aresearch design was carefully Seven groups of beginningreading framed. Its main focus was an experi- activities were developed. Primarily, these mental comparison of beginning the teach- activities provided practice in using be- ing of reading through the use of phonics ginning consonant sounds with contextual and meaning clues in kindergarten with or meaning clues to identify a printed beginning such teaching in the first grade. word. In the pilot method, phonics, i.e., Six hypotheses were formulated dealing the teaching of letter names and sounds, with the comparisons to be made among played an important role along with the four treatment groups. The main variable use of context or meaning clues. distinguishing the groups was the time of The rationale for the system used is beginning the teaching of readingkin- based on the fact that kindergarten chil- dergarten versusfirst grade. Numerous dren can recognize many thousands of other variables of importance were in- words when they hear them spoken. They cluded. These were concerned with read- know both the sounds and the meanings ing vocabulary,readingachievement, of these words. They do need to be taught reading habits, the quantity of reading, that the sounds they know are represented the incidence of reading disability, and by the particular letters and letter com- the like. binations in the printed words. This skill, Because of the complexity of the re- united with contextual or meaning clues, provides early steps toward independent 2.Terome S.Bruner, The Process of Education. Cambridge: Harvard University Preys. 1960. p.12. reading. /Jerome S. Bruner, On KnowingEssays for the Left Hand. Cambridge: Ilarvard University Press, No workbooks were used in the kinder- 1962, p. 108. garten. The reading activities combined 62 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

an auditory - visualapproach suitable to into control and research groups. group instruction. It has been suggested The children in the control classes fol- that the activities used are a kind of pre- lowed the regular kindergarten program. reading or reading readiness program. Childreninthe kindergarten research Since readiness is a complex term subject classes received instruction in the begin- to many understandings and misunder- ning reading activities every day. There- standings,theteaching procedure was maining two hours and ten minutes of designated either beginning reading ac- the kindergarten session was devoted to tivities or simply reading. The method established kindergarten procedures. was designed to capitalize upon individual When the children in the study entered differences innate in children. Teachers firstgrade,theresearchand control were encouraged to advance at arate groups were in turn divided into two consonant with the interest and ability of groups. This division provided four first the children. grade groups. Group I which had the regular pro- Research Procedure gram inkindergarten and the regular program in the first and later grades; Preliminary Study Group II which had a regular program In the Spring of 1959 the reading in kindergarten and the research program method developed was triedinfifteen in the first grade; Group III which had kindergartens. Suggestions for improve- the research program in kindergarten and ments and modifications based upon this the regular program in the first and later experience were received. While the ma- grades; and Group IV which had the terials were being revised, planning con- research program in kindergarten and the tinuedandapreliminarystudy was adjusted program in the first and later inaugurated in approximately 30 kinder- grades. gartens during the second semester of the Group I provided a necessary basis for 1959.1960 school year. Purpose of the comparisonwiththe researchgroups. preliminary study was to test the research Group II permitted a comparison between design which had been developed and to groups who had received the same in- discover what problems might arise. struction introduced at different times. Group III made possible the assessment Maio Study of the effect of early reading not followed In the Fall of 1960 the Denver Public upa danger often encounteredina Schools, with the aid of a grant from the short-term study. Group IV was estab- CooperativeResearchBranchofthe lished to test the full effect of beginning United States Office of Education, began a reading in the kindergarten followed by longitudinal research study to determine a program accelerated to take advantage the effectiveness of beginning the teach- of any gains made. ing of reading in kindergarten. Progress The study is longitudinal and thepu- of pupils in the project is to be studied pils are to be tested periodicallyusing through the fifth grade. standardizedreadingtestsandother When the main study began, the Den- appropriate measures including specially ver Public Schools had about 9,000 kin- devised tests and evaluative techniques. dergarten children divided into about 300 The primary variable consideredwas time classes. This study involved 122 classes of beginning reading. Other variables in- randomly assigned by school to control cluded mental age, chronological age, I.Q., and research groups. This resulted in 61 and family characteristics. The principal classes in the control group and 61 classes statistical technique used has been analysis in the research group. Thus, approxi- of covariance. This method of analysis, mately 4,000 pupils were divided equally in effect, equates or matches the groups 69 7

BRZEINSKI 63 being analyzed. Because the design has group had an average loss of 3.47 large numbers of experimental subjects, points. The difference in these means is not statisticallysignificant, seeming the statistical work has been programmed to indicate that the children in the pilot for computers. group maintained their advantage dur- ing the summer months. Results That not all children progressed at the Since the present study is of a longi- same rate is among other findings shown tudinalnature,anyconclusionsbased by an analysis of kindergarten data. Some upon the findings reported at this time children made little progress in learning must be considered tentative and applica- the seven steps of the trial program. Some ble only to the trial procedures. Indeed, children were able to read preprimers and it would be well to view these conclusions primers during the latter part of the kin- as hypotheses subject to further testing, dergarten. Most children were able to modification, or verification. recognizeletters,to learn letter names Kindergarten Findings and sounds, and with the help of context, to read words. Analysis of teacher ques- Analysis of data gathered in the kin- tionnaires and interview data showed that dergarten was made to determine the growth in reading in the kindergarten suitability of the trial method and ma- could be achieved without greatly modify- terials for that age level. This analysis ing existing kindergartenprograms involved a pretest administered in Octo- through betterutilization of the time ber 1960 and a post-test in May 1961. A presently available. teacher questionnaire provided additional information. The results indicated that: First-Grade Findings Tests used to gather data in the first 1. Kindergarten children could and did grade were the Gates Primary Reading learn certain beginning reading phonic Test and the Gates Advanced Primary skills. Reading Test. Analysis of adjustedtest Kindergarten-agechildren were able to learn letter forms, letter scores suggests that names, and letter sounds. 1. The children who had the beginning 2. A planned program of systematic in- reading activities in kindergarten scored structionin beginning reading skills significantly better at the end of first appeared to be more effective than a grade than did the children who had program whichincidentallyprovided the regular kindergarten program. opportunityforthe development of . The pilot group which began read- readinggrowth.Bothgroups made ing in the kindergarten had an gains. The classes with the planned adjusted mean score of114.35, program gained an average of21.8 which was 18.45 higher than that points while the classes with an inci- of the control group which started dental program gained 12.9 points. This reading in the first grade. difference was statistically significant at 2. Optimum reading achievement was ob- the .001 level of confidence. tained when adjustments were made in 3. Children taught the btginning reading the first grade program to take advan- skills in kindergarten did not forget tage of gains made in the kindergarten. them during the summer intermistion. Pupilsinthiscategory had an The possibility existed that sine the adjusted test score mean of 118.06. pilot classes seemed to learn more than This was 10.70 higher than the did the control classes, they would also group which had the beginning forget more during the summer stnnths. reading activities in kindergarten To test this hypothesis 49 et;Idren in and the regular program in the the pilot group and 49 chilch,n in the first grade. control group were tested in S«;,--;ember 3. The time of introduction of the begin- 1961 when these children 4V4.1 just ning reading activities had a significant entered first grade. The children in the effect on achievement. Those children pilot group had an average loss of 1.45 who were taught the pilot materials in points while the children in the control kindergartenweresignificantlybetter

70 64 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

readers than those children who began the same method in the first grade. combination with context, seemed to help The children who had the begin- children progress successfully when they ning reading program in kinder- later began to read in books. A practical garten and continued with an ad- implication would be the provision of justed program in the first grade appropriate possibilitiesfor growth in scored13.92 higher than those who began the pilot method in reading in the kindergarten. Growth in the first grade. reading is too important to be ignored These differences reported are significant or left to chance incidental development. beyond the .001 level of confidence. Results of the present research studysug- gest planning must occur if kindergarten- Conclusions age children are to experience continuous growth in reading concomitant with their Results at the end of two years of study growth in other important areas. Experi- appear to establish an advantage for chil- ence has shown such growth need not be dren who had an opportunity to learn at the expense of growth in other areas elements of beginning phonics and read- vital to a sound, balanced program of ing in the kindergarten. Knowledge of kindergarten instruction. letter names, sounds, and forms, used in Common Sense about Phonics

JOSEPHINEPIEKARZs ON THE subjectofof phonics arc: (1) that phonics is WRITERS"phonics in reading" are di-taught and used apart from word vided into two widely opposing fac-structure and other word identifica- tions, which seem to be separated bytion techniques, and (2) that phonic a vast chasm of incommunicationprinciples or generalizations apply to due largely to misinformation andallsyllables within written words. missing information concerning theMany champions of phonics behave relationship between the spoken andas if our language were a consistently written forms of our language. Fewspelled one and belittleall except feuds in history have had the endur-phonic techniques for word identifi- ing quality demonstrated by the pro-cation, unaware that they themselves tagonists and antagonists of phonics.rely on a combination of techniques No other single subject or area hasin identifying words. On the other had as much written about it over ashand, many antagonists of phonics long a period of time. Scarcely adeny the underlying relationship be- month passes that the professionaltween speech and writing in our lan- and lay periodicals do not carry aguage, discredit the use of phonics by number of articles on the subject.pointing out what appear to them to These articles reveal that the twobe exceptions to phonic rules, un- factions are as widely separated asaware of how heavily they themselves everdespite the fact that writtenrely on phonics in identifying printed English has been in use for manywords. The place of phonics in the centuries. The defenders of phonicsreading of our language can be un- seem to believe that a rigid set ofderstood only in the light of the rela- rules governing the pronunciation oftionship between the spoken and letters is the one true road to successwritten forms of our language. Some in reading; the opposition seems toof the relevant facts are: believe that there is no point in teach- 1. Language is man-made, having been ing phonic generalizations becausedeveloped by man over eons of time. Oral there are so many exceptions. The forms of language always precede written forms of language. Each individual child fact is that one belief is as wrong as in his turn recapitulates the development the other. Only people naive about of the language of his species, progressing from a stage of gestures and random bab- the relationship between the spoken bling sounds in orderly sequence through and written forms of the English lan-speech and oral reading to silent reading. the most advanced stage of language devel- guage believe in either view. opment. The two most frequent and serious 2. Language is symbolic and has expres- sive and receptive aspects. Meaning exists in errors or shortcomings suggested by reality, for which spoken and written words the popular writings on the teaching are merely symbols. Reading is the recep- The Reading Teacher,18 (November 1964) , 114.117.

72 66 GENERAL PROGRAMAND POLICIES

tine end ofthewritten communicative process. manly upon knowledge of the sounds S. Various forms of graphic languageof the spoken form of the language, have been attempted during the course of time, bearing different kinds of relation-knowledge of the letters of the writ- shipstoreality and speech. Alphabeticten form of the language, as well as writing, the written form of our language. is based on the premise that spoken wordsknowledge of the various circum- consist of combinations of sounds. Alpha- stances and conditions under which betic characters are graphic representations of sounds. In other words, writing is man- particular letters and combinations ually recorded speech. of letters represent particular sounds. -1. And finally. since the number of dif- ferent alphabetic characters in our written This constitutes what is usually called language does not equal the number ofphonics and word structure. It is es- different sounds in our spoken language. we do not have a one-to-one relationshipsentially a process of reconstructing between sound and letter. The disparityspeech from written symbols. Phon- between sounds (approximately forty-five) and letters (twenty-six. three of which do ics cannot be used apart from word not have sounds of their own) means thatstructure since they are inseparably smut- letters must represent more than one sound. Experience with our written lan-interrelated.Letters and combina- guage also indicates that a single sound maytions of letters in our written lan- be represented or spelled by different letters and combinations of. letters. This does notguage (particularly those represent- mean, however, that our language is noting vowel sounds) represent given alphabetic and phonetic. It merely means that there isn't a one-to-one relationshipsounds depending upon the structure between letter and sound, and that it prob- of the word in question. Therefore, it ably takes longer and is more difficult to learn the correct associations between what is impossible to apply phonic princi- is seen and what is heard than in many ples until the structure of the word is other more consistently spelled languages. Difficulties in learning to read our languageknown. Attempting to teach phonics are due to the large number of associations between letters and sounds which must be apart from word structure or struc- learned, as well as to the surface variability tural analysis will inevitably lead to of these associations. The letters though, however arbitrarily they seem to be put frustration, discouragement, failure, together in written words, stillrepresent and loss of confidence in our written sounds, so our language is phonetic. language. It will most certainly result in countless exceptions to rules. Learning to read our language in- Even one-syllable words have to be volves learning to associate spokenestablished as one-syllable words be- sounds and written symbols while atfore any phonic principles or gener- the same time mentally reconstruct-alizations can be applied correctly. ing the things and ideas representedThe syllable "can"is pronounced bythelanguagesymbols.Withwith a short a sound in the root word enough practice, the process of asso-can. The same syllable is pronounced ciating sounds and symbols recedeswith a long a sound in the word chi- to a subconscious level, while atten-canery, since chicanery isderived tion is directed toward the meaningfrom the word chicane. The verb be represented by the words. However,is pronounced with a long e sound; in learning to read, the associationsthe same combination of letters in must be made on a conscious level.bedim, bemoan, and beset, however, The correct associations depend pri-are pronounced differently because

. 73 PIEKAFtZ 67

"be" serves as a prefix in these wordsassociation with sound. Large com- and is not part of the root wordsplex bodies of learnings cannot be themselves. learned unless there is underlying or- Furthermore, the phonic rules order.If teachers do not teach this generalizationsthat arefrequentlyorder in readingthe logical and taught apply only to single-syllablecorrect relationships between spoken root words and to the accented sylla-and written wordspupils are left on ble, but not to the unaccented sylla-their own to try to figure this out for bles, of polysyllabic words. For exam-themselves.Itis amazing that as ple, many teachers teach that "whenmany children manage to do this as two vowel letters such as ai appearwell as they do. together, the first vowel letter has a It is ordinarily neither desirable long sound and the second vowelnor necessary to analyze phonetically letter is silent," and then complaincomplete words while reading. Doing bitterlyaboutalltheexceptions.so makes reading a laborious and Words like certain, captain, and cur-time-consuming process. Pupils tain are not exceptions to the rule, ifshould be taught to use only as much the rule has been taught correctly.phonetic analysis as necessary to iden- A short a sound will not be found intify printed words correctly. Once a orphan, woman, or husband, sinceword is identified, there is no further the letter a appears in the unaccentedprofit to be gained in continuing its second syllable of these words. Noranalysis. In actual reading situations can a long a sound be expected in thethe tedious analysis of entire words unaccentedsuffixofwordslikeis seldom necessary if pupils have wreckage, portage, and spoilage, norbeen taught to use configuration, pic- a short a sound in errant, servant, orture, and meaning clues in conjunc- occupant. None of these words is antion with structural-phonetic analy- exception to the rules or generaliza-sis. Fluency in word identification tions governing the pronunciation ofand reading results from the com- vowel sounds. Phonics cannot bebined use of the various word recog- separated from word structure andnition techniques, not from struc- taught in isolation. Doing so simplytural-phonetic analysis alone. Struc- will not work in our language; it willtural-phonetic analysis, however, cor- only result in disillusionment. Struc-rectly applied remains the most valu- tural-phonetic analysis would be aable single technique for identifying more appropriate term for this proc-the written words of our language. ess since both aspects have to be Valuable as itis, structural-pho- considered simultaneously. netic analysis cannot always identify There is a great deal more con-words correctly in our language. The sistency and logic inherent in ourcorrect pronunciation, for example, written language than most peopleof words like close, live, tear, and apparently see, but it is not at therebel can be determined only in the level of a consecutive letter-by-letterlight of context. For this reason, also,

74 68 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES other word identification techniquesassociations between sounds and let- besidesstructural-phoneticanalysis ters or groups of letters. When the must be taught.Pupils must beassociations can be made easily and taught how toutilize the variousefficiently, they recede to a subcon- word identification techniques sepa-conscious level, leaving the reader rately and together. None of themfree to attend to the meaning of the alone will succeed in identifying allsymbols.Structut al-phonetic analy- the words of our written languagesis accounts for the major portion of accurately. Any pupil who cannot usethe fixed relationships between the them all is handicapped to some de-sounds and written symbols of our gree or other. language. Phonetic analysis must al- Since writing is manually recordedways be carried on in relation to the speech, reading is the process of re-basic structure of words. If word constructingspeech fromwrittenstructure is ignored, phonetic analy- symbols in addition to mentally re-sis fails. Efficient word identification constructingthe things and ideasand reading are possible only through represented by the symbols. Learn-thecombined useofstructural- ing to read our written language in-phonetic analysisandother word volves learning to make the correctrecognition techniques. Phonics Materials: A Big Seller

DOLORES DURKIN*

EVEN A QUICK STROLL up andplace of honor. For the market place down the aisles of a conventionthe confusion has resulted in "a big exhibit hall provides ample evidenceseller." that educational materials are "big But there is another characteristic business." In fact, corporations likeof phonics that makes it especially IBM, RCA, and CBS can now beenticing to the publisher: its content viewed as the latest alphabet to dotcan be packaged.It canfill up the educational marketplace (4). workbooks. It provides material for Predictably, the involvement ofcharts,cards,pictures, and film- large corporations has led to somestrips. It can be pressed into phono- changes. Clearly apparent are moregraph. records. It can be put into sophisticated advertising, more col-the form of a "game." It can be orful materials, more urbane salesused to take advantage of the cur- personnelin fact, more of every-rent interest in programmed instruc- thing; for quantity is the very essencetion. And, even more recently,it of big and successful business. Inprovides ideal material for the teach- spite of the new and more glamorousing kit. Could we expect such poten- facade, however, one thing remainstial to be overlooked by the pub- unchanged: the unique importancelisher? Hardly. But as a result the of reading to the entire educationalquestion of discriminate buying takes enterprise continues, making materi-on paramount importance. als connected with the teaching of reading skills one of the most lucra-The Discriminate Buyer tive pieces to be found in the whole Discriminate buying, whether of a of the market place display. It isworkbench or a workbook, always probably equally accurate to addrequires a knowledge of the product that, of all the various skills compris-what it can and cannot do, for ing reading, phonics is the juiciestinstance. And this will not change. from an economic point of view.But what is changing, even as the Why? nature of the market place changes, A variety of factors account foris an ever increasing need for the the special market value of phonics.buyer to possess what might simply For one thing, phonics instructionbe called the will to resist temptation. oftenis equated with reading in- To be sure, temptation has always struction. While such an equationbeen the goal of the merchant and, confuses a part with the whole, thecorrespondingly, the downfall of the association has still resulted in as-buyer.But today the temptation signing to phonics a rather specialheld out to the man with money in

The Reading Teacher,20 (April 1967), 610.614. R. 70 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

his pockets must be, and often is,the sole reason why phonics materi- uniquely enticing and even manipu-als are a big seller. Certainly other lative. Reflecting the technologicalfactors are at work too. One, for and affluent society which surroundsexample, is related to the kind of it and which also has created it, thepublic criticism that has been leveled modern market place no longer isat the schools for about the last ten characterized by the classical supply-years. Starting with the publication demand relationship. The "good oldof Why Johnny Can't Read in 1955, days" in which what was producedthe focus ofa surprisinglylarge was what was needed are now partamount of criticism has been the of past history. Replacing them is ancharge thattheschoolsarenot economic era in which the tradi-teaching phonics (2). This has been tional sequence has been reversed.the case whether the complaint was Today the first step is production,about reading, as in the Flcsch book, and the second step is the deliberateor something as broad as the possible creation of a need for what has beeninferiority of American schools as produced.Indeed,thewaresofcompared with those of Soviet Rus- Madison Avenue have never been sosia (5). importantnor so effective. Withinthepastdecade,too, Although in the past the schoolsphonics instruction has worked its have sometimes been accused of re-way into public debates about man- maining apart from the realities ofners and morals, and frequently into life, nobody now could deny thatpolitically conservative publications. they are very much immersed in theAs recently as November 1966, for reality of this new market place.instance, Russell Kirk was writing There is not only a larger number ofin National Review statements like, children to educate and teachers to"Among the educationists phonics is help; there is also that "man witha dirty word," and, as he gave his money in his pockets." Thanks todescriptionofelementary schools, .6. the generosity of federal funds, the .actual teaching by phonics is educator has become a wealthy man,taboo" (3). and so a most welcome customer in That statements like these could the aisles of the market. Perhaps itnever be documented has less effect goes without saying that, among theon practice than ought to be the counters devoted to reading, phonicscase.Infact,publicaccusation materials are richly and enticinglyabout too little phonics is one of the displayed. factors that has resulted in too much phonics, in certain schools. In their Motives for Buying Phonies eagerness to have very visible phon- Materials icsprograms,forinstance, some While the allurement of the mod-administrators have gone all-out in ern market place is undeniably great,stressing phonics and in purchasing it would be erroneous to think it isphonics materials. While these efforts

t DURKIN 71

to "prove a point" are psychologi- results,ifthe cures are followed. cally understandable, they are notMost of these prescriptions, of course, always professionally defensible. Justcall for immediate and frequent in- as some schools would profit from astruction in phonics. little more and a little better phonics To thinkthatparents'beliefs instruction, others could probably beabout the best way to teach begin.- improved simply by having less. ning reading have no effect on a Related to the public indictmentschool's reading program would be of too little phonics instructionis naive. As a minimum they encour- still another factor which sometimesage the administration to take an- enters into a school's decision to in-other look at the way reading is troduce more and more phonics and,taught in the early grades. At the as is generally the case, to buy moreotherextreme, however,parents' and more phonics materials. Thisbeliefs about the special advantages source of influence is parents. of much phonics instruction have Typically, what a parent knowsbeen known to result in obviously about reading comes from the news-increased amounts inthe schools, paper or, perhaps, one of the month-sometimes as early as the kinder- lymagazines written for women.garten year. When this popular press reporting While nobody would deny the deals with reading,it most oftenright of a parent to be concerned focuses on itsbeginnings and, inabout his child's education, and even conjunction with this, the advan-to have definite ideas about the way tages of phonics instruction com-it ought to be effected, the priority pared to what is called the "look-of professionalism needs to be recog- say" approach. nized when decisions about educa- It is doubtful that the frequenttion are to be made. In the case of selection of beginning reading as thephonics instruction, decisions about topic of popular press articles is ac-its timing and, for instance, about cidental: never is a parent so con-whether it ought to be taught induc- cerned about reading as when histively or deductively are professional child is just starting to learn. Howmatters: When these decisions are well the child progresses at the be-affected by such factors as the desire ginning is viewed not only as anto "pacify" parents, the results are indicator of future progress in read-not always what is best for the chil- ing, but even as a forecast of how dren learning to read. well the child will do in all areas of There is still another reason !nod- thecurriculum. Such uncommonvatins; the purchase of volumes of importance makes the parent eagerphonics materials.Itisneither a to learn about beginning reading."right" reason nor a "wrong" one; And it also makes him somewhatbut when it existsit ought to be gullible as popular press articles offerrecognized. This isthe fact that sure cures and even "guaranteed"some teachers are not prepared to 72 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES teach phonics and, as a result, feel Certainly the first stepwhether an unusual need for materials. the intentisto help children or Citing the possibility of this sourceteachers or bothis clarification of of motivation is not to be critical ofthe role phonics is to play in a total teachers. Rather, it is to be awarereading program. In the opinion of that many were elementary schoolthiswriter, phonicsinstructionis children when phonics was givenvery important. It is one way to help scant attention. As a result, unlessa child develop independence as a these teachers are lucky enough toreader and as a learner. But to rec- have had a reading methodologyognize its importance is not to lay course which actually taught themaside the fact that phonics is just one the content of phonics, they enterkindofreadinginstructionand, their own classrooms with feelings oftherefore, only one part of the total inadequacy and insecurity. Thesereading program. Without this per- feelings arc natural, but they leadspective, phonics all too easily be- to overreliance on materials. Andcomes isolated as an end initself, while this might be advantageous foractually losing its very reason for the merchant, it is hardly good forbeing: a means toward identifying the classroom. What the dependencyunfamiliar words. creates is a situation in which the Once clarity about the contribu- teacherismerely an assistanttotion of phonics is achieved, decisions materialsa role hardly worthy ofhave to be made about whether its a professional person, and hardlycontent will be taught inductively productive of the best kind of in-or deductively, and also when the struction. teaching will begin. It is not the in- tention of this article to take a stand Some Factors in Considering on these questions; that has been Materials done with considerable detail else- If a school should decide to buywhere ( / ). Rather, the purpose is materials to help its faculty learnto recognize the existence of the more about phonicsand this surelyquestions, and to point out the need is laudablethe reason for the pur-for a faculty to arrive at some an- chase ought to be taken into accountswers, hopefully for "right" reasons. as particular materials are selected.Once this is accomplished, a school Thisisimportant because whatis ready to consider whether there might besuitableforeducatingare materials in the market place teachers is not necessarily best forthat might be helpful. teaching children. With them, dif- When materials are being consid- ferent criteria have to be kept inered for children, the first kind to mind. With both, however, the re-look for are those that might actual- quirements of prudent choices beginly instruct. Assuming, for instance, even before the customer gets to thethat a school has some children who market place. are able to learn at least part of the 79 7 w ".".S'""

DURKIN 73

content of phonics from a bookphonics materials are an especially rather than a teacher, it then makesbig seller in the educational market sense to try to find materials whichplace. Its content has been devel- allow for independent learning, andoped on the assumption thatan also for a pace of learning that canawareness of some of the factors that be matched to the abilities of indi-affect buying will make the buyer vidual children. more perceptive and discriminating. More frequently, though, schools In no sense does the selected focus will be looking for materials whichinfer that educators must rely only review instruction given by a teach-on published materials to do their er, or which provide practice in thejob. Certainly some of the best ma- use of what has been taught. Whenterials used by children are what materials are being considered tothey themselves make, or what a serve these functions, the most im-teacher makes out of her knowledge portant requirementand the onlyof what needs to be taught and prac- one that will be discussed hereisticed. However, itis unrealistic to that they facilitate a teacher's effortsthink that all the materials required to individualize instruction. by classroominstructioncanbe For example, materials providing"home made." This being the case, practice in the use of short vowelthere is the need to look to the pub- sounds ought to have enough cover-lisher for help. But, as this article age for the child who will need ahas tried to show, there also is an great deal of practice. But, in addi-ever increasing need to be wary of tion, the various kinds of practicehis products, knowing that the mo- ought to be assembled in a way thattivation of the educator and that of allows a teacher to make selectionsthe publisher will not always be the on the basis of what is needed bysame. individual children, and at a time when the need is identified. As prac- (Dolores Durkin is Professor of tice material is now generally putEducation at the University of Illi- together by publishersin the typi-nois.) cal workbook, for instanceindivid- ualized instruction is not only not References facilitated; itis often made moreI. Durkin,Dolores.Phonicsandthe Teaching of Reading. NewYork: Du- difficult. In fact, materials like the reau of Publications, Teachers College, typical workbook are the most pro- Columbia University, 1965. 2. Flcsch, Rudolph.Why Johnny Can't lific reminder that what is needed by Read. New York: Harper, 1955. a teacher is not always what is pro-3. Kirk, Russell. "Why Don't They Teach Phonics?"National Review,18 (Nov. duced by the publisher. 15. 1966) , 1169. 4. Silberman, Charles E. "Technology Is Knocking atthe Schoolhouse Door," A Summary Fortune,74 (Aug. 1966) , 120 ff. This brief article has tried to enu- 5. Trace, ArthurS.What Ivan Knows That Johnny Doesn't. NewYork: Ran merate a few of the reasons why dom House, 1961. 80 The Differences between Linguistics and Phonics

DOROTHY Z. SEYMOUR*

READING TEACHERS who have taken the time to examine articles on linguistics and reading have not always been convinced that a linguistic approach offers anything new. On first examination. linguistics seems merely to be returning certain advantages of old- time phonics to reading instruction. The similarities are inescap- able. There is a renewed emphasis on sound, on association of sound with letter, and on the use of sound-letter associations in the analy- sis of new words. But a true linguistic approach offers muchmore than phonics. It contributes the discoveries of scholarship in the field of linguistics to the field of reading instruction. Themost important contribution can be summed up in one word: exactitude. The precepts of phonics suffered from terminology thatre- vealed a basic misconception about readingthat "words"are basically written words, and that children must be taughtto "pro- nounce" them, or "sound" them. To this end, children were first taught about letters and then were told to -pronounce" the letters or "make" their sounds. Teachers constantly referred to "the sounds of the letters," and even to "silent" letters. The letterswere also endowed with the power of dictating sounds and changes in sounds: "the e makes the a long." It is this same preoccupation with the letters which made teachers say there are five vowels, when actually there are fourteen. The linguists have shown how confused this approach was.

MISCONCEPTIONS IN PHONICS 1."Words" are written words In giving phonics instruction, teachers constantly referredto words as though they did not exist outside of the world of print, whereas, of course, speech developed before ameans of symbolizing that speech in print. The assumption that "words" meant "written words" gave children the impression that wordswere not first oral words, or mental words, before the author put them into symbols. But of course, printed words did not spring into existence from nothing. Another way of saying this is that wordswere words before they were printed words.

The Reading Teacher,23 (November 1969), 99.102, 111.

81 SE YN1OUR 75

2."Pronounce" the word In phonics instruction, children were often givena list of new printed words to analyze. The teacher's job was to teach the chil- dren to "pronounce" them. This approach did not recognize the fact that children already know how to pronounce words when they conic to school. In fact. they usually need no instruction at all in how to pronounce their own language. What the teacher needsto do is to teach them to read it. Directing a six-year old to "pronounce" the printed word Mother is patently ridiculous. 3."Sound" the word Phonics instruction was based on sounds that were associated with letters. Children were thus instructed to "sound"a word that is, make the sounds that each letter "had." Children would then produce a series of pre-taught sounds, and the resulting chain of sounds was supposedly "a word." The fact that the child didnot always recognize the result as a word he had spoken countless times, and was often just mouthing a chain of sounds, made the conscientious phonics teacher frustrated and confused. Itwas this aspect of phonics which brought complaints that phonics-taught children were not readers, they were sound-makers. The instructional process should have been reversed. Teachers were presenting children with letters, and then superimposing the instruction of sounds upon those letters.,But children already know the sounds ofieir language; they have had five or six years of instruction in language sounds (phonetics) by the time theycome to school. What they need is not to learn a series of sounds that "letters have," but which symbols i letters)go with the sounds they already know. It is the letters that are new co them,not the sounds. If children can be shown that they are making certain sounds when they say words, then they can be shown which Letterssym- bolize those sounds. Thus they proceed from the known (sounds) to the unknown (letters). 4."Each letter has a sound" The phonics teacher's confused procedurestartingwith a letter instead of a language soundcompelled herto find a way of dragging in the sound after the letter was already introduced. So she used a phrase which implied that, somewhere insideor behind each letter, there lurked a sound, and that each time that letter appeared, the child should "make" that sound. The truth is that the sounds have been around a lot longer than the letters, and the letters merely stand for the sounds; they do not "have" the sounds somewhere inside them. If it is made clear, and kept clearto the 82 76 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

child (by presentation in a logicalmanner and by judicious word- ing) that the letters represent the sounds made in speech,the child has a better chance of analyzing the printed wordas a word he already can speak, such as "Mother," instead ofas a series of extra- neous sounds. 5."A letter makes a sound" The relation between speech and writingwas often explained by the phonics teacher as a series of letters which"make" certain language sounds, as though if you pinched the letter i,it would squeak out the sound /i/ as in pin. Lists of phonics rulesmight claim that a makes the sound a in man,a makes the sound a in angel, ai makes the sound i in train,ay makes the sound 5 in say. or the e in bake makes the letter a say its own name. The linguist expresses the relation between speech and writing by pointing to the different symbols which can be used torepresent our language sounds. He refers to the (a phoneme) before listing the symbol that can be used to represent it (a grapheme): the sound /ey/ may be represented by a in angel, all in say,a-e in bake, and ci in vein. In this presentation there is?to attempt to claim that itis the letter which "makes" the sound. It is people who make sounds, which are then symbolized in alphabetic spellings for the purpose of reading. 6."Silent fetters" Letters do not "make" sounds; similarly, they donot "make" silence. The existence of a letter like k ina word like know, which no longer represents a sound we speak, does not mean that the letter is "being silent." It means that the English word knowwas once pronounced /knew/, and that its spelling still reflects that pronunciation. In the present day. the sound /n/ in the wordknow is represented by the letters krt. Thus it is not the letter kwhich is failing to "make a sound;" people have stopped using the sound /k/ at the beginning of that word. English spellings simplydo not keep up with changes in English speech. and examples likeknow provide the best opportunity the teacher willever have to demon- strate this phenomenon for the children. 7. The -e makes the a longintake" pie phonics teacher often explained language sounds as being controlled by letters. But letters do not have thepower to control a change in sound. People utter the sounds, and the lettersrepre- sent and reflect their utterances. And it is not the length of the vowel sound that is distinctive in the word take. What thephonics

83 SEY HOUR 77

teacher meant is that this wond is said withthe phoneme /ey/ rather than with the phoneme Al.as in man. One of the ways of symbolizing the phoneme /ey/ in Englishis with the vowel pattern a-e, as in take. 8. We have "five vowels" It is the suppos'ed pre-emption of printover speech which made the phonics teacherassume that English has five vowels. She meant that the written version of our languageutilizes only five vowel letters. This statement obscures theimportant reality that English has at least fourteen vowel sounds;seven of them move from one sound to another and are called glided vowelsounds. The approach of the phonics teacher to vowelswas to explain the dif- ferent vowel sounds as functions of lettercombinations--as if a letter could dictate spech. Actually, the lettercombinations are the result, at least in part, of the vowel sounds. Olderword-pronuncia- tions explain much of the rest ofour spelling. 9."Write the sound" The phraseology of phonics instruction oftenassumed that a letter was a sound. Childrenwere told to look at the sound. and teachers were directed to write the soundon the board or on a chartas though a sound were something that couldbe seen or written. In fact, teachers even referredto a letter as if it actually were the sound. When teaching the sound /a/ as in apple, teachers would point to the letter a andsay. "This is the sound /al." This tactic caused further confusion whenat another point, the teacher pointed to the same letter a and stated, "This isthe letter /ey/." The fact of the matter. ofcourse, is that the sound /a/ as in apple can be represented by different symbols,but is most regularly represented by the letter a, particularly whenthe vowel letter is between two consonant letters. The lettera can stand for other language sounds aswell. Teachers must clearly differentiate between letter; and the sounds theyrepresent. in order to avoid confusion between the two.

CONCLUSION The main difference betweenalinguistic and a phonic approach to reading instruction is that phonicsgave primacy to letters, and seemed to place spoken languageunder their control, whereas linguistics points to the priority of speech,and demon- strates that writing is merely a way of recording thatspeech by the use of symbols. Proof of the antecedenceof speech to writing 84 78 GENERAL PROGRAM AND POLICIES

lies in the work of linguists with "primitive" tribes; they often dis- covered that these groups had very elaborate languagesystems which were, however, as yet unrecorded in writing. The job of these anthropological linguists was to devise a method of writing such languages, But the fact remained that, before the linguists entered the scene, those languages were already in existence, albeit in oral form. A linguist, in devising a method of writing the language,was not creating the language itself; he was merely recording it. Thus the language was not controlled by the symbols: the symbolswere dependent upon the language. Similarly, the child who is taught reading froma linguistic point of view is led to understand that a person who can speakcan also learn to read and write. Ile knows that he will be in control of the symbols he uses to represent his language; the symbols will not control him.

85 PART III: The Curriculum in Word Recognition The twelve articles in Part III present data that will determine which elements in phonics are important enough to justify inclusion in thecur- riculum and which elements are rarely usedor may actually be misleading and harmful. The pioneer study by Clymer in January 1963 is included. His study covered the primary grades only. Inmore recent years, follow-up research by Bailey. Emans, and Burmeister includes only grades above the primary or the entire range of the elementary schoolyears. All the articles deal with sounding out the letters in words except for the lone article by Winkley which evaluates generalizations for syllables. To date, recent publications of the International Reading Association have not included the following aspects of the word recognition curricu- lum: structural analysis and contextual clues.

86 Fundamental Principles Underlying Phonics Instruction

DOLORES DURKIN*

VOLUMES have been written about the to assume that students in my methods fundamental principlesunderlying courses needed help with how to teach phonics instruction. Not all writers, of phonics, but not with its content. And so, course, agree on what is "fundamental." during that first year of college teaching, I Some, for instance, insist that "good" in- discussed how I thought phonics should struction begins with the teaching of let- be taught and why I took this position. It ter sounds which, later on, are blended happened thoughand I suppose this is into syllables and then into words. Other experienced by most new teachersthat authors, to the contrary,maintainthat for aclass which was concerned with "good"instructionstartswithwhole phonics, I ran out of material before I ran words, from which letter-sounds can be out of time. I recall how, with a kind of identified. Thus, the debate about deduc- desperation,I suggested to the students tive 11. inductive phonics continues. that the remaining time would be used to I will not enter into this debate here; I review their knowledge about the content have done that elsewhere.' Instead, I have of phonics. I remember, too, that I wrote selected one principle concerning phonics the word ice on the board, and began to which would be considered fundamental ask questions: "What is there about this by anyone gifted only with common sense. word that might help a child identify the In general, the principle is: One cannot sound which the letter i probably records?" teach what one does not know. Applied to "What is the likely sound of c, and what the theme of this meeting. the principle in the word suggests that sound?" can be rephrased to suggest that one can- What took me completely by surprise notteachphonicsunlessone knows was that such simple questions as these phonics. had absolutely no meaning for the 30 col- Teachers' Knowledge of Phonics lege seniors who sat before me. The sur- prise was followed by embarrassment What do teachers know about the con- embarrassment because I had failed even tent of phonics? Are they adequately pre- to wonder whether the students knew pared to teach phonics, whether a deduc- what they were expected to teach. tive or an inductive approach is chosen? Because, in 1957, phonics instruction These are the questions to which I ad- still was not sufficiently fashionable to 'tie dress my comments. given detailed coverage in reading meth- Actually, the questions have been of ods textbooks, feelings of surprise, and special concern to me since the fall of then of embarrassment, were followed by 1957. That was the beginning of my col- efforts to put into writing some of the lege teaching, and of my responsibility more basic content of phonics.Irecall for reading methods courses. Having been that in preparing the material I assumed educated in schools which, evidently, by- its use would be unnecessary for the sum- passed the popular thinking of the 1930's mer session classes because, in these, most and the 1940's,Imyself was taught students would be experienced teachers, phonics, beginning in first grade. Conse- not inexperienced college seniors. quently, it probably was natural for me But, that summer session led to the next 'Dolores Durkin. Phonies and the Teaching of surprise, for the experienced teacheidid Reading (Second Edition). New York: Teachers Col. lege Bureau of Publications. 1965. not know the content of phonics either. Reading and Inquiry,IRA Proceedings, Vol. 10. 1965.127-430.

8'7 DURKIN 81

In fact, I still remember one teacher from was published, preliminary copies were that first summer class. 1 remember her sent to elementary schools in various sec- because, following aclass session con- tions of the country. The intent, ofcourse, cerned with phonics, she came tome and was to use the reactions of teachers to said, "I'm so glad you're helpingus with improve the test. Reactions thatwere re- phonics because, to tell you the truth, 1turned can be summarized by quoting only know second-grade phonics." What from part of one letter sent bya principal: she meant, of course, was that she knew We found these tests useful, especially for the phonics material included in the sec- the teachers. It turned out that they found ond-grade basal reader. itdifficultto administer the test because Since 1957, 1 have taught other stu- they themselves know so little about phonics. dents at other universities. In all of these This problem has been discussed ata staff meeting, and we have decidedto use places, and with most of the students, 1 the test in a kind of in-service course for have continued to finditnecessary to the teachers. teach, first, the content of phonics; and Reactions, typified by this one letter, then, secondly, how the content might be indicated that my initial efforts incon- taught to others. This need to teach the structing tests had begun at thewrong content, of course, is no longer surprising. place.1, should have started with the What I have learned is that while 1, as a teachers. Very briefly, this explains how child, was being taught phonics, many of the test called a "Phonics Test for Teach- thepeople now inreading methods ers" came into being.2 courses were attending other elementary Phonics Survey schools in which phonics instruction was "unfashionable." I bring this test into the discussion be- cause 1 want to spend the remaining time The Teaching of Phonics reporting briefly on the results ofa kind of national survey, just completed. For While helping teachers with the con- this survey, the "Phonics Test for Teach- tent of phonics 1 also have tried, over theers" was given to 603 students enrolled past seven years, to discourage what might in reading methods courses. The test be labeled a"grade-level approach" to was teaching phonics. That is, I have tried to administered by 13 professors fuin five help teachers see thatit does not make state and city colleges, and from six state good educational sense to think of the universities. To make the surveysome- content of phonics as being divided into what national in scope, these schoolswere located in the East, the Midwest, andon sections, each to be taught at a prescribed the West Coast. At some future date, grade level. Rather, I have stressed that a as with all phases of reading instruction detailed report of the survey will bepre- pared. For now, I will just mentiona few we should first find out what children findings. In fact, for the sake of brevity, already know about phonics, and then 1 will mention only findings regarding the move on to teach what they do not know. 204 participants who were experienced To facilitate more individualizedin- teachers. Here it is important to pointout struction, 1 recently constructed a diagnos-that of these 204 teachers, only 57 tic test in phonics, for children; this was were taking their first course in readingat the done with the help of the principal and time the phonics test was administered. staff of an elementary school. I mention the test now because reactions to it con-Results of Survey vinced me again that much still needs to be In this very brief report of what these done to help teachers learn at least the most basic of the phonics content. *Dolores Durkin. Phonics Test for Teachers. New York: Teacher:CollegeBureau ofPublications. &lore this diagnostic test for children 1964.

88 82 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

204 people did and did not know about core of the reading program. I emphasize phonics, I am confining my comments to this becauseIhave come to learn that those aspects of the content of phonics when one writes about a particular topic which,Ithink, most educators would and I have written about phonicsa consider basic and elementary. For in common conclusionisthat the writing stance, what did the 204 teachers know was done on that topic because the author about vowel sounds? Using averages, 89 thought it to be the most important of all per cent could identify long vowel sounds, possible topics. Let me say that I chose to but only 81 per cent recognized short write about phonics because I consider it vowel sounds. When asked how vowels to be one of the many important com- in a syllable can offer help in arriving at ponents of a good reading program; and, a correct pronunciation of that syllable, secondly,because thestudentsin my the teachers' performance dropped con- courses seemed to know so little about its siderably. For example, when asked to content. explain why, in a combination of such I do not assume that the "Phonics Test letters asek,theewould probably record for Teachers'. is flawless. In fact, what I its short sound, only 29 per cent of the learned from putting the test together is 204 teachers were able to give any ex- the very real difficulty of constructing a planation. really good test. Nonetheless, I stillfeel When the phonics test moved to ques- there is evidence to indicate that much tions about matters like the "hard" and more must be done, in reading methods "soft" sounds of the consonants c and g, courses abd inservice workshops, to help this was what was found. Again based on teachers and teachers-in-preparation learn averages, 90 per cent of the 204 teachers both what to teach, and how to teach, in identified the "hard" and "soft" sounds. this area of phonics. However, only 9 per cent could describe Finally, I would like to make a predic- when c and g generally record the "soft" tion.Itis simply to suggest that when sounds, while a small group of 2 per cent teachers really know the content of phon- seemed to know when the "hard" sounds ics, there will be far less of the deadly predominate. phonics drill now going on in classrooms Test questions about diphthongs and all over the countryeven, I am sorry to consonant digraphs resulted in very low say, at the kindergarten level. I am con- percentages of correct responses, and def- vinced, you see,that teachers who are initions of these terms sometimes were secure in their own knowledge of phonics sadly humorous. For instance, one teacher do not need to depend on high piles of wrote that a consonant digraph istwo phonics workbooks, or on long, tiresome consonants of different sizes." miles of rote drill. It seems even common Concluding Statements sense would suggest that it is the teacher who really knows phonics who also is the Perhaps these few test findings are suffi- one who will never teach too little, or too cient grounds for me to move, now, to much. some concluding statements about my own And this brings us back to theone feelings regarding this matter of phonics principle underlying phonics instruction instruction. Let me say, immediately, that which I have focused on; namely, thatone I do not believe that phonics is the very cannot teach what one does not know.

89 The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in thePrimary Grades

THEODORE CLYMER*

THEORIGINS of this study go backmethods teacher, we were carefulnot to Kenneth, an extraordinary ele-to call the generalizations "rules," mentary pupil. Prior to my encounterfor all our statements had a number with Kenneth I had completedaof exceptions. As the class finally reading methods course ina smallformulated a generalization regard- teachers college which providedaing the relationships of letters, letter backgroundintheprinciplesofposition, and sounds, such defensive teaching reading as well asa goodphrasing as "most of the time," introduction to techniques. Among"usually," and "often" appearedas these techniques were procedurestoprotective measures. We also spent develop phonic generalizations andtime listing some of the exceptionsto also the list (not a list) of the mostour generalizations. valuable generalizations to develop. At this point Kcnncth entered the (To those of you who might likediscussion. While the class was busily copies of the list, I am sad to reportengaged in developing the generali- that somehow through theyears itzation, Kenneth had skimmed his has been lost.) dictionary, locating long lists ofex- Difficulties with Kenneth beganasceptionstothe generalization.In the class reviewed phonic generaliza-fact, he often located moreexcep- tions at the start of the schoolyear.tions than I could list applications. Our procedures were like those usedWhen I protestedsomewhat weakly inmany classrooms:Croups ofthatthedictionarycontained words were presented, and the classmany unusual words, Kcnncth con- analyzed their likenesses and differ-tinuedhisrole as an educational ences with a view toward deriving ascientist. He turned to the basic reader generalization about relationships be-word list in the back of his text and tween certain letters and sounds orproduced nearly similar results. To- the position and pronunciation ofday, of course, Kenneth's behavior vowels. would be rated as "gifted," "tal- Throughout these exercises,fol-ented," or "creative"although I lowing the dictum of my readingremember discussing him in other This paper is an extension of a reportterms as I sat in the teachers' lounge. given at a joint meeting of the Interna- As Kenneth had provideda mem- tional Reading Association and the Na- tional C.onference of Research in English, orable and even a "rich" learning May 1961. Thomas Barrett, Harriette An-experience for me, he furnished the derson.Joan Hanson. and David Palmer provided invaluable assistance in variousimpetus for a series of studies which phases of the study. will attempt to answer threequcs-

The Reading Teacher, 16 (January 1963),252-258. 84 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION tions: (1) What phonic generaliza-ation the miscellaneous type of gcncr- tions are being taught in basic read-alization, a total of 121different ing programs for the primary grades?statements were located. Thcrc were (2) To what extent are these gener-50 vowel gcncralizations, 15 conson- alizations useful in having a "reason-ant generalizations, and 28 generali- able" degree of application to wordszations in each of the ending and commonly met inprimary gradesyllabication groups. In evaluating material? (3) Which of thc generali-these figuresitshould be kept in zations that stand the test of qucstionmind that any statement was con- 2 can be learned and successfullysideredaseparategcncralization applied to unknown words by pri-when its phrasing excluded or in- mary children? cluded different sets of words than another statement. For example, the What Generalizations Are generalization, "When there are two Taught? vowels side by side, the long sound of Four widely used sets of readersthe first is heard and the second one were selected to determine the phonic isusually silent" and "Whcn ea generalizations being taught in thecome together in a word, the first primary grades. After a preliminaryletter is long and thc second is silent" study of the manuals, workbooks,were counted as two separate gener- and readers, the manuals were se-alizations, although the second state lected as the source of the gcncraliza-mcnt is a special application of the tions. The manuals presented thefirst. generalizations in three ways: (1) While not dircctly related to our statements to be taught to the pupils,discussion here, note should be made (2) statements to be derived by theof the wide variation of grade level pupils after inductive teaching, andof introduction, emphasis, and phras- (3) statcments with no cicar indica-ing of thc gencralizationi. Of the 50 tion as to what was to be done. Gen-different vowel gcncralizations, only eralizations presented by allthree11 were common to all four series. means wcre included in the analysis.Nonc of these11was presented Five general types of gcncraliza-initially at the same half-year grade tions emerged from thc study of thelevel in all four series. Some series teachers manuals. These types dealtgavc a much grcatcr emphasis to the with (1) vowels, (2)consonants,gcneralizations than did othcr series. (3) cndings, (4) syllabication, andOne publisher introduced only 33 of (5) miscellaneous rclationships. 'Ar-the 121 generalizations, while an- bitrary decisions were made in as-other presented 68. These comments signing some gcncralizations to oneare not meant to detract from thc or another of the five types since cer-usefulness of bask matcrials, but tain statements might easily be classi-simply to point out some of their ficd under two or more headings. differences. That dificrcnces do call If we eliminate from our consider-for careful adjustments in the class-

91 I

CLYMER 85

room when pupils arc moved fromGates Reading Vocabulary forthe one set of materials to another. ThePrimary Grades. Once this list of teacher who changes from series Xsome twenty -six hundred words was to series Y.may need to makesomcprepared, the followingsteps were important revisions in his wordrecog-taken: nition program. These findingsmay 1. Thc phonetic respelling and the indicate also thc need for furthersyllabic division of all wordswere experimentation on emphasis and therecorded. Webster's New Collegiate developmental aspects ofour wordDictionary was usedas the authority recognition program. for this information. 2. Each phonic generalizationwas Which Generalizations Are checked against the words in the Useful? composite list to determine (a ) the Forty-five of the generalizationswords which were pmnouncedas the given in the manualswere selectedgeneralization claimed and (b) the for further study. The selection ofwords which were exceptionsto the these was somewhat arbitrary. Thegeneralization. maincriterionwastoask,"Is 3. A "per cent of utility"was com- the generalization stated specificallyputedfor each generalizationby enough so that it can be said to aidordividingthenumberofwords hinder in the pmnunciation ofa par-pronouncedasthegeneralization ticular word?" An exampleor twoclaimed by thctotal number of will make our criterion clear. Thewords to which the generalization generalization, "Long o makesacould be expected to apply. Forex- sound like its name," is undoubtedlyample, if the generalization claimed a valuable generalization, but it wasthat "When the letters oaair to- not specific enough to meet our cri-gether in a word, o always gives its terion. On the other hand, thestate-long sound and the a is silent," all ment. "When a vowelisinthe words containing oa were located in middle of a one syllable word, thethe list. The number of these words vowel is short." was included becausewas the total number of words to we could judge by reference to awhich the generalization should apply. wordlist how often one syllableThcn the phonetic spellings of these words with a vowel in the middle dowords were examined tosec how in fact have a short vowel sound. many words containing oa actually Our next problem was to developdid have the long o followed by the a word list on which we could testsilent a. In this case thirty wordswere the generalizations. A reasonableap-located which containedoa. Twenty- proach seemed to be that of makingnine of these were pronouncedas up a composite list of all the wordsthe generalization claimed:one was introduced in thc four basic scripnot. The per cent of utilitybe- from which the generalizations werecame 29/30 or 97. This procedure drawn, plus the words from thewas followed for all generalizations. 86 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

When the per cent of utility wasization 33.) This criticism assumes, computed for each generalization, weof course, that the purpose of a gcn- set two criteria as to what constitutedcralization is to help the child unlock a "reasonable" degree of application.the pronunciation of unknown words. We have no scientific evidence to The usefulness of certain gcncrali- demonstrate that these criteria arezations depends upon regional pro- valid; it can only be said that theynunciations. While following Web- seem reasonable to us. ster's markings, generalization 34 is The first criterion was that therejected. Midwestern pronunciation composite word list must contain amakesthisgeneralizationrather minimum of twenty words to whichuseful, although we reject it because the generalization might apply. Gen-we used Webster as the authority. eralizations with lower frequencies ofSuch problems are natural, and we application do not seem to meritshould not hold it against Mr. Web- instructional time. sterthathecamefromNcw The second criterion was a perEngland. cent of utility of at least 75. To state If we adhere to the criteria set up the matter another way, if the pupilat the beginning of .the study, of the applied the generalization to twentyforty-five generalizations only eight - words, it should aid him in gettingeen, numbers 5, 8, 10, 16, 20, 21, the correct pronunciation in fifteen22, 23, 25, 28, 29, 30, 31, 32, 40, of the twenty words. 41, 44, and 45 are useful. Some of The table gives the results of ourthe generalizations which failed to analysis of the forty-five phonic gcn-meet our criteria might be useful if cralizations. An inspectionofthestated in different terms or ifrc- data leaves me somewhat confused asstrictcd to certain typcs of words. We to the value of generalizations. Someare studying these problems at the time- honored customs in the teachingpresent time. We are also examining of reading may be in need of revision.other generalizations which we did Certain generalizations apply tonot test in this study. large numbers of words and are rather constant in providing the cor-Conclusion rect pronunciat.cm of words. (Sec, In evaluating this initial venture in for example, gcncralizations 19, 35,testing the utility of phonic generali- and 36.) zations,itseems quite clear that A group of generalizations seemmany gcncralizations which are com- to be useful only after the pupil canmonly taught are of limited value. plottout:(c the word. GeneralizationsCertainly the study indicates that we which specify vowel pronunciationshould give careful attention to point- in stressed syllables require that theing out the many exceptions to most pupil know the pronunciation of theof the generalizations that we teach. word before he can apply the gener-Current"extrinsic"phonicspro- alization. (Sec, for example, general-grams which present large numbers

93 7

CLYMER 87

THEUTILITY OF FORTY-FIVE PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS

'Generalization No. of Words No. of Per Cent of Conforming Exceptions Utility I. When there arc two vowels side by side. the long sound of the first one is heard and the second is usually silent.$09 (bead) t 377 (chief) t 45 2. When a vowel is in the middle of a onesyllable word, the vowel is short.408 249 62 middle letter 191 (dress) 84 (scold) 69 one of the middle two letters in a word of four letters 191 (o) 135 (told) 59 one vowel within a word of more than four letters 26 (splash) 30 (light) 16 3.If the onit vowel letter is at the end of a word. the letter usually stands for a long sound. 23 (he) 24 (to) 74 4. When there are two vowels.one of which is final e, the first vowel is long and the e is silent. 180 (bane) - 108 (done) 63 3 The r gives the preceding vowela sound that is neither long nor short. 181 (horn) 134 (wire) 78 6. The first vowel is usually lung and the second silent in the digraphs ni, ea, on, and M. 179 92 ai 66 13 (nail) 24 (said) 64 en 101 (bead) 51 (head) 66 oa 34 (boat) ta I (cupboard) 97 I(suit) 16 (build) 6 7.Inthe !Monogram ie, the iis silent and the e has a long sound. 8 (field) 39 (friend) 8. Words having double e usually have 17 the long e sound. 85 (seem) 2 (been) 9. When words end with silente, the 98 preceding a or i is long. 164 (cake) 108 (have) 10. In ay the y is silent and givesa its 60 lung bound. 36 (play) 10 (a I wa ys) II. When the letter iis followed by the 78 letters gh, the i usually stands for its long sound and .he gh is silent. 22 (high) 9 (neighbor) 12. When a follows w in a word, it usually 71 has the sound a as in was. IS (watch) 32 (swam) 13. When e is followed by w, the vowel 32 sound is the same as represented byoo. 9 (blew) 17 (sew) 35 11. The two letters ow make the longo sound. 50 (own) 35 (down) 13. IV is sometimes a vowel and follows 59 the vowel digraph rule. 50 (crow) 75 (thre) 40 16. %Viten v is the final letter ina word, it usually has a vowel sound. 169 (thy) 31 (tray) 84 17. When y.is used as a vowel in words,it sometimes has the sound of long i. 29 (fly) 170 (firmly) IS 18. The letter a has the same sound (6) when followed by I, w, andu. 61 (all) 65 (Lanai) 48

tWords in parentheses arc examples ei her of words which conform or of exceptious, depending on the column. Generalizations marked with an asteriskwere found "useful" according to the criteria. 88 THE CURRICULUM INWORD RECOGNITION

Generalization No. of Words No. of l'er Cent of Conforming Exceptions Utility 19. When a is followed by r and final e, we expect to hear the sound heard in care. 9 (dare) I(arc) 90 '20. When c and h arc next to each other. they make only one sound. 103 (peach) 0 100 '21. Ch is usually pronounced as itis in kitchen, catch, and chair, not like sh. 99 (catch) 5 (machine) 95 '22. When c is followed by e or i, the sound of s ix likely to be heard. 66 (cent) 3 (ocean) 96 23. When the letter c is followed hy o or a the sound of k is likely to be heard.113 (camp) 0 100 24. The letter g often has a sound similar to that of j in jump when it precedes the letter i or e. 49 (engine) 28 (give) 64 '25. When ght is seen in a word, gh is silent. 30 (fight) 0 100 26. When a word begins kn, the k is silent. 10 (knife) 0 100 27. %%lien a word begins with tor, the t' is silent. ti (write) 0 100 IA. When two of the same consonants are side by side only one is heard. 334 (carry) 3 (suggest) 99 When a word ends in ck, it has the same last sound as In look. 46 (brick) 0 100 30. In most two-syllable words, the first syllable is accented. 828 (famous) 113 (polite) '31.If a, in, re, ex, de, or be is the first syllable in a word. Itis usually un- accented. R6 (belong) 13 (insect) 87 32. In most twosyllable words that end in a consonant followed by y, the first syllableis accented and the lastis unaccented. 101 (baby) 1 (supply) 96 33. One vowel !ma in an accented syl !able has its short sound, 517 (city) 356 (lad)) 61 31. When y or ey is seen in the last syl- lable that is not accented, the long sound of e is heard. 0 157 (baby) 0 35.%Viten ture is the final syllable in a word, it is unaccented. 1 (picture) U 100 16. When tion is the final syllable in a word. it is unaccented. 5 (station) 0 100 17.In many two- and three-syllable words, the final e lengthens the vowel in the last syllable. 52 (invite) 62 (gasoline) 16 3$.If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by two consonants, the first syllable usually ends with the first of the two consonants. 404 (bullet) 159 (singer) 72 39.If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single consonant, that consonant usually begins the second syllable. 190 (over) 237 (oven)

'1-Words in parentheses arc examples ei her of words which conform or of exception. depending on the column. Generalizat ions marked with an asterisk were found "mein!" according in the criteria. CLYMER 89

Generalization No. of Words No. of Per Cent of Conforming Exceptions Utility 40. If the last syllable of a word ends in le,the consonant preceding thele usually begins the last syllable. 62 (tumble) 2 (buckle) 97 1. When thefirstvowel clement ina word is followed byth, ch,orsh,these symbols are not broken when the word is divided into syllables and may go with either the first or second syllable. 30 (dishes) 0 42. In a word of more than one syllable. 100 the letter v usually goes with thepre- ceding vowel to form a syllable. 53 (cover) 20 (clover) 73 13. When a word has only one vowel letter. the vowel sound is likely to be short. 433 (hide 322 (kind) 57 '141. When there is one e in a wordthat ends in a consonant, thee usually has a short sound. 85 (leg) 27 (blew) 76 45. When the last syllable is the soundr, it is unaccented. 188 (butter) 9 (appear) 95 -tWords in parentheses are examples ci her of words which conform or of exceptions, depending on the column. 'Generalizations marked with an asteriskwere found "useful" according to the criteria. of generalizationsare open to ques-ages of utility should be taught be- tion on the basis of this study. cause they encourage children to This study does not, ofcourse,examine words for sound and letter answer the question of which gener-relationships. alizations primary childrencan apply The most disturbing fact to come in working out the pronunciation offrom the study may be the rather unknown words. The answer to thedismal failure of generalization 1 to question of the primary child's abilityprovidethecorrectpronunciation to apply these and other generaliza-even 50 per cent of the time. As one tions will come only through class-teacher remarked when this study room experimentation.Also,thiswas presented to a reading methods study does not establish the per centclass, "Mr. Clymer, for years I've of utility required for a generalizationbeen teaching 'When two vowelsgo to be useful. The percentage sug-walking, the first one does the talk- gested here (75) may be too high.ing.' You're ruining theromance in Classroom research might reveal thatthe teaching of reading!" generalizations with lower percent-

86 The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in Grades One through Six MILDRED HART BAILEY* PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS haveDictionary was used as the authority long constituted a significant partin recording the phonetic respelling of instruction in phonics. Many au-and syllabic division of the words. thors of textbooks in the teaching ofClymer then checked the forty -five reading recommend phonic general-phonic generalizations against the izations as an important facet of thecomposite word list to identify all reading program, and basal readingwords that conformed or were ex- series include phonic generalizationsceptions to each of the generaliza- in the program of instruction. De-tions. A percentage of utility was spite such wide acceptance, little re-computed for each generalization, search on the utility of phonic gener-and criteria were formulated by Cly- alizations had been reported untilmer to determine a " 'reasonable' de- Theodore Clymer, in the Januarygree of application" for the general- 1963 issue of The Reading Teach-izations. Only eighteen of the forty- er,* reported the results of an inves-five phonic generalizations met the tigation of the utility of forty-fivecriteria as set forth in the Clymer phonic generalizations in the primarystudy. grades. The results of the above-described Clymer examined basal readinginvestigation proved disturbing to materials for the primary grades andmany people concerned with read- selected the teachers manuals of fouring instruction. One question fre- basal series as the source of phonicquently posed was, "Would the re- generalizationsto be investigated.sults differ greatly if the forty-five Forty - fivegeneralizations recom-phonic generalizations were applied mended in the manuals were identi-to a vocabulary list for grades one fied for study. A list of some twenty-through six, rather than just the pri- six hundred words was then assem-mary grades?" In the belief that the bled by Clymer through inclusion ofanswer to this question might pos- all words introduced in the primary-sibly contribute toward an improved level reading textbooks of the fourprogram of instruction in phonics, previously identified basic readingthe following investigation was un- series, plus the words from the Gatesdertaken by the present writer. The Reading Vocabulary for the Primarypurpose of the study was to investi- Grades. Webster's New Collegiategate the utility of phonic generaliza- tions in reading instruction through Theodore Clymer, "The Utility of Phonicapplication of recommended gener- Generalizationsinthe Primary Grades," Rending Teacher, 16 Uan. 1963) , 252.258.alizations to a list of words represen- The Rending Teacher, 20 (February 1967) , 413.418. BAILEY 91 tative of words encountered in read-termined according to the 1961 edi- ing in grades one through six. tion of Webster's New Collegiate Procedure Dictionary, and percentage of utility was computed by dividing the total Forty-five phonic generalizations,number of conformations identified previouslyidentified by Theodoreby the total number of incidents in- Clymer in the above-described inves-vestigated for each generalization. A tigation, were selected for study. Asummary of the findings pertaining list of words was collected from theto the utility of the forty-five phonic entire vocabularies of all textbooks,generalizationsis presented in the grades one through six, of the fol-table. lowing tight basal reading series: Emmett A. Betts and Carolyn M. Welch,Conclusions; The Betts Basic Readers (New York: Amer- ican Book Company, 1963). A review of the literature failed to Guy L. Bond and others, The Develop- reveal either scientifically-evolved or mental Reading Series (Chicago: Lyons and Carnahan, 1962). widely-accepted criteria for judging William S. Gray and others, The Newthe results of this study. Nevertheless, Basic Readers (Chicago: Scott, Foresman, 1962). the following conclusions were drawn Paul McKee and others, The Reading forupon the basis of the evidence gained Meaning Series (Boston: Houghton-Mdffin,in the investigation: 1963). Mabel O'Donnel, The Alice and ,Jerry 1.Inclusion in the reading pro- Basic Reading Program (Evanston, Illinois: Harper and Row, 1963). gram of generalizations 1, 7, 12, 13, David H. Russell and others, The Ginn15, 17, 18, and 34, all found to Basic Reading Program (Boston: Ginn and Company, 1961). possess low percentage ofutility, William D. Sheldon and others, Theshould be thoughtfully reconsidered Sheldon Basic Reading Series (Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 1963). by all persons concerned with read- RussellG. Stauffer and others. Theing instruction. Winston Basic Readers (New York: Holt, 2. Since certaingeneralizations Rinehart and Winston, 1960). were found difficult to interpret and Certain limitations regarding in-to apply, it is believed by the writer clusion of words were observed. Forthatchildrenintheelementary example, only words that appearedgrades would experience the same in two or more of the eight seriesuncertainty. For this reason, general- were included, and place names,izations 13, 16, 30, and 45 should proper names, and foreign wordsbe attended with caution. were excluded. A composite list of 3. Only generalizations 20, 22, 5,773 words resulted. 23, 28, 32, and 40 were found to be Computers were utilized for thesimple to understand and apply, to identification ofall words in thebe applicable to large numbers of composite word list to which each ofwords, and to have few exceptions. the forty-five phonic generalizations applied. Conformations andexcep-Recommendations tions to each generalization were de- The findings of this study empha- 92 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

TIIE UTILITY OF PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS IN GRADES ONE THROUGH SIX No. of No. of Words No. of Percent. Generalization IncidentsConforming Exceptionsof Utility 1. When there are two vowels side by side, the long sound of the first vowel is heard, and the sec- ond vowel is usually silent. 1732 :46 (leader) t 1146 (breath) 34 2. When a vowel is in the middle of a onesyllableword,the vowel is short. 1021 730 291 71 Middle letter 430 335 (flank) 95 (her) 78 One of the middle two letters in a word of four letters 478 325 (glen) 153 (long) 68 One vowel within a word of more than four letters 113 70 (depth) 43 (knight) 62 3.If the only vowel letter is at the end of a word, the letter usually stands for a long sound. 38 29 (go) 9 (do) 76 4. When there are two vowels, one of which is final e, the first vowelis long and the eis silent. 578 330 (crttdle) 248 (judge) 57 5. The rgivesthepreceding vowel a sound that is neither long nor short. 1604 1378 (depart) 226 (merit) 86 6. The first vowel is usually long and the second silentin the digraphs al, ea, oa, and ui. 497 298 199 60 tli 121 87 (acclaim) 34 (plaid) 72 ea 259 143 (bean) 116 (create) 55 oa 66 63 (roam) 3 (broad) 95 ui 51 5 (pursuit) 46 (biscuit) 10 7. In the phonogram it, the i is silent, and the a has a long sound. 88 27 (grieve) 61 (brier) 31 8. Words having double a usually have the long a sound. 171 148 (exceed) 23 (deer) 87 9. When words end with silent e, the preceding a or i is long. 674 340 (amaze) 334 (give) 50 10.Iii ay, the y is silent and gives a its long sound. 50 44 (spray) 6 (prayer) 88 II. When the letter i is followed by the letters gh, the i usually stands for its long sound, and the gh is silent. 35 25 (flight) 10 (weight) 71 12. When a follows w in a word, it usually has the sound a as in was. 78 17 (wand) 61 (sway) 22 13. When a is followed by w, the vowel sound is the same as rep- resented by oo. 35 14 (shrewd) 21 (stew) 40 1I. The two letters ow make the !on 0 sound. III 61 (flow) 50 (scowl) 55 13.IV is sometimes a vowel and follows the vowel digraph rule. 180 60 (arrow) 120 (drew) 33 Se Conclusions for a discussion of the usefulness of these generalizations. 1-Words in parentheses are examples, either of words followsfig the rule or of exceptions, depending on the Column. 99 BAILEY 93

UTIUTY OF PIIONIC GMERALIZAIIONS, GRADES ONE TIIROUGII SIX Continued

Generalization No. of No. of Words No. of Percent. IncidentsConforming Exceptions of Utility 16. When y is the final letter in a word, itusually has a vowel sound. 518 462 (lady) 56 (key) 89 17. When y is used as a vowel in words,it sometimes has the sound of long i. 596 63 (ally) 533 (silly) 11 18. The lettera has the same sound (6) when followed by I, w, and u. 346 119 (raw) 227 (laugh) 34 19. When a is followed by r and final e, we expect to hear the sound heard in care. 24 23 (flare) I (are) 96 20. When c and h are next to each other,theymake only one sound. 225 225 (charge) 0 100 21. Ch is usually pronounced as it is in kitthen, catch, and chair, not like sh. 225 196 (pitch) 29 (chute) 87 22. When c is followed by e or 1, the sound of s is likely to be heard. 284 260 (glance) 24 (ancient) 92 23. When the letter c is followed by o or a, the sound of k is likely to be heard. 428 428 (canal) 0 100 24. The letter g often has a sound similar to that of j in jump when it 'precedes the letter i or e. 216 168 (genius) 48 (eager) 78 25. When ell is seen in a word, gh is silent. 40 40 (tight) 0 100 26. When a word begins Ian, the k is silent. 17 17 (knit) 0 100 27. When II word begins with wr, the w is silent. 17 17 (wrap) 0 100 28.%%lien two of the same conso- nants are side by side, only one is heard. 826 809 (dollar) 17 (accept) 98 29. When a word ends in ck,it has the same last sound as in look. 80 80 (neck) 0 100 1 30.In most two-syllable words, the i first syllable is accented. 2345 1906 (bottom) 439 (attire) 81 31.If a, in, re, ex, de, or he is the firstsyllable in a word, itis usually unaccented. 398 336 (reply) 62 (extra) 84 32.In most twosyllable words that end in a consonant followed by y, the first syllable is accented and the last is unaccented. 195 190 (pony) 5 (apply) 97 33. One vowel letter in an accented syllable has its short sound. 3031 1960 (banish) 1071 (fortune) 65 94 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

UTILITY OF PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS, GRADES ONE THROUGII SIX - Continued

Generalization No. of No. of Words No. of Percent. IncidentsConforming Exceptionsof Utility 34. When y or ey is seen in the lastsyllablethatisnot ac- anted. the long sound ofeis heard. 449 0 449 (ferry) 0 35. When Lure is the final syllable in a word, it is unaccented. 22 21 (future) I (mature) 95 36. When lion is the final syllable in a word, it is unaccented. 102 102 (notion) 0 :00 37. In many two- and three-sylla- ble words, the final e lengthens the vowel in the last syllable. 430 198 (costume) 232 (welcome) 46 38.If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by two con- sonants, the first syllable usual. ly ends with the first of the two consonants. 1689 1311 (dinner) 378 (maple) 78 39.If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single consonant, that consonant usu- ally begins the second syllable. 1283 638 (china) 645 (shadow) 50 40.If the last syllable of a word ends inle,the consonant pre- ceding the le usually begins the last syllable. 211 196 (gable) 15 (crackle) 93 41. When the first vowel element in a word is followed byeh, ch, orsh,these symbols are not broken when the word is di- vided into syllables and may go with either the first or second syllable. 74 74 (fashion) 0 100 42. In a word of more than one syllable,the letter v usually goes with the preceding vowel to form a syllable. 184 119 (river) 65 (navy) 65 43. When a word has only one vowel letter, the vowel sound is likely to be short. 1105 759 (crib) 346 (fall) 69 44. When there is one e in a word that ends in a consonant, the cusually has a short sound. 149 137 (held) 12 (clerk) 92 45. When the last syllable is the sound r, it is unaccented. 761 601 (ever) 160 (prefer) 79 size the need for the supplementation 2. Theabilityofelementary of future research to establish theschool children to apply phonic gen- value of phonic generalizations ineralizations in reading has not been reading in the elementary grades: considered in this or any previous 1.Research deigned to establishinvestigation known to this writer. scientifically - evolvedcriteriaforFuture research, conducted through judging the usefulness of phonic gen-classroomexperimentation, should eralizations should be undertaken. contribute toward a better under- BAILEY 95 standing of the usefulness of phonicwords were not considered in the generalizations to children. present study. Resulting utility of 3.In the present study, phoniccertain generalizations would have generalizations were applied only tobeen considerably different had the words collected from basal readingpronunciation of words been consid- series. It is recommended that futureered upon the basis of pronunciations researchrelative to the utility ofcommonly used, for example in the phonic generalizations include vo-SouthernorMidwesternstates. cabulary derived from the variousTherefore, itis recommended that subject-matter areas in the elemen-future research be designed to con- tary schoolsuch as science, socialsider the effect of regional pronun- studies, and arithmetic. Vocabularyciations upon the usefulness of phon- collected from children's trade books,ic generalizations. magazines, and newspapers should Finally, this study does not con- also be included. clusively establish the utility of phon- 4. The necessity of utilizing theic generalizations in reading for chil- 1961 edition of Webster's New Col-dren in grades one through six. It is legiate Dictionary, rather than theagreed with Burrows and Lourie* more recent 1963 edition, is of sig-who, in reporting an investigation of nificance to the present study. Thethe utility of the "when two vowels schwa symbol, utilized in pronuncia-go walking" generalization, stated tions of words in the 1963 edition ofthat "to know what not to teach that dictionary, is incompatible withwhen error is so apparent is one step many phonic generalizations that areforward, but only a short one." concerned with vowel sounds. Be- (Mildred Hart Bailey is Associate cause the schwa symbol is also beingProfessor of Education and Director employed in several widely-used chil-of the Reading Center at Northwest- dren's dictionaries today, it is recom-ern State College of Louisiana at mended that investigations be con-Natchitoches. This article reports the ducted to ascertain the possibilitiesresults of her doctoral dissertation of evolving new phonic generaliza-completed in 1965 at the University tions that would utilize the schwaof Mississippi.) sound and symbol as presented by those dictionaries. Alvina Trent Burrows and Zyra Lourie. "When 'Two Vowels Go Walking:"Read. 5.Regionalpronunciationsof ing Teacher, 17 (Nov. 1963). 79.82.

1C2 'T I

The Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations above the Primary Grades

ROBERT EMANS

MOST MODERN alltitOrit;eSof read-phonic generalizations may be more ing advocate thetaching ofappropriately learned? phonics in some form. The consensus isthat children need visual andProcedures auditory clues in word recognition. The procedures of this study were Phonics is one of the helps in pro-the same as those used by Clymer in viding these dues. Nevertheless, thehis study.* They induded the selec- issue of phonics remains cloaked intion of the vocabulary, the identifi- an aura of controversy. cation of the words which might ap- An artide in The Reading Teach-ply to each generalization, and the er, "The Utility of Phonics Gener-testing of each word against the alizations in the Primary Grades,"generalization. by Theodore Clymer (2), was re- In Clymer's study the sample in- ceived with interest by many people.cluded words found in four basal In the article Clymer reported areading series. This study used a study of the use of phonic general-random sample of 10 per cent of the izations in primary school reading.words (1,944 words) beyond the He concluded, "It seems quite dearprimary level (grade four) in The that many generalizations which areTeacher's Word Book of 30,000 commonly taught are oflimitedWords by Thorndike and Large (4). value." The spellings, phonetic respelling, Clymeranalyzed2600wordsand syllabic division of the words found in four widely used sets of pri-were recorded, As in Clymer's study, mary grade readers. Such authoritiesWebster's New Collegiate Dictionary as Dolch (3) believe that phonic(I) was used for this information. generalizations are learned to helpA list of the words which applied to pupils recognize words independent-each of the various generalizations ly, not only in the primary years,was made. The phonetic generaliza- but in later years of schooling andtions were checked against the words throughout life. Many words in theto find which were pronounced as primary years are learned by "sight"thegeneralizationsclaimedand because authorities recognizethatwhich were exceptions. Those gener- many of the common, basic wordsalizations which met Clymer's cri- do not reflect phonic generalizations. teria as to a reasonable degree of Therefore, do the generalizations The present research was supported by hold true for the words which the the Cooperative Research Program of the Office of Education, U.S. Department of child later meets and for which the Health, Education, and Welfare. The Reading Teacher, 20 (February 1967) ,419.425. EMANS 97 application were recorded. Clymer'sestablished criteria; in this study six- criteria were as follows: teen met the criteria. (See Table 1.) I. The word list must contain a mini- There are important differences be- mum of twenty words to which the tween the results of this study and generalization might apply. 2. The generalization must have a per the one Clymer conducted using cent of utility of at least 75. primary grade words. Five general- This study went one step furtherizations (10, 21, 25, 29, and 44) than Clymer's study. Clymer madewere found by Clymer to be useful no differentiation between primaryfor words on the primary level, al- and secondary generalizations,al-though in this study they were not though some generalizations are ex-found to be useful for words beyond planations for the exceptions to other the primary level. Generalization 21 generalizations. For example, Cly-had a utility of only 67 per cent in mer's seventh generalization, "In the this study, as compared with 75 per phonogram ie, the i is silent and thecent in the previous study. In this e has a long sound," is an exception study, generalizations10,15,29, to his first generalization, "Whenand 44 failed to meet the criterion, there are two vowels side by side, "The word list must contain a mini- the long sound of the first one ismum of twenty words to which the heard and the second is usually si-generalization might apply." How- lent." Since generalizations may beever, generalization 44 would have learned as aids, and not hard andmet the criterion if a proportionate fast rules, levels of generalizationsnumber of words to the length of may be established. If a high priority the word list i been used. generalization fails to aid in the rec- Three generalizations(24,36, ognition of a word, a generalization and 38) met the criteria on words which accounts for the exceptionsbeyond the primary level, although may be attempted. Thus, the man-they failed to meet the criteria for ner and the order in which general-words on the primary level. General- izations are applied may be as im- ization 24 had a percentage of utili- portant as the generalizations them- ty in this study of 80, while in the selves. Therefore, the analysis wasprevious study it had a percentage repeated, only this time both theof only 64. Generalization 36 was primary generalization and the sec- found to apply to only five words in ondary generalization were appliedthe study by Clymer but to 85 words to each of the words. In addition, in this study. Generalization 38 had other generalizations which appeared a utility percentage of 80 in this to be useful as the analysis proceed-study but only 72 in Clymer's study. ed were studied. Therefore, there was found to be a difference between the two studies Results in a total of seven, and perhaps Clymer t, 4 1.itat eighteen of the tight, generalizations. forty-fivegeneralizations met the Some of the findings related to 98 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

TABLE 1 THE UTILITY OF PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS ON WORDS BEYOND THE PRIMARY LEVEL No. of Words No. of Percent. Generalization Conforming Exceptions of Utility 1. When there are two vowels side by 87 (resourceful.393 (devout) 18 side, the long sound of the first one ness) is heard and the second one is usu- ally silent. 2. When a vowel is in the middle of a 101 38 73 one-syllable word, the.vowel is short. Middle letter. 44 (blink) 10 (hew) 81 One of the middle two letters in 52 (grit) 21 (jolt) 71 a word of four letters. One vowel within a word of more 5 (strung) 7 (berth) 42 than four letters. 3.If the only vowel letter is at the end I(thru) 2 (ma) 33 of a word, the letter usually stands for a long sound. 4. When there are two vowels, one of 37 (baste) 22 (bronze) 63 which isfinal e, the first vowel is long and the a is silent. 5.The r gives the preceding vowel a459 (certainly) 99 (insecure) 82 sound that is neither long nor short. 6. The first vowel is usually long and 54 39 58 the second silentin the digraphs, ai, ea, oa, and ui. ai 19 (container) 4 (moun- 83 taineer) ea 23 (leakage) 14 (deafness) 62 oa 12 (hoarsely) 2 (oasis) 86 ui 0 19 (builder) 0 7.In the phonogram ie, i is silent and 5 (siege) 17 (impatience) 23 the a has a long sound. 8. Words having double a usually have 24 (volunteer) 0 100 the long a sound. 9. When words end with silent a, the 96 (autliorize) 102 (elective) 48 preceding a or i is long. 10. In ay the y is silent and gives a its 6 (rayon) 0 100 long sound. II. When the letter i is followed by the 3 (blight) 0 100 letters gh, the i usually stands for its long sound and the gh is silent. 12. Wien a follows w ina word, it 5 (swan) 13 (renewal) 28 usually has the sound a as in was. 13. When a is followed by at, the vowel 1 (shrew) 6 (stew) 14 sound iy the same as represented by oo. 11. The two letters of ow make the long 9 (minnow) 9 (trowel) 50 o sound. 15.IV is sometimes a vowel and follows 11 (widower) 25 (renewal) 31 the digraph rule. 16. When y is the final letter in a word,265 (currency) 5 (repay) 98 it usually has a vowel sound. 17. When y is used as a vowel, it some- 14 (personify) 312 (surgery) 4 times has the sound of long i. 'Generalizations marked with an asterisk were found usefu according to the criteria. }Words in parentheses are examples, either of words following the rule or of exceptions, depending on the column.

1G5 EMANS 99

TABLE 1

UTILITY OF PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS ONNVORDSBEYOND TIIE PRIMARY LEVEL- Continued

No. of Words No. of Percent. Generalization Conforming Exceptions of Utility 18. The letter a has the same sound (6) 27 (awning) 86 (awakening) 24 when followed by 1, w, and u. 19. When a is followed by r and final e, 2 (flare) 0 100 we expect to hear the sound heard incare. '20. When c andh arenext to each 53 (poacher) 0 100 other, they make only one sound. 21.Chis usually pronounced as it is in 35 (rancher) 17 (champagne) 67 ki',12::..1.catch,andchair,not like sit. 22. Tlibco.is followed by e or i, the 79 (excel) 9 (racial) 90 s -WOofs is likely to be heard. 23. %:),"0 six letter c is followed by 0151 (sarcasm) 0 100 or 14,tete sound ofkis likely to be heard. 24. The lettergoften has a sound Simi 60 (drudgery) lb (trigger) 80 lar to that of j in jump when it precedes the letter i ore. 25.Whenght isseen in aword, gh is 3 (blight) 0 100 silent. 26. When a word begins im, the k is 3 (knuckle) 0 100 silent. 27. When a word begins with wr, the 4 (wreckage) 0 100 w is silent. 28. When two of the same consonants274 (alley) 26 (illegal) 91 are side by side only one is heard. 29. When a word ends inck,it has the 9 (barrack) 0 100 same last sound as inlook. "30. In most twosyllable words, the first396 (haggard) 134 (annul) 75 syllable is accented. 31. If a, in,re, ex, deorbeis the first179 (devout) 36 (intellect) 83 syllablein a word,itisusually unaccented. 32. In most two-syllable words that end 57 (clumsy) 0 100 in a consonant followed by y, the first syllable is accented and the last is unaccented. 33. One vowel letter in an accented syl-959 (possum) 531 (urban) 64 lable has its short sound. 34. When y oreyis seen in the last 3 (yanky) 266 (powdery) 1 syllable that is not accented, the long sound ofeis heard. 35. When titre is the final syllable in a 6 (fracture) 0 100 word, it is unaccented. "36. Whentionis the final syllable in a 85 (estimation) 0 100 word, it is unaccented. 37. In manytwo-andthree-syllable 95 (concentrate)132 (elegance) 42 words, thefinale lengthensthe vowel in the last syllable. "38. If the first vowel sound in a word is648 (pension) 163 (atheism) 80 followed by two consonants, the first syllable usually ends with the first of two consonants. 'Generalizations marked with an asterisk were found useful according to the criteria. `Mr

100 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

UTILITY OF PHONIC GENERALIZATIONS ON WORDS BEYOND THE PRIMARY LEVELContinued

No. ot Words No. ot I'ercen t. Generalization Conforming Exceptions of Utility

39.If the first vowel sound in a word is313 (superb) 346 (tenor) 47 followed by a single consonant, that consonant usually begins the second syllable. '40. If the last syllable of a word ends in53 (monocle) 15 (squabble) 78 le, the consonant preceding the le usually begins the last syllable. '41. When the first vowel element in a 44 (atheism) 0 100 word is followed by th, ch, or sh, these symbols are not broken when the wordis divided into syllables and may go with either the first or second syllable. 42. In a word of more than one syllable, 36 (avocado) 55 (elevator) 40 the letter v usually goes with the preceding vowel to form a syllable. 43. When a word has only one vowel95 (grill) 41 (torch) 70 letter, the vowel sound Is likely to be short. 44. When there is one e in a word that15 (clench) 3 (berth) 83 ends in a consonant, the e usually has a short sound. '45. When the last syllable is the sound165 (sinister) 7 (volunteer) 96 r, it is unaccented.

'Generalizations marked with an asterisk were found useful according to the criteria.

specific generalizations are of specialHowever, this rule fails to indicate interest. Clymer found, "When therewhat the sound is likely to be, only arc two vowels side by side, the longwhat it is likely not to be. The gen- sound of the first one is heard anderalization may best be applied in the second is usually silent" to havethe modification of some of the other a utility of 49 per cent. This studyrules, inasmuch as it indicates possi- found it to have 18 per cent. Whenble exceptions to them. the specific vowel combinations of Table 2 presents these and other generalizations 6, 7, and 8 are omit-modifications to the various original ted, the percentage of utility droppedgeneralizations.Such modification to 3. Therefore, the usefulness of thiswould raise the percentage of utility generalization must be questioned. and enablethegeneralizationto Generalization 5, dealing with rmeetthecriteriaestablishedby as a modifier of preceding vowelClymer. sounds, appears to be an important rule. The rule could be applied toSummary and Implications more than one-fourth of the words Mr. Clymer found 18 generaliza- in this study and had an 82 per centtions meeting the criteria in his study utility. Clymer's results were similar.of words within the primary level; Pft 7. - "RI 7

EMANS 101

TABLE 2 MODI FICA TIONS or rut: ORIGINAL FOR 1Y -FIVE C ulz.vrioxs

Percent. Generalization of Utility I. The letters io usually represent a short u sound as in nation 86 2. The letters oo usually have the long double o sound as in food or the short double o sound as in good. They are more likely to have the double o sound as in food 100 3. When a vowel is in the middle of a one-syllable word, the vowel is short except that it may be modified in words in which the vowel is followed by an r 80 4. When the vowel is the middle letter of a one-syllable word, the vowel is short. 80 5. When the first vowel in a word is a and the second is i, the a is usually long and the i silent 83 6. When the first vowel is o and the second is a, the o is usually long and the a is silent 86 7. The vowel combination ui has a short i sound 79 8. The two letters ow make the long o sound or the ou sound as in out 100 9. When y is used as a vowel, it most often has the sound of long a 92 10. The letter a has the same sound (6) when followed by w and u 84 II. One vowel letter in an accented syllable. has its short sound if it comes before the end of the syllable and Its long sound if it comes at the end of the syllable 78 12. One vowel letter in an accented syllable has its short sound if it comes before the end of the syllable and Its long sound if it comes at the end of the syllable except when it is followed by an r 92 13. When y or ey is seen in the last syllable that is not accented, the short sound of i is heard 97 14. A -lion at the end of a four-syllable word indicates a secondary accent on the first syllable with a primary accent on the syllable preceding the -lion 95 15. Taking into account the original rules 5, 28, 29, 31, and 41. if the first vowel sound in a word is followed by two consonants, the first syllable usually ends with the first of the two consonants 96 16. Except in some words with a prefix, if the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single consonant, that consonant begins the second syllable and the vowel sound in the first syllable will be long, or if the consonant ends the first syllable the vowel sound will be short 84 17. A beginning syllable ending with a consonant and containing a short vowel sound is likely to be accented 95 18. When a word has only one vowel letter, the vowel sound is likely to be short unless the vowel letter is followed by an r 78

this study found 16. Although 13beyond the primary level. Therefore, generalizations demonstratedtheirdifferent generalizations may need usefulness in both studies, four andto be learned at different levels of possibly five of those which provedschooling. useful on the primary level failed in This study has been limited to the usefulness for words beyond the pri-45 generalizations used by Clymer, mary level; and conversely, three ofalthough additional possiblerules thosegeneralizations which werehave been suggested. There may be judged not useful at the primaryother generalizations, including some level were judged useful for wordsparticularly suitable for the interme- 102 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

'hate grades, unstudied in this in-trolled experimental conditions. vestigation, which would be more Inasmuch as some generalizations helpful than the ones included. Sim-are exceptions to others, children ilarly, the generalizations might bemay profit from learning which gen- reworded to correspond better witheralizations should be tried first and the immature understanding of chil-which generalizations should be tried dren. second if the first effort fails. Some The percentage of utility used asof the generalizations may be ap- a criterion may be too high. Possiblyplied best in conjunction with others, 50, 25, or an even lower percentageinstead of in isolation. The selection of utility would be better than noof such generalizations, their word- aid at all. A better criterion may being, their placement into a system- the total number of words in whichatic framework, and their study in a generalizationfunctions,ratherempirical settings would make chal- than the percentage of utility. lenging and appropriate areas for This investigation has left unstud-further investigation. ied the problem of which generaliza- References tions should be taught. While some 1.Bethel. J. P., Ed. Webster's New Colle- giate Dictionary. Springfield, Massachu- rules may be so complicated that setts: G. & C. Merriam Co., 1959. children have difficulty in applying2. Clymer,Theodore: "TheUtilityof Phonic Generalizations in the Primary them, other rules may be so obvious Grades," Reading Teacher,16(Jan. that children learn them without ex- 1963), 252-258. 3. Dolch, E. W. Teaching Primary Read- plicit guidance. Future studies need ing. Champaign, Illinois: Garrard Press, to be conducted to develop proce- 1941. 4. Thorndike. E. L.. and Lorge. I. The dures far teaching generalizations Teacher's Word Rook of 30,000 Words. and to try the procedures under con- New York: Teachers College, 1944.

1C9 Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations

Lou E.BURMEISTER

ALTHOUGH MOST EDUCATORS TODAYfavor the teaching of phonic generalizations, very few of them are able to enumerate with any degree of certainty the generalizations which are worthy of being taught. Evidence is beginning to accrue which would make any interested observer question the value of many phonic generaliza- tions which have appeared for years in the literature and teaching materials in the field of reading.

Purpose The purpose of this paper is to report and compare findings of seven recent studies which were designed to investigate scientific- ally the value of many commonly found phonic, structural analysis, and accent generalizations and in some cases to inductively formu- late new generalizations which may prove useful.

Studies used The studies were reported by Oaks in 1952, Clymer in 1963, Fry in 1964, Bailey in 1965, Emans in 1966, Burmeister in 1966, and Wink ley in 1966. All but Fry and Wink ley utilized a "utility level" concept in determining the usefulness of generalizations. Fry used a "frequency approach," and Wink ley selected useful gener- alizations "because of their applicability to multisyllabic words" (similar to the. utility level concept) "or because of their demon- strated usefulness to children in the identification of unknown multisyllabic words" (1965).

Content of the studies Variations in findings among the studies might be expected because of the following factors: 1.differences in the types of materials from which the sample words were taken 2.differences in the method of selecting the sample words from these materials when the materials are alike 3.differences in dictionaries, or phonemic systems, used as "authorities" for accepted pronunciation The Reading Teacher,21 (January 1968), 399.356, 360.

110 104 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

4.differences in the author's definition of "short" and/or "long" vowel sounds (e.g., Is the unique sound ofa vowel before an "r" considered in a separate category? Isa schwa sound considered to be a short vowel? etc.) 5.differences caused by various ways of determining use- fulness. So that the reader may better understand basic similarities and differences among the studies, the following brief explanation of each study is offered. Oaks looked at "vowel and vowel combinations wind.) appear in certain basal readers designed for use in the primary grades" (1952, p. 604). She used Webster's New International Dictionary (1936). Fry's frequency count is based on his 300 "Instant Words" (1960) and on comparisons made with findings by Moore (1951), Cordts (1925), Black (1961), and Kottmeyer (1954). He looked at phonic rules which were formulated from his own experiences in a reading clinic situation. The phonemic system which he used "was taken from Moore, which was based on work by Bloomfield. However, the rules make several departures from their system, most notable of which is the Y rule which states that Y on the end of a word containing another vowel makes a long E soundas opposed to the short I sound as is contended by most dictionaries" (1964, p. 759). Clymer (1963) reported the utility levels of forty-five phonic generalizations found in grades one to three in four basal series. He used the combined vocabularies of these three levels from the four series plus the words from the Gates Reading Vocabulary for the Primary Grades to determine the percent of utility of these generalizations. Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary was used. Bailey's (1965) and Eman's (1966) studies were partial replications of Clymer's study. Both Bailey and Emans studied the same forty-five generalizations as Clymer. Bailey used as her source of words the entire vocabularies ofalltextbooks, grades one through six, of eight basal reading series:"only words that appeared in two or more of the eight series were included, and place names, proper names, and foreign words were excluded. A composite list of 5773 words resulted." Emans (1966) focused on words beyond the primary level (grade 4). He took "a random sample of 10 per cent of the words (1,944 words) beyond the primary level in The Teacher's Word Book of 30,000 Words by Thorndike and Lorge." Such a sample BURMEISTER 105 would include a heavy loading of words which occur infrequently since there are, for example, almost twice as many (5200) words that occur only once per million running words (according to the Thorndike and Lorge source) than there are words (2780) which occur from twenty to forty-nine times per million running words. (A frequency of forty-nine per million running words is the begin- ning point for g ide 4.0 according to Thorndike and Lorge 11944, p. x -xil). Inidition, Emans inductively formulated eighteen generalizatiol which he found to be useful. Both Bailey and Emans used Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary (1959). Burmeister (1966), in an attempt to obtain an even spread of easy and difficult words, chose her sample words from The Teach- ers' Word Book of 30,000 Words by Thorndike and Lorge at four- teen different "frequency of occurrence' levels. She took a 5 per cent random sample at each of eleven levels for words which occur from six to over 100 times per million running words, and a somewhat smaller (percentage wise) sample at three levels for words which ranged in frequency from one to five occurrences per million running words. She looked at generalizations which are frequently found in materials at the fourth grade level and above and also at generalizations which she had formulated through her own teaching experience. She tripled the number of sample words for her analysis of adjacent (double and triple) vowels and inductively arrived at generalizations which describe the sounds of such vowels. She used The American College Dictionary (1961). Wink ley (1965) reported on the applicability of eighteen accent generalizations suggested by Gray (1960) as they apply to multisyllabic words. The findings of her original study lent support to the teaching of seven generalizations; however her second study, the one being examined herein, suggests that twelve of these generalizations are worth teaching. By combining and rewording these generalizations she reduced the twelve generalizations to seven.

Tabulation of findings Each of the generalizations stated and examined by any one of the investigators was listed. For each, the results of each study which considered this generalization were tabulated. The per- centage of utility was recorded from the data of the studies where such computations were made. From the others, the author's con- clusion as to whether or not the generalization was useful was recorded. Two groups of generalizations were formed as a result of

! . 106 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

comparison of the findings: those generalizations which apparently are commonly included in instructional programs but, according to the studies, had limited utility value; and those which according to the results of the studies had reasonably broad application. Certain others, which seemed to be infrequently encountered in instructional programs and of little value in terms of application, were eliminated from consideration.

Conclusions and implications Usefulness of generalizations GENERALIZATIONS CONSIDERED OF LIMITED USEFULNESS.Certain generalizations appeared to be commonly taught but to have very limited usefulness according to the included studies. The following fell in this category: The vowel in an open syllable has a long sound. The letter "a" has the same sound (6) when followed by "e," "vv," and "u." When there are two vowels, one of which is a "final e," the vowel is long and the "e" is silent. In many two and three syllable words, the "final e" lengthens the vowel in the last syllable. When a word ends in "vowel-consonant-e," the vowel is long and the "e" is silent. When two vowels are together, the first is long and the second is silent. If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single con- sonant, that consonant usually begins the second syllable. When two sounds are separated by one consonant, divide before the consonant, but consider "ph," "ch," "sh," and "th" to be single consonants. It is recommended that teachers be particularly cautious when instructing children in situations in which these generalizations might apply in two or more specific ways until oral recognition is achieved. For example, the following generalizations might be helpful: Single vowels are usually short, but a single vowel may have a long sound in an open syllable (approximately 30 percent of the time), especially in a one syllable word. If a word ends in "vowel-consonant-e" the vowel may be long or short. Try the long sound first.

GENERALIZATIONS OF HIGH UTILITYVALUE. Certain other genei- 113 BURMEISTER 107

alizations appeared to be particularly useful. This in no way suggests that other statements (or generalizations) are not impor- tant. The author, for example, does not consider Fry's statement "Single consonants are quite consistent in making the same sound. They should be taught in the following order: t, n, r, m, d, s (sat), 1, c (cat), p, s, f, v, g (got), h, w, k, j, z, y" to be unimportant. She recognizes the need for teaching single consonants but not necessarily in the order listed. The generalizations listed here are the most inclusive of the group, or they are the ones with the highest utility level, or both. Thus "C followed by e, i, or y sounds soft; otherwise C is hard (omit ch)" was selected because it describes every situation in which a "C" might be found; the other generalizations are more limited.

ESPECIALLY USEFUL GENERALIZATIONS. The following general- izations are those from the studies which seemed most useful, except for the "final e" generalization and the phonic syllabication number 2 generalization. The latter two generalizations were for- mulated by the current author as a result of the findings of the utility level studies.

Consonant sounds 1."C" followed by "e," 1," or "y" sounds soft; otherwise "c" is hard (omit "ch"). (certain, city, cycle; attic, cat, clip; success) 2."G" followed by "e," "i," or "y" sounds soft; otherwise "g" is hard (omit "gh"). ( gell, agile, gypsy; gone, flag, grope; suggest) 3. "Ch" is usually pronounced as it is in "kitchen," not like "sh" as in "machine." 4. When a word ends in "ck," it has the same Last sound as in "look." 5. When "ght" is seen in a word, "gh" is silent.( thought, night, right) 6. When two of the same consonants are side-by-side, only one is heard. (dollar, paddle) Vowel soundssingle vowels 1.If the only vowel letter is at the end of a word, the letter usually stands for a long sound (one syllable words only). (be, he, she, go) 2. When "consonant + y" are the final letters in a one 114 108 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

syllable word, the "y" has a 'long i" sound; in a polysyl- labic word the "y" has a "short i" (long e) sound. (my, by, cry; baby, dignity ) 3. A single vowel in a closed syllable has a short sound, except that it may be modified in words in which the vowel is followed by an "r." (club, dress,at, car, pumpkin, virgin) 4. The "r" gives the preceding vowel a sound that is neither long nor short. (car, care, far, fair, fare) [single or double vowels] Vowel soundsfinal "vowel-consonant-e" When a word ends in "vowel-consonant-e" the "e" is silent, and the vowel may be long or short. (cape, mile, contribute, accumu- late, exile, line; have, prove, encourage, ultimate, armistice, come, intensive, futile, passage ) Vowel soundsadjacent vowels 1.Digraphs: When the following double vowel combinations are seen together, the first is usually long and the second is silent: ai, ay, ea, ee, oa, ow (ea may also have a "short e" sound, and ow may have an "ou" sound ) [main, pay; eat, bread; see, oat, sparrow, how] 2. Diphthongs (or blends): The following double vowel com- binations usually blend: au, aw, ou, oi, oy, oo ("oo" has two common sounds ). [auto, awful, house, coin, boy, book, rooster] 3."io" and "ia": "io" and "ia" after "c," "t," or "s" help to make a consonant sound: vicious,partial, musician, vision, attention (even ocean). Syllabicationdetermination of a syllable Every single vowel or vowel combination means a syllable (except a "final e" in a "vowel-consonant-e" ending). Syllabicationstructural syllabication These generalizations take precedence over phonic syllabica- tion generalizations. 1.Divide between a prefix and a root. 2.Divide between two roots. 3.Usually divide between a root and a suffix. Syllabicationphonic syllabication

115 BURMEISTER 109

1. When two vowel sounds are separated by two consonants, divide between the consonants but consider "ch," "sh," "ph," and "th" to be single consonants. (assist, convey, bunny, Houston, rustic) 2. When two vowel sounds are separated by one consonant, divide either before or after the consonant. Try dividing before the consonant first. (Consider "ch," "sh," "ph," and "th"to be single consonants). [alone, select, ashame, Japan, sober; comet, honest, ever, idiot, modest, agile, general] 3. When a word ends in a "consonant-l-e" divide before the consonant. (battle, treble, tangible, kindle). Accent 1.In most two syllable words, the first syllable is accented. a. And, when there are two like consonant letters within a word the syllable before the double consonant is usually accented (beginner, letter) b. But, two vowel letters together in the last syllable of a word may be a clue to an accented final syllable (complain, conceal). 2.In inflected or derived forms of words, the primary accent usually falls on or within the root word (boxes, untie) [Therefore, if "a," "in," "re," "ex," "de," or "be" is the first syllable in a word, it is usually unaccented.]

Implications for further research VOWEL PHONEME-GRAPHEME RELATIONSHIPS. It seems important when' thinking of vowel sounds to differentiate between single vowels and double vowels. When two vowels are together, they ordinarily compose a phoneme. To lose sight of this causes a lack of clarity and a lessening of utility level of a possibly good gene :alization. For generalizations to be especially useful and clear the gra pheme must be clearly defined. For example, some generaliza- tions listed under VOWEL SOUNDS"Single or double vowels" tend to be ambiguous. Vowels should always be looked at as single .owels or as double vowels. This can be clarified if we look at the following generalization: "When y is the final letter in a word, it usually has a vowel sound." Exceptions are, for example, words such as play, tray, repay, key, they, words which end with two vowels which often form a digraph. Notice the 99 percent utility

116 110 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

level, instead of the followingmore clearly stated generalization: "When 'consonant1- y' are the final letters in a monosyllabic word. the 'y' has a 'longsound: in a polysyllabic word the 'y'has a 'short (long e) sound." Another example might be cited. Generalization: "When the letter 'i' is followed by the letters 'gh' the 'i usuallystands for its long sound and the 'gh' issilent." Most of the common words which follow the generalization havea single vowelT: high, night, fight, flight, blight, etc. Words which donot follow the generaliza- tion are words with double vowels: neighbor, straight,weight, etc. The generalization would have a higher utility levelif it were stated thus: When the single vowel 'i' is followed by theletters 'gh' the usually stands for its long sound and the 'gh' issilent." SAMPLE MATERIAL. Apparently the variation in the sources from which sample words were selected usually makesvery little differ- ence in the findings. This becomes obvious when one compares the findings of Clymer, Bailey, and Emans in particular. Whenthe size of the sample is large, the "utility levels" of almost all generaliza- tions are fairly standard. An exception might be that of the "g" generalization. Clymer's primary materials yieldeda lower utility level for this generalization than the other studies. Thiscan be explained by noting that words of Anglo-Saxon origin supply the exceptions to this generalization and Anglo-Saxon wordsare our most common wordswords likely to be found often at the primary level (give, get, girl, tiger, finger, etc.). Another exception involves the voweldigraph,generalization. The primary materials used in Clymer's study yield a higher utility level for this generalization than do any of the other materials. The observation that level of difficulty of words in general makes little difference in the utility level fora generalization can also be affirmed by examining the findings in Burmeister's study of the utility levels of each of the fourteen frequency ofoccurrence stratifications (Burmeister, 1966).

References Bailey, Mildred Hart. An analytical study of the utility of selected phonic generalizations for children in grades one through six. Unpublished doctoral dis- sertation, University of Mississippi, 1965. Black, Sister Mary Carla, B.V.M. Phonics rules verification by a thirteen hundred word count. Unpublished master's project, Loyola University of Los Angeles, 1961. Bloomfield, L. Language. New York: H. Holt, 1955. Burmeister, Lou E. An analysis of the inductive and deductive groupap- BURMEISTER 111

proaches to teaching selected word analysis generalizations to disabled readers in eighth and ninth grade. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin, 1966. Clymer, T. L. The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 1963. 16, 252.58. Cordts, Anna D. Analysis and classification of the sounds of English words in the primary reading vocabulary. Unpublished dissertation, University of Iowa, 1925. Dale, E., and Eichholz, G. Children's knowledge of words. Columbus, Ohio: Bureau of Educational Research and Service, 1960. Emans, R. The usefulness of phonic generalizations above the primary grades. A paper given at the annual convention of the American Educational Research Association, Chicago, 1966. Fry, E. A frequency approach to Phonics. Elementary English, 1964, 41, 759. 765+. . Teaching a basic reading vocabulary. Elementary English, 1960, 39, 37-42. Gray, W. S. On their own in reading. Chicago: Scott, Foresman, 1960. Horn, E. The child's early experience with the letter "a." The Journal of Educational Psychology, 1929, 20, 161-68. Kottmeyer, W. A. Phonetic and structural generalizations for the teaching of a primary grade spelling vocabulary. Reported in Webster Publishing Company Reserve File No. 528S and 529-S, St. Louis, Mo., 1954. Moore, J. T. Phonetic elements appearing in a 3,000 word spelling vocabulary. Unpublished dissertation, Stanford University, 1951. Thorndike, E. L., and Lorge, I. The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1944. Vogel, Mabel, Jaycox, Emma, and Washburne, C. W. A basic list of phonics for grades I and II. Elementary School Journal, 1923, 23, 436 .443. Wink ley, Carol K. Utilization of accent generalizations in identifying unknown multisyllabic words. Unpublished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1965. .Which accent generalizations are worth teaching? The Reading Teacher. 1966, 20, 219.224+.

118 Utility of Vowel Digraph Generalizations in Grades One through Six

MILDRED HART BAILEY

l'IIONICGENERALIZATIONShave long ty-nine percent of the words investi- played an important role in reading in- gated in that study followed the rule. structionintheelementaryschool. Recent studies on phonic generaliza- Numerous authors of textbooks in the tions by Bailey (2), Emans (6), and teaching of reading recommend that Burmeister(3) have also reported, phonic generalizations be included in without exception, failure of the vowel the reading program, and most basal digraph generalizationtobe useful. reading series include phonic general- Bailey reported findings of thirty-four izations in the instructional prOgram. percent utility, Emans, eighteen per- Of all the recommended phonic gener- centutility, and Burmeister catego- alizations, possibly the generalization rized the generalization as one found which is most widely known and most to possess only limited usefulness. often taught is the vowel digraph gen- Purposes. The purposes of the pres- eralizationthegeneralizationcom- ent study were to 1) investigate the monly stated, "When two vowels are overall utilityof the vowel digraph side by side, usually the long sound of generalization when applied to a list of the first vowel is heard and the second words representative of words met by rowel is silent," or, in many first and childreninreadinginstructionin second grade classrooms, "When two grades one through six, 2) determine vowels go walking, the first one does the utility of all possible subgroups of the talking." adjacent vowels (an, ne, ai, ao, nu. cq, Despite such wide and time-honored ee. etc.), and 3) explore the possibili- acceptance, little research on the use- ties of evolving new vowel digraph fulness of the vowel digraph general- generalizationsthat would apply to ization had been reportedpriorto large numbers of words and possess 1963, when two studies, by Clymer high percentages of utility. (5), and Burrows and Lourie (4), Procedure. The vowel digraph gen- were published.Clymer reported the eralization,ordinarily stated, "When results of an investigation of the utility two vowels are side by side, usually of forty-fire selected phonic generaliza- the long sound of the first vowel is tions in the primary grades, one ofheard and the second vowel is silent," which was the vowel digraph general- was applied to a word list collected in ization, and concluded that many gen- a previous study by Bailey (1). The eralizations were foundtopossess original word list consisted of the en- limited value. Special attention was di- tire vocabularies of eight basal reading rected by Clymer to the vowel digraph series, grades one through six, pub- generalization, which was foundto lished in the United States during or possess only forty-five percent utility since 1960, and was pronounced, for in that study.Burrows and Lourie purposes of the present investigation, (4) explored the reliability of only one representative of words met by chil- phonic generalization,the vowel di- dren in reading in grades one through graph rule, and found that only thir- six.

Reading and Realism, IRA Proceedings, Vol. 13, Part I, 1969, 654.658. BAILEY 113

Table 1 Utility of the Original Vowel Digraph Generalization*

Number of Incidents Number of Number of Percentage Investigated Conformations Exceptions of Utility

1506 490 (paint)** 1016 (been)* 33

* When two vowels are side by side, usually the long sound of thefirst vowel is heard and the second vowel is silent. ** Examples of words that conformed orwere exceptions to the generalization.

Table 2 Utility of the Vowel Digraph Generalization When Appliedto Twenty-five Adjacent Vowel Combinations

Adjacent- Number of Number Number Percentage Vowel Incidents of Combinations Investigoted Conformations Exceptions Utility

as 1 0 I(bazaar)* 0 ac 1 0 1 (phaeton) 0 ai 118 84 (bait)* 34 (air) 71 ao 0 0 0 0 au 45 0 45 (caught) 0 ea 252 141 (peach) 111 (pear) 56 cc 166 145 (cheek) 21 (been) 87 et 30 9 (ceiling) 21 (freight) 30 co 13 1 (people) 12 (geography) 8 eu 4 0 4 (museum) 0 is 45 0 45 (giant) 0 le 86 6 (lie) 80 (friend) 7 ti 1 0 1 (taxiing) 0 io 178 0 178 (union)* 0 iu 7 0 7 (aquarium) 0 oa 66 63 (road)* 3 (cupboard) 95 oe 12 5 (toe) 7 (shoe) 42 of 43 0 43 (boil) 0 00 124 2 (door) 122 (cool) 2 ou 185 17 (four) 168 (fought) 9 ua 38 0 38 (equal) 0 uc 38 12 (continue) 26 (fuel) ui 32 SO 5 (nuisance) 45 (ruin) 10 uo 2 0 2 (buoy) 0 uu 1 0 1 (vacuum) 0 Totals 25 1506 490 1016 33

* Examples of words that conformedor were exceptions to the generalization. The vowel letters a,e,i,o, and u In every instance, only the first-listed only were investigated, andWebster's pronunciation was recorded. New Collegiate Dictionary (7)was Percentage of utility for the original used as the dictionary of authority for vowel digraph generalization wascom- the pronunciation and syllabic division puted by dividing the total number of all words considered in the study. of incidents which conformed to the 114 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION generalization by the total number of each of these four subgroups was con- incidents investigated.Likewise, per- sideredsufficiently high to warrant centages of utility for the twenty-five further investigation. subgroups of adjacent vowel combina- In an attempt to determine the pos- tions were computed in the same man- sibility of formulating new vowel di- ner. graph generalizations that would prove Results.When the original vowel di- usefultochildreningrades one graph generalization was applied to the through six, two new generalizations list of 1506 words containing adjacent were formulated andinvestigated. vowel incidents, 490 words conformed Examination of Table 3 reveals that a to the generalization and 1016 words generalization stated, "When two vow- were exceptions, resulting in an overall els are side by side, usually only one utilityofonlythirty-threepercent. vowel sound is heard," was found to Table 1 presents the results. possess a high utility of ninety-two Data obtained from the analysis. of percent. The second newly formulated each of the twenty-five subgroups of vowel digraph generalization, "When adjacent vowel combinations are pre- ai, ca, cc,oroais found in a word, sented in Table 2.Only four sub- usually only the long sound of the first groups were found to have a percentage vowel is heard," was applicable to 602 of utility above fifty percent :ai, ca, cc, words. Four hundred and thirty-three andoa.Frequency of occurrence in words conformed to the generalization Table 3 New Vowel Digraph Generalizations

Number of New Incidents Number of Number of Percentage Generalications Investigated Conformations Exceptions of Utility

1. When two vowels are side 1506 1381 (juice)* 125 (idea)* 92 by side, usually only one vowel sound is heard. 2. When oi, ea, ce, or oa 602 433 (pail) 169 (pair) 72 is found in a word, (bead) (steak) usually only the long (feel) (been) sound of the first (goat) (cupboard) vowel is heard.

* Examples of words that conformed or were exceptions to the generalization. and 169 words were exceptions, yield- groups, with low percentages of utility ing a percentage of utility of seventy- and high frequencies of occurrence, two percent. that presented definite and consistent Although no attempts were made in patterns in regard to exceptions :an, the present study to investigate the in, io, oi, oo, on,andua.Hopefully, possibilities of rewording, restricting, children should learn to recognize oi or formulating new generalizations rel- and ou as diphthongs and, thus, elimi- ative to adjacent vowel combinations nate any need for applying vowel di- that yielded low percentages of utility, graph generalizations when oi and ou it should be noted that Table 1 reveals are met in word analysis.Likewise, thefollowingadjacentvowelsub- oo was found to usually have its own 121 BAILEY 115 distinctive sounds, as in foot and cool;ties to learn that1)of and ou are an nearly always had the sound of cir- diphthongs, 2) oo usually has its own cumflex o (6), as in caught; io was distinctive sounds, 3) an nearly always usually found in the phonograms lion has the same sound as a in call and and sion and then was pronounced asraw, 4)ioisusually found in the short u, as in vacation and permission;phonograms lion and sion and then is and when is and ua were found withinpronounced as short it, and 5) when is words, usually both vowel sounds wereand ua are found in words, usually heard separately, as in giant and ac-both vowel sounds are heard sepa- tual. Furthermore, results of the pres- rately.Children should be aided also ent study reveal that the value of in word analysis by the knowledge that vowel digraph generalizations would the sounds of vowel digraphs areusu- begreatly.enhancedifelementaryally affected by the consonants that fol- school children also learn that vowellow, notably r following a vowel di- sounds are altered when followed bygraph. the letter r, as in heard and fair. Finally, care should be taken toas- sist children in the development of flex- Conclusions ibility in the use of all phonic general- Children in grades one through six izations. should gain help in word analysis if they understand that when two vowels REFERENCES are together in a word, only one vowel 1.Bailey, Mildred Hart."An Analytical sound is usually recorded.The for Study of the Utility of Selected Phonic Generalizations for Children in Grades lowing generalization might well be the One Through Six," unpublished doctoral first developed by children regarding dissertation, 1965. vowel digraphs, for it is basic and un- 2.Bailey, Mildred Hart. "Utility of Phonic Generalizations in Grades One Through derlies other, more specific phonic gen- Six," Reading Teacher,20 (February eralizations: "When two vowels are 1967), 413-418. side by side, usually only one vowel 3. Burmeister, Lou E. "Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations,"ReadingTeacher,21 sound is heard." (January 1968), 349-356, 360. Results of the present investigation 4. Burrows, Alvina Treut, and Zyra Lourie. "When 'Two Vowels GoWalking," indicate that the following, more spe- Reading Teacher,17 (November 1963), cific phonic generalization should also 79-82. prove useful to children in word analy- 5. Clymer, Theodore."Utility of Phonic Generalizations in the Primary Grades" sis: "When al, ea, cc, or oa is found in Reading Teacher, 16 (January1963), a word, usually only the long sound of 252 -35R. the first vowel is heard." 6. Emans, Robert."Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations Above the Primary The value of the newly formulated Grades,Reading Teacher,20 (February vowel digraph generalizations should 1967), 419-425. be enhanced by affording children in7. Webstes New CollegiateDictionary. Springfield,Massachusetts :G. and C. the elementary grades the opportuni- Merriam Company, 1961.

1224 When Two Vowels Go Walking

ALV1NA TRUET BURROWSandZYRA LOURIE

GEORGE BERNARD SHAW claimeddons about vowelsstillclaiming that we needed at least eighteenattendon,aninvestigationseems vowels in order to write English pho-overdue into the specific reliability netically. Among the vagaries of ourof such a widely taught rule as the language he pointed to "tough" and"two vowels together" rule in pri- "cough,""sawed" and"sword,"mary reading. "sweat" and "sweet," "wheat" and In the early elementary grades, "what." To eliminate this "recklessteachers often UP: a jingle to teach inconsistency" he suggested a newwhat is considered by many a basic alphabet withtwenty-fourconso-phonic principle to helpchildren nants and eighteen vowels (12). In"unlock" new words.Oftenthe England a thousand children are tak-"rule" goes like this: ing part in an experiment in learning When two vowels go out walking to read with an augmented Roman The first one does the talking. alphabet based on rather consistentSometimes it goes like this: relationshipsbetweenlettersand Big brother speaks: sounds (7).0ther attempts to simpli- Little brother is silent. fy English spelling continue as they have for decades. But changes in lan- A recent book presents these adja- guage arrive slowly, and we mustcent vowels as "polite" and "impo- induct our exploding primary readinglite" digraphs. The "polite" digraph classes now using our conventionalconsistsof "two vowelstogether alphabet. Surely, teachers of reading(holding hands), the first one speak- must seek those generalizations whiching and saying its own name" (3). will be helpful; but, when we offerAn impolite digraph, according to rigid "rules" to children for wordthe same author, is "two vowels to- recognition, are we really helping?gether (holding hands), the second In the case of vowel rules, we haveone speaking and saying either its the added difficulty that scholars arelong or short name." The writer goes still debating classification of vowels.on to explain that a diphthong holds For example, several linguists, amonghands, too, but in this case the vowels them Dr. Donald J. Lloyd of Wayneboth speak, making a "common State University, state that Englishsound." A child must then memorize has three semivowels and nine vowels,the "rules" and proceed to apply as in the words pit, pet, pat, just,them, one by one, to the new words putt, pot, put, the first sound in obey,met in his reading. Such complicated and the first in audacious (6). learning must surely be based on With these and similar basic ques-solid language facts! Perhaps! The Reading Teacher, 17 (November 1963). 79.82. 123 BURROWS 117

Analysis of High Frequency certainly,aisle,curtain,airplane, Words . pair, dairy, chair, hair, fairy. We decided to test the validity of Of the 122 words with the ie com- the"two-vowels-together rule" bybination, 29 follow the "big brother" analyzing the five thousand words ofrule; 81 do not. The other 12 words highest frequency on the Rinslandcomprise a category in which each list (9). We checked pronunciationvowel is pronounced separately as in ofallwords containing adjacentdiet. Among the 29 words that illus- vowels in Webster's New Collegiatetrate the rule are such common ones Dictionary (11). A total of 1,728as: dies, flies, pie, replied, tries, tie, words were found to have two adja-lies, cried. But the exceptions to the cent vowels, including a, e, i, o, u, y, first vowel domination are also com- and w. Of this number only 668 mon : friend, field, fierce, piece, niece, words followed the "big brother"candies,movie, puppies,mischief, rule. Furthermore, it should be notedstories, worried, ladies, soldier, an- that these 668 words include thecient, armies, apiece, believe, relief. words in which two vowels form Even in words containingthe diphthongs. Since the diphthong isdouble ee, where a consistent long usually not taught in the first grade,vowel sound seems inescapable, the the beginning reader who is taughteffect of a following r produces an the "easy" rule sees only the "twoimportant difference:cheerful, en- vowels together." If he applies thegineer, pioneer, deer, cheer, steer. rule he's been taught, he has less most obvious exception in the than a fifty-fifty chance of being rightce group is, of course, the word been. as the following data reveal. Another group of words in which When we examine some of theboth vowels were sounded indicated subgroupings of words with adjacentthat the first vowel had its long sound vowels, we see that the largest grouponly occasionally. The is combina- consists of words containing the eationinparticularshowedlittle combination. In this group, 157 fol-consistency: trial, diary, aviator, im- low the "rule"; 111 do not. Amongmediately, giant, colonial, piano, In- the latter are such common words as: dian. In addition there are museum, great, break, ocean, weather, steak,geography, rodeo. head, meadow, heart, heard, and A study of words containing y re- heavy. vealed the difficulties to be met in Another large group of adjacentformulating a rule for this "semi- vowels is represented by the combina-vowel." tion ai. The rule applies to 103, but The "vowel" w proved no easier! does not hold for 36 words. The ruleIs w a silent vowel or a silent con- covers more examples than there arcsonantintowards,jewel,power, exceptions,obviously;but amongtower, answer, view, now, Hallow- these 36 exceptions are such commoneen? words as: fountain, captain, certain, It must also be noted that 49 124 118 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

The Vocabulary of Beginning Y AS A VOWEL Y AS A CONSONANT (SILENT) (SOUNDED) Readers Follows the Follows No Rule Rule The stout defender of the two- canyon vowel rule, however, will claim that rye coyote beyond dye crayons lawyer the primary school pupil does not dyeing mayor bye encounter five thousand words in his early reading. Perhaps the rule is consistent for those words met most words were counted with threevow-often in primers and first readers? An els together. Among theseare suchanalysis of the vocabulary of the be- high frequency words as: beautiful,ginning readers of five publishers, eye, view, curious, delicious. Wouldlisted by Botel in 1955 (2) presented "three brothers" now be helpful infindings which are given in the tabu- word recognition? lar analysis below. There comes a time, hopefully, Afterstudyingtheaccounting when the young reader quite readily given here, or any other analysis of identifies many diphthongs at sight.the sounds of adjacent vowels, one When these words are cast out of themight rightly ask, "When two vow- list of five thousand, how much help els go out walking, what does either is the two-vowel rule? Using Betts'of them say?" If the vowels "say" definition of a diphthong, we in-anything at all to beginning readers, cluded the following list: oy, ow, oi, ou, ew (1). There are 389 wordsthey make a baffling "speech." A considerably larger number of words containing diphthongs in thefiverefute the "rule" than obey it. This thousand words of highest frequency conclusion is essentially the same as in the Rinsland list. Omitting thesethat reached by Oaks in 1952 after words, we have a list of 1,399 wordsanalyzing a representative group of containing adjacent vowels. readers dated 1932-1939 to discover SUMMARY OF VOWEL which "vowel situations"illustrate COMBINATIONS principles of pronunciation (8).

IN FIVE TDDUSAND WORDS OF HIGHEST Recently Clymer (4) reported a FREQUENCY FROM RINSLAND'S LIST study (not yet published when we collected our data)in which he Number of words containing two adjacent vowels, a, e, i, o, u 1.414 speaks of the "rather dismal failure" Number of words containing two of the adjacent vowel rule in provid- adjag cnt vowels. a, e, i, o, u, w, y1,728 Number of words containing three ing correct pronunciation even 50 adjacent %ets 49 per cent of the time. Number of words containing two adjacent vowels as diphthongs....._..... 389 Our analysis of the Gates Primary Number of words containing two adjacent vowels not diphthongs. 1,339 Word List (5) revealed essentially Number of words in which first long the same facts as Sister Mary Chris- sound dominate.__ 668 Number of exceptions to the long tine'sclassificationofthevowel vowel "rule". 1.060 sounds in the same list (10). 125 BURROWS 119

ANALYSIS OF VOWEL SOUNDS IN THE BEGINNING READERS No. Words First Vowel Other Single vowels pro. No. Two Adjacent Long Sound Vowel Sound pounced Separately Reader Words Vowels (blue, play) (you, said) (lion) Preprimer 41 8 3 5 0 Primer 67 20 9 II 0 First 124 36 13 23 0 Second 1st level 152 47 21 26 0 2nd level 207 64 17 46 I Third 1st level 283 85 38 44 3 2nd level 311 106 44 61 1 Total 1,185 366 145 216 5 Total words with three adjacent vowels (a, e, i, a, u, y, w): 10 (beautiful, flower, quiet, queer, eye, queen. toward, delicious, squeal, tower)

Applications for Teaching ing, Chicago: Follett, 1962. (List made in 1955.) What recommendation for teach- 3. Caroline, Sister Mary. Teachers Mow ing can be made from the above ual for Breaking the Sound Barrier: A Phonics Handbook, p. 42. New York: data? To know what not to teach Macmillan, 1960. when error is so apparent is one step 4. Clymer, Theodore. "TheUtilityof Phonic Generalizations in the Primary forward, but only a short one. What Grades," Reading Teacher, 16(Jan. kind of generalization can one teach? 1963) , 252.258. 5. Gates, Arthur J. A Reading Vocabulary Perhaps the only solution that can for the Primary Grades, New York: be tentatively applied is to help the Bureau of Publications, Teachers Col- lege. Columbia University, 1935. beginning reader from time to time 6. Lloyd, Donald J. "Reading American to note what vowel sound is used in English Sound Patterns." Paper read at spring meeting. May 13. 1961, of the the common words he meets. In Chicago Area Reading Association. reading again or could, in said or 7. New York Times, July 18. 1961, p. 31. 8. Oaks. Ruth L "A Study of the Vowel guess or brought or ready, he may Situations in a Primary Vocabulary." gain his greatest help from context Education, 82 (1952). 604-17. 9. Rinsland, Henry D. A Basic Vocabu- and from consonants. As he gains a lary of Elementary School Children. larger and larger reading vocabulary, New York: Macmillan, 1945. hearing that the vowel sounds vary 10. Sullivan,Sister Mary Christine. "A Phonetic Analysis of the New Gates' may be the most substantial help that Primary Reading Vocabulary." Educa- he can take to the analysis of new tional Research Monographs. Washing. ton, D.C., 1939. words. II. Webster's New Collegiate Didionary. Springfield,Massachusetts:G. & C. References Merriam, 1959. I.Betts, Emmett A. Foundations of Read- 12. Wilson, Albert Richard. The Miracu- ing Instruction. New York: American lous Birth of Language. Introduction Book Company, 1954. by George Bernard Shaw. New York: 2. Botel, Morton. How To Teach Read- Philosophical Library, 1948. When Two Vowels Go Walking and Other Such Things

ROBERT EMANS FROM TIME TO TIME, research reports will stimulate the thinking and actions of other investigators. Such was the research reported in an article, "The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in the Primary Grades," in The Reading Teacher by Theodore Clymer (1963). Clymer se- lected forty-five generalizations and developed a word test from four widely used sets of readers in the primary grades. Then he set two criteria, admittedly arbitrary, as to what was meant by a "reasonable" degree of application. The first criterion was that there must be at least twenty words to which the generalization might apply. The second criterion was a percent of utility of at least seventy-five. Only eighteen of the forty-five generalizations met the criteria of usefulness in Clymer's study. A number of generalizations which had long been considered to have merit were among those which did not meet the criteria. For example, the "When two vowels go walking, the first does the talking" or "When there are two vowels side by side, the long sound of the first one is heard and the second is usually silent," generalization was one of these. Clymer concluded that "some time- honored customs in the teaching of reading may be in need of revi- sion." The need for revision has been interpreted by some people to mean that many commonly taught phonic generalizations should be discarded. In a study supported by the Cooperative Research Program of the Office of Education (Emans, 1965) and reported in The Reading Teacher (Emans, 1967), this investigator replicated Clymer's study, but used a random sampling of words beyond the primary level. The writer reasoned that, although some phonic generalizations may not be useful with words in the primary grades, these same generalizations might have utility for the words which the child is required to recognize later. The results of the two studies showed that thirteen generalizations demonstrated their usefulness at both primary and upper grade levels; at least four generalizations which met the criteria of usefulness on the primary level failed to do so for words beyond the primary level; and three generalizations, which were found not to be useful at the primary level, were useful for words beyond the primary level. Hence, when the results of the two studies were combined, only twenty-one of the forty-five generalizations identified by Clymer were found to be useful. As this investigator's study progressed, he became increasingly aware that if certain changes were made in Clymer's generalizations, their utility could be raised. In some cases a simple rewording of the generali- The Reading Teacher, 21 (December 1967) , 262'49. PP- 7

EMANS 121

nation could increase the utility from a few percentage points to near- ly a hundred. Also, since generalizations need not be applied in isolation, but can be applied in conjunction with each other, their usefulness could be increased greatly by combining them. Therefore, this paper reports some possible modifications in Clymer's generalizations which may increase their utility. Its purpose is not to encourage keeping practices which are basically weak,but to avoid discarding practices which may require on:y modification and not abandonment. In some cases, the writer suggests the rewording of get. eralizations which already have a fairly high percent of utility. Some- times, such rewording increases the utility even more.

Related literature Various attempts have been made to find consistencies in the English language to serve as aids in word recognition. Ironically, probably more inconsistencies than consistencies have been discovered. One of the first studies (Voge:, Jaycex, Washburne, 1923) was con- ducted by a group of teachers in the Winnetka Public Schools, Winnetka, Illinois. The study, which was later expanded (Washburne and Vogel, 1928), listed common phonograms to be taught in the primary grades. In a similar study, Atkins (1926) examined the relation between the phonetic and unphonetic occurrences of high frequency symbols. He found sounds which were most apt to accompany the various symbols and concluded that the then often-taught phonetic elements were fre- quently not the most common elements. More recent studies of English have shown it to be complex and seemingly inconsistent. An examination of even a few common words such as bread, once, laugh, straight, and knife reveals silent letters, in- consistent spellings and exceptions to established rules. Hildreth (1958, p. 153) states that there are no unvarying sounds and that vowel sounds depend on associations with the other letters within words. She states that only 200 of the 350 commonest words can be written as they sound (1958, p.152). Armstrong (1949) found that the twenty-six letters in the alphabet give rise to at least 117 sounds. Anderson (1964) states that there are three hundred different combina- tions which express the seventeen vowel sounds; for example, ow has one sound in owl, cow anddown, but another sound in grow, flow, and snow. Horn (1954, pp. 129-132) points out that the short i, as in hit,is spelled twenty-two different ways. And of course, the dassic example of ough has different sounds in ought, rough, though, and through. Furthermore, nearly all letters of the alphabet are silent at some time. Hildreth (1958,p.153) notes that about two-thirds of the THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION words in an unabridged dictionary contain silent letters, e.g. here, were, through, once, one, enough, doubt, and sleigh. Sartorious (1931) found more exceptions than examples of the silent e rule. Clymer (1961) re- ported that in 35 percent of the cases involving the most common 220 words, the first of two vowels was not long when the second was a silent e. Dolch (1951, p. 35) found that there are about as many excep- tions to the two vowel rule as there are instances when it applies. However, the rile does often apply, he found, for specific vowel situa- tions such as ai, ee, ea, oa, and ay. Although many studies have shown inconsistencies in the English language, others have demonstrated consistent patterns. For example, Hanna and Moore (1953) found English to be 86.9 percent phonetic, as they defined it. They found that single consonants are represented by regular spellings about 90 percent of the time. A number of other specific studies have been conducted. Dolch (1938) lists 200 common syllables after determining that 81 percent of words studied were of more than one syllable. Osburn (1954) found the most common syllables in children's written vocabulary to be ing, ed, er, by, es, lion, and y. A study by Hildreth (1958,p. 154) showed that 25 percent of the words in English are composed of base forms with varied prefixes or endings. Thorndike (1941, p. 115) lists the common suffixes as ion, lion, ation, er, y, al, ent, ful, ity, ty, are, and ous. Stauffer (1942) found that fourteen prefixes make up 82 percent of the total number of prefixes. These are ab, ad, be, corn, de, dis, en, ex, in, pre, pro, re, sub, and un. In respect to phonograms, Dolch (1938) showed that the twenty- four most commonly taught phonograms make up only 28 percent of the syllables in words children are likely to find in their textbooks. He recommended that teachers should, therefore, teach syllabication. Gun- derson (1939) listed the phonograms found in the ten reading manuals for teaching in grades 1 and 2. Spache's (1939) study revealed that the three and four letter phonograms such as ake and tight are more phonetically constant than the two letter combinations such as en, on, at, an. Oaks (1952) computed the frequency of vowel sound combinations in basal readers. She concluded that one-third of vowel situations ap- pear as early as the primer. Anderson (1964, p. 130) states that the schwa vowel sound is found in half of the multisyllabic words. In respect to consonants, Black (1952) reports that they are evenly distributed between initial and final positions in syllables with very few consonants appearing in the middle of a syllable. In a fairly recent artide, Fry (1964) summarizes the findings of studies by Black (1952) and others in respect to phonic rules. He also

129 EMANS 123

presents a study of his own on 300 words usually taught in the primary grades and suggests twenty-one rules which he found to be worth teaching. Possible modifications In the discussion that follows, Clymer's original rule will be stated and the possible modifications will be given. Practicaluse of the modi- fications would probably indicate still otherways of restating them to correspond better with the understandings of children. 1. When there are two vowels side by side, the long sound of the first one is heard and the second is usually silent. Atotal of 480 words were identified in the author's studyas being words to which this generaliiation could apply. Only eighty-seven words conformed; there were 393 exceptions. Therefore, for the words beyond the primary level, it exhibited a utility of only 18 percent. Clymer found a utility of 45 percent for words in the primary grades. When the specific vowel combinations of ai, ea, oa, ui, andre were omitted, 318 words remained to which one might hope to apply the generalization.Of these 318 words, only eight, or 3 percent, followed the rule. Therefore, the usefulness of this geniralization must be questioned. Nevertheless, a number of specific vowel combinations hada high percent of utility. The bowel combination ai hada percent utility of 83; oa had a percent utility of 86; ee hada. percent utility of 100. In addition, a detailed study of the exceptions to thetwo vowel generali- zation indicated other possible generalizations which mightbe useful. Out of a total of 131 words, the vowel combination io hada short u sound as in nation 113 i,imes, or a utility of 86percent. The vowel com- bination ui was found to have a short i sound 79percent of the time. In addition, the oo combination had the soundas in food in 74 percent of the words and the sound as in good in the remaining26 percent of the time. Hence, the following generalizations replace,to some extent, the "When two vowels go walking the first does the talking"generali- zation. a. When the first vowel in a word is a and the second is i, thea is usually long and the i silent. b. When the first vowel is o and the second isa, the o is usually long and the a is silent. c. Words having double e usually have the long c sound. d. The letters io usually represent a shortu sound as in nation. e. The letters oo usually have the long double o sound as in foodor the short double o sound as in good. Theyare more likely to have the double o sound as in food. f. The vowel combination ui has a short i sound.

130 124 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

2. When a vowel is in the middle of a one-syllable word, the vowel is short. Thisdue hada 73percent utility for words beyond the primary level, as determined by this study, and a 62 percent utility for words within the primary level, as found by Clymer. However, of the thirty- eight exceptions for words beyond the primary level, thirteen had anr followingthe vowel. An important generalization found in both Clymer's and this study was that the vowel sound may be modified if it precedes an T. Therefore, if the clue is changed to state, "When a vowel is in the middle of a one-syllable word, the vowel is short except that it may be modified in words in which the vowel is followed by an r," it hasa utility of 80 percent, increasing its usefulness considerably. 3.When words end with silent "e", the preceding "a" or "i" is long. As stated, this generalization was found to have a utility of only 48 percent in this study. If words ending inlewere excluded, the utility could be raised to 67 percent; to 71 percent if words ending withleand words withivewere excluded. These exceptions warrant further investi- gation in that Clymer found that his generalization had a 60 percent utility for words on the primary level. If the proposed exceptionswere applied, Clynter's generalization might become useful in the primary grades, measured against the criteria used. 4. The two letters "ow" make the long "o" sound. Thegeneralization as stated had a 50 percent utility. However, if it were changed to, "The two lettersow makethe longosound or the ou sound as in out," it would have 100 percent utility in the sample of this study. 5.When "y" is the final letter in a word, it usually has a vowel sound. This generalization had an 84 percent utility using words in the primary grades and a 98 percent utility using words above the primary grades. In all exceptions in this study, the y was silent, following the geLieralization that in ay theyis silent and gives "a" its long sound which Clymer found to be a useful generalization. Therefore, the two generalizations together would have 100 percent utility for words above the primary level. Similar findings might have been noted had Clymer applied the two generalizations together in his studyperhaps increasing the rules' usefulness on the primary level. 6.When "y" is used as a vowel, it sometimes has the sound of long"i". Stated this way, this generalization has a utility of only 4 percent. However, if it were changed to read, "Wheny is used as avowel, it sometimes has the sound of longe,"it would have a utility of 92 per- cent. 7. The letter "a" has the same sound (6) when followed by "I", "w", and "u." EMANS 125

As stated, the percent of utility it 24. Only four of the al words follow this due, all but twelve of them having either long or short a sounds. Therefore, the / docs not seem to modify the long or short a. However, of the thirty-two au and aw words, all but five, or 84 percent, follow it. If the generalizations were changed to state, "The letter a has the same sound (4) when followed by w am( u," its usefulness would be greatly increased. 8. When "c" is followed by "e" or "i", the sound of "s" is likely to be heard. Eighty-eight words of the sample were possible applications. The clue had a 90 percent utility, meeting the established criteria. In the nine exceptions, the sound of sh was heard rather than s. Therefore, if the generalization were to be restated as "When c is followed by e or i, the sct'nd s or sh is likely to be hear" the utility in this study would be 100 percent. 9. When two of the same consonants are side by side only one is heard. This generalization was applied to 300 words with a 91 percent utility and, therefore, deemed to be useful. Of the twenty-six exceptions, all but three were a result of a prefix or suffix being added, for example, illegal. Ten of the twenty exceptions had an 4 added to a root word ending in I, and both i's were sounded, as in dreadfully. 10. In most tu,a-syllable words, the first syllable is accented. This clue met the criteria for usefulness in both this study and the previous study by Clymer. Approximately a third of the exceptions can be accounted for through application of the generalization that "If a, in, re, ex, de, or be is the first syllable in a word, it is usually unaccented" which was found in both studies to be useful. Additional prefixes such as con- and pre- could explain still other exceptions. 11. One vowel letter in an accented syllable has its short sound. This due applied to 1,490 words analyzed for this study and had a utility of 64 percent. If it is reworded to read, "One vowel letter in an accented syllable has its short sound ifit comes before the end of the syllable and its long sound if it comes at the end of the syllable," the utility would be raised to 78 percent. Addition of the phrase, "Except when it is followed by an r" raised the percent of utility to ninety-two. 12. When "y" or "ey" is seen in the last syllable that is not accented, the long sound of "e" is heard. As the generalization is stated, only three (1 percent) of the 269 applicable words conformed to its claim. However, if it is read "short i" instead of "long e" 261 words (87 percent) would have conformed. 13. When "-lion" is the final syllable in a word, it is unaccented. This due supports the original thinking which prompted this study that the usefulness of phonic generalizations may differ for words above 126 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

and below the primary level. Clymer found only five words from his list to which this rule might apply, while eighty-five words from this study's shorter word list conformed. In both studies the utility was 100 percent. Apparently, it is useful for words beyond the primary level, but it may be of questionable value, as defined in this study, for words within the primary level. A -lion at the end of a four syllable word seems to indicate a second- ary accent on the first syllable with a primary accent on the syllable pre- ceding the -tion. This held true in forty or forty-two words, indicatinga 95 percent utility. The primary accent failed to fall on the second to last syllable only three times out of all the -lion words on the list. The primary accent was on the second to last syllable in all the three-syllable words, all but two of the four-syllable words, and all but one of the five- syllable words. Similar results were found with words ending with -sion. Therefore, the generalization "When -lion or -sion is the final syllable in a word, the primary accent is likely to fall on the second to last syllable," would indicate the location of the primary accent inmany words to which it would apply. 14. In many two- and three-syllable words, the final "e" lengthens the vowel in the last syllable. Of the total number of words in the sample, this clue would beap- plied to 227 with a 42 percent utility; ninety-five words conformed and there were 132 exceptions. Of the exceptions, fifty ended with le and eleven ended with ive. In that no le or ive words followed it, their omission would increase the utility to 64 percent. Other consistentex- ceptions might be found to make the generalization more usable. 15. If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by two consonants, the first syllable usually ends with the first of two consonants. Clymer found that this generalization had only 72 percent utility with the words he studied. In this study, it could be applied to 811 words, meeting the criteria of usefulness with 80 percent utility. How- ever, of the 163 exceptions, twenty-four words included vowels modified with an r, thirty-six words had a double consonant withone of them silent, twenty-one words had a ck with the k sound, twentywere found to have common prefixes, and twenty-two words had one of the sounds th, ch, or sh. All five of these generalizations were found by Clymerto be useful. If these generalizations were applied in conjunction with this generalization, it would have a utility of 96 percent. 16.If the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single consonant, that consonant usually begins the second syllable.

This generalization could be appliedto 659 words of the list with a 47 percent utility. Clymer noted like results.Of the 346 words which

133 EMANS 127

were exceptions, ninety-three were root words plus a prefix, and 240 had beginning syllables which ended with a consonant with a short vowel sound. Consequently, if it read, "Except in some words with a prefix and a root word, if the first vowel sound in a word is followed by a single consonant, that consonant begins the second syllable and the vowel sound in the first syllable will be long, or if the consonant ends the first syllable the vowel sound will be short," the percent of utility would be 84. Of the 240 beginning syllables ending with a consonant and containing a short vowel sound, 227 or 95 percent are accented. Of the 237 beginning syllables ending with a long vowel, 138 or 58 percent are unaccented. 17.If the last syllable of a word ends in "le," the consonant preceding the "le" usually begins the last syllable. This clue met the criteria of usefulness in both studies. In this pres- ent study a total of sixty-eight words could be applied to it with a utility of 78 percent. Of the fifteen exceptions, ten had double consonants, as in squabble, before the le. 18. When a word has only one vowel letter, the vowel sound is likely to be Mort. This generalization could be applied to 136 words in the sample with a 70 percent utility, as compared with 57 percent in the previous study. However, of the forty-one exceptions, fourteen words had vowels which were modified by an r. Omitting these exceptions would raise the per- cent of utility to seventy-eight. This article has suggested modifications and substitutes for some of the commonly taught phonic generalizations. There are probably other modifications which would also be helpful. The belief underlying this effort has been that greater improvement will come about in helping children learn to read if we first attempt to eliminate the weaknesses in existing practices before deciding that they should be discarded.

References Anderson, P. S. Languageskills in elementary education.New York: Macmillan, 1964. Armstrong, S.How words get into the dictionary.New York: Funk and Wagnalls, 1949. Atkins, Ruth E. An analysis of the phonetic elements in a basal reading vocabulary. Elementary School Journal,1926,26, 596.606. Black, E. B. A study, of the consonant situations in a primary reading vocabulary. Education,1952, 72, 618.623. Clymer, T. The utility of phonic generalLations in the primary grades.Changing Concepts of ReadingInstruction. New Yurk: Scholastic Magazines, 1961. P. l56159. Clymer, T. The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades.The Read- ing Teacher,1963,16,252-258. Dolch, E. W. Phonics and polysyllables.Elementary English Review,1938, 15, 120.124.

134 .7

1

128 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

Do 1dt, E. W. The teaching of sounding. Champaign, Illinois: Garrard Press, 1951. Entails, R. The usefulness of word pronounciation rules: cooperative research project. No. S-340. Unpublished research report. Cooperative Research Program of the Office of Education, United States Department of Health, Education, and Wel- fare, 1965. Emans, R. The usefulness of phonic generalizations above the primary level. The Reading Teacher, 1967, 20, 419-425. Fry, E. A frequency approach to phonics. Elementary English, 1964, 41, 759-765. Gunderson, Agnes G. Simplified phonics. Elementary School Journal, 1939. 39, 593.606, Hanna, R. R., and Moore.., Jr. Spelling front spoken word to written symbol. Elementary School Journal, 1953, 53, 329-337. Hildreth, Gertrude. Teaching reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart A: Winston. 1958. Horn, E. Phonics and spelling. Journal of Education, 1954, 136, 233-235. Oaks, Ruth E. A study of the vowel situations in primary vocabulary. Education, 1952, 72, 604-617. Osborn. W. J. Teaching spelling by teaching syllabics and root words. Elementary School Journal, 1954. 55, V- II. Sartorions, I. C. Generalkation in spelling. New York: Teachers College, 1931. Spache, G. 1). A phonics manual for primary and remedial teachers. Elementary English Review, 1939, 16, 191-198. Stauffer, R. G. A study of prefixes in the horndike listto establish a list of prefixes that should be taught in the elementary school. Journal of Educational Re- search, 1942, 33, 453-458. Thormlike, F. L. The teaching of English suffixes. New York: Teachers College, 1941. Vogel. Mabel, pyrex. Emma, and Washburne. C. W. A basic list of phonics for grades 1 and 11. Elementary School Journal, 1923, 23, 436-443. Washburne, C., and Vogel, Mabel. A revised list of phonics for grade II. Elemen- tary School Journal, 1928. 23, 771-777. Vowel Pairs

Lou E. BURMEISTER*

ORDINARILY WHEN TWO VOWELS APPEAR TOGETHER they should be viewed as a grapheme; that is, together they ordinarily represent one sound, known as a phoneme. Burmeister (1966) examined the validity of this statement in 537 randomly selected instances from the Thorndike and Lorge ( 1944) Teacher's Word Book of 30,000 Words and found it to be true 84.5 per cent of the time; she found one-third of the exceptions to be instances of only one vowel pair: is (e.g., giant, liable, piano). The words were selected on fourteen levels, according to frequency of occurrence. "Every vowel combination means a sylla- ble" was true 90.6 per cent of the time for words that occur from six to over 100 times per million running words; it was true 75.8 per cent of the time for words that occur from one to five times per million running words. Several investigators have examined the applicability of the commonly taught phonic generalization: "When two vowels are together, the first is long and the second is silent." They found this "generalization" to be extremely limited in usefulness even in the controlled vocabularies of primary materials (Bailey, 1967; Clymer, 1963; Oaks, 1952) and much more so in uncontrolled vocabularies above the primary level in difficulty (Bailey, 1967; Burmeister, 1966; Emans, 1967). An important question yet to be answered from a linguistic examination of a large stock of words is: What specific phoneme (or phonemes) does each "vowel-vowel" graphemes represent? That is, what are the most common sounds of each vowel-pair? Carrying the question a step further, one might ask whether it is possible to group several vowel pair graphemes according to similar patterns in their phoneme behavior (e.g., digraphs, diphthongs) and which vowel pairs fit in each group. The question is not a new one; how- ever the manner of answering it as set forth in this article is modern.

Procedure Hanna, Hanna, Hodges, and Rudorf in 1966 published a vol- ume titled Phoneme-Grapheme Correspondences as Cues to Spell- The Reading Teacher, 21 (February 1968) , 445-452.

136 130 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

ing Improvement. In it they reported the results of their computer- ized examination of 17,310 words: 15,284 words selected from Part I of the Thorndike -Lorge Teacher's Word Book of 30,000 plus 2,026 words selected from the Merriam-Webster's New Collegiate Dictionary, sixth edition, "which were considered to be relevant to a study of a 'common core' vocabulary but had not been available from Part I of the Thorndike -Lorge word list" (Hanna, et al., 1966, pp. 12-13). The authors of the Hanna, et al. study were concerned with furthering knowledge about spelling by examining phoneme- grapheme relationships. The purpose of the present study is to further knowledge about reading, specifically about the phonic situation. To do so, it is necessary to look at grapheme to phoneme relationships. The procedure is the reverse of that used by Hanna, et al. Using as source materials the findings of Hanna, et al., and reversing them to "grapheme to phoneme" relationships, the author of the present study was able to classify the sounds of all "vowel-vowel" combina- tions which form a single phoneme for the 17,310 words examined. Results The grapheme-phoneme relationship for each possible vowel pair in the English language was examined. In Table 1 the findings are presented. Vowel-pairs not listed did not occurat least as single graphemesin the material. To analyze the data the author grouped some of the vowel pairs according to each pair's most common sound. The vowel pairs which occurred fewer than fifty times in the sample material (which means that in the sample material they occurred less often than once in every 346 words) were not included in this analysis because it was felt that generalizations covering their sounds would be of little value. (It would probably be.easier to learn words which contain these vowel pairs as sight words.) There are at least two ways of viewing the vowel pairs which are included. One way is to look at single phoneme percentages for each grapheme. Another way is to simply look at the frequency of occurrence of each phoneme for each grapheme. Each of these methods has a value. Looking at percentages will show the propor- tional value of each phoneme for each grapheme. Looking at frequencies will demonstrate the actual number value of each phoneme, both in relationship to its grapheme and also in rela- tionship to the total sample. Thus, for example, although "es" most common sound (a) occurs 40 per cent of the time when "ei" occurs as a grapheme in the sample material and "ou's" most common sound (a) occurs 41.2 per cent of the time when "ou" BURMEISTER 131 Table 1Frequency and per cent of occurrence of each single phoneme for each vowel-pair grapheme Grapheme Phonemic Behavior Pronun- NameFrequencyciation Example FrequencyPer Cent Key

ae (6) E algae 5 83.3 e aesthetic 1 16.7

ai (309) a abstain 230 74.4 It air 49 15.6 T mountain 15 4.9 a villain 9 2.3 I again 4 1.3 I plaid 1 .3 r aisle 1 .3

ao (2) 6 extraordinary 2 100

au (178) 6 auction 167 93.8 5 chauffeur 5 2.8 a laugh 4 2.2 a epaulet 1 .6 I gauge 1 .6 ay (137) 6 gray 132 96.4 T kayak 3 2.2 6 says 1 .7 T yesterday 1 .7

aw (77) 6 lawn 77 100 ea (545) I east 275 50.5 6 weapon 140 25.7 ear 49 9.0 0 earth 31 5.7 a bear 13 2.4 I heatty 18 3.3 as great 14 2.6 T guinea 2 .4 a sergeant 3 .5

ee (290) E sleet 248 85.5 fi peer 36 12.4 T been 6 2.1

ei (86) I reign 34 40.0 5 deceit 22 25.6 T foreign 11 12.8 T seismic. 9 10.5 a their 5 5.8 a 3overeigiity 2 2.3 .4., weird 2 2.3 a heifer 1 1.2

co (15) a pigeon 10 66.7 leopard 3 20.0 people 2 13.3

eu (40) u feud 29 72.5 amateur 6 15.0 sleuth 4 10.0 pleurisy 1 2.5 132 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION Table 1 (continued) Grapheme Phonemic Behavior Pronun- NameFrequencyciation Example Frequency 'Per Cent Key ey (69) l' honey 40 58.0 I convey 14 20.3 T geyser 8 11.6 7 key 6 8.7 a eyrie 1 1.4 ew (64) II news 39 60.9 e ro flew 22 34.4 3 sew 3 4.7 is T carriage 3 60 a parliament 2 40 le (156) 3 thief 56 35.9 T lassie 30 19.2 T die 26 16.7 a patient 23 14.7 4. cashier 17 10.9 e friend 4 2.6 oa (138) 3 road 129 93.5 6 broad 9 6.5 oe (22) 6 foe 13 59.1 3 amoeba 5 22.7 oo shoe 4 18.2 of (102) oi moist 100 98.0 a porpoise 2 2.0 00 (315) 66 lagoon 185 58.7 o-o wood 114 36.2 ii floor 9 2.9 5 blood 7 2.2 ou 1815) a rigorous 336 41.2 ou out 285 35.0 66 soup 54 6.6 6 four 47 5.8 a touch 30 3.7 97° your 25 3.1 u journey 22 2.7 E glamour 1 .1 oy (50) oi convoy 49 98.0 T coyote 1 2.0

ow 1250) T:1 own 125 50.0 ou town 121 48.4 6 knowledge 4 1.6 ue (43) ii due, cue 27 62.8 To clue 16 37.2 ui (34) T build 16 47.1 56 fruit 10 29.4 U suit 8 23.5 uo (2) 56 buoyant 2 100 uy (3) Y buy 3 100 139 BURNIEISTER 133

occurs as a grapheme, a look at the frequency data shows that teaching the first sound of "ou" ( which occurs 336 times) would be more valuable than teaching the first sound of "ei" (which occurs only 34 times). Such comparisons can also be made when deciding to teach, or not to teach, the second most common sound of graphemes. For example, it can be seen that "ou's" second most common sound (ou) occurs 35 per cent of the time when "ou" occurs as a grapheme, but the frequency of this sound is 285, a frequency greater than the total number of occurrences of all of the vowel pair graphemes except "ai," "ea," "ee," and "oo," and a fre- quency value greater than the first sound of any other vowel pair. In Table 2 these graphemes are grouped according to their most common phoneme. Included for each grapheme are the three most common sounds, or the number of sounds needed to reach the 90 per cent utility level, which ever occurs first. In Table 3 the data are presented in frequency of occurrence form. The same number of phonemes are included in Table 3 as in Table 2 but a shaded area is added to show the total number of occurrences of each grapheme. Conclusions Phonemes for vowel-pairs tend to fall into four categories: 1. The first vowel may dominate, as is usually true with the following combinations: ai, ay, ea, ee, oa, ow. (That is, when the above vowels "go walking, the first one usually does the talking," and what it says is its own name; however, "ea" may be a long e or a short e, and "ow" may be a long o or an ou.) 2. The two vowels may cooperate (blend), as is usually true with the following combinations: au, aw, oi, ly, and oo. "Oo" may sound as it does in lagoon or wood. (Note the high utility level for this generalization.) 3. A new sound may be created: ei=a, ou=e, ey=i, ew=u*. [It may be surprising to find that ou's most common sound is schwa (rigorous). "Ou's" second most common sound is a diphthong (out); this sound has a very high frequency.] 4. Vowel-pairs may separate. (See Limitations of This Study.) Also, except for the most common sound of "ei" (which is a), "ei" and "ie" may have the same sounds. In order of frequency they are: e, *Many will question that "ey" as in honey = i; to many it sounds like an e; to these "ey" would be a digeaph. Some will also question that "ew" as in news = u, the letter we say when we say the alphabet. It is interesting to note that Webster's Seventh New Collegiate Dictionary, 1963, based on Webster's Third New Interna tional Dictionary, gives the following keys for these words: honey /'hen -e/ and news Pn(y)ilz/.

140 134 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

Table 2Per cent of occurrence of the three most common phonemes, or the number of phonemes needed to reach the ninety per cent utility levelwhichever occurs firstfor commonly found vowel-pair graphemes

Most Common Per Cent Phoneme Behavior Grapheme 0 20 90 60 80 100

digraph ai a a

ay a ea I e- 1 ee e

oa

ow 5 011

diphthong au

aw e

of of

oy of

00 00 00

other ei ii" 1 ey r L ew Ti I ie e 1 I ou T a Implications for teaching This study furnishes proof that what discriminating educators have been teaching is not too far from being correct. It also fur- nishes additional proof that the vowel digraph generalization has limited usefulness. However, the generalization "When two vowels BURMEISTER 135 go walking the first one does the talking" is still turning up in the literature with no qualifications about the limited number of graphemes for which this generalization is appropriate. This study furnishes proof, also, that the second most common sounds of "ea"

Table 3 Total frequency of occurrence, including frequency of occurrence of the three most common phonemes, or the number of phonemes needed to reach the ninety per cent utility levelwhichever occurs firstfor commonly found vowel-pair graphemes

Most Common Frequency Phoneme Behavior Grapheme 000100200 300 900500 600700800900

digraph ai E la M ay

ea e I 1_±_3= ee I e oa 6

OW 0 ou

diphthong au

aw 6

of

oy 00 go 601

other ci 11:1111 ey 11111 ew

ie

ou ou e

142 136 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

and "ow" should be taught. This study also reinforces the value of teaching "au," "aw," "oi," "oy" as diphthongs and the two sounds of "oo." The finding that "ou's" most common sound is a schwa may surprise many, but the frequency of occurrence of "ou" as a diph- thong has been demonstrated. The study may throw light on the relative importance of the various sounds of "ei" and "ie." Teaching a generalization which covers this may have dubious value because of its cumbersomeness. However, children might be taught to try several specific sounds when first meeting a word with either an "ei" or an le" in it. Limitations of the study Only vowel-pairs which represent a single phoneme were analyzed in this study. The author found no way of examining situations in which vowel-pairs form two phonemes. Such an analysis would be possible by looking at the raw data which is sup- plied in the appendices of the Hanna, et al., study. However, Burmeister (1966) found that the generalization: "Every vowel combination means a syllable" was true 84.5 per cent of the time. The following cases are the exceptions: Per cent of total vowel-pair sample 1. "A vowel combination means no syllable." (e.g.,que) 0.7% 2. "A vowel combination means two syllables." a."ia" means two syllables (67% of the times "ia" occurs ) 5.2% b."le" means two syllables (16.7% of the times "ie" occurs c."ea" means two syllables (12% of the times "ea" occurs) 1.5% d."eous" and "ious" mean two syllables ( 47% of the times "ious" occurs; sample too small to judge % for "eous") 1.9% e.Miscellaneous 5.0% References Bailey, Mildred Hart. The utility of phonic generalizations ingrades one through six. The Reading Teacher, 1967, 20, 413-418. Burmeister, Lou E. An evaluation of the inductive and deductivegroup ap- proaches to teaching selected word analysis generalizationsto disabled readers in eighth and ninth grade. Doctoral dissertation, University of Wisconsin,1966. Clymer, T. L. The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades.The Reading Teacher, 1963, 16, 252.258. Emans, R. The usefulness of phonic generalizations above the primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 1967, 20, 419425.

143 BOMA EISTER 137

Hanna, P. R., Hanna, Jean S., Hodges, R. G., and Rudorf, E. H., Jr. Phoneme- grapheme correspondences as cues to spelling improvement. Washington, D .C.: Office of Education, U. S. Department of Health, Education, and Welfare, 1966. MerriamWebster's new collegiate dictionary, sixth edition, 1961. Oaks, Ruth E. A study of the vowel situation in a primary vocabulary. Edu- cation, 1952, 72, 604.617. Thorndike, E. L., and Large, I. The teacher's word book of 30,000 words. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers College, Columbia University, 1944.

144 A Sound-Symbol Frequency Count

DAVID R. STONE

AREAL PROBLEM faced by a readerThese were compiled for gradesone, in attacking a new word is totwo, and three.* associate correctly the printedsym- Pronunciation of each one was bol with the sound appropriate to itschecked, sound by sound, in the place in a word. In English thereareThorndike Barnhart Beginning Dic- many symbols for the same sound,tionary.t For schwa problems,a jury and a particular symbol may haveof thirty graduate studentswas used several sounds. to decide whether emphasis favored For example, a reader who hasthe short u sound. Reference to the learned that a in cat is a short a,examples in tables will give the read- finds that a says short e in any, ander a guide to any particular decision short u in about. he wishes to check. Except for some Many authorities have pointed outBrooklyn and Southern pronuncia- this problem as basic for beginningtion variations, the percentage 'of readers. Three principal solutionssuch problems is under 5 per cent. have been suggested:(1) establishSuch accented regional or colloquial a sound-symbol relation such thatpronunciation was avoided. In the there is a separate symbol for eachcase of pretty, any, and the like, the sound, (2) use cueing devices, suchy was listed as long e, rather than as color, to indicate which sound thesoft i as in many dictionaries. Inter- symbol is being used for, and (3)mediate sounds are usually given as teach the present system in terms ofeither long or short. relative frequency. The latter stresses thesound-symbolrelationsmostLong Vowels often encountered. In view of the The long vowel varieties are listed extensive effort needed to change orin Table 1. Short vowels, combina- simplify, the third approach seemstions, and consonants follow. the most practical at this time. The long a. The long a sound This study presents basic frequen-appeared a total of 138 times. For cy data for sound-symbol relations,this letter'sound the a-e combination which will allow a more. efficientwas the most frequent, accounting approachto teachingthose mostfor 43 per cent of the total. The ai used at the beginning level. The source of data was content D. R. Stone and' Vilda B. Tracy. "A Basic Word List from Basal Readers,Ele- researchdoneonapproximately mentary English,April 1963, pp. 420.427. 6,000 sounds in a list of words corn- tE. L. Thorndike and C. L. Barnhart. Thorndike Barnhart Beginning Dictionary mon to five basal reading series. (Ne York: Scott, Forcsman, 1952). The Reading Teacher,19 (April 1966), 498.504. STONE 139

TABLE I LONG VOWELS Long a Total 138 Long 0Total 125 (regular 85, irregular 53) (regular 77, irregular 48) ae cage 60 o colt, horse 0 ai wait 27 oc pole 37 a baby 25 OW arrow 22 ay birthday 17 oa railroad 15 cigh freight 4 On course. four 6 cy obey 0 oo floor 2 ea great 2 oe goes 2 aigh straight ough though Long c Total 203 (regular 82, irregular 121) y badly 62 ea bead 4 Long uTotal 5 ce agree 43 (regular 2, irregular 3) eleven 39 ti usual 9 Cr money 7 icw view, few 2 ie thief 4 eau beauty ci neither 3 eo people

Long iTotal 103 Those called regular are starred(a (regular 65. irregular 38) vowel plus c combination, vice, or the i.e fine 47 vowel alone) . behind 18 lying 13 Total Regular 311 igh light 12 Total Irregular 263 ie tied II 574 cy eye 1 ig sign

combination appeared 20 per cent ofthe total. The other combinations the time, and a was made the longtogether comprised about 1 per cent. sound alone about 18 per cent of theThese were the ie, ey, i, eo, and ei time. The only other important com-sets. They were mostly found in bination was the ay, about 11 perhigher grade levels. Teachers review- cent.Othervariationsoccurreding this study have commented on mainly in the advanced levels andthe fact that the y in pretty is listed formed less than 1 per cent of theas a long e and not short i, as many lone a sounds. These arc the ey, ea,dictionaries give it. In this report, aigh, and eigh combinations. badly and pretty are taken to have The long e. The long e soundthe same ending sound, a long e. appeared a total of 203 times. Sur- The longi. The long i sound prisingly enough it was formed mostappeared a total of 103 times. It was frequently by a lone y (approximate-formed 46 per cent of the time by ly 31 per cent). The ea and ee com-thei-e combination. The longi binations were almost equal in num-sound was made by i alone 17 per ber, each about 21 per cent. Thecent of the time. The ie, y, and igh single e accounted for 19 per cent ofcombinations appeared almost equal-

146 140 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

lv often, each about 11 per cent ofto use a short , o. Since regional em- the total. The er and ig combina-phases have not been used, these tions each appeared once. were classified as long o. The long o. The long o sound The long u. The long u sound in appeared 125 times. The o alonean initial position is a difficult sound and the o-e combination were mostto catalog. Formally, the sound is important, the first accounting forlone e plus oo. Some give it as y plus 34 per cent of the total and theoo; otherwise itis oo, as in blue, latter, 32 per cent. The ow symbolwhich is not a long u sound. was next with a total of 22 tima. It The problem appeared rather in- accounted for about 18 per cent offrequently, a total of only 5 times in the total long o sounds. The oa com-the study. Only the y plus oo and bination became quite important inthe long e plus oo sounds have been the third grade levels and producedincluded here. The oo sounds of u 12 per cent of the total. The otherare listed in oo (as in duty). Long combinationsoo, ou, oe, and oughvowels aro more irregular than reg- accounted for about 6 per cent ofular. Findings for short vowels are the total. given in Table 2. The long o sound in words like horse, born, and corn are difficult to Short Vowels classify since there is some tendency The short a. The short a appeared

TABLE2 Slum. Vowcis Short a'Dual 149 Short o Total 171 (regular 147, irregular 2) (regular103,irregular 68) hat 147 0 hopping 103 au laugh 2 a harvest 53 au because Short cTotal 176 ay hawk 4 (regular 1.18. irregular 28) ough fought 3 C egg. let 148 :igh naughty 2 a arrow 13 oa broad ai airplane 9 ea heart tte guess is friend Short u Total 291 bury (regular 103, irregular 188) ea ready ti but 103 ay prayer come (13 ci their broken 56 a elephant 46 Short iTotal 178 oat trouble 9 (regular 169. irregular 9) ollicc hit 169 O station 4 build 2 ai curtain 3 ea dear ec been Regular. except u 567 busy Irregular. except n 107 y bicycle Regular u 103 a language 2 Irregular u 188 O women STONE 141

149 times. Except for two au's, the"bossy" r has an effect, but does not a alone was used. destroy the soft o sound. "Because," The short e. The short e appearedfor example, could just as easily be a total of 176 times. It was generallyspelled "becoz," using the soft o. formed by e alone (85 per cent). The short u. Of the short vowels, The a and ai combinations each ap-the short u sound occurred most peared only once. These are the ue,often. It was formed in a variety of ie, u, ea, ay, and ei groups. ways. The vowels u (35 per cent) e While there is the possibility of hear-(20 per cent), a (16 per cent), and ing "leg" pronounced "laig," thiso (22 per cent) were the most im- kind of regional inflection is not usedportant variations. Other combina- in this study. While the long a maytions formed the sound about 6 per be heard in "prayer" and "their,"cent of the time; these were the i, ou, they are included here as short e onand ai combinations. the basis of most common usage. A The issue of "nicety versus utility" middle course between "prissy" over-is a problem here. The schwa o is exactness and coarse regionalism hasclearincome, but other schwa been used for the problem cases.sounds, such as the i in office, repre- Since examples are given for each,sent language in motion, and are the reader may observe how it washard to catalog. The examples indi- done. cate the ways in which the decisions The short i. The short i sound ap-were made. The short u showed peared a total of 178 times. Of these,more irregularity than regularity; 97 per cent of the time it was formedotherwise, short vowels are approxi- by i alone. Some schwa problemsmately five times more regular than appeared. The i in animal, for exam-irregular. ple, was counted as short u rather than short i. In music, there is noCombinations and Silent Letters problem for the short i. While many Combinations of letters and silent variations were seen, no particularletters are seen in Table 3. one occurred often. The short oo (look) sound. This The short o. The short o soundsound occurred 30 times in the study. was produced in a variety of ways,It was formed 70 per cent of the but the most important was a lone o.time by the oo combination. The This accounted for 60 per cent oflone u accounted for about 17 per the total of 171. The only importantcent of the sounds and the ou com- variation was the lone a, which ac-bination, 10 per cent. counted for about 30 per cent of the The long oo (food) sound. The total. The other combinations ac-sound occurred 63 times in the study. counted for about 10 per cent. TheIt was produced 30 per cent of the lone o is a simplification of the ahtime by an oo combination. Varieties sound. Its utility may be easily testedof combinations with the single letter by writing it in an ar word. Theo accounted for 19 per cent and the 148 142 THE CURRICU1.UM IN WORD RECOGNITION

TABLE 3 COMBINATIONS AND SILEN'IS Short ooTotal 30 Cr Total 124 (regular 21, ircgular 9) (regular 92, irregular 32) on look 21 er another 92 push 5 ar collar ou should 3 or color li 0 woman car early ti ir birthday ur picture .1 Long ooTotal 63 crc where wcr oo (regular 25, irregular 38) answer food 25 tit (55) change 45 0 to, more, two 12 tch watch 5 ew knew 8 ti question 4 -e rule 5 slt (55) shut 43 1.1 duty ti notion 8 Call beauty 2 sure 2 ut suit, juice 3 ocean 2 ou soup 2 th (35) thin 35 ough through 1 th (34) though 31 oc shoe 1 wIt (13) when 13 ue true 1 tig (26) going 26 /eTotal 29 on Total 41 le little (regular 25, irregular 16) 29 ou ground 25 Silents ow allow 16 Semi-irregular silent e e come 52 oiTotal 12 Irregular silent e (regular 8, irregular 4) house 51 oi noise 8 oy boy 4 Regular ew combination, 13 per cent. Other The er sound. The er sound oc- combinationsobservedwerethecurred a total of 124 times in the ough, u-e, oe, u, eau, ui, ue, and oudata. The er combination accounted combinations. (In a single symbolfor about 72 per cent of the total. system, if oo is reserved for the lookThe other combinations were the or, sound, then ew can be used here.) ear, ir, and are sets. The ou sound. The sound occur- Silents. Silent e was classified as red a total of 41 times. The ou com-being either irregular or semiirregu- bination was most used, with ow alar. Classification was semiirregular little more than half as often. Whatif the e was in a position to influence is "regular" is rather arbitrary here.the sound of a preceding vowel but Is ouch more regular than cow? did not make the vowel long. Some The oi sound. The oi sound oc-examples of this are:are, come, curred seldom, only 12 times in all.here, were, cabbage, careful, police- Of this total it was produced 67 perman, automobile, and given. They cent of the time by the oi combina-were classified as irregular if they tion and 33 per cent by oy. had no influence on a preceding

149 STONE 143 vowel. Two methods wcrc used tocompletely unrcl. tcd and are given determine this: separately here: tongue u, island s, 1.If there wcrc two consonantslisten I, chocolate o, two w, whistle t, between it and the preceding vowel;fasten t. These occur usually once, for example, horse,else,change,and no more than twice. ache, and waste. 2.IftherewerealreadytwoConsonants vowels together which could product The varietiesofsound-symbol the given sound; for example, house,combinationsforconsonantsare noise, please, because, leaves, choose,given in Table 4. and squirrel. Regular silent e's (the Note that the greatest kind of ones which made a preceding vowelvariation appears in the double-letter long) were classified with the vowelproblem. The function of double in the respective tables. letters is to guide the reader to a short Silentletters have usually bccnpreceding vowel sound ( the short e given with the adjacent letters (asin letter, for example). This function dj for j in adjacent), but some seemhas been obscured, however, and is TABLE 4 CONSONANTS

"b (170) basket 167 is (211) perfect 200 bb rubber 3 ppi appear 11 "d (224) duck 219 r (384) ran 370 dd buddy 4 rr error II Id would 1 wr write 2 "f (243) father 132 CT prayer I ff effect 6 's (324) stop 272 gh enough 2 c cent 37 If half 2 ss mess 14 ph phone. 1 sc scene I g (94) good 92 2 (412) terrible 392 flff egg 2 tt sitting 18 "h (76) have 72 ed watched I wh whole 4 d second I 'I (31) lust 12 v (63) very 61 g general 15 f of 2 dg wedge 3 w (97) worth 83 di soldier 1 u quick II "k (257) keel) 86 o one 2 c cream 119 Ili squirrel I ck luck 29 x (9) exit 9 q quit 14 'y (IS) yard 10 lk walk 5 u cure 3 clt chord 4 i onion I 1 (328) leave 278 "z (59) zero 14 II will 49 s flimsy 41 tl whistle 1 a watches 2 nt (163) Mother 154 zz buzz 2 mm summer 2 mb limb 7 It (366) nothing 355 Regular 3526 kit know 6 Irregular 447 lin funny 4 nd handkerchief 1 3973

150 1 7

144 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION rathcr unreliable. It is particularly Some implications for those inter- defective when all short vowels areested in phonetic efficiency which considered. The word "dcfectivc,"would require no type changes would for example, would undcr the abovebe: rulc be spelled "defecctivve." The 1.Eliminate all double letters. functionalutility of double letters 2.Indicate a long vowel with an may be seriously questioned. Thcrce following it (time, tiem). are two aspects of the problem, (1) 3. Usc a single u for all schwa the sound of preceding vowels whensounds. double letters arc found, and (2) the 4.Use a single o for almost all of kind of consonant doubles foundthe ah, ar, au variations and single i, after short vowels. For reactive con-r, and a for their respective short iistcncy, both should be functional.sounds. 5. Summary Use ou for all the ouch words, eliminating the ow (cow, cou). When over 6000 sounds from the 6.Usc the usual t in the th com- vocabulary of five basal readers werebination for thin and a double -bar t classified, it was found that greaterin the th combination for this. variability of sound-symbol relation- 7.Use z for buzz (buz), and zh ships occurred for vowels as com-for azure (azhure). pared to consonants. Thcrc were 311 8.Usc oo for the short double-o (54 per cent) regular long vowels assound in good and ew for the long compared to 262 irregular. Theredouble -o sound in duly (dewty). were 567 regular short vowels as 9. Usc k for c, eliminating the compared to 107 irregular withoutc-s confusion. the short u. The short u had 103 Variations like these could be in- regular, and 188 irregular. Thcrctroduced, where appropriate, with- were 3526 regular consonants asout undue printing problems. comparedto 447irregular. The practical application lies in making (David R. Stone is a Professor in clear the emphasis needed in teach-the Department of Psychology, at ing the most frequent sound-symbolUtah Slate University. His speciali- combinations. ties are learning and reading.) The Effect of Syllabic Position andAccent on the Phonemic Behavior of Single Vowel Graphemes

Lou E.BURMEISTER*

WHEN TWO VOWELS APPEARside-by- The findings of former studies are side they usually form a single graph- somewhat unclear because of the ap- eme, which is a written representation parent differences in the authors' defi- of a single phoneme.The phoneme nitions of short vowel sounds (e.g., Is commonly will be a long vowel sound, the short vowel sound before an r cate- a diphthong, a short vowel sound, or a gorized as a short vowel sound, or is it schwa, depending upon the particular placed in another category?Is the vowel-pair being examined. An anal- schwa sound considered to be a short ysis of the phonemic behavior of each vowel sound ?). single grapheme vowel-pair is already available (3). The section titled "Re- lated Findings" in the present article. Questions asked however, may be viewed by some asa The present study is unique because supplement to this report. it examines the phonemic behavior of The present article is an attempt to individual single-vowel graphemes in describe the phonemic behavior of each an attempt to determine whether the single-vowel grapheme(i.e., of each commonly taught generalizations are vowel when it appears without another more valid for sonic vowels than for vowel at its side, and when it does not others.Information about accent, or appear in a "final vowel- consonant -c" stress, as a possible cue to phonemic construction). behavior is also available in this study. Several investigators have examined The questions asked were: the usefulness of two "generalizations" which presumably were formulated to I. What is the phonemic behavior describe the phonemic behavior of sin- of each single-vowel grapheme gle-vowels in two syllabic positions: when it is found in an open sylla- open and closed.The open syllable, ble ? long vowel generalization, was found 2. What is the phonemic behavior not to be generalizable to words of of each of these graphemes in an more than one syllable (2), and even accented open syllable ?in an to have a relatively low (75 percent) unaccented open syllable? utility level for monosyllabic words (1, 3. What is the phonemic behavior 4). The closed syllable, short vowel of each single-vowel grapheme generalization, seems to be more valid when it is found in a closed syl- for polysyllabic word's than for mono- lable? syllabic words, perhaps because excep- 4. What is the phonemic behavior tions tend to fall into families of one of each of these graphemes inan syllable words (e.g., mild, child, wild; accented closed syllable?In an cold, bold, hold ;night, fight, right). unaccented closed syllable? Reading and Realism,IRA Proceedings, Vol. 13. Part I, 1969, 645.649.

152 146 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

Procedure accordingtosyllabicposition,and Hanna, Hanna, Hodges, and Ru- within this, according to accent pattern accented syllable (primary or sec- dorf, in 1966, published a volume titled Both Phoneme-GraphemeCorrespondences ondary) or unaccented syllable. as Cues to SpellingImprovement.In frequency and percentage data are sup- it, they reported the results of their plied. computerized examination of17,310 When available, the following pho- words:15,284 words selected from nemes appear for each grapheme:long Part 1 of the Thorndike-LorgeTeach- sound, long sound before an r, short er's Word Book of 30,000 Words (8), sound, short sound before an r, and plus 2,026 words selected from the schwa. Also included is every other Merriam-Webster's New Collegiate phoneme which reached the 5 percent Dictionary,sixth edition, "which were level in at least one percentage column. considered to be relevant to the study Phonemes not listed did not reach that of a 'common core' vocabulary but had 5 percent level in any column. not been available from Part 1 of the Thorndike-Lorge word list"(6: 12- Conclusions 13). TheIllerriam-Webster's New To answer the questions posed, the CollegiateDictionary,sixthedition author decided to use a minimum of an (7), was the source for the analysis of 80 percent utility level.Therefore, in phoneme-grapheme correspondences this section the number of phonemes (See 6: 13-14). The authors of the for each grapheme are given, in order Hanna, et al, study were concerned to reach at least the 80 percent level. with furthering knowledge about spell- 1. open syllables ing by examining phoneme-grapheme a. The most common phonemes for relationships. single-vowel graphemes in open sylla- The purpose of the present study is bles are to further knowledge aboutreading, specificallyabout thephonicsitua- e: c - 90.4% a : - 53.4%, - 32.4% tion. To do so, it is necessary to look o: 6 - 92.0% i: a - 47.8%, 1 - 37.3% u: 0 - 82.6% y: l - 87.8% at grapheme-to-phonemerelationships. The procedure is the reverse of that If we wish to disregard accent as a used by Hanna, et al. cue, the following generalization seems Using as source materials the find- appropriate: In anopen syllable, e, o, ings of Hanna, et al, and reversing and u will have their own long sounds; schwaor alonga. sound ; them to "grapheme to phoneme" rela- a will have a schwaor ashorti sound. tionships, the author of the present i will have a The findings about y in an open syl- study was able to classify thesounds of lable may be misleading here. the allsingle-vowel graphemes for Burmeister(2)looked at the following 17,310 words examined. generalizationand foundittobe highly valid(99 percent) :"When Results consonant -I-y are the final letters in a monosyllabic word, the y has along The grapheme-phonemerelation- sound ; in a polysyllabic word the y has ships for each vowel were examined. (long c) sound." Au analy- In Table 1 the findings are presented ashort i

153 BURMEISTER 147

Table 1: Frequency and Percent of Occurrence of EachPhoneme for Each Single-Vowel Grapheme According to Syllabic Position and Accent Pattern

Syllabic Position

- onen syllable closed syllable E O let Total Accented Unaccented Total Accented Unaccented f % f % f f % f % I f %

a halo 860 32.4 84993.0 11 .6 142 2.8 139 4.1 3 .2 a vary 0 0 0 0 0 0 64 1.3 64 1.9 0 0 304 11.5 1 a baboon .1 303 17.4 388876.6 2485 73.4 1403 82.7 a arm 58 2.2 49 5.4 9 .5 460 9.1 383 11.3 77 4.5 53.4 a canal 1418 0 0 1419 81.5 19 .4 0 0 19 1.1 2654 913 1741 5078 3382 1696

: or 1740 90.4 345 97.7 1395 aa.e 25 .4 18 .6 7 .2 0 O 0 0 0 64 1.0 63 2.2 1 .03 44 e I bet 2.3 1 .3 43 2.7 3272 48.3 2419 85.2 953 21.7 Z after 6 .3 1 .3 5 .3 1560 24.5 0 0 166042.2 e anon' 115 6.0 O 0 115 7.3 648 9.6 1 .04 647 16.5 0 0 O 0 her 0 0 0 313 4.6 288 10.1 25 .6 le °t able 0 0 O 0 0 0 620 9.2 0 0 620 0 524 3S3 1571 6772 2840 3932

14.2 294 china 395 94.8 101 4.1 159 3.4 158 5.7 1 .1 in 1039 37.3 0 0 103941.9 4307 91.5 2417 85.9 1890 99.0 0 pencil 1332 47.9 0 1332 53.7 15 .3 0 0 15 .8 ski 23 .8 16 5.2 7 .3 IS .3 13 .5 2 a ---arr ireir 4709 2781 1928

SQ 1629 92.0 545 97.0 1084 89.7 247 5.8 208 9.5 39 1.9 0 0 0 O cord 0 0 0 312 7.4 282 12.0 50 2.4 0 0 dot 0 O 0 0 01557 36.71425 65.0 132 6.4 0 off 0 0 O 0 0 0 123 2.9 102 4.7 21 1.0 0 carton 114 6.4 O 0 114 9.4 3497 35.3 0 1497 73.0 0 0 humnr 0 O 0 0 0 265 6.3 0 0 258 13.1 0 0 6 nan 0 0 0 112 2.6j 110 5.0 2 .1 1770 562 1208 4243 2192 2051

770 82.6 u u onion 320 82.3 45082.9 44 2.3 29 2.3 15 2.3 0 0 u U barn 0 0 0 0 20310.6 188 14.7 15 2.3 u 11 n16. 2 .2 0 0 2 .4 121062.9 996 77.a 21433.3 a subnre 42 4.5 u 0 0 42 7.7 255 13.3 0 0 255 39.7 82 8.8 u ea trnth 69 17.7 13 2.4 11 .6 11 .9 0 0 u ZSput 36 3.9 0 0 36 6.6 164 8.6 51 4,0 113 17.e_ £u 932 389 543 1923 1230 643

209 11.1 123 100.0 4.3 y Ai 2 1.2 2 1.7 0 0 myth. 160 97.9 O 7 0 164794.0 154 93.3 117 96.7 37 94.1 baby idyl 20 1.1 O 0 3 7 a 20 1.1 1.9 3 6.4 mar 0 0 O 0 0 0 t 2.4 4 0.1 7Wlo 123 1753 165 121 44

154 148 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION sis of the data in the present study af- A single-vowel in a closed syllable firms that the vowel sound for a conso- has its own short sound, except as nant + y in an accented open syllable modified by r (in the case of a, c. and is long i, and in an unaccented open u), or a schwa sound (in the case of syllableis shorti.This suggests a c, o, and u). Y has a short i sound in partial confirmation of the above gen- a closed syllable (e.g., synonym). I). The most common phonemes for eralization, since all one-syllable words single-vowelgraphemesinaccented arc in the accented category. closed syllables are as follows: I). The most common phonemes fur single-vowelgraphemesinaccented a: a - 73.4%, a - 11.3%

short before r schwa and other a: a - 76.6% a -9.1% e: - 48.3% - 24.5% -9.6% i - 91.5% o: - 36.7% a - 35.3%,- 7.4%, r: - 6.3% u: - 62.9% 6 - 10.6% a - 13.3% y: i - 93.3% open syllables are as follows: Cc - 85.2% i:I- 86.9% C: a -97.7% a: A - 93.0% o:o - 65.0%, - 12.0%,6 - 9.5% o: - 97.0% i: i- 94.8% u:u-77.8%, is - 14.7% u: is - 82.3% y: i -100.0% y:i-96.7%

Generalization : In an accented open syllable, each vowel has its own long The most common phonemes for sound (y = single-vowel graphemes in unaccented This generalization has a high valid- closed syllables are as follows: ity level, but low frequency value. a:a - 82.7% The most common phonemes for e:e - 42.2%,- 21.7%, a - 16.5% single-vowel graphemes in unaccented isI- 98.0% o:a - 73.0%, - 13.1% open syllables arc as follows: u:a - 39.7%, - 33.3%, d'o- 17.6% y: - 84.1% e: - 88.8% a: a - 81.5% o: 6- 892% is a - 53.7%,- 41.9% Accent may serve as a cue in closed u: - 82:9% y: i - 94.0% syllables fcr c, o. and u. since schwa sounds are found only in unaccented In an unaccented open syllable,c, syllables.Accent is not a strong cue o, and u have their own long sounds; a for a ori. has a schwa sound ;i has a schwa or 3. schwa sound short i sound ;and y has a shorti It may be interesting to note that a sound. andi commonly represent a schwa 2. closed syllables sound in an open syllable, but not in a a. The most common phonemesfor closed syllable. An o often represents single-vowel graphemes in closed sylla- a schwa sound in a closed syllable, but bles are not in an open syllable. 155 BURMEISTER 149

Related find ingsconsonantizing fusion) and the ch sound 13 time:: of y, i, and u (question). Y served as a grapheme 2,095 times U also served as a consonant (w- in the Fianna, et al material.It served 47 times), or as part of a consonant as a vowel 2,041 times, and as a conso- grapheme (qu = k-27 times; qu= nant 53 times, or 2.53 percent of the kw-191 times; cqu = kw-5 times), time.When it was a consonant, it for a total of 270 times, or 8.6 percent was always the first letter in the sylla- of the time that u was found in the ble.Forty times it was the first letter Hanna listings.In each case, thett of the word (yacht, yes); five times it directly preceded a vowel (u = w: was the first letter of the second root suave, suite, jaguar ; qu = k : bouquet, of a compound (barnyard).It always mosquito; qu = kw :quack, quail, directly preceded a singleor double queen; cqu = kw :acquire, acquit). vowel (buoyant, canyon) and at times REFERENCES followed a vowel pair.V. of course, 1.Bailey, Mildred Hart."The Utility of also occurs as part of several vowel- Phonic Generalizations in Grades One pair graphemes (ay,ey, oy, uy).In thiough Six." Reading Teacher, 20 (Feb- these cases it follows a single vowel. ruary 1967), 413-418. 2.Burmeister, Lou E."An Evaluation of (See 3.) the Inductive and Deductive Group Ap- I occurred as a single-vowel 7,498 proachestoTeaching Selected Word times. Analysis GeneralizationstoDisabled Rutitalsooccurred1,141 Readers inEighth and Ninth Grade," times in a consonant function.Sixty- doctoral dissertation, University of Wis- six times it served asa single conso- consin, 1966. 3.Burmeister.Lou E "VowelPairs," nant grapheme, and 1,075 timesas a Reading Teacher, 21(February 1968), part of a consonant grapheme, accord- 445-452. ing to the Hanna, et al coding 4. Clymer, T. L."The Utility of Phonic system. Generalizations in the Primary Grades." When it was codedas a consonant, it ReadingTeacher,16(January1963), had a y sound and always precededa 252-258. vowel.Tt, therefore, might be consid- 5. Emans, R."The Usefulness of Phonic Generalizations above the Primary ered by some to be part of a vowel-pair Grades." Reading Teacher. 20 (February (alien, opinion, senior).The remain- 1967), 419-425. 6. Hanna, P. R., et al., Phoneme-Grapheme ing times, when i was codedas part of Correspondences as Cues to Spelling Im- a consonant grapheme, it also always provement.Washington, D. C.: Office of directly preceded a vowel.In all but Education, U. S. Department of Health, Education and Welfare, 1966. 16 of these cases it followeda c, t. or s, 7. Alerriam-Webstes New Collegiate Dic- and usually (995 times)it helped to tionary (6th ed.), 1961. make the sh sound (facial, vicious, lus- 8. Thorndike, E. L., andI.Lorge. The Teacher's Word Book of 30,000 Words. cious, pension, mansion, diction,fic- New York :BureauofPublications, tion, lotion).It also helped to make Teachers College, Columbia University, the :h sound 51 times (erosion, vision, 1944.

156 Which Accent Generalizations Are Worth Teaching?

CAROL WINKLEY*

AS A TECHNIQUE of word identifi- pils who had not been taught the cation, several reading special- accentprinciples,pupilsinthe ists and authors of basal reader man-"accent-generalization group" were uals have advocated teaching chil-found to have greater "power" in dren to apply certain accent gener-(a) ability to attack unknown words, alizations to locate the accented syl-(b)vocabulary development, and lableinunfamiliarmulti-syllabic (c)comprehension.It was con- words. Recommendations have beencluded, therefore, that the reading made although there has been no proficiencyof intermediategrade evidence providing justification forpupils with average ability was im- teaching the application of accentproved by a word recognition pro- generalizationsasaword-attackgram that included the teaching of method. Two studies conducted byaccent generalizations. Clymer (1) and Groff (5) raised questions regarding the advisabilityThe Problem of teaching principles of accentua- From the findings presented, indi- tion at either the primary or thecating that accent generalizations intermediate grade levels. There haswere helpful in the identification of been no agreement among readingunknown words, it was inferred that experts as to whether accent gener-such generalizations should be taught alizations should be taught. topupilsof averageability and A recent study (7) has providedabove at the intermediate grade lev- thefirst experimental evidence ofels. The question then arose as to the value of teaching accent general- whichgeneralizationsshouldbe izations as a word recognition tech-taught. nique. Two groups of pupils of av- Of the reading specialists, Gray erage ability and above at the inter-has presented the most comprehen- mediate grade levels were compared. sive plan for teaching "clues to ac- Pupils in one group received instruc-cent." These are fully explained in tion in applying accent generaliza-his 1960 edition of On Their Own tions to unfamiliar words (the "ac-in Reading (4) and were incorpo- cent-generalizationgroup").Chil-rated in his basic reader series for dren in another group in a neigh-Scott, Foresman. Although linguists boring community learned only thehave discussed a few of the "stress dictionaryskill ofpronouncingpatterns" ofisolated words, their words in which the accented sylla-chief interest has been in the stress bles were marked (the "marked-patterns of connected speech and the accent group"). Compared with pu-effects of shifts of accent on intended "Th'e Reading Teacher, 20 (December 1966), 219.224, 253.

157 W1NKLEY 151 meaning. Becauseisolatedwordsof a written Accent Test, constructed have been the main concern of read-by the experimenter. This test re- ing specialists,these were investi-quired the pupils to perform three gated in this study. Eighteen accenttasks in relation to each unknown generalizations proposed by Grayword: (1) to underline the accented were tested. The generalizations ap-syllable, (2) to select the right yowl pear in the accompanying table. sound for the accented syllable, and The relative merits of each gen-(3) to choose the correct meaning eralizationwcrcassessedintwofor the word. ways. First, a word count was used The unknown words for the test to determine the perccntagc of mul-wcrc secured from two word lists tisyllabic words complying with eachDaleandEichholz, Children's generalization and the percentage ofKnowledgeofWords(2),and words which wcrc exceptions to eachDiederich and Palmer, "Difficulty in principle. Second, an attempt wasGrades 11 and 13 of 4,800 Words made to determine the relative use-from 6,000 Through 20,000 in Fre- fulness of each generalization to chil-quency"(3). A totalof10,896 dren faced with the actual task ofmultisyllabic words which could be identifying unfamiliar words of moreconsidered unknown to an average than one syllable. fourth grader were classified accord- The applicability of accent gener-ing to the acccnt generalization that alizationstomulti-syllabic words,was applicable. through the use of word counts, has The Accent Test was constructed been reported elsewhere (7). using a random selection of words Columns one and two of the tablefrom the lists obtained for each gen- show which acccnt generalizationseralization.The numberoftest had the highest percentage of wordswords exemplifying each accent gen- complying with the clue and thoseeralization reflected the percentage having the highest percentage ofof words of that type in the total exceptions. sampleateachparticulargrade The findings of this aspect of thelevel. The "foils" originally used by study seem to lend support for theDale. and Eichholz in their testing teaching of the acccnt generaliza-wcrc obtained and used as the mul- tion clues numbered 3, 4, 8, 13, 15,tiple- choice items of the test. The 16, and 18. Since Clues 1 and 10administration of the test in pilot had a relatively high percentage ofstudies provided an additional check exceptions, they wcrc not consideredon the familiarity of the words and to be of value. made it possible to improve the con- As the second step in determiningtent of the test and refine the pro- the advisability of teaching the prin-cedures of administration. ciples of accentuation, the relative Face validity, supplemented by merits of the generalizations wcrcevidence obtained in individual in- assessed through the administrationterviews with selected subjects, and

158 152 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

ACCENT GENERALIZATIONS AND THEIR USEFULNESS

V E O m._ ta 'g 2 .:: '8 C." clio Es E = 8.- 8 '111'4=. 3 Clues to tdatt.= ., u 8 Cl. Ct.,,,x a.va 8 8 .. ,c, Accent Generalizations ...lo .o b.f., (.7 - .e c.-. ,,r4...... t t .t...:= .x.. c t z.F, I, ° =8 C.1 = Is k C.(

I. Compound Word Clue In compound words, a coin- x x mon pattern of accent is a primary accent on or within the first word and a secondary accent on or within the second word. (foot" hold') 2. Noun -Verb Clue In two-syllabic words which may be used either as a noun or a verb, the accent is usually on the first syllable when the word is used as a noun and on the second syllable when the word is used as a verb. (con' test vs. con test') 3. "Ion" ClueIn words ending with ion, the primary accent falls on the next to the last syllable. x x (nt i' tion) 1. VariedSuffixClue The primaryaccentusually occurs on the syllable before the suffixes ity, ic. ital. imp, ial, or kiln and on the second syllable x x before the suffix ate. (vi tal'i ty, ty ran' ni cal) 5. Dropped Final "c" ClueA single consonant letter following a single vowel letter before an ending or suffix (beginning with a vowel) may be a clue that a final "e" was dropped and the last syllabic of the root

word is accented. (pro cur' ing) ___ 6. Unaccented Syllabic Before Ending ClueA single consonant letter following a single vowel letter before an ending or suffix (beginning with a vowel) may be a clue to an unaccented final syllabic in the root word. (coy' ct cd) ___ 7. Two Consonants Before Ending Clue Two like con- sonants before an ending or suffix are a clue to an x x accented final syllabic in the root word. (con trol' let) S. TwinC.onsonant Chic Two like consonant letters following thefirstvowelletter are a clue to an s N accented first syllable. (ag' gra vate) 9. "ck" ClueThe letters "ck" following a single vowel letter are a clue to an accented first syllable. ((lick' er) x x M. Final "e" Clue Two vowel letters, one of which is final "e" in the last syllable of a word, may be a clue x s N to an accented final syllable. (con Wye) II. Two Vowels Together ClueTwo vowel letters to- i Filter in the last syllabic of a word may be a clue to x x an accented final syllable. On peach')

12,Final "y" Clue In a twosyllable word that ends in a consonant followed by'y," thefirst syllabicis x x usually accented. (pal' sy)

159 7

1

WINKLEY 153

ACCENT GENERALIZATIONS AND THEIR USEFULNESS(Cola ailed)

. to e e .9 Ze - 9 0 ., 3.7e ...N -6 I PLE 00 6,,v 1" Chios to II i : F., ti.0.)0 "O .0 gO a a. tiC I e2 At- Accent Generalizations 136,-. ig12 2(201Z 6 i ..-- 726 YE 0 8 & z.a k 0.g

13. Common Beginning Clue- If de-, rc-, be -, ex-. in-, or a-, is the first syllable in a word, it is usually x x x unaccented. (re sort', a dicu') If. "tare" le" Clue- If the final syllabic in a word is -lure, or -le preceded by a consonant, that final x x syllable is usually unaccented. (nue titre, am' ble) 15. Koot-Word Clue- In inflected or derived forms of words. the primary accent usually falls on or within x x x the root word. (list' less, nu mixed') 16. Long Word Chic - In words of three or more sylla Ws. one of the list two syllables is usually accented. x x (cat' a pull)

17. TwoAccent Clue - In longer words where there isa secondary as well as a primary accent, often the sec- ondary accent falls on the first or second syllabic, which is then followed by one unstressed syllable before the primary accent. On' per sedel IS. woSyllable Word Clue - In a word of two syllables the first syllable is usually acc: ted unless it is a x x prefix. (mer' chant) reliability coefficients of .92 and .95which had their accented syl ablcs (using the Spearman-Brown "split- marked (the "marked - accent half" technique) established the Ac-group"). Each subject had an IQ of cent Test as a dependable instru- 100 or more,had attended his pres- ment of measurement. ent school system since the begin- The subjects of the study werening of fourth grade, and had been 409 pupils at the fourth, sixth, and taught by teachers in the intermedi- eighth grade levels. Approximately ate grades who had had at least two half of these pupils (207), using theyears of experience in that school Scott, Foresman basal readers, hadsystem. The reading consultant and been taught accent generalizationsthe principals in each system en- as a part of their word recognition couraged theuse of the basal reader program at the intermediate grademanuals and believed that concepts levels(the"accent-generalizationconcerning accent were taught as group"). The remaining 202 stu-suggested. dents, using the Lyons and Carna- Following the administration of han basal readers, had received in-the tests, all data were coded and structioninpronouncingwordspunched on IBM cards. An item 154 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION analysis of test responses related towhom they had been taught could each generalization was made. Pro-be determined. portions were then computed com- Eight of the generalizations had paring the total responses that wereproportions significantly higher than correct with the total possible re-the remaining generalizations. These sponses for all of the test items exem-generalizations are indicated in the plifying each generalization. A sta-third column of the table. The prin- tistical analysis of the differences be-ciples that appeared to have the tween the resulting proportions re-highest utility, listed in the order of vealed any differences that were sig-their apparent usefulness, were: (9) nificant. The relative merits of each "ck" Clue, (11) Two Vowels To- accent clue were thus assessed. gether Clue, (10) Final "e" Clue, Statistically significant superiority (12) Final "y" Clue, (13) Com- in proportions was shown for themon Beginning Clue, (14) "ture," "accent-generalization group" over"le" Clue, (7) Two Consonants Be- the "marked-accent group" in manyfore Ending Clue, and (15) Root- generalizations at the end of fourthWord Clue. (Clue 18 was not tested and sixth grades. In the fourth gradesince the children in the "accent- at the beginning of the year and ingeneralization group" had not been the eighth grade at the end of the taught to apply it.) year differences were not consistently in favor of either the "accent-gener- Discussion alization group" or the "marked- Comparing the whole list of gen- accent group," and the differenceseralizations with those identified as did not reach the 5 per cent level of"useful" through word counts, one significance in any instance. finds Clues 13 and 15 identified as Since the differences found at the useful by both techniques. Although end of the fourth and sixth gradesthe generalization clues numbered 3, were frequently statistically signifi-4, 8, and 16 were not as helpful to cant and, for each generalization,the subjects of the "accent-general- proportions were higher in the "ac-ization group" as were the eight cent-generalization group" than in clues enumerated above, their appli- the "marked-accent group," the datacability to a relatively high percent- for the two grade levels were com-age of multisyllabic words warrants bined and new proportions com-consideration in the final selection of puted for the "accent-generalizationaccent generalizations worth teach- group." The purpose of combining ing. the data was to secure a single pro- Because about one-third of the portion foreachgeneralizationwords to which Clues 1 and 10 were which would make itpassibleto applicable were found to be excep- compare one generalization with an- tions,theadvisability of teaching other. Thus, the accent clues whichthese clues was questioned. Omitting were most helpful to the pupils tothese clues, the passible "utility" of

161 WINKLEY 155 twelve of the generalizations wassyllable before the double consonants dcmonstratcd. is usually accented. (Clues 7 and 8) Ex.: beginner, letter. Conclusions and 6. The primary accent usually Recommendations occurs on the syllable before the, Twelve generalizations (Clues 3,suffixes -ion, -ity, -ic, -ical, -ian, -ial, 4, 7, 8, 9, 11, 12, 13, 14, 15, 16, andor-ious, and on the second syllabic 18) appeared to be worth teachingbefore the suffix -ale. (Clues 3 and to children. With these findings in4) Ex.: affectation, differentiate. mind, thc recommended list of ac- 7.In words of three or more syl- cent generalizations to be taught waslables, one of the first two syllables shortened and simplified by conibin-is usually accented. (Clue 16) Ex.: ing and rewording the twelve gen-accident, determine. eralizations. Further research should be con- The following seven generaliza-ducted to assess the usefulness of the tions are proposed as those worthgeneralizations, as restated above, in teaching to children because of theirunlocking unknown words; to deter- applicability to multisyllabic wordsmine the advisability of teaching ac- or because oftheir demonstratedcent generalizations to pupils of be- usefulness to children in the identi-low average ability; and to recheck ficationof unknown multisyllabicthc merits of teaching the general- wcrds. Examples of words to whichizations eliminated in this study. In the clues are applicable are provided.thc meantime, the list of seven clues 1. When there is no other cluepresented above can serve as a useful in a two syllabic word, the accent isguide to thc accent generalizations usually on the first syllable. (Elim-worth teaching. inates necd for teaching Clues 8, 9, (Dr.Winkley is now Assistant 12, 14, and 18) Ex.:basic, pro- Professor of Education in the read- gram. ing clinic a! Northern Illinois Uni- 2.In inflected or derived formsversity. She formerly served as a of words, the primary accent usually falls on or within the root word.reading consultantinthe Aurora (Clue 15) Ex.: boxes, untie. public schools.) 3.If de-, re-, be-, ex-, in-, or a- References is the first syllable in a word, itis I.Clymer.Theodore. "TheUtilityof Phonic Generalizations in the Primary usually unaccented. (Clue 13) Ex.: C odes."TheReadingteacher. 16 delay, explore. (Jail.1963) , 252-258. 2.Dale,Edgar. and Eichhok Gerhard. 4. Two vowel letters together in Children's KnowledgeofWools. Co- the last syllabic of a word may be a lumbus. Ohio: Bureau of Educational Research and Service, 19G0, clue to an accented final syllable. 3. Diederich, PaulB.. and Palmer, Os- (Clue 11) Ex.: complain, conceal. mond E. "Difficulty in Grades 11 and 13 of 4,800 Words from 6,000 Through 5. When there arc two like con- 20,000 in Frequency." Princeton, N. J.: sonant letters within a word the

162 156 THE CURRICULUM IN WORD RECOGNITION

Educational Testing Service, 1956. 4. Cray. William S. On Their Own in COM M ENT Reading, pp.121-225. Chicago:Scott. This study provides teachers, pub- Forest:tan, 1960. lishers and authors of reading texts 5. Groff. Patrick. "To Teach or Not to Teach Accents?"Elementary School with valuable information hitherto Journal, 62 (Jan. 1962), 218-221. unobtainable.Itprovidesspecific 6. Wink ley, Carol K. "The Applicability ofAccent Generalizations,"Academic guidance for an instructional pro- Therapy Quarterly, 2 (Fall 1966), 2.10.gram in higher level word analysis 7. Winklcy. Carol K. "Utilization of Ac- cent Generalizations in Identifying Un- skills. Studies of this type are impor- known Multisyllabic Words." Unpub tant and welcome additions to our lished doctoral dissertation, University of Chicago, 1965. k nowledge.s.w. PART IV: Instruction in Special Aspects of Word Recognition The twelve articles in Part IV deal with particular aspects of the word recognition program. Chambers discusses the extension of the sight vo- cabulary; Emans tells how to use contextual clues; Schell deals with the teaching of structural analysis. What research has revealed about the more effective ways of attacking new words is reported by King in terms of sensory cues, Muchl in terms of visual discrimination, and Glass with di- rections for perceptual conditioning.Railsback discusses procedures for substituting consonants; Hanson shows how children work with variant word endings; the more effective strategies for teaching the sound-letter relationships are presented by Botel. The innovative phonetic and/or alphabetic approaches for teaching beginning reading are presented by Dietrich, Heilman, and Fry. It is evident that speakers and writers in the past seven years have been more or less preoccupied with the sounding of words in the teaching of word-attack skills. Few whose articles have been published by the Inter- national Reading Association have concerned themselves with visual and structural analysis and the constantly important use of contextual clues. Analysis that deals with meaningful prefixes, suffixes and stems of words, and with the clues that context affords would seem to give more emphasis to comprehension in reading, and it is to be hoped that such elements of word attack will receive more attention in the future. Certainly teachers who do stress phonics should constantly give parallel emphasis to the meaning of the words that have been so analyzed and to the contextual clues given in the passages in which such words appear. Extending the Sight Vocabulary

J. RICHARD CHAMBERS*

SIGHTvocabularybelongs under the have words to express them. Words seem broad category of word recognition to be necessary tools with which to think." regardless of the grade level span one It appears that to read with compre- considers. Disregarding grade level fur- hension, the ultimate goal of all reading ther, word recognition(and therefore, instruction, one must be able to recognize sight vocabulary) also implies word mean- words immediately and to transfer these ing since the necessity of understanding symbolsintotheir proper meaning as the meaning behind words read is without determined by the particular setting in question. As everyone knows, there is no which the words occur. Children should meaning on the printed page since only he given the responsibility of demanding the symbols of meaning are there, stimu- meaningfrom material read even though lating the reader to make his own mean- historically itis known that children are ings. Therefore, word recognition becomes often not good judges of words needing the ability of the reader to recognize the clarification. The scale becomes one of word asa form he readily knows (a reading with comprehension on one ex- "sight" word), to correctly pronounce the treme to verbalism on the other. word, and to know the particular meaning At the intermediate grade level,the of that word in its present context. problems associated with vocabulary are Importance of a Sight Vocabulary multiplieda hundred-fold as compared Lacking an adequate sight vocabulary, with those inthe primary grades. The the child ;s without word recognition skills number of words met by the child in the and necessarily then, without word mean- middle grades is almost without limit, yet ings. When meaning is not present neither his power tolearn words is obviously are comprehension and/orrecall. The limited to a small fraction of this total. effect of the absence of an adequate sight The sheer number of new words is further vocabulary is also reflected in incorrect, complicated by the specialized nature of inadequatephrasingorword-by-word the vocabulary associated with the content reading resulting, then, in poor oral read- subjects of social studies and science and, ing. Though one might approach the indeed, with any of the many subjects development of word recognition skills taught within this three-grade span. through word analysis, without immediate Limitations of Sight Vocabulary recognition of words, speed of reading is Stockpiling words in his sight vocab- too slow and results in an interruption of ulary permits the child to develop his flow of thoughtsand, consequently,a reading power and increases his reading reduction in memory of the content read. horizons. Yet, as he reads more widely, Traxler's study' of the relationship be- he must inevitably meet new words which tween vocabulary and general achievement are not a part of his ready supply of points out that a child's word meaning words. Here his "go-power"islimited score on the Stanford Achievement Test and the usefulness of his sight vocabulary is a reasonably good predictor of his total diminishes. Unless he has been taught score on the complete test. It has also been some independent method of analyzing suggested that,Some psychologists think words his only alternatives are to ask his th-at we never have any ideas until we teacher what the words say (assuming she 'Arthur Traxler, "The Relationship Between Vo- is available), to guess at the words, or to cabulary and Ce,eral Achievement in the Elemen tart'School,"Elementary School Journal, XLV Mabel Vinson Cage. Reading in High Gear. New (Februar 1945), pp. 331.33. York: Harper and Brothers. 1938, p. 30. Reading and Inquiry, IRA Proceedings, Vol.10, 1965, 29-30. 165 CHAMBERS 159

forget about them and to try to go on increases in rapid word recognition with without figuring out what the new words meaning, speed of oral reading, reduction might be. in the number of errors in oral reading, Simply relying on his ability to remem- and overall reading achievement. ber the configuration of a word limits his When teaching vocabulary it is impor- knowledge to words already presented. If tant to add the dimension of depth to the he has learned to examine the context existing one of breadth. Depth inthis surrounding the word he has increased his context means two things: a level of learn- "octane" but still has restricted his poten- ing going beyond simple recognition, and tial power. These perception and associa- a knowledge of the multiple meanings tion abilities must be implemented by the which most words have. addition of analytical abilities. Structural Word analysis ability, a necessary ex- and phonetic analyses become particularly tension of skill in word recognition for valuable and necessary at the intermediate the development of independence in ex- grade level when the range and infre- tending sight vocabulary, offers many op- quency of new words met in the many portunitiesformiddle-gradeteachers. different types ofreading materials Exercisescombiningstructuralanalysis increases. (especially prefixes, suffixes, root words) Techniques of Teaching and word meaning are probably the most As one considers ways in which upper valuable kind of word analysis practice for elementary level children may be helped developing the reading vocabulary of in- to extend their sight vocabulary it becomes termediate-grade children. The relation- necessary to consider the element of quan- ship of these sub-skills to reading vocab- tity or the practical question of the number ular is higher than the relationship exist- of words children may be expected to ing among any of the other commonly successfully learn per day. The number of taught word analysis skills and vocabulary. words met by children at thislevelis Since studies show a positive relation- apparently high. Yet all of these words ship between phonic ability and reading will not be learned by all of the children. achievement it is necessary to suggest, too, This suggests that establishment of the that phonetic analysis be added to the learning rate of students at this level is elements that need emphasis when devel- just as necessary as itisat the reading oping independent vocabulary power. The readiness and primary levels. Teaching in inductive approach to word analysis as relation to the established learning rate is reflected in the Word Analysis Practice an acknowledged method of providing for Cards by Durrell and others' will help individual differences since it recognizes children develop a "phonic sense" without the existence of varying progressrates the necessity of relying on a multiple and is a sound criterion to consider when rules - exception program. attempting to set up reading groups. Summary Childrenintheupperelementary Procedures in word recognition must grades must be encouraged and, if neces- continually emphasize meaning and ex- sary,re-taught to make use of context tend 'over into the area of word analysis clues as an aid in word recognition. Evi- with attention to the use of context clues dence indicates that students at this level and structural and phonetic analysis. The do not appear to use these clues as fre- vocabulary demands placed upon children quently as they should even though they at these grade levels, because of the spe- have probably been given this instruction pecialized nature of the content in the in the primary grades. various subject areas and because of the Evidence suggeststhattachistoscopic lack of a rigidly controlled vocabulary, drill not only produces gains in quick make it imperative that they become self- word perception ability but also results in sufficient in their vocabulary needs.

. 166 Different Sensory Cues as Aids in Beginning Reading

ETHEL M. KING and SIEGMAR MUE111.*

SEVERAL METHODS ofbeginningthe classroom and relevance to learn- reading instruction make use ofing theory. What sensory cues,or the so-called "sight-word" approachcombination of cues, make for the as abasis for subsequent phonicmosteffectivelearningsituation teaching. The success of the sight-when children are being taught sight word approach depends on the abilitywords? When a sight word (visual of children to learn rapidly to associ-cue)is presented, the appropriate ate a meaningful sound sequencesound sequence can be indicated by with a specific graphic configuration.the teacher's saying the word (audi- Many basal reading series have em-tory cue), by presenting a picture phasized the value of using pictures(visual cue), or by doing both (vis- to facilitate this sight-word learning.ual and auditory cues). In addition, Russell (6) suggested that picturethe child may be asked to say the cards "... may be used many timesword aloud (self- produced auditory until recognition of the word sym-and kinesthetic cues). An increase in bols is possible...." Smith (7)the number of cues accompanying rationalized this approach by sayingpresentation of the printed word can that pictures "...offer the childbe hypothesized to act in two quite valuable assistance in making thedifferent ways on the learning proc- transition from recognizing an objectess. The increase in cues may (1) and naming it to recognizing a sym-interfere with learning by distracting bol which stands for the objcct...."attention from the printed word or In contrast, Dechant (2) questioned(2) facilitate learning, particularly if the emphasis on pictures in preprimcrthe sight words are similar in appear- and primer materials: "Reading per-ance or sound. The second outcome haps too often becomes an exercise inwould be in accord with the acquired picture reading rather than in iden-distinctiveness of cues hypothesis of- tification and understanding of thefered by the psychologist Miller (5). word." A sharp break in the tradi-The hypothesis states that if a sub- tion of using pictures to accompanyject learns to make distinctive re- beginning reading material is illus-sponses to similar stimuli, these re- trated by the complete absence ofsponses will become part of the re- pictures in Bloomfield and Barnhart'sspective stimulus patterns, making Let's Read series (I). the original stimuli more distinctive. Out of these conflicting viewpointsThe use of pictures with similar sight a question can be framed, and thewords could elicit distinctive verbal answer has practical implications forresponsesormneumonicdevices The Reading Teacher, 19 (Dumber 1965), 163.168.

. 167 KING 161

which could aid in discriminationorfollowed on the next four pages; mediate between the word and thethese four presentations comprised appropriate sound sequence. Kines-one test trial. The training and test- thetic stimulation produced by theing pages were repeated in different child's saying words would presuma-orders until twelve testtrials had bly confer additional distinctivenessbeen made. to the words, particularly if the visual To provide for the five methods of cues of the words were similar whenpresentation, two different bookscon- the auditory cues were dissimilar. taining the same set of wordswere The purpose of the present studyneeded for each word list.In one was to compare systematically thebook the training pages contained the relative effectiveness of different sen-words with an appropriate picture sory cues and combinations of cuesabove each word. In the other book for kindergarten children as theythe training pages contained only the learnedtoassociateprinted andwords. spoken words varying in similarity. The training cue was centeredon Procedure each page. All words were typed in lower case on a primary-style type- Different methods were used inwriter. When the method of training training five groups of subjects toinvolved a picture, a simple black read a common list of four similarline drawing illustrating the word words. The same methods of trainingwas presented above the word. were repeated using five other groups Words were selected according to learning to read a common list of fourthe following criteria: (1) suitability dissimilar words. for illustration (nouns), (2)evi- A kindergarten-sized table and twodence of their presence in the speak- chairs were provided in a classrooming vocabulary of most kindergarten apart from the regular classroom. Achildren, and (3) similarity in the wooden stand with an inclined sur-number of letters in each word soas face was placed on the table. Spe-to keep the length constant. For pur- cially constructed books rested on theposes of this study one list of words sloping surface of the stand. Thesewas chosen because they were ap- books were of a loose-leaf type, 51/2proximately the same in configura- by 81/2 inches. The pages were oftion and contained some of the same heavy white paper covered with ace-sounds. These "similar" words were: tate to protect them during frequentdoll, ball, bowl, and bell. By way of handling. contrast, another list of words was Each book had two alternatingselected because of their differences parts: training and testing. In thein configuration and in sounds. These training part of the book each of four"dissimilar" words were: gate, drum, stimulus words was presented on anest, and fork. separate page. A test on each of the Five different methods of training words presented in a different orderwere used. In all methods the printed

168 162 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

word appeared as a visual cue. Themarker above the word to focus at- methods differed with respect to thetention on it and said, "Look care- additional cues appearing with thefully at the word. The word is ." printed word. These included: pic-Subjects trained by the auditory and ture, auditory, picture and auditory,echoic response method were pro- auditory and echoic response, andvided with the same materials as the picture and auditory and echoic re7auditory group but with the follow- sponse cues. inf.modificationininstructions. The picture groups were shown"Look carefully at the word. The the word and corresponding picture.word is .What is the word?" After about two seconds the experi-Again, an interval of about two sec- menter held a white cardboard mark-onds was allowed in which the sub- er over the picture to focus attentionject was expected to respond by say- on the word. The subject was in-ing the word. Table 1 shows the ten structed to "Look carefully at thetraining groups used in the study. word." Subjects trained by the pic- TABLE I ture and auditory method used the TRAINING GROUPS same book, but in addition to seeing the picture and the word they were Training: Kinds of Words also told the word. After the picture Printed Word +Dissimilar Similar had been covered by the marker, in- Picture Croup 1 Group 2 structions were given to the subject Auditory Group 3 Group 4 to "Look carefully at the word. The Picture + word is ." A further dimen- Auditory Group 5 Group 6 sion was added to the method for the Auditory -1- Echoic Group 7 Group 8 picture and auditory and echoic re- Response sponse groups. After the picture had Picture + been covered by the marker each Auditory subject was asked to, "Look carefully +Echoic Group 9Group 10 at the word. The word is Response What is the word?" An interval ofSubjects about two seconds was allowed in which the subject was expected to The subjects were 210 children respond by saying the word. from eight community kindergartens When the method of presentationin Calgary, Canada.* Twenty-one did not involve a picture, as in thesubjects were assigned in rotating auditory groups and auditory andorder to each of the ten groups. Two echoic response groups, a differentsubjects were not included in the book was used. The second loose-leaf The authors wish to thank R. Warren. book contained words only. The Superintendent of Schools in Calgary, and the following kindergarten teachers: Mrs. auditory group was presented with D. M. Arbuthnot. Mrs. R. Dean, Mrs. W. one word on a page. After about two Frise, Mrs. E. J. Miller, and Mrs. A. Ruther- ford. whose cooperation made this research seconds the experimenter placed the possible. 169 KING 163

data because they could read the testthe trials by kinds-of-word interac- words before training. Ages rangedtion (p<.001). Examination of the from 61 to 77 months. The meandata showed the trials by kinds-of- age was 70.9 months. There were nowords interactionresulted from a significantagedifferences amongtamer learning rate for the dissimilar groups. The experiment was con-word groups. There was a significant ducted between May 25 and Juneinteraction between training-methods 10. The training and testing periodand kinds-of-words (p<.05). Table for each subject lasted about fifteen2 shows the nature of this interaction. minutes. It will be noted that the order of Results difficulty is completely reversed when similar and dissimilar words are com- In analyzing the data by learningpared. The picture method of train- trials, the 12 trials were divided intoing provided the most efficient learn- 6 blocks of 2 trials each. Table 2ing with similar words, and the audi- summarizes the mean number of cor-tory method of training provided the rect responses and the standard de-most efficient learning with dissimilar viations for all groups, averaged overwords. trial blocks. A two-factor analysis was con- A three-factor analysis of varianceducted for both similar and dissimilar was used for analyzing the data (3).word groups. In each analysis train- The between-factors included train-ing methods was the between-factor ing methods and kinds of words; theand trial blocks the within-factor. within-factor included learning trials.The two-factor analysis of variance The analysis of the data revealed aon the number of correct responses significant difference inthe meanfor dissimilar word groups indicated number of correct responses betweensignificance (p<.001) for trials only. the kinds of words (p<.00I) withAn identical analysis on the number similar words being more difficult.of correct responses for similar word The trials effect was significant in thegroups revealed a significant differ- expected direction (p<.001), as wasence in the mean number of correct

TABLE 2 MEAN NUMBER OE CORRECT RESPONSES AND NIANDARII IWSIVIIONS FOR ALL Gkores

Training NIcthod: Similar Word GroupsDissimilar Word Groups Printed Word + Mean (rank) SI) Mean (rank) SD

Picture 2.10 (I) 1.03 3.15 (5) 1.00

Picture + Auditory 1.97 (2) 1.09 3.18 (4) 1.24

Picture + Auditory + Echoic Response 1.90 (3) .68 3.52 (3) 1.63

Auditory + Echoic Response 1.86 (4) .69 3.68 (2) 1.27

Auditory 1.27 (5) 59 3.89 (I) 1.67 Acraged over trial blocks.

170 164 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION responses among the training meth-ing the printed word made little dif- ods (p<.05). The trials effect wasference in learning performance, al- also significant (p<.001). When allthough there was a tendency for pairs of group means for the similarhearing the word alone to be the word groups were compared, I-testsmost effective method. indicated that performance in the Practically speaking, the findings auditory method was significantlydo not support the view that pictures poorer when compared to each ofshould be dispensed with in teaching the other methods. None of the othersight words or in beginning reading differences was reliable. material. For the beginning reader Prior to the experiment the Lorge- itislikely that many words look Thorndike Test of Intelligence wassomewhat similar. Therefore, provid- administered to small groups of theing additional cues may aid the visual subjects. Subjects within the similarand auditory discrimination, which and dissimilar word groups were di-is necessary if a consistent spelling-to- vided into high and low I.Q. groups. sound association is to be made. The analysis for methods showed no The present results agree with the reliabledifferences associated withfindings of McNeil and Keislar (4). I.Q. Similar results were obtainedThese authors investigated the value when subjects were grouped by sex. of an oral response in beginning reading. The program of words in Discussion their study was presented orally and The study showed that the mostvisually. Pictures were used only oc- appropriatemethodforteachingcasionally for motivation. Kindergar- sightwords in beginning readingten subjects were divided into "oral" varied with the similarity of the wordsand "non-oral" groups. The oral introduced. When the words weregroup was instructedtosaythe similar, faster learning resulted when words; the "non-oral" group to look the printed word was accompanied at the words. A follow-up silent read- by a picture or by having the childing test showed better performance say the word, as compared to hearingby the "oral" group. Table 2 from the word alone. This finding, when the present study shows that the same interpreted according to the acquiredcombination of auditory and echoic distinctiveness of cues hypothesis, in- cues producedsignificantlybetter dicates that the cues did elicit dis-learning than an auditory cue alone tinctiveverbalorkinestheticre- with similar words. In the McNeil sponses which served to make the and Keislar study some of the items similar words more distinctive, there- required the subjects to identify the fore facilitating discrimination among test word from among words similar them. When the words introduced in letter configuration. were dissimilar, however, the results The finding that words similar in indicated that the kind of sensory cue shape and sound were more difficult or combinations of cues accompany-to learn to read than dissimilar words

. 171 KING 165 is not surprising. However, it would Educational Psychology,33 (1963) , 162- 168. be interesting to determine whether 5.Miller, N. E. "Theory and Experiment the visual or auditory dimension of Relating Psychoanalytic Displacement to StimulusResponse Generalization,Jour- similaritycontributes most to the nal of Abnormal arid Social Psychology, learning difficulty. Are ball and bell 43 (1948). 153-178. 6. Russell, D. H. Children Learn to Read. difficult to discriminate because they p. 161. Boston: Ginn and Co.. 1961. look alike, sound alike, or both? 7. Smith, Nila B. Reading Instruction for Today's Children, p. 168. 1963.

(Ethel M. King is at the Univer-COMMENT sity of Alberta at Calgary and Sieg- mar Muehl is at the University of This investigation provides empiri- Iowa.) cal insight into a question some lin- guists have raised about the value of References pictures in beginning reading mate- 1. Bloomfield, L., and Barnhart, C. L Let'srials. Although the results cannot be Read, Parts 1-3. Bronxville, N.Y.: C. L. Barnhart, 1963. viewed as unequivocal, they certainly 2. Dechant, E. V.Improving the Teachingshould generate further study in this of Reading,p. 205. Englewood Cliffs. N. J.: Prentice-Hall, 1964. area. Moreover, the careful attention 3. Lindquist, E. F.Design and Analysis ofthe authors have given to writing ExperimentsinPsychology and Educa- tion.Boston: Houghton - Mifflin, 1953. their research report, particularly the 4. McNeil, J. D., and Kcislar, E. R. Valuesection on procedures, should permit of the Oral Response in Beginning Read- ing: An Experimental Study Using Pro-others interested in the problem to grammed Instruction,British Journal of replicate the present study.

172 Recent Research in Visual Discrimination: Significance for Beginning Reading

SIEGMAR NiUE1111. and ETHEL M. KING

MANY SYSTEMS of beginning reading This paper is concerned with the kinds instruction use the so-called "sight- of training that are most effectivein word" approach to introduce children to helping childrenlearntodiscriminate their first reading material. The children among graphic patterns. Our purposes are presented with a basic set of words are twofold: to present an overview of which they learnto read.Later these the research bearing on this question of words provide the basis for formal in- discrimination learning and to suggest struction in word analysis skills including implicationsforteaching which seem structural and phonic analyses. warranted on the basis of this research. Learning sight words is an association We say, ".suggest implications" advisedly, process. As such, certain general factors since the majority of the research cited are known to affect the case with which willbe experimentalinnature.This the association is learned. First, the re- means that it was carried out in highly sponses to be learned must be availablecontrolled andartificialcircumstances. and readily discriminated from one an- For this approach we make no apology. other. Secondly, the stimuli to be asso- This step in educational research is im- ciated with the responses must be dis- perative in order to assess, unambiguous- criminated so that recognitioniscon- ly, the operation of certain factors in the sistent for each appearance of the same learning process. However, this type of stimulus. research can only be a beginning. Experi- In teaching reading, we have been well mental research needs to be followed up aware of the importance of making read- withclassroomstudiestodetermine ing responses readily available. This has whether or not the manipulations and been done by selecting beginning reading variables which produce significant effects words that are of high frequency in chil- in experimental situations will also pro- dren's speech. Unless children have cul- duce differences in the classroom that are turally disadvantaged or culturally differ- practicallyas well as statistically signifi- ent backgrounds these words are avail- cant. able; at most, we need to provide some auditory discrimination training to sharp- Historical Development en discriminations among similar sound- Before we look at the recent research ing words. in discrimination learning, it would be The stimuli or printed words in begin- useful to review briefly the history relat- ning reading present an entirely different ing to the question of how children see problem. In the beginning these graphic words, particularly why whole words have patterns are not readily discriminable or been considered the child's:natural" unit consistentlyrecognized.Assuming the of perception(14).Several sources have beginning reader has the spoken responses contributed to this thinking. American available, he is faced with two learning educators writing in the 1850's opined tasks with respect to these graphic pat- that words are more easily remembered terns. First, he must learn to discriminate than letters because they are not such among the visual symbols; second, heminute objects and because they are more must learn to associate each of the graphic meaningful. At the turn of the century, patternswithanappropriatespoken Huey (6) cited evidence from tachisto- word. scopic research using adult subjects indi- Vistas in Reading, IRA Proceedings. Vol. 11, Part 1, 1967, 434-429. r73 MUEHL 167

cating that word "form"wasthe critical Goins left off (5). Goins' study was the cue for perception. In 1922, Buswell's first that made a systematic attempt to ( I ) studies of eye movements showed trainfirst grade children in visual dis- that the mature reader seemed to organize crimination. The fact that the results of his reading perceptions by words and hertachistoscopictrainingwithnon- even phrases. Although the data from verbal type visual stimuli did not facili- tachistoscopic and eye movement research tate reading performance suggested the could be used to support the "whole- need for visual discrimination training word view,"these same studiesalso with stimuli that are immediately relevant showed that as the reading material be- to learning to read; namely, printed words came more difficult, or when young and and letters themselves. Our first study inexperienced readers were used as sub- (1 I )confirmed the specificity of transfer jects, discrimination of words proceeded in the area of reading (Table1).Three mostly on aletter-by-letter basis. The groups were given visual discrimination historical clincher for the "whole word" training or matching practice with three approach, however, appears to be psycho- types of material; words that appeared on logical theory, not experimental evidence. a subsequent reading task, words that did In 1924, Max Wertheimer, the Gestalt not appear on the subsequent reading psychologist (3), stated that the whole is task, and geometric forms. The results on greater than the sum of its parts, and the reading test favored the first group that the form or grouping that is most that had practiced discriminating among naturalis the one which involves the the same words that appeared on the smallest interval. Since words are letter reading task. Since there was no differ- groupings set apart spatially from other ence between the geometric and different words, this theory seemed to provide the word group, the results indicated that answer to word perception. what is learned in discrimination is very Recent Research specific and that this learning transfers only when the words in the reading task What does more recent research say are highly similar to those in discrimina- about how children learn to discriminate tiontraining. These resultsraisedthe visually among words? Our research with question: What specifically are the chil- prereaderstakesitsbeginning where dren attending to as they learn to dis- TABLE 1 TYPE OF TASK AND ORDER

First Second

Visual Discrimination Training Reading Words (Matching)

Groups Materials All groups

Same boat, help, play, come boat help Different make, jump, work, find play come Contra (1-e\'i

1 el"'II .1Li ver

168 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

criminate among words? Is it the "form" words and the other groups.Inthis or shape of the word as a whole, or is it group, the children saw a picture of the individualletters within the word? A word when the word was first presented study designed to assessthesefactors and,in addition,the word was pro- (12)showed two interestingfindings nounced. The results showed two groups (Table2).First, word shape or form did clearly superiorintheir word reading not seem to be an important cue to kin- performance:thedifferent meaningful

TABLE2 TYPEOF TASK AND ORDER

* First Second

Visual Discrimination Training Reading Words (Matching)

Groups Materials All groups

...... Same form :feu' geu, reu feu - "blue"

Different form FA, gjd, rid geu- "red" *t.f

Letters only f, g, r reu-"white"

dergarten childrenin discriminating word group and the relev.ant letter match- among words. Specific letterdifferences ing groups. In visual discrimination train- between the words seemed to be the sig- ing, the letter matching task was the nificant cue. Second, although discrim- easiest of all; the different meaningful inatinglettersinisolationwas much word most difficult. What skillstrans- easier than discriminating letters embed- ferred from these two different types of ded in words, the isolated letter group training? The different meaningful word did just as well on the reading test as did group presumably learned that itis ap- the groups that had discriminated the propriate, to respond to printed stimuli letters in the words. A similar study by with meaningful responses, which, after Staats, Staats, and Schutz(15),however, all, is reading. By contrast the letter group failed to confirm these findings. They apparently learned to attend to the visual found that visual discrimination training features that provided a basis for dis- using the whole words was more effective criminating among the words. A subse- than training with the letters in isolation. quent study by the Cornell group (9) ProfessorKingsdoctoraldissertation indicated that training with the letter at (7) attempted to resolve these conflicting the beginning of a word isrelatively findings. Her study design is shown in more effective than with letters that occur Table 3. It is important to note the differ- in other positions. Another factor we ence between the visualdiscrimination attempted to assess was the relative effec- training group with different meaningful tiveness of presenting the materials in rlr MUEHL 169 the matching tasks either simultaneously with a word in visual discrimination train- or successively (7). Table 4 shows the ing led to the question: What is the difference inthese two approaches. In most effective combination of cues for the top example, the matching stimulus helping children learn sight words? The is presented at the same time as the re- study designed to answer this question TABLE3 TYPE OF TASK AND ORDER

First Second

Visual Discrimination Training Reading Words (Matching)

Groups Materials All groups

4.4 Different word nest,c2,r ng,kit' hand (meaningful)

Different word nest, cake, ring, duck coat

Same letters h,a,n,d,c,o,a,t, g,i,r,l,s,h,o,e. girl

Same word hand, coat, girl, shoe shoe

4 Control A il A7A A 1 I vor 1 gvi sponse choices; in the bottom example, (8) is shown in Table 5. Children were the matching stimulus is presented first asked to learn to read two lists of words. and then removed before the response TABLE5 choices are presented. We found no re- TRAINING GROUPS liable differences between these two meth- Training Kinds of Words ods of matchingasmeasured in perform- Dissimilar Similar ance on the reading task. (gate, drum,(doll, ball, TABLE4 Printed word -I- nest, fork) bowl, bell) TYPES OF VISUAL DISCRIMINATION TASKS Picture Group 1 Group 2 Simultaneous (same page) Auditory Group 3 Group 4 I hand hand coat girl shoe I Successive (different page) Picture + Auditory Group 5 Group 6

I hand Auditory. + hand coat girl shoe I Echoic Response Group 7 Group 8 Positive transfer to reading resulting Picture + Auditory + Group 9 Group 10 from associating a meaningful picture Echoic Response 76 170 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

In one list, the words were highly simi- provided the least effect method. Since it lar in sound and appearance; in the other is likely that most printed words appear list, highly dissimilar in sound and ap- more similar than different to the begin- pearance. The same table also shows the ning reader, it would seem safe to con- different combination of cues that were clude that the use of pictures and other used in teaching the words to different accompanying cues will be helpfulin groups of children. In each group the mastering sight vocabulary. printed word was always presented. In One final area of our research remains addition, the words were accompanied by for summary. The question can be asked: one or more additional cues. For exam- If training in discriminating among let- ple, in groups three and four, the printed tersfacilitates word discrimination and word was accompanied by the teacher's reading, what effect does knowledge of saying it and the child repeating it.In the letter-names have on this process? groups nine and ten, the word was ac- The results of a study just completed are companied by cues provided by a picture, shown in Table 7 (13). The columns the teacher's saying the word, and the TABLE 7 child's saying it. After each of the four DISTRIBUTIONOFHIGII AND Low READERS words and accompanying cues were pre- ONTHE HARRISONSTRoUD sented to the various groups, a test trial LETTER-NAMINGCONTEST was given using the word alone, with Giving Names of Leters the child trying to recall the correct re- sponse, The results at the end of the Lowest 50% Highest 50% learning session are shown in Table 6. High readers 0 24

TABLE6 Low readers 13 12 MEANNUMBEROFCORRECTRESPONSES represent above and below the median Training Word GroupsWord Groups groupsbasedonthe Harrison-Stroud Method: (gate, drum, (doll, ball, letter-naming subtest administered in Sep- nest, fork) bowl, bell) tember of first grade. The rows represent Printed Word +Mean Rank Mean Rank extreme reading groups (matched in IQ) - Picture 18.62 5 12.62 1 based on end-of-first grade year Metro politart reading test performance. Letter- Picture 4- Auditory 20.94 naming ability appears to be markedly 4 11.86 2 associated with later reading success. No Picture -F- child below the 50th percentile in letter-, Auditory 4- 21.14 3 11.43 3 Echoic Response naming ability was in the high reading group. Twice as many children above the Auditory + 50th percentile appeared in the high as Echoic Res onse 22.10 2 11.19 4 comparedtothe low reading group. Auditory 23.38 1 7.67 5 These results support Durrell's (2) ear- lier findings. We are not certain whether They indicatethat when words were thisrelationship indicates that training highly similar, additional cues helped the in the knowledge of lettel.nantes would child discriminate and recall the printed facilitate word discrimination and read- word and sound association. Thus, groups ing,or whether knowledge ofletter- 1-4 were reliably better in performance names is an indication of a basic ability than the group that just heard the teacher to form associations between abstract vis- say the word. For the dissimilar word ual stimuli and auditory sequences. Dur- groups, just the opposite order resulted. rell's methods study involving teaching Hearing the word alone provided the best letter-names and sounds supportsthe learning condition while the picture cue training notion. MUEHL 171

Implications for Teaching 5.Since the transfer of visual discrim- ination training seems to be very specific, Keeping in mind that we are now mov- ing from controlled experimental situa- the teacher would do well to give chil- tions to the classroom, the results of this dren practice in discriminating among the new words to be learned at the beginning experimentation appeartohave some practical implications for the classroom of a reading lesson. At this point, a suc- teacher. cessive presentation could be used in the discrimination training. This type of prc- 1.Viival discrimination training from sentation demands greater attention to the the very beginning should be with word words in order to recall their letter char- and letter stimuli. Having the child match animal pictures, geometric forms, or any acteristics since the word is not in view kind of non-verbal graphic stimuli does when the choices are presented. not appear to transfer to word discrim- 6. When presenting new vocabulary ination. Our data confirms a point of words, particularly words that are easily view expressed by McKee (10) almost 20 confused with other words because of years ago. sound and letter similarities, providing additional cues in the form of pictures, 2. The simultaneous matching format when possible, and having the children which exists in most prereading books pronounce the words will probably facili- and charts at the present time seems ade- tate learning. quate. To date we have found .that simul- If the above research has answered a taneous matching is easier than successive few questions, it has raised many more. matching and that the transfer of dis- Much investigation needs to be done. We crimination learning appears to be about hope to continue our efforts. as effective. 3.Since matching lettersis an easy REFERENCES task for kindergarten children, the earliest I.Buswell, Guy T. Fundamental Reading visual discrimination exercises should use Habits: A Study of Their Development (Supplementar Educational Monographs, letter stimuli. Althih we do not as yet No. 21.) Chicago: University of Chicago, know how knowledge of letter-names 1922. works in the discrimination and reading 2.Durrell, Donald D. "First.Grade Reading process, the evidence strongly indicates Success Study: A Summary," Journal of Education, Boston University School of that teaching the letter-names is probably Education, 140:3 (February, 1958), 2.43. highly useful. Letter discrimination and 3.Ellis, William Davis. A Source Book of naming can be taught simultaneously. Gestalt Psyhology. Ncw York: Humani 4.Just prior to beginning reading in- ties Press, Inc., 1955. 4.Fries, Charles C. Linguistics and Reading. struction,visual discrimination exercises Ncw York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, should include training in making the 1962. "three-way association" of the sound and 5.Goins, JeanTurner.Visual Perceptual the meaning with the visualform.If AbilitiesandEarlyReadingProgress (Supplementary Educational Monographs, words are accompanied by a representa- No. 87). Chicago: University of Chicago tivepicture (when possible)and the Press, 1958. pronunciation of the word, then a child 6. Huey, Edmund B. The Ps)cbology and gradually learns to attach a meaningful Pedagogy of Reading. New York: Mac. label millan Company, 1908. verbal to the printed word each 7.King,EthelM. "EffectsofDifferent time it is encountered in a matching exer- Kinds of Visual Discrimination Training cise. Such training provides not only for on Learning to Read Words," Journal of skillinvisualdiscriminationbutalso Educational hybology, 55:6 (December, 1964), 325.333. skill in responding to graphic symbols in 8. King,Ethel M. andMuch!, Sicgmar. a manner similartothatrequiredin "Different Sensory Cues as Aids in Be. learning to read. ginning Reading," The Reading Teacher, 8 172 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

19:3 (December, 1965), I6i.168. Journal of Educational Psychology, 52:4 9.Marchbanks. Gabrielle and Levin, Harry. (August, 1961), 215-221. "CuesbyWhichChildren Recognize 13. Muehl. Siegmar and Kremenak. Shirley. Words," Journal of Educational hycholo- "The Ability to Match Information With- gy, 56:2 (April. 1965), 57-61. inand BetweenAuditory andVisual 10. M:Kee, Paul. The Teaching of Reading Sense Modalities and Subsequent Reading in the Elementary School. Boston: Hough- Achievement," JournalofEducational ton Mifflin, 1948. Anthology, (in press). 1 I.Much!. Siegmar. "The Effects of Visual 14. Smith, Henry P. and Dechant, Emerald Discrimination Pretraining on Learning to V.Psychology inTeaching Reading. Read a Vocabulary List in Kioderjarten Englewood Cliffs, New Jersey: Prentice- Children,"journal of Educational hy- Hall, 1961. chology. 51:4 (August, 1960), 217.221. 15.Staats,CarolynK.,Staats, Arthur W., 12.Nfuehl. Siegmar. "The Effects of Visual and Schutz, Richard E. "The Effects of DiscriminationPretraining With Word Discrimination Pretraining on Textual Be- and Letter Stimuli on Learning to Read a havior," Journal of Educational Psycholo- Word ListinKindergartenChildren." gy, 53:1 (February, 1962), 32.37.

'179 Consonant Substitution in Word Attack

CHARLES E. RAILSRACK THE TEACHING of consonant substitution is a prominent part of several of the basal reading programs currently being used through- out the United States. The procedure is taught, however, for differ- ent purposes in various reading programs. Some reading programs teach the use of consonant substitution in the skills portion of their program as a way of reinforcing knowledge of letter-sound associations. As children make new words based upon changing the letters that represent one consonant sound, the letter-sound associ- ations are strengthened through practice. For most students such practice is an essential part of learning to quickly associate letters with the sounds that they represent. Consonant substitution exer- cises can, in addition to reinforcing the letter-sound associations, be valuable as a way for students to incidentally learn many new words. For example, as students make the word "dike" from "like," they not only reinforce the letter-sound associations for "i" and "d," but they may incidentally add words such as "dike" to their recognition vocabularies. In addition to consonant substitution being taught for the pur- poses of reinforcing letter-sound associations and developing recog- nition vocabulary, some of the most widely used basal reading series teach yet another use for the procedure, consonant substitu- tion as a method of word attack. The procedure, when taught for this purpose, is often referred to as a part of "phonetic-analysis." The teacher's guide of a primer that emphasizes this method of word attack states: "Consonant substitution is a simple method of phonetic analysis that will enable children to attack new words while reading" (Robinson, et al., 1962). The way in which such phonetic analysis is to be used by the pupil may be read in the teachers' manuals, an excerpt of a typical lesson being: Conclude by having children identify new words formed bycon- sonant substitution. Place on the upper part of the holder the words let, find, ride, and good. Place on the bottom ledge, or hold inyour hand, the letter cards for s, w, m, h. Read aloud the sentence The fence around the ponies was made of . Quickly place w over the g in the word good and let children complete the sen- tence with the word wood .... (Robinson, et al., 1962) QUESTIONABLE POINTS The question must be asked, "Is this an efficient method of The Reading Teacher, 23 (February 1970), 132.135. 174 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

word attack that is actually used in independent reading?" To help determine an answer to this question, the author and a classroom teacher presented to an above average group of second gradersa list of nonsense words and a list of words the students had not yet met in their basal readers. After the students had attacked each word, they were questioned by the teacher as to how they identified each of the unknown words. Words from their readers thatwere presented included: "truck," "cap," "dry," "best," "snow," and "chop." Nonsense words which were presented included: "nog," "ped," "door," "plock," "tell," "cloy," "stot," and "pilt.' For a few of the words one student reported using consonant substitution in the identification process. In all other instances, however, no students reported attacking the words by usinga known base word from which a consonant sound was dropped and another added. Instead, students reported using their knowledge ofletter- sound associations to attack the words directly. For instance,in attacking the word, -truck," they reported using their knowledge of the sounds that "tr" and "ck" stand for to identify the word. Ata later time the teacher repeated this experiment with her slowest group of children. Although this group was by no means seriously handicapped in reading, no studentwas capable of attacking the words by use of consonant substitution,a procedure which had been taught them since the primer level of instruction. The question about the practical value of the consonant sub- stitution procedure as a method of word attackmay also be answered by considering the way in which the adult attacksstrange words. As the reader of this article meets suchnonsense words as "clib," "ning," "zick," or "pelp," it may be predicted thatvery few, if any, would naturally attack such words by first thinking of "bib," "sing," "quick," or "help," dropping the first sound, and then adding the sounds represented by the first letters of thenonsense words. The use of consonant substitution as a method of word attack seems extremely inefficient. The reader must look at the word and decide whether he knows a word from which he changes the begin- ning or the ending sound. He then must mentally "remove" the sound and substitute the one from the strange word. Forno other reason than the excessive amount of time it takes to think of a word that is similar and then to make the substitution, theprocess cannot be recommended as a word attack procedure. A far more serious weakness in this process, however, is the matter of determining the "known" word that is similar to the "unknown" word. To "know" the word from which the derivation is to be made means that the reader must havea mental image,

ISM. RAlLSBACK 175

i.e., the spelling, of the word. For instance, with the word "truck" how else will the student be able to utilize the procedure unless he can mentally picture "buck," "luck," or "duck?" Only after he has a mental image of the similar word can he remove a beginning letter and substitute "tr." It is tempting to say that the child does not have to mentally spell the similar word, that he can hear that "ruck" rhymes with "duck." It must be remembered that such a rhyming procedure will not work for the reader who has not yet identified the sound of "truck;" hence he does not know with what words it rhymes. The students who most need efficient methods of word attack are the ones least capable of creating the necessary mental images to use consonant substitution to unlock strange words that they meet in print. Any person who has worked with below average stu- dents knows that while they usually have trouble remembering sight words, they seldom, if ever, can create the mental images in their minds with which to successfully use the substitution pro- cedure. Observation of first grade students in a related area, spelling, also verifies the point that students usually will not use the substi- tution procedure. Students seldom will independently learn the spelling of a word by substituting from a word that they can spell. For instance, seldom will a first grade student who knows how to spell a word such as "duck" drop the "d" and add "tr" to independ- ently determine the spelling of "truck." If such a procedure is not used in spelling, there is little reason to think it would be used in reading. In skills lessons, basal readers attempt to skirt the problem of creating the image of the known word by putting the word from which the substitution is to be made before the child. In independ- ent reading, however, no such word sits on the chalk tray! The publisher previously alluded to recognized the problem under discussion by mentioning it in the manual of the first reader: "Whenever you help children use consonant substitution to attack unknown words, they must call up mental images of known words that arc similar in form to the unfamiliar printed words. The fol- lowing exercise will reinforce this ability" (Robinson, et al., 1962). An exercise typical of many in the program follows the quotation. A known word is placed before the children, and it is pronounced. This word then becomes the basis for substitution to form a new word. For example, the word "right" may be placed before the children. After it is identified, an unknown word, such as "night" is displayed. The student is to attack "night" by dropping the "r" 182 176 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

from "right" and substituting the "n" of "night." Such exercises are of questionable value for two reasons: 1J In independent reading no "known" word from which a substi- tution can be made is conveniently sitting on the chalk tray. 21 The exercise is inefficient in that it will generalize to only a very few words. To practice visualizing "right" so that "night" may be derived from it will help the reader identify but a few wordsthose that are spelled just like "right" except for the first sound"sight," "blight," "fight," "light," "might," "night," and "tight." In many instances not even this many words could be derived from one word for which the mental image could be recalled. Textbook authors apparently have great faith that such round- about word attack procedures will actually work for it is suggested that the unknown words "keeping" and "snowing" be identified from the known words "sleep" and "show" or "slow" (Robinson, et al., 1963). With these words the child is expected to go through four mental steps:11 dropping the suffix, 2J thinking of a word that is similar except for one sound, 31 dropping the sound, and 41 substituting the sound represented by the letter(s) of theun- known word that are different from the known word. Both personal observation of students and common sense would indicate that such procedures will seldom, if ever, be the actual way in whicha student identifies an unknown word. For slower students, particu- larly, it is completely unrealistic to think that such word attack procedures will work in independent reading.

SUMMARY The utilization of consonant substitution exercises within the reading program can serve to strengthen letter-sound associations as well as to provide a way in which some new words may be added to students' recognition vocabularies. However, to teach chil- dren to use consonant substitution as a primary word attack skill is to teach them a skill that is not only inefficient in terms of the time it takes, but also one that is of very limited usage. Few childrencan independently create the necessary mental image of a word that is spelled like the unknown word except for one consonant sound.

REFERENCES Robinson, HelenM., et al.Fun with our friendsteacher's manual. Chicago: Scott. Foresman, 1962. Pp. 87, 134-135. Robinson, Helen M..et al.More fun with our friendsteacher's manual. Chi- cago: Scott, Foresman, 1962. Pp. 62.63. Robinson, Helen M.,et al. Friends old andnewteacher's manual. Chicago: Scott, Foresman, 1963. P. 85.

/7. 1 1/4.10 First Grade Children Work with Variant Word Endings

IRENE WHANSON

OF THE continuing concernsInge measure. to the learner's ability to 0" perceiveidenticalelementsinseparate of primary grade teachers and activities. Since this is true. one very useful authors of beginning reading mate- approach to the teaching of the slow learner rials isto make him consciousofidentical is when and how instructionelements.... should be given in theuse of variant The syllable that recurs most frequently is final "ing," which is found in 881 of word endings. That thisconcern is the 9.000 words. With perfect transfer, a an important one is shown by a studychild who has learned the "ing" syllable in, say "going." will know that syllable in by Edward Dolch (2), in which he 880 more words. reported that the three most com- (non syllables in 8,409 samples of These studiesestablishwithout 14,00() running words in elementarydoubt the frequency of occurrence school textbooks in arithmetic, his-and the importance of teaching the tor, and geography were ing, ed,use of variant word endings. The and er. big occurred 240 times withfollowing investigation conducted by 135 different words. Ed occurredthe author attempted to answer the 165 times with 90 different words.question of when such instruction Er occurred 223 times with 72 dif-could most profitably be given. ferent words. Est occurred 53 times Twenty-six first grade classes from with 17 differentwords.SincetheSt.Paul,Minnesota, public 14,000runningwordsrepresentschool system were used in the study. about fifty pages of textbook mate-Thirteen of the classes were in the rial, this investigation pointedup theexperimental group and thirteen in great frequency of occurrence ofthe control group. The classes in the variant word endings and also theirgroups were equated for socioeco- breadth of applicability to words innomic: level. Pretest data revealed no the Englisl' language. significant differences between the Worth J. Osborn (3) investigatedexperimentalandcontrolgroups the frequency of occurrence of ini-with regard to reading ability, men- tial, medial, vnd final syllables in talability,chronologicalage, or words in the Rinsland word list. Heknowledge of the useof variant reported that the syllable ing oc-word endings. The total number of curred 881 times, ed occurred 338children included in the analysis of times, er occurred 323 times, and estthe study was 554. occurred 65 times. Osburn also made Over a period of four weeks the the following interesting comment: experimental classes received eight- We are told that the amount of transfereen periods of special instruction in that takes place in learning is due, in ageneralizationsconcerning variant The Reading Teacher, 19 (April 1966). 505-507. 511.

184 178 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION word endings and the application of Personal data and the differences them incontextual material. Theamong class means of the experi- endings taught were: s added tomental and control groups on the nouns and verbs, ed, d, and ingtests were compared by means of the added to verbs with no change in theF ratio for analysis of variance. Then root word and also with dropping orall the children in the experimental doubling of a final consonant in theand control groups were divided into root Awrd, er and est to form thethreementalabilitygroups,five comparative and superlative forms,reading ability groups, and three and er as a noun agent. Each day'ssocioeconomiclevelgroups.The lesson required about twcnty min-means of these groups were then also utes and was given in addition to allanalyzed by using the F ratio for regular reading and phonics instruc-analysis of variance. tion. The lessons were conducted with each class as a whole and con-Findings sisted of a preliminary presentation I.There was a significant differ- of a variant ending generalization byence between pretest and posttest the teacher, largely through the in-means of class means on the Variant ductive method, and a period ofEnding Test in favor of the experi- pupil practice, both oral and written,mental group over the control group. including the use of a worksheet.The difference was significant at the Teachers' manuals and daily work-.01level for the total number of sheets were supplied to the experi-children and for the boys, but was mental classes. Vocabulary for in-in the region of doubt (between the struction was chosen from the set of.01 and the .05 level) for the girls. readers already in use in the classes, 2.There was no significant dif- and the format of the worksheetsferencebetween the experimental was similar to that found in primaryand control groups in reading scores reading workbooks. Thus attentionbefore or after instniction in using could be concentrated on the variantvariantword endings whenthe endings rather than on new vocabu-Bond-Balow-Hoyt NewDevelop- lary or new methods. The controlmental Reading Test was used as the classes used the same periods of timemeasuring instrument. No response for independent reading. items on this test required a utiliza- The study was conducted duringtion of knowledge of variant word March and April of the first gradeendings; hence, it can only be said school year. The Bond-Balow-Hoytthat general reading ability as meas- New Developmental Reading Testured by this test was neither im- ( / ) and a specially constructed Var-proved nor harmed by the special iant Ending Test were administeredattention to the endings of words. to all classes during the pretest and 3. There were no significant dif- postteli periods. IQs and ages wereferences among the gains on tne obtained from school records. Variant Ending Test of children in 185 HANSON 179

high, average, or low mental abilitya considerable knowledge of such groups. The three groups profitedword endings independently. about equally from the instruction. 2. Since the meanscores on the 4. There was a significant differ-Variant Ending Test were signifi- ence among the gains on the Variantcandy different for1'eachreading Ending Test of children in the fiveability group and increased regularly reading ability groups. The threefrom the low group to the highone, lowest reading groups, which hadthe investigator suggestsa planned measured averagereadinggradesequence of teaching variant word equivalents of 1.5, 1.8, and 2.0onendings, starting with noun plurals the pretest, profited somewhatmoreat the preprimer reading level, verbs than the two highest readinggroups,adding s, ed, or ing withno change which had average reading gradein the root word at primer level, equivalents of 2.4 and 2.9on theverbs with changes in the root word pretest.Thesignificantlyhigherand noun agents at first reader level, scores of the two highest readingand comparative and superlative ad- groups on the Variant Ending pre-jectives at the second reader level. test showed,however, thattheseThis sequencediffers from most groups had already acquired muchpresently used ones in beginningear- knowledge of variant word endings.lier with verb endings and complet- 5. The differences amongmeaning instruction at the second reader scores of children assigned to thelevel rather than at the third reader three socioeconomic levelswere in level. the region of doubt (significance be- 3. The findings of this study in- tween the .01 and the .05 levels) ondicate that the present practice of the Variant Endings pretest. Thereseverely restricting the use of variant werenosignificantdifferencesword endings in first grade reading among their means of posttest scoresmaterials is unnecessary since most or mean gains. These data seem tofirst grade children arc already fa- suggestthat socioeconomic statusmiliar with their use in speech and may have had some effect on pretestarc learning to use them indepen- scores but that this effect was lesseneddently in reading, even though they by the instruction in variant wordhave not been specifically taught to endings. do so. Authors of first grade reading Educational Implications textbooks should be able to makeuse of the s, ed. and ing verb endings 1. The findingsofthisstudythroughout primer and first reader clearly indicate that the teaching oflevels, provided a planned sequence generalizations concerning the use ofof instruction in the generalizations variant word endings is possible andconcerning the use of variant word effective in the second half of firstendings is maintained in the teach- grade and that many of the childreners' manuals and pupil workbooks. by that time have already acquiredHopefully, this increased use ofcom-

186 180 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION mon verb endings in first grade read- References ing materials should permit authors I.Bond, Guy L., Ba lo, Bruce. and CyrilJ. New Dept topiarian! !leading to produce beginning reading mate- Tots, Form 1-1 and. L-11, Lower Primary rials which can conform more easily Reading. Chicago: Lyons and C.:tot:than, 1963. and naturally to English speech. 2. Do Ich, Edward W. "Sight Syllables Ver- (Mrs. Hanson is Assistant Profes- sus Letter Phonics," Elementary School Journal, 41 (Sept. 19-10) . 3842. sor of Education at Augsburg Col- 3.Ilanson, Irene W. "An Investigation of lege, Minneapolis, Minnesota. This the Effects of Teaching Variant Wool Endings to First Grade Children." Cit material was taken from her disser- published doctoral dissertation, Univei- tation for the Ph.D. degree, which sity of Minnesota, 1965. 1. Osburn, Worth J. "Teaching Spelling she received from the University of lw 're:Kiting Syllables and Root Words." Minnesota in June 1965.) Elementary School Journal, 55(Sept. 1954), 32-41.

. 187 Use of Context Clues

ROBERT EM ANS° Mary had a little lamb, using context clues well (4:321). Its fleece was white as Obviously, statements praising the FEWfour - year -ofd children would be worthofcontextcluesareeasily unable to complete the sentence with found.Much has been written about snow. Likewise,"Becky andherhow the use of contexthelpsthe mother wanted to go shopping.Theyreader develop the meaning of words. climbed into the to go toAs important as it may be this topic is the store."Few children, even beforenot the concern of this paper.Rather, entering school, would have difficulty the purpose is to attack the more elu- with supplying any one ofseveral sive problem and explore what aid the words which would make sense :car, use of context clues gives the reader in automobile,bus, streetcar.Children respect to word recognition. use context in their oral language, eas- There are at least four uses of con- ily and naturally.Children, and adultstext clues in word recognition.These too, use context clues to aid them in can be summarized as follows : their reading. 1. Context clues can help children It is easy to find testimonials to theremember words they have identified importance of context clues in reading.earlier, but forgotten.Most teachers Open almost any textbook on the sub-can cite examples of a child's having ject and there are such statements as difficulty with a partially known word It would be difficult to overestimateand then recognizing the word in a the value of the context in children'snew setting after being told that the word perception in reading (7:16).word is a familiar one and thatit Contextual clues provide one of the makes sense.For example, if a child most important aids to word identi-reads "Bill was a cow" for "Bill saw a fication and interpretation (6: 84).cow," asking, him if what he reads Context clues are perhaps the mostmakes sense will often enable him to important single aid to word percep- correct himself. tion (11:25). 2. Context clues may be combined The person who has not developed with other word-analysis clues (phonic skill in the use of verbal context has and structural analysis) to check on not become a mature reader(22: the accuracy of words tentatively iden- 23). tified by the use of other clues. Bond It is important to know, however,and Wagner state that context clues that many children who are thought serve as "Checks on the accuracy of all to be in difficulty in reading becausethe other techniques that are used" (5: of limited skill in analytical tech- 172). niques or because they have insuffi- 3. Context clues help in the .rapid cient knowledge of phonetic, struc- recognition of words for all readers by tural, or visual elements are usually helping one anticipate whata word indifficulty because they are not might be.The ability to draw an ac- Reading and Realign', IRA Proceedings, Vol.13, Part I, 1969, 76.82.

1 88 182 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OP WORD RECOGNITION

curate inference to what a word is can guage, and the meaning of the words, serve as a timesaver.It is a faster phrases,sentences,andparagraphs technique than other word recognitionsurrounding theword to anticipate aids such as phonics.It enables thewhat the word might be.Bond and reader to use only those phonic andWagner state, "Instead of having to other analytical techniques which arerecognize the word from the total of necessary to distinguish one word from words in the English language, the use another.For example, instead of hav- of context clues limits the choice of ing to sound out a word, the efficient words to the few that would fitthe render uses only enough phonic clues meaning of the passage being read" to recognize the word quickly when (5:172). By also using ti:e other word combined with the meaning clues. recognitionskillsthechildhasat 4. Context clues are required for thehis disposal, he tentatively identifies correct identification of some words. the word and checks to see if the word Gray states, "The pronunciation of makes sense.For example, if a child many words (permit. for example) de-reads the sentence, "Jenny picked up pends upon their meaning in a given her to draw," he can, from context" (11:148). Other words which his experience, limit the words to such require the use of context clues arepossibilities as pen, crayon, pencil, or lead in a pencil or lead the way, wind chalk.He would not have to select a ball of string or the wind blew, tear a the word from the 800,000 or so possi- piece of paper or a tear flowed downble words inthe English language. her cheek, and piggy bank or a bankBy combining this information with to fish from. various phonic clues, the child could recognize the exact word more quickly How do context clues work in word than by using only phonics. recognition? With respect to the point that con- Following the importance of context text clues may be used in conjunction clues and some of the uses of contextwithotherwordrecognitiontech- clues in word recognition, one should niques. Bond and Tinkerstate. ask how context clues work? By an-"Meaning clues alone are not enough swering this question. one can gain a for good reading at any level.They better idea as to how to teach the usemust be accompanied by the use of a of context clues. flexible set of word recognition skills. To understand how context cluesIt is the interaction of all the word- work it must be recalled that the child study skills that forms the foundation brings to reading a background of ex-on which a competent reader builds his perience ant, oral language.Likewise, reading structure" (4:322). DeBoer the child must bring to the reading sit-and Dallmann state. "Context clues uation a habit of demanding meaningare most effective when they are em- from reading. A child must com-ployed along with other methods of bine his experience, his oral language.word attack" (8:111). In actual read- and the meaning he gets from his read- ing, the use of context clues is prob- ing if he is to use successfully context ably so closelytiedinwith other clues in recognizing words. When he word recognition techniques that nei- meets a word which he cannot recog- ther can be separated.Context clues nize, he uses his experiences, oral Ian- aloneare seldomadequate because

. 189 EMANS 183

they provide only one aid to wordrec- schemes may becloselyrelatedto ognition.They may suggestone ofthose for developing word meanings. severalpossible wordsbutseldom they also have relevance for the use of point to the specific word.For exam- contextcluesinwordrecognition. ple, in the sentence. "Themouse nib- Art ley (3) identified ten types of con- bled a piece of ," any textual aids the reader might find in number of possible words could fitthe printedmatter :typographical(e.g.. meaning.However, by combining the quotation marks, parenthesis). struc- sense of the sentence with the phonic tural appositives. nonrestrictive clue that the word begins withaeh clauses). substitute words (synonyms, sound. the reader can readily supply antonyms), word elements (roots, suf- theword cheese. Childrenshould fixes.prefixes).figuresofspeech probably be discouraged from using (similes. metaphors), pictorial repre- context clues alone.By combining sentation(e.g.,pictures.diagrams, themwithother wordrecognition charts). inference. direct explanation, techniques a child would be discour- backgroundofexperienceofthe aged from wild guessing.Therefore, reader, and subjective clues (e.g.. tone, in relation to the discussion that fol- mood, intent).Likewise. McCullough lows,itshould be remembered that (15)identified two general classes of whenever context cluesarctaught, clues,idea andpresentation. Idea they should probably be combined with cluesincludedpictorialillustration, other word recognition aids. verbal,experience,comparisonand Sonic people might regard theuse of contrast. synonym, summary, mood. context clues as untutored or guess- definition,andfamiliarexpression. ing; and in a sense it is.However, it The presentationclues included the is probably more accurately described position of words, the sequence of a as inferential reasoning which must be sentence or paragraph, and the general developed. guided, and used inmany organization of a selection.From a areasoflife. LilaBantonSmith studyof500,000 runningwords states. "Surely this process of examin- Deighton(9)identifiedfourkey ing meanings. reasoning, and deducing wordsclasses(definition,examples. an unrecognized word is not just a modifiers, and restatement) andinfer- matter of chance guessing" (20: 186). ential cluesfor which the reader has Kolson and Ko linger state, "Guessing no direct clue excepthis abilityto is the mainstay of the contextual clue draw inferences. In his dissertation, skill and should be encouraged, but for which he won the IRA research wild guessing is a symptom ofa disa- award, Ames (1) found fourteen dues bility in contextual clue use" (14: 65). from'his case studies of mature readers The sophisticated use of context clues, using a variety of contextualsitua- therefore. should probably be devel- tions: oped along with reasoning and theuse of other word recognition skills. I.Clues derivedfromlanguage experience or familiar expres- The classification of context clues sions Various attempts have been made to 2. Clues utilizing modifying classify the wide variety of context phrases or clauses clues. Although theseclassification 3.Clues utilizing definition or de-

S . 0 184 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

script ion tence or sentences surrounding the un- .1. Clues provided through wordsrecognized word.The categoryin- connected or in series cludes such clues for recognizing un- 5. Comparison or contrast clues known words as familiar expressions 6.Synonym clues and idioms.definitions. descriptions. 7. Clues provided by the tone, set- examples.synonyms. antonymsin- ting. and mood of a selection cluded in the text, as well as compari- 8.Referral clues sons andcontrasts,andthetone. 9.Association clues mood, and setting of what isbeing 10. Clues derived from the main read. idea and supporting details pat- The languagebearing cluesuse tern of paragraph organization knowledge of syntax, the structure of II. Cluesprovidedthroughthe sentences, as aids in word recognition. question-and-answer pattern of Hildreth states. "The use of context paragraph organization clues has its roots in linguistics" (13 : 12. Preposition clues 156).There are a number of exam- 13 Cluesutilizingnonrestrictive ples of language bearing clues. One clauses or appositive phrases such aid is the noting of phrases which 14.Chtes derived from cause and may serve as a clue in recognition of effect pattern of paragraph and modified unknown words.Another sentence organization such aid is the recognition of unknown words through referral signal words. Concerning the Artley and McCul- such as these and same, which refer to loughclassifications,Russellstates, what has been stated previously.The "these are often too technical for sys- associating of known words of one part tematic use in the elementary of speech with closely related unknown school...." (10:300-301). Likewise, words of another part of speech (such Ames states in respect to his own study as nouns and verbs or adjectives and that "It must he stressed that muchnouns) may serve as anotherclue. more researchis necessary and one For example. birds fly and fish swim ; would be ill-advised to try to develop grass is green, and old ladies usually animatein structional procedures have gray hair.Finally the recogni- based onthepresentclassification tion of the relationship of nonrestric- scheme" (2:81 ). An appropriate task tiveclauses,appositive phrases,or regarding the implementation of con- prepositional phrases to other parts of text clues in word recognition woulda sentence may serve as another lan- appear to be the development of a guage clue. simplified scheme for classifying the numerous kinds of context clues iden- Another group of clues involve how tified. The next few paragraphs will sentences or paragraphs are organized. suggest such a possible scheme. Within this group arc such aids as the Most context clues for use in word realization that an unknown word is recognition seem tofallinto one of part of a series of words and an appre- three main categories: meaning bear- ciation of the relatedness of main idea ing clues, language bearing clues, and to details, of questions to answers, and organization clues. The meaning of cause to effect within sentences Or bearing cities use the sense of the sett- paragraphs.

. 191 EMANS 185

Therefore,itwould 'appearthat recognizing words" ( 12 :138). many context clues fall within one of The goal of instruction for the use thesethreeclassifications:meaning of context clues in word recognition is bearing, language bearing, and organi- probably to develop such skill that con- zation.The scheme is not allinclu- text clues are used easily and automat- sive. For example, the scheme does ically.If a child makes as many er- not include contextual aids from pic- rors in contextual reading as he does tures or the typography.However, it in reading a list of words, he is proba- seems to simplify the complex classifi- bly failihg to make extensive use of catkm schemes previously suggested. context clues.The problem becomes Possibly to date, sufficient emphasis one of developing instructional proce- has not been placed on the role that dures. context clues play in word recognition There is little evidence that children because the classification schemes have will use context clues more effectively appearedto be too complex. The ifthey have knowledge of sentence preceding simplified.yet comrehen- patterns. McKee states, such knowl- sive. scheme for the classification of edge "contributes littleif anything to context clues is proposed in the hope the pupils' comprehension of the sen- that it will foster further investigation tences" ( /8 :185). However, children in this area.Because of the simplifica- willprobablybenefitfrom knowing tion, teachers may feel encouraged to thatI a word makes sense within a teachthe use of context clues as sentence; 2) readers can use sentence knowledge of the structure and impli- meaning to help recognize an unrecog- cations for teaching context clues is nized word ;and 3) more than one more attainable. word may fit the meaning of a sentence Teaching context clues and, therefore, structure and phonic Although the teaching of context clues are often necessary. clues seems complex. there is evidence In preparing to teach the use of con- that teachers should attemptto do text clues, materials should be care- so.McCullough (15) concluded that fully scrutinized to determineif the adults fail to use context clues because content gives adequate clues to words they were never taught how touse which children have not yet learned to them.In a study by McKee, children recognize in their reading but possess were found to use context clues effec- in their speaking-listening vocabular- tively in only about one third of the ies.Only a few such words should be opportunities presented (17:73), while presented at any one time, as too many Porter (21 :316) found that third grade unknown- words make using the con- childrencould givean appropriate text difficult and might encourage wild meaning of a word left out of context guessing. The exact ratio of unknown in about 80 percent of the cases. Since words to known words probably de- children may not develop the skill of pends on the children's intelligence, using context clues without specific maturity, and background, although training, they should be given help in Bond and Tinker suggest that about its development. Hester states, "Sys- one word in every forty running words tematic guidance is necessary to help should be unknown (4:321). Never- him learn this important technique for theless, the materials used should be 186 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

easy enough for children to recognize carriedby picturesinthereaders. the unknown word without too much However, Weintraub found that chil, difficulty. (ken do not make as much of illustra- The materials should reflect the lan- tions as they might (23). Therefore, guage patterns of the child whenever children may be taught to look at pic- possible, be at a concept level appro- tures to get clues for unknown words priate to the children, and have the un- since pictures are a part of the total known words evenly distributed context. and since they may be helpful throughout the text. in demonstrating to children the con- .After the materials have been se- cept of. using context clues.Picture- lected, provisions need to be made, as word cards, picture dictionaries, and in any reading activity, to assure that introducing new words in advance of children have the background to read reading with the aid of pictures can be the materials through prior direct and helpful in getting children to use pic- vicarious experiences,including dis- tures as aids in word recognition. cussions, explanations, demonstrations, As soon as children have developed and field.trips. The children need enough sight words to read sentences, then to be given direct guidance in pictures become less and sentences be- reading usingcontextclues. Such come more important for developing guidance may include talking about the the use of context clues. Sentences, idea so that knowing the meaning of a paragraphs, riddles. and stories with sentence or paragraph will help in rec- parts of words omitted may be given to ognizing unknown words ;encourag- ing children to read the entire sentence children.Enutns (10) found the fol- before deciding on an unknown word. lowing hierarchy of exercises, easiest showing how context clues can be to most difficult, to be significant at the combined with other word recognition .001level of confidence for children techniques, such as phonics; reading from grades three to ten : the exercises orally to the children and 1. No clue given other than context having the children supply the un- 2.Beginning letter given known word ; covering a few lines in a 3.Length of word given story and having the children antici- 4.Beginningandendingletters pate what will come ;asking which given part of a sentence or paragraph gives a 5. Four word choice given clue to an unknown word ; and showing 6. Consonants given that some words must be recognized in Teachers can probably think of varia- context, such as, wound the clock and tions to the exercises.For example, wound a deer. in the multiple-choice type of exercise, Pictures may be used in the lower words with the same sounds or words grades to develop an orientation to- withsimilarconfigurations may be used. ward theuse ofcontext clues. Hildreth states, "The use of picture In summary, this paper shows the clues is similar to the tie of context importance of helping children develop clues for deriving the meaning of new skills in the use of context clues in or forgotten words" (13:156). In the word recognition and makes sugges- lower grades much of the content is tions as to how to teach these skills. EMANS 187

REFERENCES man, 1960. 1. Ames, W. S."A Study of the Process 12.Hester, Kathleen B.Teaching Every by whichReadersDetermine Word Child to Read.New York : Harper and Meaning Through the Use of Verbal Row, 1964. Context," unpublished doctoral disserta- 13.Hildreth,Gertrude. Teaching .Read- tion, University of Missouri, 1965. ing: A Guide to Basic Principles and 2. Ames, W. S."The Development of a Modern Practices.New York: Holt, ClassificationSchemeofContextual Rinehart and Winston, 1958. Aids," Reading Research Quarterly,2 14.Kolson, C. J., and G. Koluger.Clinical (1966), 57-82. Aspects of Remedial Reading.Spring- 3. Art ley, A. S."Teaching Word-Mean- field, Illinois: Charles C. Thomas, 1963. ing through Context,"Elementary En- IS.McCullough, Constance, M. "Learning ghsh Review,20 (1943), 68-74. to Use Context Clues," Elementary En- 4.Bond,G.L.,andM.A.Tinker. glishReview,20 (1943), 140-143. Reading Difficulties:Their Diagnosis 16.McCullough, Constance M. "The Rec- and Correction.New York : Appleton- ognition of Context Clues in Reading," Century-Crofts, 1967. Elementary English Review,22 (1945). 5.Bond,G.L.,and Eva B. Wagner. 1-5. Teaching the Child to Read(3rd ed.). 17.McKee, P.The Teaching of Reading. New York : Macmillan, 1960. Boston : Houghton Mifflin, 1948. 6. Carter,H.L. J.1 andDorothyJ. 18.McKee, P.Reading: A Program of In- McGinnis. TeachingIndividualsto structionforthe Elementary School, Read.Boston: D. C. Heath, 1962. Boston : Houghton Mifflin, 1966. 7.Cordts,AnnaD. Phonicsforthe 19.Russell, D. H.Children Learn to Read Reading Teacher.New York: Holt, (2nd ed.). Boston: Ginn, 1961. Rinehart and Winston, 1965. 20.Smith, Nila Banton.Reading Instruc- 8. De Boer, John J., and Martha Dall- tion for Today's Children.Englewood mann.The Teaching of Reading.New Cliffs, New Jersey : Prentice-Hall, 1963. York :Holt, Rinehart, and Winston, 21.Spache, G. D.Reading in the Elemen- 1964. tary School.Boston: Allyn and Bacon, 9. Deighton, L.Vocabulary Development 49&. in the Classroom.New York :Teach- 22.Tinker, M. A.Bases for Elective Read- er'sCollege, Bureau ofPublicatio ts, ing.Minneapolis: University of Minne- 1959. sota Press, 1965. 10. Emans, R., and Gladys Mary Fisher. 23.Weintraub, S."The Effect of Pictures "Teaching the Use of Context Clues," on theComprehension of a Second- Elementary English,44 (1967), 243-246. Grade Basal Reader," unpublished doc- 11. Gray, W. S. OnTheir Own in Read- toral dissertation, University of Illinois, ing.Glenview, Illinois :Scott, Fores- 1960. New Approaches to Easing Word Attack atthe Beginning Reader Levels

DOROTI1Y OVER THE YEARS, new methods of teaching reading have been proposed regularly. In the past ten years, the number of new ideas has appeared to be astounding. Because no one approach works for all pupils, teachers need to be aware of a variety of approaches which might he used. A brief description of some of these approaches follows.

NEW APPROACHES i.t.a. The Initial Teaching Alphabet was designed by Sir James Pitman for the purpose of providing an easier alphabet to use in the beginning stages of reading. The alphabethas forty-four characters. Of these, twenty-four are the same as our known lower case characters (q and x have been omitted). Fourteen other symbols are combinations of known symbols and the rest are specialized symbols used to representdifferent sounds of the same letter. The alphabet is not a method but amedium that may be used with the teacher's usual teaching style. i.t.a. was designed to permit easier decoding of printed sym- bols during the early stages of learning to read. It also enables children to spell and Write more easily at first grade level. Transition to traditional orthography usually takes place at the end of first grade or beginning second grade. Since twenty-four of the i.t.a. symbols are similar to t.o., transition is not seen as a problem for most pupils. Basal readers using i.t.a. have been developed in England and in the United States. In addition, supplementary materials are available from several companies. Before deciding to use i.t.a. it would be advisable to consider the following points: 1.Approval from the administration to insure the necessary follow-through from grade one to grade two. 2.Training of all teachers who are to use i.t.a. 3.Purchase of newi.t. a.readers and supplementary materials. 4.Rewrite all classroom dittos, charts, and labels in i.t.a. 5.Subject area materials written in t.o. could probably be The Reading Teacher,23 (March 1970), 511-515. DIETRICH 189

used by the brighter students but unit material would need to be developed in i.t.a. for slow pupils or provision made to teach without the use of published texts. Research studies seem to indicate that some children taught to read through the medium of i.t.a. become better readers, especi- ally if they are the type who learn best by a phonic approach or a decoding method. Other children's preferred and most advantageous mode of learning puts emphasison meaning, with skills of decoding taught as need arises. The greatest advantage of i.t.a. seems to be in the early learning to write independently and to spell well. While first graders who use i.t.a. often make quite rapid progress in learning to read, this advantage tends to disappear after transition to t.o. and there seems to be but little residual advantage by the time third grade is reached. More careful research is needed before one can say for sure whether there is much if any permanent advantage in using i.t.a. Words in color As an aid in helping pupils identify similar sounds of our language, various colors have been utilized by Gattegno (1962) in his Words in Color. While color is the most obvious aspect of this approach, the method also places the responsibility for learning and developing principlesupon the learner. It is expected that the child will use his own ability to draw generalizations concerning the sounds ofour language, he will develop an attitude toward checking his work and he will become critical of his own work and that of hispeers. The method uses a number of different colored chalks, each representing a particular sound found in the language. Symbols used to represent the same sound use the same colored chalk. Gattegno has developed his own materials embodying Words in Color which might be used by the teacher. These include word charts, phonic word charts, sets of word cards, books, plus work sheets. Many opinions have been voiced on the effectiveness or ineffectiveness of this method, but there are seemingly no defini- tive research studies. Words in Color was one of the methods not included in the national first grade study done several years ago. Glowing testimonials do little to help one evaluate the strengths and weaknesses of this approach. Carefully controlled studies done with a substantial number of children are needed. LinguisticsIn recent years, linguists (those who have made a scientific study of the language) have proposed a number of 190 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS 01: WORD RECOGNITION

lettersis emphasized. They learn that every sentence begins with a capital letter and ends with a period or question mark. These language skills which are developed through much repeti- tion provide a good foundation for later use in creative writing. LATER DEVELOPMENTS The Daily Story soon becomes a news story which serves as a communication link between school and home. Every daily story is recorded on a chart tablet as it appeared on the board. Tkis enables children to have the story where they can read it during the day, while the board is used for other class work. On Friday, the story for each day of the week is typed in manuscript and mimeographed, one story to a page. The stories are then stapled together in a booklet form. Friday afternoon is an enjoy- able time in the first grade room, as each child receives a copy of the week's stories. Upon receiving the booklet, the children num- ber the pages and immediately begin to look for familiar words. As they recognize a word, they are encouraged to draw a ring around it. They like to count the number of words they have remembered. At the beginning someone might volunteer to read a sentence of his choice. After a few weeks, they are ready to read all of one day's sentences. Before proceeding to the following page, they read the story in unison. In this way all children have a chance to participate. When someone is capable of reading an entire story, the child is allowed to go to one of the other primary rooms in the building, and read the story to that class. It may be that as many as five are ready to do this. In that case, all five of the children go, each reading a different day's story. This keeps the interest high for both the bright child and others who now have an incentive to try a little more to reach that goal. CONCLUSION The communication skills are related and the experiences of children, including their language experiences, are essential for a successful educational program. The Daily Story Approach permits originality and initiative. It is very evident that language experi- ences do make great contributions to the reading process. The Daily Story Approach is by far not the only method of instruction, but it has proved to be a very successful one for the beginning reader. It offers children a very exciting, provocative, and successful beginning reading experience. Its effectiveness lies in its simplicity and in personal reading of stories and the

ic,17 DIETRICH 191

approaches in using their scientific knowledge of our language as it applies to the decoding process of reading. While linguistic methods vary, all begin with a study of the alphabet and the speech sounds. Regular phoneme-grapheme relationship are pre- sented early, giving the child a mastery over a goodly number of words. Basic words following an irregular pattern are taught as sight words; for instance, the and of. In addition to teaching the phoneme-grapheme relationship, attention is also given in some linguistic readers to syntax, pitch, and stress. Syntax relates to ways words are used in phrases, clauses and sentences, pitch relates to intonation, and stress has to do with accent given to the more important words within a sentence. There are numerous series available which incorporate a linguistic approach. Many follow a very routine pattern with little stress being given to comprehension skills. A few of the newer linguistic series are stressing comprehension to a greater degree. Many early linguistic readers were rather dull for both teach- ers and children. Newer reading series using linguistic approaches have turned from the rather dull, rote material to more realistic material. While there has been some research done on the effec- tiveness of a linguistic approach, there are no clear-cut indications that the method produces any better results than any other method using a strong decoding approach.

Programmed instructionBoth machines and materials have been developed to teach beginning reading skills. The basic prin- ciple of programmed instruction is to provide children with small bits of information or with piecemeal skills, with constant rein- forcement, to enable them to build a larger body of information or a major skill. One example of programmed books is the Buchanan Pro- grammed Readers prepared by Sullivan Associates. The material is linguistically based. Each frame has its own answer in a column along the left side of the page. The child covers the answer with a marker while he is working and as he finishes each frame he uncovers the answer and checks his work. The material begins with regular letter sounds and progresses to the reading of a variety of literary forms. The Aud-X machine by Educational Developmental Labora- tories uses a visual, auditory, kinesthetic approach to beginning reading. Pupils hear the sound or word, see the written form on the screen and usually reinforce their learning in the earliest

198 192 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OP WORD RECOGNITION

stages by tracing or writing the letter or word. As the difficulty of the material progresses, pupils begin to react to the material present and then are given the correct response by which they may evaluate their work. As with the other approaches just mentioned, there are few research studies to indicate the effectiveness of programmed instruction. In a controlled study conducted in the Uniondale Dis- trict several years ago, at the end of second grade there was no difference in performance of those pupils using Sullivan's Program Reading Series as compared with those pupils using a basal reading series. The major difference noted was the response from the teachers who felt that the time and energy spent in working with numerous groups and preparing supplementary material did not warrant the adoption of the program. PhonicsPhonic series in the form of workbooks, worksheets, charts, and books have been available for many years. A few basal reading series begin with a strong phonics program before story readingisintroduced. Most basal readers include well developed phonics programs introduced in grade one and extend- ing to grade three and included as one part of a total reading approach. Each phonic program and phonetically based reader series uses Its own progression of skills. Some methods begin by teaching consonants, others begin with vowels. Some teach through a faniily approach (ake, make, cake, lake); others a helper approach (ba, bad, bath, bat). For this reason it is wise for a teacher to restrict initial use to one series in order to form a core for the phonics program and to supplement when necessary by carefully selecting skills practice from other sources. Recent research by Clymer (1963) indicates that many of the generalizations previously taught hold true less than 50 per cent of the time and thus should not be taught as hard and fast rules. Other research studies indicate that pupils at the end of first grade who have been taught to read through phonics-supple- mented basal programs usually do better than children using just a basal program which neglects word attack skills. By the end of third grade the differences appear to level off. Again this area needs more research. Teachers need to recognize several factors before considering phonics programs to begin to teach reading or to supplement basal readers: DIETRICH 193

1.Each child differs in the amount of phonics needed to make him an independent reader. 2. Too much phonics can be as harmful as too little. 3.Supplemental programs must be dovetailed to the phonics program found in basal readers. 4.As much time and attention needs to be devoted to the pupils' application of isolated phonic elements to total word attack as to their learning the element initially.

SUMMARY While there are many ways of teaching children to learn to read and many ways of helping children to decode at the beginning levels, teachers should not rely on one method or approach for all children within their classes. Little research has been done to show the superiority of one decoding method over another. In some instances there is little research that tells conclusively how effective any one particular method is. Teachers must become skillful enough in knowing children and approaches to reading that the best approach might be found for each child. Flexibility in teaching is still the byword.

REFERENCES Buchanan, Cynthio Dee, and Sullivan Associates. Programmed reading. New York: Webster Divisic.n, McGraw-Hill. Clymer, T. The utility of phonic generalizations in the primary grades. The Reading Teacher. 1963, 16, 252-258. Gattegno, C. Words in color. Chicago: Learning Materials, Inc., 1962. Phonic Emphasis Approaches

ARTHUR W. HEILMAN*

TWO OBJECTIVES of this discussion are to identify salient features of a limited number of phonic approaches to beginning reading and to stress the educational issues which are related to the debate on phonics emphasis which has characterized the past decade. Background The problem of what is the proper role of phonics in reading instruction is not new. Undoubtedly the pressures on schools and teachers today, which arc traceable to this issue are unprecedented. In general, the most vociferous critics of present day reading instruction start from a number of erroneous premises which suit their purposes. These premises include: 1. There is a sight word method of teaching reading which makes no provision for teaching phonic skills. 2.All words met at various instructional levels are taught exclusively as "sight words." 3. There is an educational conspiracy in American education which is opposed to the teaching of phonics. 4.Phonics instruction is "good" and children cannot get too much of a good thing. 5.If we go back to the phonics emphasis of the 1890's, present reading problems in our schools would disappear. To illustrate the present state of affairs we will turn to the Council on Basic Education, a non-profit organization dedicated to the improvement of American education. In so far as the Council on Basic Education has been concerned with reading instruction, that concern has focused entirely on the issue of phonics. In the January 1964 Bulletin (p. 1) of the Council we read: Perhaps the most encouraging development of the year (1963), was the increasing understanding on the part of the public of what the phonics versus sight reading controversy is all about. The issue has often been confused in the public mind, especially when the advocates of the sight, or look-and-say method, insist that they also are believers in phonics instruction. In the CBE Bulletin, February 1962 (p. 2), one finds: From our observation of good teachers in the fields of reading and language we discern a few guiding principles and methods of operations. First Grade Reading Programs,Perspectives in Reading No. 5, 1965, 56-71. r i4.1 HEILMAN 195

I. The good teacher of beginning reading, where she is not bound byan imposed methodology, operates on the theory that beginning reading is not a 'thought getting' process but is basedon translating letters into sounds.

No evidence was cited as to source of this data. In discussing an article by Paul Woodring, (Saturday Review,January 20, 1962) which made a pica for less heat and lessname calling in the discussion of reading methodolcgy, the editor of the Council's Bulletin stated, "It seems to us that anyone who suggests,even by indirection, that the present reading controversy does not involve fundamental issues butis a reflection of temperamental differences between reading theorists, misreuls the true nature of the controversy. There isa real war on in :r. ,ling, and for the future well-being of American educationitis important that the right side win." [Emphasis added.] CBE Bulletin, March 1962, pp. 8-9. The individual who wants to make beginning readinga meaningful and enjoyable experience may have cause to feel threatened by the almost unlimited number of phonic systems which deal only with thislimited facet of reading. This factor, coupled with theemergence of the philosophy that a "fast start" in beginning reading basedon early stress of phonics is an unmitigated virtue, poses an educational issue of major importance. The writer has long been of the opinion that phonics is second in importance to no other reading skill, and that "the reader must have the ability to pronounce or to approximate the pronunciation ofwords he does not know as sight words. This is amplereason for teaching phonics and sufficient justification for teaching it well" (9). Nevertheless, holding such a view does not preclude resistance to critics and materialswhich would push us into: 1.Teaching more phonics than is necessary for the childto learn to read. 2.Neglecting procedures for differentiation of phonics instruction. 3. Overemphasizing phonics in beginningreading. 4. Teaching phonics "steps" in illogicalsequence. 5.Developing an over-relianze on phonics, (orcontext, or any other word analysis skill), as all suchextremes are uneconomical and indefensible. 6.Behaving as if we believed thata child's memorizing phonic rules assures the ability to apply these rules in reading situations. With these cautions in mind we move toa brief analysis of a limited --- 'T "-- -V1,111,- 7

196 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS Of WORD RECOGNITION

number of phonic-emphasis approaches to first grade reading, The Carden Method The Carden Method is a set of materials and a methodology developed and published by Mac Carden (4). Although the method stresses compo- sition and spelling, its chief aim is the teaching of phonics. Book I deals primarily with the two-vowel rule. Book II teaches the sound of single vowels, and Book III introduces combinations which include r and w. The author suggests that after pupils have completed the Carden readers they move on to basal readers. Thus, the initial stageof reading is saturated with letter sound analysis. This approach: I.Stresses Rules Initial teaching is limited to "regular spellings," or words which follow the given rule. The child is taught to "cross off" silent vowels and the famous two-vowel rule is modified to read: "When there arc 2 vowels in a word the second vowel is crossed off and thefirst vowel keeps its name sound." The following steps are utilized in teaching two-vowel patterns: A. The term vowel is not used until after children have studied all vowels. B. a, e, i, o. = 'The little letters that change their sounds," C. Find the two letters that change their sounds. 1.Think! Which one comes first in the word and which one comes second in the word? 2.Cross off the one that comes second. 3.Sound out the word letter by letter (coal = cull' o tub). Later a goodly number of amendments or new rules arc advanced or taught. Examples: A. "There are ea words where the a drops out." (head, bread) B. "There are words in which the o drops out in the ou combination and has the sound of u." (double, trouble, country) IL Stresses Synthetic Sounding Each consonant (or digraph) is given the weight of a syllabic, (bbuh; jjuh; gguh; ththuh; chchuh; etc.). The word the is pro- nounced thuh and once the sound of a few letters have beenintroduced the child is ready to read a sentence: suh' c thuh buh' o tuh see the boat (preprimmy manual, p. 42)

g-2 HEILMAN 197

Since an ordinary three- or four-letter word is soundedas though it consists of three syllables, the teacher is instructed to place theaccent on the initial letter. (boat = buh' o tuh; coat = kuh'o tuh) III.Stresses Writing and Spelling from the Beginning of Instruction The letter c plays an important role in the teaching ofletter forms. The child is taught that: A. "Seven letters begin with c." Theseare: c, a, d, g, q, o, e, (The point is that there is a c imbedded somewhere in each of these lettcrs.) B. "Two letters make a c backwards." Thesearc: b, p. C. "The letter s is a c up in the air and ac backwards." The following is from "A Note on Learning to Spell." The teacher constructs a word letter by lcttcr applying the two-vowel rule. Example: The word dime is given. The teacher says, "Write duh." She adds, "What did you write ?" The pupils answer, "D." The tcachcr says, "Write i next to the d." Then she says, "m, write m, mm." The pupils write m. The tcachcr asks, "How do you make the i stay i?" The pupils answer, "Add an e." (The Carden Method, 11 Briefon How To Get Started.) In the opinion of the writer, this approach is extremely mechanisticand ritualistic. However, it should not be concluded that children exposedto this type of instruction in grade one will not "learn" what is commonly measured at the end of grade one as "reading achievement." This isa tribute to children, but perhaps an unfortunate educational phenomena since the fact that "learning takes place" is often citedas a justification for the use of such procedures. Words in Color Words In Color (7) is a system developed by Caleb Gattcgnowho also developed Numbers in Color basedon the Cuisenairc Rods for teaching arithmetic. That materials arediscussedas a "phonics emphasis approach" because instruction begins and continues with the systematic teaching of letter sounds. The child is exposed toa dual visual stimuli :( I ) the letter configuration, (2) in color. He hears hundreds of repetitions of each letter sound while his attention is focusedon the letter configuration 2C4 "II "."- 1

198 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

in color. In this approach 39 colors are used, each of which represents a speech sound in Englishregardless of what letters represent that sound. Initial teaching is done entirely at the blackboard using different colored chalks, and through the use of some 29 large wall charts done in color. On these charts the various letters and letter combinations in a given word are shown in different colors. Thus, the word and utilizes three colors; sand, four colors; and impossible calls for the use of seven colors. (The writer being color-blind could not verify this latter fact for himself, but accepted the word of a trusted colleague.) The following are some pertinent facts about Words In Color: 1. Sounding, or the association of colored-letter forms with speech sounds, is taught in a most systematic, repetitive fashion. The term drill used in this setting would not be an overstatement. In the teaching of letter sounds the individual consonants are not taught in isolation but are blended with vowel sounds. 2. At this moment there is, to the best of the writer's knowledge, not a single piece of research which even hints that the addition of color to letter forms adds one iota to the learning of letter sounds or phonic principles. 3. The child actually never reads anything in color (except letters and words on the wall charts or in blackboard drill). All reading 1 materials used are printed black on white ! 4. Thirty-nine colors is a goodly number of colors. It is a tenable hypothesis that children might develop problems in discrimination of just noticeable differences in color in the absence of letter configurations. Examples of one series of colors include: cadmium green, yellow green No. 15, yellow green No. 47, dark green, olive green, light green, deep green, emerald green No. 45, emerald green No. 26, leaf green, gray green, yellow ochre, brown ochre. 5. Materials include: for teachers, Background and Principles, Teach- er's Guide, 21 Wall Charts, 8 Phonic Code Charts; and children's materials (printed black on white), Books 1, 2, 3, First Book of Stories, set of worksheets. Programmed Reading (2) While there are a number of materials which might merit the designa- tion programmed reading our discussion is limited to one set of materials which meets the criterion of being a phonics emphasis approach to beginning reading (Programmed Reading, Webster Division, McGraw- HEILMAN 199

Hill Book Co.). These materials are primarily a program for gradesone and two, and for remedial instruction. The program begins with the teaching of word symbols which have "a constant sound value." Thus, we meet again the "word family" of previous eras or "regular spellings" of one linguistic approach. However, before beginning to work with the programmed materials the child must have mastered a sizeable portion of word analysis or the phonic skills program which includes the following: 1. The names of the letters of the alphabet (capital and small). 2. How to print all the capital and small letters. 3. That letters stand for sounds and what sounds to associate with the letters a, f, m, n, p, e, th, and i which are used as the points of departure for the programmed re,Iders. 4. That letters are read from left to right and that groups of letters form words. 5. The words yes and no by sight; how to discriminate the words ant, man, and mat from each other, and how to read the sentences, I am an ant, I am a man, I am a mat, I am a pin, I am a pan, I am thin, I am tan, I am fat. These skills are taught in a stage called programmed pre- reading which, by the nature of what is taught, emphasizes the association of letter symbol with corresponding letter sound. The programmed materials are workbook type exercises calling for the child to circle the correct word or write a letter or letters. For instance, a picture of an ant is followed by the sentence: Iam anant.A later frame man will be I am an -nt. It should be noted that these materials rely heavilyon visual discrimination and that there is considerable emphasison "word parts" or individual letters. Without doubt programmed materials holdsome promise for teaching certain facets of beginning reading. A promotional brochure heralds these particular materials as "the most significant breakthrough in the teaching of reading in 50 years!" This might bean overstatement. One of the virtues of programmed learning is the fact that the childcan instantly check his response to see if he is correct. As the child writesa vowel, a consonant blend, or a word in a blank space he can look to the left on the same frame and sec if he wrote correctly. He can also look before he writes if he learns this approach. One strength of programmed reading is that it could be called an individualized approach. However, in this case every child uses the same 200 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

material or book, but each goes through it at his own rate. Programmed materials deal with mechanical aspects of the reading process. These are important, but they are not all there is to reading. There has been little, if any, programming of critical reading or thought processes. The Phonovisual Method (14) The Phonovisual Method is a supplementary phonic instruction program designed for use with existing basal and other approaches. The authors offer their materials as a "middle course" stating, "The Phonovisual Method is not intended to be used instead of sight reading, but as a parallel teaching" (14, p. 6). As is the case with many other phonics instructional materials there are no major differences in the "content" of the phonics program of phonovisual and basal materials. The major differences arc: 1. The degree to which phonic teaching dominates beginning reading instruction. 2. The emphasis on drill in associating letter sounds and graphic letter symbols. It should be noted that the teacher is cautioned to always use the material as a game, not as drill. Yet, it is obvious that the amountof emphasis on teaching letter sounds makes this a difficult goal to achieve. "For best results the teaching [of letter sounds] should begin the veryfirst week of school if possible the very first day. At the demonstration school one half-hour daily is given to each of these two subjects, (a) sight reading and (b) phonetic instruction ..." (14, p. 11). Materials. The Phonovisual materials are describedas a phonics program for kindergarten, primary, and remedial instruction, and include the following: 1. Two 26" x 40" wall charts one for teaching consonant sounds, the other for vowel sounds. 2.Phonovisual method book (a teacher's guide). 3.Pupils' books: Readiness book, transition book (preprimer), sepa- rate consonant and vowel workbooks. 4.Miscellaneous items: Phonograph recording, "Sounds on the Phono- visual Charts," skill building flash cards, consonant and vowel filmstrips, magnetic boards, phonic rummy games, and the like. Of all the numerous phonic approaches, phonovisual probably most closely parallels the philosophy of a majority of basal materials. Consonant sounds are taught first, auditory-visual training is coordinated, learning words as sight-recognition units is encouraged. One of the major potential "Or AV

HEILMAN 201

drawbacks in the use of phonovisual is that the actual instruction caneasily go beyond simply systematicteaching and become a ritual. The matter of difTcrentiation of amounts of phonicinstruction for different pupils is not stressed. Despite the warningsof the authors, and particularly in light of the procedures outlined, the teachingof sounding can easily become an end in itselfrather than the means to achieving a balanced program whose goal is fluent, critical reading. On theother hand, the materials provide a concrete step -by -step programwhich introduces practically all of the phonics teaching in grade one.Some teachers may profit from this systematic approach while atthe same time modifying techniques so as to minimize rote teaching. Phonetic Keys to Reading Phonetic Keys to Reading (8) is a quite ,accurate descriptionof the materials which bear thistitle.The "keys" arc the multitude of "rules" that have accumulated over a numbcr of decades as aresult of attempting to deal with the vagaries of lettersounds in written English. Phonetic Keys provides for teaching children all of the known rulesincluding some which have very limited application. The data reported byClymer (5), Oaks (13), and Burrows/Louric (3), relative to the per centof time various phonic rules actually apply, should be kept in mind as prospective users attempt to evaluate the materials under discussion. Phonetic Keys is not a method of teaching reading but a set of supple- mentary phonics materials. The first two months ofbeginning instruction are devoted to teaching approximatelyforty separate phonic skills including both long and short vowel sounds, all consonant sounds, and anumber of blends and consonant digraphs. For the first several weekschildren are taught to sound letters (vowels) which are invariablylocated in the middle of words. Following this unique experience they are taughtinitial consonant sounds. A reasonable rationale for attacking the middle of wordshas never been advanced. Promotional materials point out, "Since each wordcontains one or more than one vowel, no words canbe sounded independently by the pupil until he has a knowledge of the more commonvowel sounds." It might be pointed out that all words exceptI, a and eye contain consonants which must be noted if the wordis to be solved. As a result of the emphasis on sounding in beginningreading the child is in essence taught, and many learn, toacquire a set to sound out words. The same word is sounded out time after time, long afterit should have been mastered as a sight word. Theteacher's manual in discussing the 202 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL. ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

use of expericnce charts cautions teachers against teaching words as sight words, "At first these charts arc to be read by sight, butas soon as the children havc learned enough sounds and phonetic principles to analyze words with the help of the teacher, teaching cxpericncc charts and reading charts by sight should be entirely eliminated" (Teacher's Manual for First Grade, 1964, p. 18). Materials. First grade materials consist of three paperback workbook- type books, Tag, Dot and Jim, All Around with Dot and Jim, and a teacher's manual. The materials were developed by Cornelia Sloop,an elementary teacher in Texas. The major educational issues with which users and prospectiveusers of these materials should be concerned includc: 1.Should beginning reading instruction concentrate on sounding letters to the degree these materials advocate? 2.Should initial sounding begin in the middle of words? 3. Should children learn dozens of complicated phonic rules in the process of beginning reading? 4. Can sounding be "overemphasized" to the detriment of future reading facility? 5. Can beginning instruction result in pupils' developinga "set" to sound out each word met? 6.All facile readers recognize wordsas units and sound out only those few words they do not recognizeas sight words. Should children be taught to sound out all words to the neglect of developinga sight vocabulary? Linguistic Approach (Emphasizing Spccch Sounds in Words) This discussion does not attempt to deal with all linguistic thcory and suggested practices which relate to beginning reading instruction.Reference is made only to one approach, that of limitinginitial teaching to words which enjoy "regular spellings" in English. Whilethe term phonics might be offensive to the linguist-authors, the materials meritdiscussion here since their chief aim is the early association of spccchsounds with written letter combinations. The first such serious proposal bya linguist was presented publicly in 1942. The April and May issues of The Elementary EnglishReview contained articles by Leonard Bloomfield in which he outlinedwhat in essence was later published in 1961 under, the title, Let's ReadA Linguistic Approach (1). Reducedto its essentials this approach simply HEILMAN 203

advocates teaching beginning reading through the process of teaching words which follow "regular spellings" in English. This approach relics on a very rigid vocabulary control. Each lesson introduces and stresses what in the past was called "word families," such as cat, rat, fat, mat, bat, sat. The child first learns words in isolation, then phrases and sentences: a bat, a cat, a rat, a mat, a fat cat, a fat rat, a fat cat ran at a fat cat (Let's Read, Bloomfield and Barnhart, p. 61). In 1963, Charles Fries' book Linguistics and Reading was published. There are minor differences between Bloomfield's and Fries' approaches. The former advocated that children learn letter names and learn to distinguish both capital and lower case letters, while Fries advocated teaching only capital letters in beginning reading. However, in regard to methodology Frics and Bloomfield advocate almost identical content. The same words, phrases, and sentences are found in both sources, for example in Fries, "a cat, a rat, a fat rat, pat a fat rat, cats bat at rats" (6, p. 203). Both Bloomfield and Fries (in the materials referred to above) oppose the practice of making beginning reading instruction a meaning-making process for the. reader. In the words of Clarence Barnhart, coauthor of Let's Read, we find:"Bloomfield's system of teaching readingis a linguistic system.Essentially, a linguistic system of teaching reading separates the problem of the study of word-form from the study of word- meaning" (p. 9). In Fries we find: "Seeking an extraneous interest in a story as a story during the earliest steps of reading is more likely to hinder than to help the efforts put forth by the pupil himself" (6, p. 199)*. When a person in reading raises questions as to the actual contributions made by linguists he is put in the position of a mere educational practi- tioner questioning the role of the scientist. When a linguist has reservations about the contributions of linguists to reading we might listen. Raven I. Mc David of the University of Chicago stated: I am diffident about how much the linguist can contribute to the complex operation of teaching reading, which may utilize everything from informa- tion theory to the doctrine of original sin. Like many of my colleague!, I am 'Certain other linguists do not reject meaning in reading. Lelerre stresses "that language is meaningful behavior and that reading is the reproduction of sound patterns which carry meanings." In addition there has been a considerable body of writing which stresses the various signal systems. Under this heading the linguist discusses it:too:Mims patterns, pitch, stress, juncture, transitions, pauses, and the like. Teachers of reading have used different terminology without neglecting these important facets. (Reading terms :night be. "reading with expression," "read it like you would say it." Juncture pausestops would be taught as functions of punctuation and phrasing.

. 210 1

204 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

disturbed that linguistics is currently fashionable, often considered a panacea for all the woes of education and society. For with the supply of professional linguists low and the demand for their services high, we find a proliferation of store-front linguists clamoring to perform these services... And when the linguist attempts to produce readers, he can expect them to be criticized on both linguistic and other grounds ( /0). The Augmented Roman Alphabet The objective ofthe AugmentedRoman Alphabet is to present an initial teaching mediumwhichapproximates a one-to-one relation between letter- symbolseen and speech-soundheard when a word is pronounced. To accomplish this,19new letter characters were added to the present English alphabet after dropping the letters q andx. (For ITA materials secMazurkiewicz and Tanyzcr,/2.) Some of themethodological features of this system arc: 1.Phonics or letter sounds associated withgraphic symbols arc taught systematically and thoroughly. 2.Child learns symbol sound, not lettername. 3.Children write this new symbol system from thevery beginning of instruction. 4.Separate capital letters arc not used. Capitalsare indicated by making the letter larger. 5.Children are expected to make a transition from this initial teaching alphabet to traditional orthography and spelling withina year; four to five months for the rapid learners; seven to eight months for the average pupils. In data reported from England, the transition was reportedly made without difficulty. However, pupils transferred from Augmented Roman to the same materials printed in regular or traditional orthography. Some Issues: 1.Teaching children to write the Augmented Alphabet. A very important question is, "If the child is to transfer from the initial teaching medium to traditional in from 4 to 8 months, why should he learn and reinforce the Augmented Alphabet in hisown writing?" The ITA is much more difficult to writeas one meets symbols such as: Ch, x, rg, ih, ic, S, cc, to. 2. The compatibility of ITA and Traditional Orthography. In the promotional materials for ITA it is claimed that there isa high degree of compatibility between spellings in ITA and traditional spellings. A fact that is often overlooked is that in addition to the changed alphabet HEILMAN 205

in ITA a great number of words arc changed to phonetic spellings, some of which do not involve any of the new letters. The re-spellings result in visual configurations which arc radically different from those the child will meet once he transfers to traditional English. This is, in essence, not a system for "cracking the code" but rather the teaching of a substitute code. Do no children experience difficulty in transferring from phonetic spellings to the traditional irrational spellings? The following examples come from one small first-grade book of less than primerdifficulty. waswox watched wot1ht walkedwaukt excitedeksietcd enoughend also aulsa: wifewief sixthsiksrh rightnet calledcaulk' findfiend thoughtPut largelurj crossedcrosst next nekst pleased pl«xd climbdiem boxes boksex busybixv night niet Georgejorj quieterkwicter castles - easlx sixsiks (a sccsied holick for ja:n and tceby, .inn Ihwaite, cunStabl anti Cumpanylimited,Iundon.) In the promotional material the question is asked, "Is the traditional alphabet and spelling of English an important cause of failure in beginning reading?" It might be stated that failures do not stem primarily from the traditional alphabet, but from spellings of words. As noted above, ITA. does not rely solely on a new alphabet but actually utilizes a considerable number of re-spellings of English words. ITA actually attempts to follow the traditional "rules" found in most phonic approaches, particularly with regard to the "two vowel" and "one vowel in medial position" rules. When words do not follow the rules they are spelled phonetically: onewun halfhav comecum somesum havehay moneymuny saidsed headhed oncewunz moremor werewer 3. Emphasis on leaching Phonics A point that is seldom stressed on the popular writings about ITA is the 4 fact that in this approach sounding or phonic analysis is taught both early and systematically. A large number of letters (consonants) are the same in structure and sound in both the augmented and traditional alphabets (b, d, f, h, j, k, m, n, p, r, t, y). The digraphs ch, th, sh, wh have the same sound and are very similar in visual pattern, although joining the two

isd912 206 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

symbols in writing is more difficult. The early stress on sounding or phonic analysis thus becomesa con- taminating factor in research which purports to demonstrate thatuse of the Augmented Alphabet is the primary independent variable in com- parative studies. The efficacy of the Augmented Alphabet could be tested only if the traditional alphabet approach with which it is compared included the same amount of early phonics emphasis. 4. Results of instruction using ITA There have been many reports in the popular press which contain suggestions, intimations and projections which often ri:sult in a reader's inferring that initial instruction using ITA, followed by transfer to traditional print sometime in grade one, will lead to significantly higher reading achievement at the end of grade one. The data covering the Bethlehem, Pennsylvania study, 1963-64, reported by Mazurkiewicz does not bear out this assumption, although that writer's tentative conclusions continue to be optimistic. There was no significant difference in reading achievement between groups taught by ITA and traditional basal instruction: TABLE I (II) ITA Traditional Total Raw Scores Mean S.D. MeanS.D. T. Test

California Reading Test 59.6017.42 61.1516.15 0.433 (Lower Primary) California Reading Test 41.11 19.28 41.2916.90 0.064 (Upper Primary)

Summary: The above phonic emphasisapproaches cannot justifiably be labeled "methods of teaching reading." Eachdeals primarily with one important reading skill, namely, phonic analysis.To qualify as a method a set of materials would have to embrace a broad teachingprogram which attempts to deal withdllof the essential skills which needto be taught in the entire program. A second criterionof a method would be that learning to read would of necessity be treatedas a developmental process, extending over a relatively long period of time. Each of the above approaches,as is the case with other phonic materials not discussed, deals only with beginning reading andattempts to saturate this instructional period with analysis of letter sounds.This extensive and intensive drill on sounding letters often hasa salutary effect on reading achievement scores during the early stages of formalinstruction. Stand- 213 HEILMAN 207

ardizcd reading tests for grade one deal primarily with word recognition, and thus tend to measure what phonics-emphasis approaches tcach. Rcading vocabularies of pupils at this stage of development do not approximate their capacity for dcaling with oral language. The point of view of this paper is that this fact of human devclopment does not serve as a justification for the position that beginning reading need not be a meaning-making process. Languagc is a system of agreements as to the meaning ascribed to particular spccch sounds as found in words and words- in-combination. Meaning is the only thing that can transfer from oral language usage to facile rcading of thc graphic representation of oral language. This question, as to whether beginning rcading should be a meaning- making process is one of the major educational issues in thc present controversy over thc proper role of phonics instruction. Starting from the premise that students must acquire the ability to sound out words not recognized nt sight words, othcr questions teachers of reading should answer as / think of methology and materials arc:

1.When initial reading instruction centers on letter-sound analysis is it not likely that a number of pupils will develop a "set" to sound out all words met? 2.Will some pupils continue to sound out the same words long after they should have been mastered as sight words? 3.Is it not true that smooth, facile is characterized by a minimum of recourse to "sounding outout words"? 4. Do some phonic-instructional materials overemphasize what teach- ing letter sounds can actually accomplish in reading English? 5. Does the large number of irregular spellings in English mitigate against teaching over-reliance on sounding? 6. Do phonic instruction materials tend to under-dmphasize the value of combining phonic analysis and context clues in solving unknown words? 7.If a set of materials and instructional procedures results in higher first-grade reading achievement, is this prima facie evidence that this approach is a better learning experience than any approach which achieves lesser results over this relatively short segment of the total educational continuum? 8.Specifically, is the "fast start" which may accrue from hours of drill on analyzing letter sounds inevitably the best introduction to the long-term developmental process called learning to read?

214 208 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OP WORD RECOGNITION

The point of view emphasized in this presentation is that the educational issues in phonics instruction center around (1) initial learning set which materials develop; (2) that letter-sound analysis is limited by the nature of written English; ( 3 ) over-reliance on phonics can be taught; (4) that not to use all word-analysis clues (context, structure, pictures) is uneco- nomical; (5) the sequence in which steps are taught should be basedon a psychologically sound rationale. If one concludes that numerous phonic approaches err in saturating initial reading instruction with letter-sound analysis, and over-emphasize what this can accomplish in learning to read English, the original premise still remains intact. Namely, that children must learn sounding techniques and that these should be taught effectively. Simply because approaches A, B, C, etc. over-emphasize analysis in beginning reading dues not establish that materials which present much less phonic analysis have hit upon the "right combination." To not systematically teach vowel sounds in grade one is a methodological decision which handicaps a majority of pupils in first grade. All reading-instructional-materials are offsprings of the free enterprise system. Promotional materials which describe and sell reading materials are often much more imaginative than the materials themselves. "Let the buyer beware" applies as well to the adoption of teaching materials as it does to the purchase of a used car. Education will have nothing to fear from this philosophy and practice when teachers and administrators develop the professional competency to ask the right questions and evaluate materials in light of their questions.

REFERENCES 1.Bloomfield, Leonard, and Barnhart, Clarence L. Let's ReadA Linguistic Approach, Detroit: Wayne State University Press, 1961. 2.Buchanan, Cynthia Dee, et. al. Programmed Reading. St. Louis: Webster Division, McGraw-Hill Book Co., 1963. 3.Burrows, Alvina T., and Lourie, Zyra. "Two Vowels Go Walking," The Reading Teacher, XVII, (November 1963). 4. Carden, Mae. The Carden Method. Glen Rock, New Jersey. 5.Clymer, Theodore. "The Utility of Phonic Generalizations in the Primary Grades," The Reading Teachcr, XVI, ( January 1963). 6.Fries, Charles C. Linguistics and Reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1963. 7.Gattcgno, Caleb. Words in Color. Chicago: Learning Materials, Inc., 1962. HEILMAN 209

8.Harris, Theodore, Creekmore,Mildred, and Greenman, Margaret. Phonetic Keys to Reading. Oklahoma City: The Economy. Company, 1964. 9.Heilman, Arthur W. Phonics in Proper Perspective. Columbus, Ohio: Charles E. Merrill Books, Inc., 1964. 10. Mc David, Raven I., Jr. "Linguistics and Reading," Reading and the Language Arts, Supplementary Educational Monograph No. 93, Univer- sity of Chicago Press, 1963. 11.Mazurkiewicz, Albert J. "Lehigh-Bethlehem ITA Study Interim Report Six," Journal of the Reading Specialist, Vol. 4 (September 1964). 12.Mazurkiewicz, Albert J., and Tanyzer, Harold. ITA Program. New York: ITA Publications, Inc., 1963. 13. Oakes, Ruth E. "A Study of the Vowel Situations ina Primary Vocabu- lary," Education, LXXII (May 1952). 14.Schoolfield, Lucille D., and Timberlake, Josephine B. Phonovisual Meth- od. Washington, D.C.: Phonovisual Products, Inc.

2 1 6 R. New Alphabet Approaches

EDWARD FRY* the most provable thing about new alphabet approaches is PERHAPSthat they are not new at all. One need only scratch the surfaceof a library, and a cornucopia of revisedalphabets, spelling reforms, and diacritical marking plans for beginning reading pours forth. The history of the development of written language is exceedingly voluminous. There is no time in recorded history when it was not under attack and change. Probably the invention of movable type andthe printing press has done more to slow down the change in writtenlanguage in the last few centuries than any other single factor. In these lastfew centuries we have attained a high degree of rigidity in writtenlanguage. We now find ourselves in the somewhat strange positionof having dictionaries largely agree on exactly what letters should be used to spell each word in our language; it has not always been thus. We could start a discussion of the need and desirability of new alphabets with a look at Cuniform and Linear B, but let us jumpahead thirty centuries to the annual report of the National EducationAssociation in 1910. Your committee is able to make a report of progress that is very encourag- ing to those interested in the simplification of the spelling of English, as the year has been notable in the things that havebeen done and in the coopera- tion that has been secured. .. . The ridicule and the contumely heaped upon the movement five years ago has almost entirely disappeared, and the opposition to a reasonable consideration of its demands and claims is gone. Alas the bright hopes of the educators of 1910 have been dashed on the hard rocks of history. The whole problem of alphabet reform orspelling reform is largely a political one. Any scholar could singkhandedlyimprove English spelling or the alphabet. However, the problem is that people with powerpresidents, senators, giant corporations who publish books, even adults who votedo not want a change because theyalready know the established system. On the other hand, the people who really need the change, the first-graders and the adult illiterates, have virtually no power at all. Apparently the education profession has verylittle power in this grave matter either. The Reader's Problem Lest one feel that we have gotten off the track by discussing simplified First Grade Reading Programs, Perspectives in Reading No. 5, 1965, 72-85. 217 1

FRY 211

spelling in a new alphabet approaches article, let us put the whole area in "perspective." The basic problem of the first grade teacher is to train the child to decode a set of symbols which constitutes what we call written language. This set of symbols is rather closely based on spoken language. The average first grade child already possesses a good knowledge of spoken language. His vocabulary is something in excess of 5,000 words and his knowledge of the rules and structure of the English language is amazingly good. An interesting proof of the first-grader's facility with structure is to compare him with a newly arrived foreign university student who, even though well-educated, uses torturous and often erroneous sentence struc- tures and intonations. Since it is the problem of the first grade student to learn to decode the written symbols, quite obviously the manner in which the language is incoded will have a profound effect. If the symbols follow strange and highly irregular principles it stands to reason that the child will have greater difficulty learning to decode them. Hence, anything that will improve the coding system, even temporarily, for beginning readers has been a concern of educators and many other intelligent people for at least centuries and probably for as long as there has been inaccurate coding systems. Thus, new alphabets, spelling reforms, and devices which temporarily add regularity, such as diacritical marks, are all part of our problem. Spelling is simply the obverse of the same problemthat of incoding, or having to write the symbol, as opposed to decoding, or having to read the symbol. The Alphabet Problem A good bit of the problem of incoding English developed during the 15th century in what scholars call "the great vowel shift." In this period between Chaucer and Shakespeare spoken English went through a notice- able change while written English failed to keep abreast. It was towards the end of this period that the spelling of English became somewhat crystallized due to dictionary makers like Samuel Johnson and technical innovators like Gutenberg. The situation was further compounded by the inadequacy of the sound symbols or alphabet that the English chose to use. The alphabet which we use is really designed for writing Latin, not English. Hence, we note that modern Latin or Spanish and Italian have far fewer problems in spelling than does English. There are something like 44 different phonemes or distinctive sound units used in speaking English; however, our alphabet has only 26 letters and at least three of these are no good at all as they overlap or duplicate the work of other letters (C sounds like an S or a K,

. 218 212 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL. ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

X sounds like KS, and Q is used only with U and sounds like KW). We get around this problem of an inadequate number of letters several ways. In consonants the situation is not too bad as we tend to use digraphs, or a special combination of two letters, to make a different phoneme.For example CH does not make a blend of C and H, but almost always makes the sound heard at the beginning of chair. But in the vowel sounds the situation is much more unpleasant, in that one letter makes many sounds. For example note the A in at, all, above, ale. A Backward Glance It will perhaps help to put the modern systems in better perspective if we have a quick trip through some historical antecedents. In 1551 John Hart wrote an essay entitled "The Opening of the Unreasonable Writing of our English Toung." He later followed this up by suggesting a new orthography, and by 1517 hc had zeroed in on the first grade problem with "A Mcthode of Comfortable Beginning for All Unlearned, Whereby They May Bee Taught To Read English, In A Very Short Time, With Pleasure." In the next ccntury Charles Butler (1634) developed a phonetic alphabet for overcoming "uncertain writing and difficulty in learning." Because hc found that the Latin Alphabet was "troublesome to the novice reader and writer." America's own Benjamin Franklin, that intelligent dabbler in many things, developed in 1786 his own reformed alphabet. While technically not very good, he did see the need forconsistency and the addition of six new letters. Franklin's alphabetapparently never received any acceptance but he did manage to carry on a small correspondence using it. Leaping ahead another century, we find that Bcnn Pitman was producing phonetic readers for the American school market using a greatly modified alphabet. Another 19th ccntury innovation was Fonotypy, a phonetically regular alphabet developed by Isaac Pitman and A. J. Ellis in 1884. We have already noted the interest in spelling reforms around the turn of the century. From that time until this very day there are a number of simplified spelling organizations which actively correspond with each other in simplified spelling and regularly induce some local congressman to put a bill before the United States Congress ordering thatspelling in all government documents be simplified, or proposing to write into law some other scheme which will insure the modification of the American English written language.

219 FRY 213

HISTORICAL "NEW" ALPHABETS AND DIACRITICALLY MARKED READERS

it stand Ann't,

1§ lamp neat The EaffeR and Speediell.way, bath for the I troy (palling and refhog of tiiglifh, as Ala for the Trilewritiiii thereof: a mat the stand that ever was pubbeklz knawn ta this day. See the lamp!Itis on a mat. The mat is on the stand.

MC GUFFEY 1881 HODGES 1644

Ewen Jeri WoRyton woe Dsir Madam aldfickflyx.in meek to rat:Alio abot siks yen old, hie (riter our altalml. "441 it sod di atesda rs7t iwkamoininsui gar him a hm-et,ov hwig he woe and diftlyilis." istisaisral eye; far is ohms alyrs 'CM fond, and woe konttantli Antos mu' rifarnwilyx ss prepared An4yr iw mlidif re,

BENN PITMAN 1855 BENJAMIN FRANKLIN 1768

WUNS UPON A TAM LiTaL RED HEN LIVD IN A BARN AVIA HUR FAV EIKS. A PIG. A KAT AND

ISAAC PITMAN & A.4. ELLIS FONOTYPY 1844

But the attic Einsialfr was pantiles'.In camti the wag& wind. Down to the BO want the lititlf girls. They did not want any sickness.

EDWARD WARD 1.894

. 220 214 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

CURRENTLY USED NEW ALPHABET AND DIACRITICALLY MARKED FIRST GRADE MATERIALS Sample of Primer Printed in ITA "dam% run awc," ben sed tco his cat. "dcalit Piet," miek sed tco his cat. "wzt hoer," sed ben and miek. "WEE will bee at sccsol." Sample of Primer Printed with DMS "Look, Bill," AM Lind& "Here comes Riiky. He is all reedy f6r school. Look up and se0 funny Rieky."

Sample of Primer Printed in Unifon 1EN MoNCT3R HoPP0 TRAD. HI

PoNKT UP MI HibB3RT. HELD HO 41N Hon SSG ORMZ AND CAN

U LULUBA. Sample from first drill book for use with Words in Color

at to ut to

it ti et to

of to England too has had its full share of alphabet and spelling reformers. No less a celebrity than George Bernard Shaw was so seriously interested in spelling reform that he left a major proportion of his estate to a trust which now is working on the simplification of the alphabet. In the late 1950's Sir James Pitman, the head of the publishing firm and a member of Parliament, introduced a bill for greatly modifying the English alphabet. This bill was defeated before it became law but, more or less as a direct consequence, Sir James did receive the blessing of the

9°11..2 A*4 FRY 215

Minister of Educationto experiment with An Augmented Roman Alphabet in the British Schools. Some British parents, being not too well versed in the history of their language, prefer not to have their children learning from a "Roman Alphabet." Hence, the name of this new alphabet was changed to Initial Teaching Alphabet, or as it is popularly called the ITA. The name also reflects the current usage of this alphabet, namely that it is to be used for beginning instruction in literacy and later a transition is made to the traditional alphabet which ITA people like to call TO for Traditional Orthography. Though it would be impolitic to say so publicly, there is probably at least a secret thought in the minds of some supporters that ITA could become the English alphabet to be used by everybody at all times. Needless to say, one clever way of gaining acceptance for this would be to raise a generation of children who had equal facility in TO and ITA. Some future decades from now there might be considerably less resistance to changing spelling or an alphabet if the Prime Minister, the cabinet members, the senators, the heads of corporations and a sizeable portion of the population all had equal reading and writing facility in ITA. An interesting thing about ITA is the wide acceptance of it before adequate research results are reported. Most of the research results (see bibliography) are reported by investigators who are closely identified with the ITA movement and in many instances have authored or published materials in the ITA medium. These research results are almost uniformly glowing. Typical reports show that teachers using the ITA materials have results that are definitely superior to other teachers using the old basal reader or traditional system. While we cannot deny that high reading achievement is a good thing no matter how it is obtained, it is something else again to prove that the superior results were due to this particular alphabet rather than the fact that a new method was used. United States educators have recently had a multitude of claims from super-phonics enthusiasts. There is little doubt that some teacher who has been graying along through her basal reading series and suddenly gets inspired by the new super-duper phonics system is going to get better results. In fact, she would probably get as good results if the situation were reversed and all the humdrum ordinary teachers were using phonics systems and she suddenly discovered a super-duper new hot-shot word reading method. In fact, it must be a little discouraging for the educational researcher to learn that in 1851 the Massachusetts Teachers Association appointed a committee to consider the subject of "Phonetics" and that this committee reported in 1852 that teachers should study the merits of the

22.2 216 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

phonetic system by themselves by actual trial in their schools. When one sees such a basic problem in existence forwell over a century and yet so completely unresolved, the problem of whether or not a new alphabet will really prove superior can hardly be expected to be solved in the nextfew years. The Initial Teaching Alphabet is a 43 character alphabet. It uses24 Roman or Latin characters that Pre used in traditional English (X andQ are missing) plus 19 augmented or additionalletters. Most of the new letters are formed by a fusion of two lower case Roman letters. Five of these new letters, the long vowels, are made by placing a lower caseE immediately adjacer.t to the preceding vowel. Dipthongs, other vowel sounds, and consonant digraphs are frequently similarly formed by placing two letters in close juxtaposition. Writing inthe ITA is also constrained by a special set of spelling rules. Hence, some words using no newletters look different because of the spelling rules only. Both the new letters and the spelling rules add a further constraint in that so far aspossible traditional word form is preserved so that in a later stage of training the students may transfer to the traditional orthography with aminimum of difficulty. In an early paper Pitman estimated that 39.25 per cent of the words were radically changed when written in ITA,10.50 per cent were moderately changed, 23.75 per cent had minor modifications, and 26.50 per cent were unchanged. The goal of ITAis to have a consistent phoneme- grapheme relationship so that one letter will always make the same sound, and vice versa. This goal is not always achieved, partially because ofthe concessions to word form, but it is a tremendous improvement over the traditional orthography. Despite the lack of conclusive research results, the spread of the use of the Initial Teaching Alphabet has been extremely rapid. The firstregular use of the ITA in the publicschool was in Britain in September of 1960. The experimental population consisted of approximately 600 four- and five-year-olds. The British begin reading instruction a year earlier than the Americans. In the school year 1964-65 Pitman Publishing Company, the main publishers of Initial Teaching Alphabet materials, estimates that there were approximately 60,000 British children using ITA. ITA use in the United States began in 1962 in the Bethlehem, Pennsylvania school system with a Ford Foundation grant to Dr. AlbertNlazurkiewicz at Lehigh University. During the school year 1964-65 the author estimates that approximately 10,000 United States children were learning to read FRY 217

using the ITA. Most of these children were taking part in either continuing research in the Bethlehem schools or in research projects sponsored by the U. S. Office of Education. However, there are also many school systems "trying it out" without special foundation or government research grants. Despite these seemingly large numbers this is still far less than one per cent of the U. S. first grade population. Words in Color Another British import aimed at taking advantage of some phonetic regularity principles is the Words In Color system by Caleb Gattegno. This system which has been commercially available for the past five years is used by fewer schools than is the ITA. Proponents of the Gattegno system also make some interesting statements such as, "Words In Color makes the English language phonetic through the use of color, enabling the learner to master the mechanics of reading in eight weeks or less." Forty-seven different sounds of English are taught by using different colors for each sound. Since it is difficult for children, or even adults, to distinguish 47 different colors some of Gattegno's symbols are split in half so that they are really two-colored rather than a unique color. This system does not have the color printed in children's workbook material, but rather there are 21 drill charts containing 270 letters or letter groups that make a phoneme and some phonetically regular words. The teacher drills the student to make the sounds in isolation, then blended together. For example, the first page of the first book simply contains a number of A's of various sizes. The second page consists of a number of U's. The third page consists of groups of A's and U's together which the student would blend as a word. After the five vowels are introduced (short sounds), consonants are introduced and the student can now begin to blend vowels and consonants to form short meaningful and meaningless words. The teacher is also instructed to get a large selection of colored chalk so that she can write letters and words on the board for drill. Spache points out that this idea for the use of color to identify the common sounds was introduced by Nellie Dale in 1899. The memorization of the 47 different sounds with their corresponding colors, as well as the 270 letter combinations which commonly are used in writing these sounds, appears at least on the surface to be the heighth of a mechanical memorization approach to beginning reading. Demonstrations put on by the publisher, Encyclopaedia Britannica, managed to be interesting and lively but, as yet, research proof is lacking for the superiority of this method over any other.

224 218 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

Diacritical Marks Diacritical marking plans to aid beginning readers are not exactly new. One of the oldest currently-used languages, Hebrew, has a system of marks used to indicate vowel sounds for beginning readers. After a certain degree of reading maturity is established, the marks vanish and the reader and writer use only consonants. In 1644 an English schoolmaster Richard Hodges developed a set of diacritical marks to be used with beginning readers which the publisher put forth as, "The easiest and speediest way both for the true spelling and reading of English as also the true writing thereof that was ever publicly known to this day." In the 1890's E. G. Ward authored a set of diacritically marked beginning reading texts published by the Silver Burdett Company for the American schoo;s. In addition to diacritical marks that made the letters- sound relationship much more regular, Ward also underlined groups of letters that were "Phonograms." A large number of readers in the 19th century, including the famous McGuffey series, used diacritical marks for the introduction of new words. In 1964 Edward Fry published an article in Elementary English describing the Diacritical Marking System (DMS) to be used for beginning reading instruction. Later that year an experiment was begun, using the DMS marks on the Allyn & Bacon readers, in seven first grades. This Diacritical Marking System has over 99 per cent phoneme-grapheme regularity and aims to achieve essentially the same goals as the Initial Teaching Alphabet without distortion in word form or change in spelling. The DMS is somewhat simpler than diacritical marking systems found in most dictionaries, since the intention is to aid beginning readers rather than give extreme accuracy. Regular consonants and short vowels are not marked since these are the most common usages of the letters. Long vowels have a bar over them. Regular two-letter combinations which make unique sounds such as the consonant digraphs and dipthongs, have a bar under both letters. Silent letters have a slash mark through them. These marks plus a few others, such as those used for the broad A and other sounds of U, constitute the bulk of the marks used. Nearly every word the child sees in the first grade reading books is marked and likewise all work that the teacher duplicates or puts on the board has the DMS marks. In writing children have the option of using the marks or not. Uniphon Another new phonetically-regular alphabet currently being used with FRY 219

some experiments in the Chicago area is Uniphon, developed by John Malone. This 40 letter alphabet, which the author calls "a single sound alphabet," uses block letters which have an additional interesting charac- teristic of being specially designed so that they can be read by computers for automatic translating purposes. Simplified Use of Latin Alphabet Though not really new alphabets, there are several systems of beginning reading instruction which achieve a high degree of initial phoneme- grapheme regularity by simply carefully selecting the words used in initial instruction. For example, under one type of "linguistic approach" that is putforth by Bloomfield and Barnhart, the child uses only regular consonants and short vowels. The introduction of new words is further graded so that only one short vowel is introduced per lesson. This method of controlled introduction of regular letters, while purported to be "new" in some circles, actually has 4s very close historical antecedent in the NIcGuffey Readers of the 1850's which used a similar introduction of sounds and words in its primers. While a number of other readers of the 1800's used a similar approach, one unique variation was used in the books written with words of one syllable published by McLoughlin Brothers in 1901. Whole books were written using only one syllable words. Methodology New alphabet approaches to beginning reading have some strong inferences for methodology but in and of themselves are only partially a method. For example, the most widely used set of ITA readers in the United States, written by Albert Mazurkiewicz and Harold Tanzer, have a relatively strong emphasis on the "language arts approach." The materials, which consist chiefly of a set of paperbound books with interesting stories not greatly different from a basal series, tend to have a relatively stronger phonic emphasis, but much of the uniqueness of method is due to the emphasis on children's writing activities in the teachers manual. While the authors would claim that it is the regularity of the ITA alphabet which facilitates children's ability towrite, nonetheless the emphasis on the language arts approach is not unique to the type of alphabet used. The DMS methods used in the current experimentation arc largely overlays on the basal series. The actual pages from a basal series have been reproduced after marks have been added. The same basal series teachers manual is available, in fact required, for the teacher's use. The chief 220 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

methodological feature of the DNIS instructional program is the use of a number of phonic charts which accompany the basal series manual. Each time a new story is introduced in a preprimer or primer a small chart is introduced which explains one letter and its corresponding sound. The chart contains a key word (for example the word cat with a picture of a cat) plus several other common words which use the letter being discussed. Regular consonants and short vowels are thus introduced at the rate of one per reading lesson, and gradually digraphs, second soundsof con- sonants, and other vowel sounds are introduced with DN1S marks. After a chart has been introduced, itis left hanging in the rooms so that the children may refer to the chart when coming across a new word. For example, if a child comes across a new word beginning with the letter L and he cannot remember the sound made by the letter, he needs only to look up on the board and find the L chart with its key picture; he may do the same with each of the letters in the word. Conclusion At the present time active experimentation is going on in the United States using theInitial Teaching Alphabet (ITA), the Diacritical Marking System (DN1S), Uniphon, Words In Color, and the gradual introduction of phonetically regular letterssometimes referred to as the linguistic approach. Of the new alphabets the only one which has published research reports is the Initial Teaching Alphabet. These published research reports, both from the United States and England, show that the ITA is definitely superior to the "old method." Whether or not this superiority is dae to the special characteristics of 1TA or the fact that it is a "new method" has yet to be established. Three of these methods, ITA, DN1S, and the linguistic approach, are being included in the large U. S. Office of Education Coordinated First Grade Reading Research Projects, a study now under way. The other two methods, Uniphon and Words In Color are beinggiven field experimentation by their authors or publishers. Hence, all of these methods are being giver. classroom experimentationusing method A classroom vs. method B classroom type of research design. While this type of research design may yield some interesting information, it is also quite possible for it to yield conflicting information such as we have seen coming from the phonics studies over the last hundred years. It is possible that for addi- tional valuable information the reading researcher is going to have to team. up with the experimental psychologist or learning researcherwho designs much smaller and much more carefully controlled studies. For FRY 221 example, is ITA superior to the basal reader method because it is new and because it uses a language experience approach or because ituses a phonetically regular alphabet? Current investigations will probably not satisfactorily answer this question. A further important question might be, "If ITA or DMS or any of the other systems is superior to the traditional method, is the ITA or the DMS the best method of achieving these goals?' While each of the methods that we have discussed has gone through some developmentalgesticulation, each of them is essentially the work of one man. Hence, if one of the new alphabets turns out to be definitely superior to the traditional alphabet the next question should be, "Is this the best possible alphabet to do this type of job?" American educators should seek the answer to this question before any large scale adoption of a beginning reading method utilizin' a new alphabet is seriously contemplated. It is perhaps a necessary step in the evolution of new ideas that there be reasonably widespread interest in the current new alphabets and classroom experimentations of the type now under way. However, for the education profession to demonstrate itself to have achieved a reasonable degree of maturity, much more money should now be spent for research and development of new alphabets rather than wide scale adoption of them. REFERENCES 1.Downing, J. A. The 1TA Reading Experiment. London: Evan Brothers, Ltd., 1964. 2. Downing, J. A. Experiments With An Augmented Alphabet for Begin- ning Readers in British Schools. Pamphlet published on a talk to the 27th Educational Conference sponsored by the Educational Records Bureau, New York, November 1962. 3.Downing, J. A, "The Value of ITA," NEA Journal. Ivan Rose, Part 1; Warren Cutts, Part 2. 4.Downing, J. A. The Augmented Roman Alphabet. A Neu. Trio-Stage Method to Help Children to Learn to Read. Thirteen page pamphlet printed at Pitman Press, Bath, England, undated, circa 1962. 5.Downing, J. A. Too Bee Or Not To Be, The Augmented Roman Alpha- bet. London: Cassell and Company, Ltd., 1962. 6.Downing, J. A. The I.T.A. (Initial Teaching Alphabet) Reading Experi- ment. The Reading Teacher, November 1964. 7.Franklin, Benjamin. "A Scheme For A New Alphabet And Reformed Mode Of Spelling With Remarks And Examples Concerning The Sante and An Inquiry Into Its Uses In A Correspondence Between Miss Stephenson and Dr. Franklin Written In The Characters Of The Alpha- bet," 1768, reprinted in Complete Works of Benjamin Franklin, John 228 222 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL. ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

Bigelow, Editor, G. P. Putnam's Sons, New York, 1887. 8.Fries, Charles C. Linguistics and Reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, Inc., 1962. 9.Fry, Edward. "A Diacritical Marking System To Aid Beginning Reading Instruction," Elementary English, May 1964. 10.Gattegno, Caleb. "Words in Color" (mimeographed). Chicago: Learning Materials Inc., circa 1963. 11.Harrison, Maurice. The Story of the Initial Teaching Alphabet. London: Pitman Publishing Co., 1965. 12.Hodges, Richard. The English Primrose. London: Richard Cotes, 1644. 13.Keller, Linda. Herbert The Hippo (children's book in Uniphon). Chi- cago: Peppermint Press, Inc., 1962. 14.Mazurkiewicz, Albert J. "The Lehigh-Bethlehem ITA Study"(First Year), ITA Report, mimeographed, undated, circa 1964. 15.Mazurkiewicz, Albert J. "Teaching Reading in America Using the Initial Teaching Alphabet," Elementary English, November 1964. 16.National Education Association. Journal of Procedures & Addscsses. Winona, Minnesota: 1910. 17.Pitman, Benn. First Phonetic Reader. Cincinnati: American Phonetic Publishing Association, 1855. 18.Pitman, Sir James. The Ehrhardt Augmented (40 sound-42 character) lower-case Roman Alphabet. The reasons and intentions underlying its design together with a specimen. Twenty-three page printed pamphlet. London: Pitman House, 1959. 19.Pitman, Sir James. "Learning to Read," Journal of the Royal Society of Arts, February 1961. 20.Pitman, Sir James. "Learning to Read. A Suggested Experiment," The Times Educational Supplement, May 29, 1959. 21. Pitman, Sir James. "Spelling With The Augmented Roman Alphabet." Ten-page mimeographed pamphlet circulated by Mr. Downing at teacher training meetings. 22.Pitman, Sir James. "The Future of the Teaching of Reading," Keeping Abreast of the Revolution in Education, edited by Arthur E. Traxler. Report of the 28th Educational Conference, American Council on Edu- cation, 1964. 23. Spache; George D. "Interesting Books for the Reading Teacher," The Reading Teacher, April 1964. 24. Ward, Edward R. The Rational Method of Reading, First Reader. Silver, Burdett & Co., Boston, 1894. 25. No author. Mc Guffey's Eclectic Primer, Revised Edition, copyright 1909, Henry H. Vail, American Book Company, New York. Strategies for Teaching Sound-Letter Relationships

MORTON BOTEL

PiODAY'S TEACHER has a real problem Standards for Teaching Sound-Letter 1in planning and teaching phonics or Relationships word attack. He has his choice of the use 1. The program must provide linguis- of simplified alphabets, color, linguistic tically sound content and structure. programing, "programed learning" ma- 2. The program must provide for self- terials, new phonics programs in basal discovery by the pupil. readers, spellers, language books, work- 3. The program must provide multi- books, filmstrip, tape and computer form. sensory experiences including touch and These alternativeshaveincreasedthe movement. problem of making judgments about what 4. The program must provide pupils should be taught and how itis to be with opportunities for the application of taught. skills in many settings. Not too many years ago, the matter 5.The program must provide oppor- was more simple. Word attack was taught tunitiesfor each pupil to work at his as the fourth or fifth step in a reading own level and rate. lesson. The content was built into the Obviously, the "strategies" referred to design of the lesson by the authors of in the title of this paper have to do with basal readers. With a minimum of sup- a technology which provides strong af- plementation,teachersfeltthatthese firmative answers to the questions sug- guidebook and workbook activities, cor- gested by the standards. related as they were with the story in the For each standard Iwill discuss ele- basal reader, provided the material needed ments of a program that contribute to to do the job. But in the last decade or betterpupilunderstanding of sound- two, methods and materials for teaching letter relationships. pupils how sounds are represented by 1.Is the program linguistically sound? letters have proliferated. This has hap- In the light of present day linguistic pened partly, of course, because of a research, a program designed to teach pendulum swingbackto"phonics." sound-letter relationships should,Ibe- Much vital and productive activity, how- lieve, have the following characteristics: ever, has come from new insights into a.It should include strong emphasis linguistics, the perceptual process, induc- from the beginning on the idea that tive or discovery methods of teaching, and vowel and consonant speech sounds are the overriding concern for intensifying represented by letters. instruction for every pupil at his own b.It should develop this understand- level and rate. ing by programing its beginning vocab- I would like to suggest five standards ulary in large measure on words in which my colleagues and I have used as the there is a one-to-one correspondence be- basis for the development of a program tween sounds and letters. forteaching sound -letterrelationships. c.It should present these words in Perhaps these standards might serve as spelling patternsin which thereisa guidelines for teachers who are develop- minimal contrast in sound and letter rep- ing or evaluating the validity of programs resentation from word to word in the in this aspect of reading and spelling. pattern.

I'istas in Reading, IRA Proceedings, Vol. II, Part I, 1967, 156-159. 224 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

d.It should lead pupils systematically chalkboard: and logically from such spelling patterns to those which are less regularly repre- Patter 1 Pattern2 Pattern 3 sented. hat hat hat mat hit had e.It should include the study of vowel sound-vowel letter relationships practical- We ask pupils (1) to read the words ly from the beginning. in the pattern aloud, (2) to sort such f.It should immediately provide oral words as hat, cat, hut, ham and hot into experience in which the words studied the pattern sets, (3) to extend the pattern are used in phrases and sentences so that sets with other words that belong, and pupils become aware of the need for (4) to formulate the rule. Actually pu- thinking of the way the words sound in pils who can sort given words into the the larger intonational setting. right patterns and who can find other g.It should be flexible enough to in- words that belong to the patterns, have clude words which are less regular or "found the rule" in the most essential even irregular in the early stages if these sense. The ability to verbalize the pattern words are needed to make language more orformulate therule should not be interesting, more meaningful, or more hurried. Indeed, it is questionable wheth- like the sound of language. eritis helpfulatallto many young Inshort,linguistic research supports pupils or to slow pupils to focus on the two major ideas which must be wed in abstract statement of the rule. programingsound-letter relationships: Here are further pattern contrasts pro- (1)it must capitalize on the consistent gramed into the What's My Rule? form. sound-to-spellingcorrespondences and Notice thai they provide minimal con patterns orrulesinAmerican-English trast between words not only within a spellings, and (2)it must immediately pattern but also from pattern to pattern: provide contextualsettingsfor words studied that cause them to be used in a more natural linguistic environment. Pattern 4 Pattern 5 2.Isthe program structured to pro- bat bet vide for self-discovery by the pupil? All mat met of us as teachers respect the importance sat of discovery. Discovery is one of the most rewarding experiencesinteaching.It might be called the "Aha!" experience Pattern 6 Pattern 7 Pattern 8 ... a gasp, thelight dawns! "I've got it!" tan tin ten These are events teachers strive to bring pan pin about. How isthis done inteaching man sound-letter relationships? As we have noted already, pupils must Here are further examples of patterns understand that spelling patterns repre- designed for discovery activity. Patterns sent sounds. We want this understanding 9 to 12 maintain the one letter-one sound to grow by their involvement in and change in successive words and between exploration of these patterns. In this way patterns as a way of introducing the spell- pupils will be led to discover significant ing patterns of long sounds. sound-letter relationships.Ishould like Pattern9 Pattern 10 toillustrate a way of programing for pan pane discovery through patternstudies. We man mane sometimes call such exploration the game of "What's My Rule?". can Put thefollowingpatterns on the 1 -1" v

BOTEL 225

Pattern11 Pattern 12 are too strongly directed toward the ver- pan - 4 pain balization of the rule rather than to the ran - 4 rain pattern of words which are the substance man - 4 of the formulation of the rule. But even when the primary emphasis The more complex aspects of sound- is on the pattern itself and when a dis- letter relationships in English are grad- covery technique is used, we still will not ually presented in related patterns like get transfer to reading and spelling unless these: we program for it. Application of what the pupil learns about spelling patterns Pattern 13Pattern 14Pattern should be provided for in several differ- hope snow boat ent settingsin context and in lists. home low coat Let us use the silent e spelling pattern mole yellow soap as an example: Here are some ways in which we can help pupils make this pat- in which pupils get to know the alterna- tern a tool for independence in word tive ways a consonant or vowel sound attack: (long o in above patterns) is represented a.In independent wide reading. Have in English. pupils read in self-selected materials. In 3. Does the program provide multi- such books, the transfer occurs naturally sensory experiences including touch and in a highly motivated reading environ- movement? Certainly our knowledge of ment. Certainly many new and unfamiliar the psychology of perception should sug- words will belong to the silent e pattern gest a strategy for improving the quality and with the help of the context and the of perceptual response to words and to high interest, such words will be attacked spelling patterns. in their most natural setting. This medi- In the earliest experience with words um for application is like going into the and patterns, all pupils should have ex- water to practice a new swimming stroke. perience in tracing and writing as well as Is there a better way? seeing and saying these elements. Fur- b.In contrived sentences. Have pupils ther, for those pupils whose memory is read sentences made up to give emphasis poor as evidenced by lack of recall or to the principle. In such sentences, all weak recall from day to day, tracing and words except the new ones should be writing should be extended. known. At one time it was widely believed I hope to get a jump rope and kite that tactile-motor experiences in learning for my birthday. words should be restrictedto the lan- Please write your name, the date, and guage disabled pupil. Today, we are more place you came from. and more coming to the view that such In the dim light, he saw a dime in perceptual training. should be develop- the. street. mental and preventive. He made us mad when he spit in 4. Does the program provide oppor- spite of the warning. tunities for application of skills in many c.By er;anding patterned lists. We settings? It is quite common for teachers have already indicated under the discov- to say, "Johnny knows the rule when we ery method how pupils may extend any work with it on the board, but he can't patternedlistsas,farasthey can by apply it when he meets a new word in finding words that fit. his reading or spelling." d. By having pupils invent and read The reason for a pupil's inability to aloud nonsense words.Perhaps pupils apply rules in part resides in the way in can give meaning to these words by re- which rules are taught. Frequently, pupils lating them to naming some new discov- 7

226 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION ery in medicine or interplanetary travel. materials in which pupils read with at sane least 95% oral fluency and with at least Gibe 75% comprehension. dufe b. Using discovery activities such as e.By having pupils read aloud pat- those described earlier in which open- terned lists of words with clues built in. ended activities provide automatically for cap cope different levels and rates. cape cape c.Testing to determine which spelling tape tape patterns pupils have mastered and which tap take need to be reviewed or introduced. tapping bake In summary, by considering such mat- snapping brake ters as linguistic validity, self-discovery, snipping broke perception, transfer of learning, and in- snipe bloke_ dividualization of instruction, the cur- sniping riculum builder will be able to develop f.By having pupils spell dictated lists or adopt programs which deal efficiently and sentences which include words that andeffectively with the problems of fit the silent e pattern. sound-letter relationships. g.By having pupils spell words in REFERENCES self-dictated tests.In such phrases and sentences as those below, pupils spell the 1.Botel, Morton; Holsclaw, Cora; Brothers, Aileen.Pattern, in Spelling and Writing, word called for. These words are cued Levels A throughF;3140 Important by a sound-spelling representation of the Words;and1620 Power Words. Chicago: word, by context, or both. Follett Publishing Company, 1963. to /tayst/ the food 2.Fries, CharlesC. Linguistics and 'Reading. a pretty snow /seen/ New York: Holt, Rinehart and Winston, Inc., 1963. on the ice to slip and 3.Hall, Robert.Sound and Spelling in Eng- The Navahos are an Indian /tr____/. lish.Philadelphia: Chilton Co., 1961. 5.The program mutt provide oppor- 4. Hodges, Richard E. "The Case for Teach- tunities for each pupil to work at his own ingSoundtoLetterCorrespondencein level and rate.The word attack program Spelling," Elementary School Journal, 327 - must be tailored to the individual differ- 336, March 1966. 5.Lefevre, Carl A.Linguistics and the Teach- ences in level and rate to be found in ing of Reading.New York: McGrawHill, every group. Many ways should be com- 1964. piled by the teacher or by a staff for 6. Lloyd, Donald J., and Warfel, Harry A. accomplishing this task of intensifying American English inits Cultural Setting. the instructional process. Three very im- New York: Alfred A. Knopf, 1956. portant techniques in this category in- 7. The ReadingTeacher."Linguistics and Reading." Newark, Delaware: international clude: Reading Association, December 1964. a.Placing pupils at their instructional level in basal readers and textbooks; i.e., The Teaching of Word Analysis through Perceptual Conditioning

GERALD G. GLASS

T IS MOST important that we carefully both the student and the teacher a spuri- Iand objectively examine what isin- ous idea of the actual ability of the stu- volved in the teaching of word analysis. dent in word identification. It leads to ap- It is this writer's objective to demonstrate parent ability vs. actual ability. It is best that many of our "approved" methods that while the student has the helpful are either unreasonable or unwarranted in guidanceoftheteacheravailablehe light of the tasks involved. Some of these should not also have the contextual aids may actually be detrimental to the devel- available. Most often if one can "sound opment of proficiency in word analysis. out" out of context one will surely be able The following points need to be made. to identify the word with the added clues The teaching of word analysis should of context when the reading is for actual not be considered the teaching of read- content learning and not word analysis ing. Reading, at even its simplest level, training. Many of the word analysis dif- is responding to meaning. Just "calling" ficulties which are "discovered" by mid- a word is not reading. (We took a pound dle grade and junior high teachers are the of Flesch when someone said it was.) In result of the context's becoming too diffi- a sense word analysisis a pre-reading cult to shelter the previously concealed ability.Itcouldbeconsideredthat weakness in word analysis. "reading" comes into play only after one It is dishonest to say that words in our knows the sound of a word. Actually, language can be sounded out phonetically, even if this were not accepted, the prob- i.e., letter-by-letter sound. Other than that lem of meaning in word analysis work there are over 2,000 ways the twenty-six below the middle grades would rarely be letters of our alphabet can be sounded. a problem. This writer surveyed the new The sounds of the vowels (the most dif- words introduced in major basals and (to ficult problem in word analysis) are usual- no one's surprise) close to 99 per cent of ly controlled by their place in the syllable. the words were already within the average So, one cannot discover the sound of any youngster's vocabulary. Because of this vowel before first determining the syllable an efficient word analysis "method" need structure of the word (structural analy- not include the teaching of basic mean- sis). The e in pepper can only be known ings, for to do so may be superfluous. (if you are really using "approved" tech- Word analysis work may actually be niques) when you decide that the ris hampered by having the word analysis within a first syllable and is followed in skills taught utilizing words within a the syllable by a consonant that is not part phrase or sentence context. Too often is of a digraph. Only after this process of the context (and not the early grader's syllabicationcanphoneticanalysisbe ability to discover phonic and structural used. However, it is also dishonest to ad- elements in words) a major factor in vise the use of syllabication as a technique identifying the correct sound of a word. in word analysis. The narrowness of its Words in context may undermine the rules, the contradictions, the complexity need of a developing word analysis skill's of its "ifs," the mental gymnastics neces- coming to the lore and being practiced. sary to store information in order to de- Context (and pictures)frequently give cipher a word of two or more syllables, Reading and Inquiry, IRAProceedings, Vol. 10, 1965, 410-413. 228 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION makes it ludicrous to suggest that the tech- one) will say "a pencil." Actually, the ob- nique be used in word analysis. (In fact, server will see a great deal more. Your one set of rules actually requires that you fingers, hand, arm, shoulder, head, torso, know the sound of the word[motor, the area behind and around you, etc. are rapid] before you decide where the break in his view. But he has "learned" to dis- occurstodiscoverthe sound of the criminate out of the field based upon his word!) Fortunately itis of little import. learned mental sets. These factors suggest That people use syllabication to discover that in word analysis itis possible, and the sound of a word is a myth now too maybe even natural, for a child to find long in the literature (almost every meth- a three- and four-letter combination such ods book on the teaching of reading). as ing, ate, and ight as easy to learn as Syllabication, in populations totaling over singleletter phonic sounds; such phono- 500 adults and children, was applied after grams may be seen as one whole rather the word was identified, never (yes, not than three or four separate wholes. In ad- once) before the sound of the word was dition, as one learns to "see" only a cer- known. To this point the writer seriously tain part of a complete view depending questions the necessity of the teaching of upon one's mental set, one might be able meaning with word analysis, the use of to look at an unknown word like distena- context and contextual clues in the teach- tinning and sec only what one has learned ing of word analysis, and the use of syl- to see. labication and phonic principles. Remem- The reader is advised to say the un- ber, we are concerned with word analysis known word aloud. Did you read the and not with the operationally defined word at sight or did you sound the word problem of teaching reading. out? You sounded it out. Did you use Other factorsthat, because of space syllable and phonic rules such as vccv, limitations, can only be mentioned here, vcv, open syllable and closedsyllable, should be taken into consideration when two vowels together, etc.? Of course not. attempting to explore what is involved in Did you notice the little words in'the big learning and utilizing word analysis tech - one and use the sound of these words in niques. the analysis; e.g,at,on, in, nation?I The Gestaltists have long ago made us hope not. aware that the size of a configuration has What then was used? The reader had little or nothing to do with the person's nochoice.He has beenconditioned ability to "learn." It is the familiarity and through his consistent and extended deal- the meaning one can bring to the object ings with words used in American writing, that will determine what one will do with uniquely to notice clusters of letters in it. The eleven letter word grandmother an unknown word that seem naturally to for obvious reasons is less difficult than come together. We see (and hear) with- the four letter word here. in the whole word dis/ten/a/tion/ing or We know that objects composed of dis- possibly d/h/t/en/ation/ing, but never crete elements initially can be seen as the Me/ or /ena/ or /tio/ or /nin/ wholes and then, if necessary, seen as (all of which are orthographically pos- composed of various parts. A child, by sible). Any activity, then, which fosters the nature of his being, first identifies a seeing the "correct" clusters would con- car, a house, and a toy before he needs tribute a great deal toward developing to deal with the parts which make up the word analysis. proficiency. whole. The belief of this writer is that un- We can "learn" to see what we want to knownwordsarecorrectlyidentified see and subordinate what we do not. Hold through a process which utilizes a condi- a pencil up to someone: and say only tioned perceptual set that scans a word for "What do you see?" Everyone (yes, every- itsstructural sounding elements(letter GLASS 229 clusters) as they have been historically in words correctly so that they will be consistent in the reader's experience. The able to transfer these !earnings when they reader of this article is asked to examine read unknown words. the unknown words "blassment, trepula- The whole wordmustalwaysbe lion, from and deplistrationer." It is most presented. Parts of words should not be certain that you "read" the words letter- covered up then exposed. Letters should cluster by letter-cluster, as you have learn- not be added on to structural elements to ed these letters arrange themselves in your form the whole word. The elements experiences with the language.Itisal- (phonograms) must be seen with the let- most asifthere were unique spacing ters before and after as when seen during between the common structuresinthe reading. The ong in isolation is not the word. You probably used nothing more same perceptual image asthe ong in than your instant (conditioned) knowl- stronger. Readers must perceptually iso- edge of these "phonograms." (For words late the letter clusters within a word (as which have structures that do notfit a with distenationing). consistent pattern [police, great] no word The teacher directs how a pupil will analysis method can apply). If we agreed examine a word. She always directs the upon how successful readers identify un- examination with two crucial questions to known words,it would be fruitfulto develop mental set. These are: "What backtrack and attempt to develop ways letters make what sounds?" "What sounds of learning word analysis which would be do what letters make?" For example, in consistent with what we desire the end re- the word black, what letters make the sult to be. buhll sound; what letters make the ack An approach called "perceptual con- sound? In the word black what sound ditioning" has been developed to be con- does the bl make? What sound does the sistent with the positive factors which seem ack make? Here we have required the to be valid in word analysis. The "meth- student to examine the word in a way od" requires youngsters to examine known that we know will be of benefit to him words (out of context) both visually and when he sees other words with the same auclitorially in a way that would foster the phonograms. We requiredthat he ex- correct visual clustering of letters. Mental amine the letters(cluster fashion) and sets to see "correctly" are controlledand apply a sound. We did not allow him to fostered by the teacher'scomplete di- examine the word in any way other than rection of how a youngster is to examine what we think would be consistent with a word and identify the sounds within it. the way he will examine words when he A note should be made here. In the word becomes proficient in word analysis. He analysis process itis important not only did not, and never will be allowed to, to identify sounds in a word and the let- insult the integrity of a digraph, phono- ters that make that sound, but it is crucial gram, or any of the meaning or pronunci- that a reader see letters and then come up ation units which are structurally con- with the sound that the letters make. One sistent in our written language. If the is from auditory to visual and the other youngster is consistently required to ex- from visual to auditory. Both skills are amine whole words in terms of these com- necessary for effective word analysis. mon internal structures (e.g., team, dream- ing, streamline) utilizing the mental set Perceptual Conditioning questions, he theoretically must condition to the phonograms as they are visually Words to be used must be within the isolated as a part within the whole word. child's listening vocabulary and listed out This, as has been suggested, is just what of context. The child should either know the whole word or be toldits name. we want. Youngsters are learning to identify sounds The question that immediately comes 230 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

to mind is how many of these phono-has a place in a syllable or any other such grams or letter clusters do we have in our"rule.Careful count was made of the language? Are they so numerous that it consonant blends and the consonant di- would be unreasonable to become condi- graphs. It was found that when teaching tioned to their existence in words. Or, in word analysis through perceptual condi- fact,do theyreallyexistfrequently tioning, these sound elements are learned enough for us to depend upon them in relatively easily compared to the vowel sounding out unknown words? This writ- phonograms; that their random distribu- er and his staff have intensively examined tion throughout words which are con- the words used in the basals of the first trolledjustforthe vowel phonogram three grades. All new words were ex- gives enough repetition to bring about the aminedtodetermine structuralsound conditioned effect when the consonant ele- make-up. It was discovered that less than ments are setn within a whole word. 100 vowel phonograms are included five To date the approach has been used by or more times in the new vocabulary of the the writer, his colleagues, and graduate first three grades. Only 42 vowel phono- students. The results have been gratifying grams are used 10 times or more. If these enough for us to do continued research. vowel phonograms could be consistently Lists of words have been developed which identified in whole words the youngster include the specific vowel phonograms will have the vowel sounds introduced in that are to be worked with during the over 90 per cent of the new vocabulary. lesson. For a ten-minute period pupils are For example, it was discovered that lug directed to examine selected words accord- was repeated at least 109 times in new ing to the rigid requirements discussed. words; er 105 times; ar 47 times; ad 17 If this is doneconsistentlyfor two ten- times; ear 6 times; ore 5 times. (The com- minute periods a day, each of the five pleteset of approximately 100 vowel days in a school week, the writer predicts phonograms and their frequency is avail- that within a three- or four-month period able throughthe writer.) The reader the average first-grader will be able to should recall that the writer feels that it is analyze unknown words with the profi- just as easy to learn that three or four let- ciency of the average third-grader. The ters make one sound as it is to learn that only words he will not be able to sound one letter makes a sound. Also, it should out would be those words which need to be noted that if clusters of letters are be learned at sight. There is evidence that taught as producing a sound, rather than this approach should be considered where single letters, the problem of teaching vow- early training in reading is desired. Be- el sounds and their variations is virtually cause it minimizes factors of an emotional eliminated. A vowel here makes a sound and intellectual nature it may be "easier" as part of a letter cluster not because it and therefore applicable.

ri,tv"1 Teaching Structural Analysis

LEO M. SCHELL

TO TEACH CHILDREN how to figure out the pronunciation and/or mean- ing of an unrecognized wc,rd through the use of phonic and structural analysis is one important goal of reading instruction. Yet Clymer (1963) has shown that many phonic generalizations lack either validity or fre- quency of application and that consideration should be given to some possible revisions in the commonly taught content of phonic analysis. Recently, Wink ley (1966) has raised some similar disturbing questions about the worth of certain generalizations about accenting. It there- fore seems a propitious time to examine some of the problems concerning the content of structural analysis as found in both professional methods textbooks and basal reading series.

The meaning of affixes Methods textbooks typically list prefixes and suffixes which the ele- mentary school pupil should know. Usually, itis not clear whether it is the pronunciation or meaning or both which pupils should know. If it is the meaning which is intended, some problems arise. Few prefixes are valuable for meaning. Four prefixes commonlyrec- ommended to be taughtand their meaningsarc: con- ...together, with, very ex- ...out of, from, beyond, without pre-...before in time re- ...backwards, again Yet, consider the plight of the intermediate grade pupil trying to apply what he has been taught to any of these words commonly found in elementary school textbooks: conserve, exclaim, present, and resin. The obvious problem is that these prefixes have meaning to an ele- mentary school child only when there is a known base word. Even though in Latin these prefixes were attached to base words, over the years these bases have lost their independent standing and. the prefixes have become known as "absorbed prefixes." In such words, knowledge of the meaning of the prefix is virtually worthless. Suffixes present difficulties in two ways. Many suffixes have multiple meanings; e.g., -ment may mean act, condition, or concrete instance. To use the suffix to help derive the word's meaning, the reader must1] recall these three meanings,2] choose the appropriate one, and 33

The Reading Teacher, 21 (November 1967), 131612.7-.- 232 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

apply this meaning to the base forma prodigious task for most ele- mentary school pupils. Surely, there must be a more efficient procedure. Suffixes are most valuable to the reader when they are affixed to a known base word; e.g., development. But, in some cases it does the reader little good to detach the suffix in order to locate the base word because the base changes forms when a suffix is added, e.g., admission and deceptive. In such instances, recognition and knowledge of the meaning of the suffix seem of little value in determining the meaning of an unrecognized word. Inaccurate instruction Not only are there difficulties inherent in the construction and mean- ing of affixed words, but the accuracy of the content taught is sometimes questionable. Instructional techniques frequently fail to distinguish be- tween reading and spelling.For example, one prominent reading specialist writes, "When a word ends in y following a consonant, y is usually changed to i when adding an ending as in cried, ponies, tinier, happily" (Smith, 1963). This rule goes from the base to the inflected form, a spelling sequence. The reading sequence should go from the inflected form to the base. Perhaps it isn't too confusing to children, but it seems to indicate some lack of understanding by those engaged in teaching teachers. It appears that sometimes itis not clear whether pronunciation or syllabication comes first. The senior author of one of the best selling basal series writes in his professional text, "Lead pupils to note that the vowel sound (in "paper") is long and therefore, the first syllable is probably "pa" (Gray, 1960). Similarly, another basal series author writes in her professional text, "When two vowels conic together and each keeps its own sound, they form separate syllables, as pi-o-neer" (Hester, 1964). Both of these principles imply that the pupil must pronounce the word before he can syllabicate it. But, if he can already pronounce the word, why should he need to syllabicate it? Furthermore, the latter generalization is a tautology: the dependent and independentclauses say the same thing, just in different words. Thc definition of a syllable is such that this statementdoes not really tell one anything; it only masquerades as information. Unfortunately, most principles of syllabic division are based on the vocabulary entry in the dictionary rather than on the respelling, a pro- cedure excellent for composition but questionable for reading. Troup outlines the historical process by which the present system. evolved and points out that The way a word is pronounced has nothing whatsoever to do with the

. 239 SCHELL 233

way it is divided at the end of a line in writing.... Over the years, edu- cators set up a kind of a phonics system based on a mechanical practice that was never intended to relate to speech.' In any dictionary, for example, you will find the word vision divided vi-sion in the entry and re- spelled the only way it can be pronounced in English, the first syllable being vizh. (1961, p. 142). Pupils are told that affixes form separate syllables and then asked to divide and pronounce words such as "building" which everyone pro- nounces in context as bil-ding rather thanbild-ing. It is surprising there is not a complete generation of skeptics, unwilling to accept what is told them. Maybe teachers are lucky students do not take instruction too seriously! Directions for how to use structural analysis to unlock an unrecog- nized word may be inefficient even if not inaccurate. One fourth grade basal workbook tells pupils to syllabicate the word and then look for affixes (Russell,1961).Since affixes are predominantly monosyllabic, it seems easiest to locate known affixes first. This leaves only the base word to be syllabicated. Practice exercises in workbooks, basal series, etc. leave much to be desired. One fifth grade workbook presents this principle, "If a word ends in le preceded by a consonant, the consonant is included in the last syllable," and then asks the pupil to apply it to the word "single" (Unknown,1961).A widely used methods text uses "don-key" as ap- propriate practice for the rule about dividing syllables between medial consonants (Bond and Wagner,1966).These words may follow the respective generalizations, but they would be of little help in pronounc- ing an unknown word. Valuable principles? It may he a waste of pupils' time to teach than certain structural principles. One methods text recommends teaching this accenting gen- eralization:"In multisyllabic words, the first or second syllable has either a primary or secondary accent (su-per-vi'sor, re- spon'si- bil'i -ty)" (Harris,1962).If the reader syllabicates correctly and then accents in- correctly, is he at much of a disadvantage? The purpose for learning accenting principles is to allow the readei to recreate the sound of a word he already knows, to activate its auditory memory. Should the reader give the first rather than the second syllable a primary accent, it hardly seems that this minor deviation will hinder the desired recrea- tion. Having children learn this principle seems to be the educational equivalent of "overkill." From here, where? The foregoing discussion was not intended to be a comprehensive survey of all the problems associated with the content andmethodology 234 INSTRUCTION IN SPECIAL ASPECTS OF WORD RECOGNITION

of structural analysis. It was meant only to highlight, to call attention to, certain facets of the topic which deserve critical scrutiny. Some sug- gestions for dealing with these problems seem warranted. That pupils should be trade aware of the limited applicability of the meaning of prefixes seems an obvious first step. But, if it has ever been previously suggested, it has never been practiced systematically. Intermediate grade pupils should have a realistic perception of what they canand cannotdo with their learnings. To help them attain this goal, after introducing and practicing on prefix meanings, it would be wise to piesent and discuss some common exceptions, e.g., disaster, illusion, and uncouth. In dealing with the meaning of suffixes, perhaps it would be more efficient to stress the grammatical function of the suffixed word in the sentence than to teach the meaning of individual suffixes. For example, rather than teach that -a/ means "pertaining to" and that therefore musical means "pertaining to music," have pupils examine the location of "musical" in the sentence and note its relation to other words. In "We went to a musical comedy," the grammatical function of the word is an obvious cue to the grammatically informed that music had a dominant role in the play. To rectify inaccurate instruction, there must be more precise under- standing of how structural analysis aids in unlocking unrecognized words. Possibly better than teaching a set of separate and independent rules (tactics), concentration should be on a general technique (strategy) applicable to various situations. The first three steps of such an approach could be: 1-:-Visually- loenie.and. isalate- any- recognized affixes. 2.Ifthe base word is not recognized, syllabicate it. 3.Determine the vowel sound in each syllable. Thisapproach begins with the affixed form and goes to the base form, a realistic reading procedure. It also treats syllabicationas an integral part of phonics, which is its correct role. It seems probable that linguists with their emphasison the sub- ordination of writing to speaking may provide a more functional set of principles governing syllabic division than thosenow used. Teachers need to know howbestto handle the syllabication of words such as connect and how to cope with the problem of neutral(schwa) sounds in unaccented syllables. The most practical way to teach accenting generalizations would seem to be to coordinate them with dictionary work rather than to present and practice them in isolation. This approach not onlygives pupils independence with unrecognized words in their listening vocabu- lary, but also helps them pronounce words not already in their listening

. 241 SCHELL 235

vocabularythe two functions of accenting. "Two birds with one stone." There must be studies using the techniques of Clymer (1963) and Wink ley (1966)to provide information about as yet uninvestigated areas of structural analysis and to extend their ideas to other material and grade levels. However, twiddling collective thumbs until someone produces this information hardly seems the proper attitude. Teachers need. to be selective in what they teach and how they teach it. They should feel free to omit a principle that has little applicability or to revise suggested procedures so they are correct and accurate. A teacher is primarily a decision maker and should not be shackled to suggestions in teachers' manuals. There seems to be sufficient evidence that some of the content of structural analysisis incorrect and seldom applicable and that some current methodology may be inefficient and questionable. All teachers must be aware of these shortcomings and must search for more accurate content and better methods of instruction.

References Bond, G. L., and %Vaguer, Eva. Teaching the child to read. New York: Macmillan, 1966. p. 167. Clymer, T. The utility of phonic generalizations inthe primary grades. The Reading Teacher, 1963, 16, 252.258. Gray. W. S. On their own in reading. Chicago: Scott. Foresman, 1960. p. 128. Harris, A. J. Eflective teaching of reading. New York: McKay, 1962. p. 369. Hester, Kathleen. Teaching every child to read. New York: Harper & Row, 1964. p. 149. Russell,I).H., and McCullough, Constance M. My do and learn book to at company roads to everywhere. Boston: Ginn, 1961. p. 23. illy do and learn book to accompany trails to treasure. Boston: Ginn. 1961. p. 13. Smith, Nila Banton. Reading instruction for today's children. Englewood Cliffs. N. J.: Prentice. Hall. 1963. p. 225. Troup, Mildred. Controversial issues related to published instructional materials. Controversial issues in reading and promising solutions. Supplementary Educational Monographs, No. 91. 1961. 135-144. Winkley, Carol K. Which accent generalizations are worth teaching? The Reading Teacher, 1966, 20, 219-224.

. 242 4 -.7%

PART V: Articles Partially Related or Very General Word Analysis and Comprehension

DoNALn A. BENZand Roman' RosEmm.R*

BECAUSE OF THE IMPORTANCEplaced on reading ability in typical classroom learning experiences, educators have been greatly con- cerned over the procedures by which reading ability is developed. While some investigators have considered the format of presenta- tion and methods for teaching reading. others have been interested inthe particular skillsinvolved in reading development. The present study. as one of the latter type, employed two methods to determine the relationship between performance on tests designed to measure certain reading skills and their relationship to reading comprehension at the fourth grade level. One method involved observations of the ways in which chil- dren of high comprehension levels differed from children at lower levels. This particular grouping was selected rather than one based on the factor of intelligence, such as those which might result in over-under achiever groupings or classification into those of high- average-low IQ. Although these latter types would seem appropriate for the study of effective teaching methods with various levels of student abilities, such as those performed by Dolan (1963) and Strand (1956 ). it was felt that a clear insight into the fundamental elements of reading comprehension could come only from study of children who were classified by their comprehension level alone and not by any other criteria. If those at a low comprehension level were observed to perform significantly lower on a skill than those at a higher level, this skill would seemingly be related to reading comprehension ability. It might be inferred then. and could subsequently be studied experi- mentally, that additional work on this skill could aid the develop- ment of reading comprehension. The second method of determining the relationship between performance on a skill and reading comprehension involved the computation of partial correlation coefficients. These partial corre- lations can be interpreted in essentially the same way as the more familiar simple correlation coefficients, except that these partialS would indicate the relationship between two variables when the confounding effects of the other variables were removed. This tech- nique is especially valuable when one realizes that much of an observed relationship between two variables may result from the mutual dependence upon a third variable. If the effects of the third The Reading Teacher.21 (March 1968). 558.563.

244 BENZ 239

variable were to be removed. the coefficient would give sonic meas- ure of "pure" relationship between the first two. In the present study, the partial correlations were computed between reading comprehension scores over all levels and scores on each of six tests of word analysis skills while removing the effects of performance on the other five skills. The subjects The participants consisted of all fourth grade children in six towns and cities in five northeastern states. These fifty-five class- rooms of 1490 children included what the authors believed to be a representative cross-section of school environments. Included were a college town ( n273), a resort village ( n = 148), a state capital (11,-115).an industrial-professional community (n.---,274), an agricultural center ( n = 248 ), and a so-called "bedroom" community serving a nearby metropolitan center (n = 432). The specific cities were essentially selected by random proce- dures. with stratification according to geographic location, size, and environment type. Although none of the communities was consid- ered **culturally deprived", a wide range of socio-econornical levels did exist among and within the systems considered. Likewise, these schools represented a wide variety of instructional methods for teaching reading. with varying degrees of emphasis on word analy- sis skills. The wider sampling would allow more generalizing of findings than would a more restricted sample which represented only one method for teaching reading. The number of classrooms in the systems ranged from five to eighteen. The population appeared to be only slightly above normal (mean IQ107) on the basis of intelligence test scores which were available for 94 per cent of the population. The instruments Each fourth-grade class was administered the Gates (1958), Level of Comprehension Test, Type LC, to determine reading com- prehension and the Bond, Clymer. Hoyt (1955), Silent Reading Diagnostic Tests, Form ll-A, to measure proficiency in syllabication, locating root words, words in context, word elements, beginning sounds, and rhyming sounds. The classroom teachers were trained to administer the tests which were given in consecutive half-day testing sessions. Children scoring between 6.0 and 9.2 on the Gates test were considered high level readers (n=474); those between 4.6 and 5.9, middle level (n=450); and those from 2.0 to 4.5, low level( n = 478.).Eighty-eight subjects were dropped from the

245 240 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

analysis either because of absence during testing or failure to achieve the minimum level reported on the comprehension test, thus leaving 1402 children available for analysis. The analyses and results The statistical test of analysis of variance was conducted to determine whether or not differences existed between these three comprehension levels of readers for each of the six word-analysis skills. When a skill showed a significant difference among the three levels, a t test was conducted between pairs of levels to determine which levels differed from one another on this skill. The results of this first phase, presented in Table 1, indicate that on the basis of mean scores, the high readers tend to perform better than middle readers, and that middle readers tend to perform better than low readers on each of the six word-analysis skills.

Table 1Meanscores for high, middle. and low readers in fourth grade on six word-analysis tests

Skill High Middle Low L Words in context 25.5 23.3 20.6 2. Syllabication 16.0 13.2 10.7 3. Root word 23.8 22.1 19.1 4. Word elements 26.7 24.9 22.6 5. Beginning sounds 27.6 26.2 23.9 6. Rhyming sounds 25.6 22.7 19.1

All comparisons between adjacent groups within a skill were statistically signficant at the .01 level. It might be concluded that performance on these word-analysis skillsis related to reading comprehension level, assuming of course, that the opportunity to learn these skills did not favor a particular comprehension level. The validity of this assumption, which confounds every investiga- tion within existing classrooms, is questionable, and could be the sole or major cause of skill differences among comprehension levels. However, until better measures of opportunity as reflected in the ability to learn are derived and can be tested for importance, this conclusion seems at least partially justified. The previously-mentioned partial correlation coefficients be- tween each of the six word-analysis skills and word comprehension were computed over all levels, with the effects of the other five word-analysis skills statistically removed. These values are shown. in Table 2.

fi BENZ 241

Table jPartial correlation coefficients between tests of each of six word-analysis skills and reading comprehension

Skill Coefficient 1. Words in context .5548 2. Syllabication .4300 3. Root word .3230 4. Word elements .2651 5. Beginning sounds .0975 6. Rhyming sounds .4793

For this group of fourth grade children, the skills of words in context, rhyming sounds, and syllabication attained the higher partial correlation coefficients with reading comprehension. The coefficient for the skill of beginning sounds appears much lower than the coefficients for the other five skills. An equation of multiple regression (prediction) ) was also com- puted for the six analysis skills with reading comprehension as the criterion. The weights with which each skill entered Lao the regres- sion equation also allowed a ranking of the skills (Ezekial, 1959). These weights appear in Table 3.

Table 3 Weights (beta) assigned to each word-analysis test in predicting reading comprehension scores

Skill Coefficient 1. Words in context .3189 2. Syllabication .1830 3. Root word .1102 4. Word elements .0673 5. Beginning sounds .0101 6. Rhyming sounds .2295

Again it is observed that the three skills of words in context, rhyming sounds, and syllabication have the larger values and that the other three skills (especially beginning sounds) have smaller values. The multiple correlation coefficient (R) between the reading comprehension scores which could be predicted from this regres- sion equation and the actual reading comprehension scores attained by the subjects provides a global picture of the importance of the

247 242 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

selected variables as a group with respect to all the variables which might have been considered. This view is attained by squaring the multiple correlation coefficient (R2), which indicates the proportion of fluctuation in reading comprehension scores %vhich may be accounted for by fluctuation in the six word-analysis skill scores. For the six word-analysis skills. the multiple correlation coefficient was .734, indicating that approximately 54 per cent of the variation in reading comprehension could be accounted for by these variables. By employing an extension of a formula by Tate (1955) this percentage can be subdivided into the component parts from the unique contributions of each of the six skills and from the joint effects of the skills. These percentages are shown in Table 4.

Tubb,4Per cent of variation in reading comprehension contributed by the word-analysis skills

1 2 3 4 5 6

1. Words in context 10.2 6.4 3.5 2.2 8.3 2. Syllabication 3.4 1.9 1.3 4.5 3. Root word 1.2 2.1 4. Word elements ' 1.8 5. Beginning sounds 6. Rhyming sounds 5.3 Note:I) diagonal values represent unique contributions. offdiagonal represent joint effects Note: 2) ,these variables contributed less than 1 per cent Approximately 20 per cent of the variation in reading compre- hension at this grade level (for this sample) can be accounted for by the unique contributions of the skills of words in context, syllabi- cation, root word and rhyming sounds, while 33 per cent can be accounted for by joint contributions of the six skills. It is probably this joint influence that caused the skill of beginning sounds to be adequately discriminated by comprehension levels (analysis 1), while allowing it to appear virtually unimportant in explaining variation in reading comprehension (analysis 2). Further investigations of the relative influence of these skills on reading comprehension should be conducted at othergrade levels. These studies along with experimental verification, may sug- gest that some skills could be more fruitfully taught at certain grade levels than at others. Of incidental interest, the preceding analyses were also con- ducted by statistically removing the effects of intelligence. Since intelligence scores were available for only 94 per cent of the sub- jects on a variety of intelligence tests, the results from this portion

4110 BENZ 243

would, at best, be only grossly indicative of theeffects of this variable. When intelligence was considered in the analysis,none of the six original determinations varied bymore than .02. The varia- tion in reading comprehension attributableto the seven variables was raised only to 57 per cent. Although the improvementresulting from the addition of the variable of intelligencewas slight, this variable became the third highest in accountingfor variation in reading comprehension. Implications Because both phonic and visual skills were among those relat- ing most closely to reading comprehension, these results would further support the need for employing both kinds of skill instruc- tion in primary grade reading. Since all word-analysis skills (except for. perhaps. beginning sounds) seemed important at this grade level (Lc. contributed to differential comprehension scores). it is recommended that each teacher make every attempt to identify the status of skill attainment among children in his classroom and make individual efforts to improve these skills. possibly by changing the order in which these skills arc typically taughtor the relative empliasi:, they receive. In general, it would seem that certain skills can be identified as "most influential" in affecting reading compre- hension at this grade level and that good proficiency in these skills is highly desirable. Further research would seem vital in identifying causes why certain skills pose difficulty for certain children. Of particular challenge would be the derivation ofan answer to the question of whether or not curricular or instructional practices could be altered to improve the word skill performances of "low" and -middle" level readers, thus significantly improving their read- ing comprehension performance.

References Bond, G. L.. Clymer. T.. and Hoyt, C. Silent reading diagnostic tests. form D.A. Chicago: Lyons and Carnahan. 1955. Dolan. Sister Mary Edward. A comparative study of reading achievement at the fourth grade level under two methods of instruction: modified linguistic and traditional basal. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. The University of Minnesota. 1963. Ezekial, M.. and Fos. K. A., Methods of correlatiou nod rearessioa aaalysis linear and curvilinear. New York: John Wiley, 1959. Pp. 192197. Gates. A. 1. Level of comprelten. test. type LC. New York: Bureau of Pub. Watkins. Teachers College, Columbia University. 1958. Strand. Helen A. The differential contribution of specific word recognition techniques to reading ability at various levels of educational advancement. Unpub- lished doctoral dissertation, 'rho University of Minnesota. 1956. Tate. Merle W. Statistics in Education. New York: The Macmillan Company, 1955. Pp. 316-318.

249 Linguistics and the Teaching of Reading

1)01.01tES 1)119KIN*

IIEMES and titles of recent articlesguistics), and of comparisons among (6, 8, 10), and even of some notthem (comparative linguistics). so rcccnt ones (I, 11), continue to call attention to a linguistic approachThe Linguist and Reading to the teaching of reading. Now, too, Early concern shown by a linguist a few reading texts "based on a lin-for the teaching of reading can be guistics approach" are beginning tofound in two articles by Professor trickle from the presses. Before theLeonard Bloomfield, both published reading field gets swamped by stillin 1942 in Elementary English (/). another panacea,itisimperativeSubsequently, but especially through- that ample time and careful thoughtout the fifties and into the sixties, the be given to the very basic ques-number of similar kinds of articles, tion, "What, in fact, is a linguisticeach generally offering a proposal for approach?" tcaching rcading, has grown steadily. Any effort to deal with such aAt the same time the interest of pro- question immediately suggests a stillfc.ssional educators in these proposals prior one; namely, "What is linguis-has also grown. Now, in fact, the tics itself?" The quickest and theinterest can be characterized as being most general answer is to say thatwide and enthusiasticbut, generally the field of linguistics is concernedspeaking, not as probing and con- with a scientific study of language.structively critical as it might be. One major focus of this study is the What factors have encouraged this structural features of the sound sys-kind of reaction to the pedagogical tems of language. Here, there is in-proposals of linguists? Many factors, tcrestinthephysicalfeatures ofof course, but two seem particularly speech (phonetics). There would also influential. be concern for the identification and The first factor is the ever present study of the sounds oflanguage one of too easy acceptance of cure- (phonemics). ails when the thing to be "cured" or The second major focus of linguis- improved is as baskally important as tics is the structure of language on areading ability. The second factor is grammatical level. Here, attention is more modern for it is rooted in the given to word order patterns, intona-tcmper of the times in which we now tion, and inflection. live. This is a tcmper that continues At times, both of these major focito beget not only great respect for also include a study of the evolution specialistsbut, sometimes, even a of particular languages (historical lin-naive kind of homage toward them. The Heading Teacher, 16 (March 1963). 312.3.16. DURKIN 245

And, when homage rather than crit-might be incorporated into the teach- ical respect predominates, two tend-ing of reading, and (2) pedagogical encies result. One is the tendency forproposals that might be investigated, pseudospecialists to talk and to writeA third category suggested can be as if they were specialists. To a small labeled as "hoped-for contributions extent this has happened in current that are yet to come." attempts to bring together linguistics and the teaching of reading. Technical Findings A second and equally undesirable The technical findings having spe- tendency fostered in this climate of cial relevance for reading are pri- unquestioning adulation isfor spe- marily concerned with the language cialiststospeak"authoritatively" of teaching reading, in particular, of about a field that is actually outside teaching phonics. Here, the possible their own area of competence. And contribution of the linguists lies in it is toward this tendency that many correcting misconceptions. linguists have drifted as they now For example, it is routine to hear make proposals for school programs in a classroom such pedagogical talk in reading. Such a tendency in any as "The letter b has this sound," or area of study is unfortunate, mostly "The letterssit have that sound." because it is unfruitful. It too often In response to this, linguists correctly leadstoresponses ofrejectionin point out that letters do not have which the authoritative as well as the sounds; they are not "pronounced." nonauthoritative proposals are lost. Rather, they suggest, it is oral speech To avoid, therefore, the loss of help thatiscomprised of sounds, and for reading that might come from these sounds are then represented or the field of linguistics, itis essential recorded by various letters of the to distinguish between valid proposals alphabet. that are the direct outgrowth of this Another illustration. References to specialization, and those which are the long sound of o or, for example, only the conjectures of individual to the short sound of a are common linguists.Once thedistinction is occurrences inphonics instruction. made, the first set of proposals will In this instruction the terms are used merit serious attention from reacting to refer to a particular sound repre- specialists. The second set might serve sented by o, and to another particular to suggest new directions for research sound represented by e. - -or, perhaps, old paths that must Not so, say the linguists. Instead, be trudged again,this time more they explain, the terms "long vowel" carefully. and "short vowel" actually refer to Linguistic Publications the duration of a vowel sound as it With the question "What help for is heard within a particular word. reading?" as the focus, a careful sur- According to this linguistic classifica- vey of linguistic publications suggests tion, therefore, the letter c represents there are (1) technical findings that a long vowel sound in a word such 246 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL as be, but a short sound in a word attcntion to thc largcr structures of like beet. Or, to cite anothcr example, our languagc ... and muchless to thc letter o would rccord a long sound `phonics' and word mcthods.... in road but a shorter sound in rote. Children who arc taught reading With the hclp of linguistic publi- with thc main emphasis on largcr cations other examples could bc given pattcrns would be expcctcd to dc- of misconccptions about languagc, vclop thcir own generalizations of especially prcvalcnt in thc teaching spelling-sound relationships... ."(8). of phonics. But, essentially, the qucs- Role of the Linguist tion raised by all of thc examples is Thc position takcn by this writcr whether it makes good pcdagogical assumcs that thc gap bctwccn lin- scnsc to shift, at this point in time, guistics and instructionalmcthods from traditional but technically in- for rcading is wide, and that thc gap corrcct usage to more prccisely ac- will bc bridged most effectively and curate language. Regardless of thc sccurcly not by proclamations like dccision, of coursc, thc linguist ccr- those cited above, but only by the tainly has the right to suggest cor- findings of careful research. Thc posi- rcctions that arc dircctly conccrncd tion also assumes, and cvcn includes, with the conceptions and thc lan- thc hope that some of the foci of this guage of his specialization. rcscarch will comc from pcdagogical But, it might bc askcd at this point, proposals now being madc by lin- does the linguist also have thc right, guistsmany of which, it must bc qua linguist, to issuc proclamations addcd, havc been made ovcr thc about how reading oughttobc years by rcading spccialists. taught? For instance, is this particu- lar linguist "out of his ficld" when Pedagogical Proposals hc suggests that "Thc entire struc- What arc somc of these proposals? ture of English graphemics can easily In addition to thosc already citcd, bc presented in tcn elementary rcad- thc most frcqucnt proposals of lin- ers, which can be covered in not over guists arc notcd bclow, each followcd by a listing of somc of thc questions two years...,after which a pupil of ordinary intelligence canstart that nccd to bc askcd about thcm. rcading in the third gradc what is 1. The vocabulary of beginning normally read in the fifth" (5 ). texts in reading ought to include only words that show a regular cor- Is this next linguist also going bc- respondence between their spelling yondhisspecialization when hc and their sound. (What, in fact, is claims that "Thc ability to rclatc thc a "regularly spcllcd" word? Arc lin- melody of speech to thc written pagc guistsreadytoprovideuswith is thc kcy to good rcading" (9). groupings of words that show grad- What about the following proposal ual change from thc most rcgular from still anothcr linguist:"I bc- to thc most highly irrcgular? Docs hew: we should pay much greater thcproposal amount to an ovcr-

r: ck A .# DURKIN 247 simplification of our spelling system? children use pictures as a source of Will this kind of introduction to read- help with word identification? Would ing make children less flexible and, the omission of pictures make for less therefore,less ready to cope with interesting texts? Would the omission the many words in our language that of pictures make a text unlike the ma- show an almost capricious kind of terials children read outside the class- spelling?) room? And, ideally, shouldn't the 2. In initial reading instruction, situation in which a skill is learned concentration should be on single, and practiced be typical of the one regularly spelled words, and even on in which the skill is to function?) nonsense syllables. (Doesnot the call- The few proposals noted here, ing off of lists of individual ords, combined with a study of other pro- and even of lists of syllables, inevi- posals made by still other linguists, tably become monotonous and also suggest two conclusions. First, there meaningless? Does. the proposal en- seems to be no such entity as "a lin- courage a return to the rote drill guistic approach to the teaching of that prevailed inthe earlier years reading" and, secondly, many of the of this century? Will it lead to over- individual proposals of linguists pose concentration on individual words important questions which are still and insufficient attention to mean- without an answer. ings of groups of words, sentences, and paragraphs?) Hoped-for Contributions 3.The style of textbook language Even a limited knowledge of lin- ought to be like the style of children's guistics also suggests, however, that spoken language.( What, precisely, is it is still a kind of specialization which meant by "style of language"? Might ought to hold much for the teaching it not be that "style" varies greatly of reading that has not yet been among children?Forinstance, realized, but which is needed. For wouldn'tdifferentfamilyback- example, itis the linguist who can grounds make for major differences offer scholarly help with the impor- inspeech styles?Ifcertainstyles tant job of teaching reading to bilin- were clearly identified and if, in turn, gual childrennot so much by ad- they became a part of reading texts, vocating particular methods as by would this necessarily simplify the identifying major similarities and dif- task of teaching reading? Would it, ferences in the alphabetical system, for example, improve reading com- and in the grammatical structure of prehension? Would the material ex- the child's two languages. cite new interest in learning to read?) It is the linguist, again, who can 4.Pictures should be omitted from help teachers gain competence in reading texts. Helpful cues for word developing newer kinds of language identification should come from let- study. Even inelementary school ter-sounds, not pictures.(To what classrooms attention could be given extent, and in what kinds of ways, do to the history of American English, 248 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL to the factors that have affected itsown area of competence, will the development, to the histories of in- field of linguistics be given optimum dividual words, and to the ways in opportunity to make genuinely pro- which pronunciation and meaningsductive contributions to the field of of words have changed over time.reading. For too long, it seems, the most in- teresting aspects of language study have been unknown to teachers, and thereforeunavailabletochildren. References Linguists have the ability, and the I.Bloomfield, Leonard. "Linguistics and Reading."Elementary EnglishReview, opportunity, to bring this study into 19 (Apr.. May 1942) , 125130, 183-186. 2.Bloomfield.L.. and Barnhart. C.L. focus now. Let's Read: A LinguisticApproach. In addition, the linguist can also Detroit: Wayne State University Press, offer much more guidance than he 1961. 3. Carroll, John B.The Study of Lan has thus far given in helping teach- gunge.Cambridge: Harvard University ersorganize and systematize,for Press, 1933. 4. Dawkins. John. "Reading Theory An themselves and ultimately for the Important Distinction."Elementary children they teach, both thc regulari- English,38 (Oct. 1961) , 389.92. 5.Hall. Robert A., Jr.Sound and Spelling ties and the irregularities found in inEnglish. Philadelphia: Chilton Com- our writing system. With this kind pany. 1961. Page 30. 6.King, Harold V. "Linguistic Aspects of of systematizationand with a will- the Reading Program."Language ingness on the part of teachers to Learning,9, No. 1(1959). 19.23. .7.Lelevre. Carl A. "Reading Instruction study the resultsphonics instruc- Related to Primary Language Leant. tion, for example, could become much ings: A Linguistic View."Journal 01 Developmental Reading(Spring 1961) , more productive because it would be 147-158. so much more knowledgeable and 8. Lelevre. Carl A. "Language Patterns and Their Graphic Counterparts: A secure. Linguistic Vicw of Reading," Changing Concepts of Readinginstruction, IRA In Summary Proceedings, 1961. Pages 245-249. 9. Lloyd. Donald J. "Reading American The hope expressed in this article English Sound Patterns," A Monograph is that the specialist in linguistics will forElementary Teachers No. 104. Evanston, Illinois: Row, Peterson, 1962. be willing to share, with both pre- Page 2. ciseness and simplicity, findings that 10. McDavid. R. I. **The Role of the Lin- guistinthe Teaching of Reading," arc the fruit of his special areas of Changing Concepts of Reading lnstruc study. It is also hoped that educators lion,IRA Proceedings, 1961. Pages 253 256. will study these findings, and then II.SoMetti. James B. "Why Children Fail test those proposals that seem to have to Read: A Linguistic Analysis." Har- vardEducational Review(Spring 1955) , special relevance for the teaching of 63-84. reading. Only with this combination 12. Womack. Thurston, "Is English a Pho netic Language?"Elementary English. of specialists, each working in his 34 (Ot. 19V) , 386-88. The Effectiveness of Code- and Meaning-Emphasis Beginning Reading Programs

ROBERTDYKSTRA INher recent book, Chall (1967) concluded that code-emphasis reading programs tend to produce better overall reading achieve- ment, at least in the initial stages of instruction, than do meaning- emphasis programs. She defined code-emphasis programs as those which aim at the beginning to teach the pupil mastery of the alphabetic code rather than expecting from him a mature reading performance. Meaning-emphasis programs, according to Chall, are those which emphasize from the very beginning the necessity of reading for meaning, undoubtedly a more mature skill than mere code-breaking. The typical basal reading series belongs to the meaning emphasis category. Many current programs, however, are characterized by an early concentrated emphasis on learning the alphabetic code which characterizes printed English. This is especi- ally true of a number of recently-published "linguistic" programs. The Cooperative Research Program in First-Grade lnstructioli provided considerable data with which to test Chall's conclusion. Many of the projects which participated in this research venture compared the relatiVe effectiveness of basal programs and certain innovative instructional programs. a number of which belong to Chall's code-emphasis category. The Coordinating Center at the University of Minnesota reported, in two separate volumes, the results of the combined analysis of the data which compared basal and various other programs at the end of grades one and two (Bond and Dykstra. 1967; Dykstra. 1967). The present report draws together specific data from the Cooperative Research Program pertinent to the issue of the relative effectiveness of code-emphasis programs in initial reading instruction. PROGRAMS Information about three relevant types of reading programs evaluated in the first grade reading studies is presented in Table 1. Programs are categorized as conventional basal, linguistic, and phonics-first basal in accordance with Chall's classification scheme, The Programmed Reading Series was not labeled "linguistic" by Chall, but because of its claim to be linguistically based and its similarity in many respects to linguistic materials it has been placed in that category for purposes of this report.

The Reading Teacher,22 (October 19 68), 17 -23. 250 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

Table IClassification of reading programs used in the cooperative research study according to publisher, common label, and certain instructional variables. Conventional Phonics Basal Linguistic ApproachesFirst Basal Variables 2 3 2 3 4 1404, Goals of Beginning Instruction: Reading for Meaning (M) or M M M C C C C Learning the Code(C) Motiv. Appeal at Beginning: Content (C) or Process (P) of C C C PPP P PIC Learning to Read Major Criterion For Selecting Words: Meaning Freq. (MF) or MF MF MF SR SR SR SR SR Spelling Regularity (SR) Vocabulary Load: First Year LOW LOW LOW HIGH HIGHHIGH HIGH HIGH Phonics Instruction: Analytic (A) or A AA A A S S Synthetic (S) Phonic Load: First Year LOW LOW LOW HIGHHIGH HIGH HIGH HIGH Cues to Use: Structural (S) or M M M SS S S Meaning (M) "Set" For Regularity (R) or Diversity (D) D D D R R R R Structural Clues Employed: Sounding and Blending (SB), Visual Analysis and VAS VAS VAS SP SP SB SB SB Substitutions (VAS), or Spelling (SP) Response Modes: Whole Words (WW) WW WW WW LET LET LET LET LET or Letters First (LET) I° Scott, ForesmanThe New Basic 2 MerrillBasic Reading Series Reader: Developed on Linguistic Principles 2 GinnGinn Basic Readers 3 SingerStructural Reading Series 3 Allyn and BaconSheldon Basic 4 McGrawHillProgrammed Readers Reading I**BarnhartLet's Read 1*** J. B. LippincottBasic Reading Certain of the instructional variables which differentiate the three types of programs arc presented in Table 1. More complete descriptions of these variables are found in Appendix A of Chall's DYKSTRA 251

book. It is apparent that conventional basal, linguistic. and phonics first basal programs are differentiated by a number of instructional variables such as vocabulary load, type of vocabulary control, phonics load, and initial response modes. The first row of the table indicates that conventional basal programs are characterized by an early emphasis on meaning. while the various linguistic programs and the phonics-first basal program belong to the code-emphasis group. It was possible, therefore, to make certain comparisons of the relative effectiveness of meaning-emphasis and code-emphasis instructional materials in beginning reading. The experimental design utilized by the Coordinating Center enabled comparisons between basal and linguistic materials and between conventional basal programs and phonics-first basal series. The rationale underlying the analysis is presented in detail in other reports of the Cooperative Research Program (Bond and Dykstra, 1967; Dykstra, 1967). The analysis conducted by the Coordinating Center utilized data collected from various projects participating in the research program, thereby giving information about the relative effectiveness of various types of materials across a number of projects. Achievement was measured inthe Cooperative Research Study by means of a number of instruments. Oral word pronouncia- tion was measured by the word list from theCates-McKillop Read- ing Diagnostic Testand the Fry Phonetically-Regular Word List which was developedspecificallyfortheresearchprogram. Accuracy of connected oral reading and rate of reading were assessed by theGilmore Oral Reading Test.TheStanford Achieve- ment Testwas used to evaluate spelling, silent reading, word recognition, and silent paragraph comprehension.

FINDINGS The findings of the analysis comparing basal and linguistic programs are reported in Table 2. Relative performance of basal ( meaning-emphasis ) and linguistic ( code-emphasis) pupilson the various measures is reported for both the end of gradeone and the end of grade two. At the end of first grade, 1357 pupilswere used to evaluate spelling and silent reading ability. This number dropped to 959 by the end of the second grade. Approximately 250 pupilswere used at both testing points to analyze oral word pronounciation, accuracy of of al reading. and reading rate. Linguistic pupils were better in oral word pronounciation and silent reading word recognition at the ends of both grades one and 252 Airrictils PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

Table 2 Reading and spelling achievement of pupils in code-emphasis and meaning-emphasis reading programs. Programscompared Conventional Basal Conventional Basal ( Meaning Emphasig) (Meaning Emphasis) Achievement variahles vs. Linguistic vs. Phonic/First Basal (Code Emphasis) (Code Emphasis) Oral Word Pronunciation Grade 1 CODE CODE Grade 2 CODE Accuracy of Oral Reading Grade 1 MEANING CODE Grade 2 MEANING Spelling Grade 1 MEANING CODE Grade 2 CODE CODE Silent Reading Word Recognition Grade 1 CODE CODE Grade 2 CODE CODE Silent Reading Comprehension Grade 1 MEANING CODE Grade 2 MEANING CODE Rate of Reading Grade 1 MEANING CODE Grade 2 CODE two. Pupils in the code-emphasis linguistic programs were also better spellers at the end of grade two, although the reverse was true at the end of the first grade. Therefore. it is apparant that early emphasis on learning the alphabetic code resulted in superior ability at decoding words in isolation as well as superior ability at encoding spoken words by the end of the second grade. It should be emphasized. however, that not all of the differences favoring either linguistic or basal pupils were statistically significant. The analysis was very complex because of the number of projects involved and in many cases it was impossible to come up with a simple straightforward comprehensive test of the relative effective- ness of the two treatments for this reason, the data presented in this article may best be used to illustrate trends. Details concerning the significance of observed differences are presented elsewhere ( Bond and Dykstra, 1967; Dykstra, 1967). Differences in accuracy of reading a connected passage orally and in understanding paragraphs read silently favored the basal pupils at both testing points. This finding lends some support to the view expressed by many reading authorities that concentrated early emphasis on learning the code to the virtual exclusion of reading for meaning may have a negative effect on comprehension, However, the differences favoring basal pupils on the second- 2.58 DYKSTRA 253

grade comprehension test were negligible ( Dykstra. 1967. P. 105). The evidence concerning reading rate is less clearcut. Basal pupils were faster readers at the end of grade one, but pupils whose initial instruction had been in linguistic materials were reading at a higher rate by the end of the second grade. Therefore, it does not appear that an early emphasis on learning the alphabetic code necessarily produces halting word -by -word reacting at least through the second grade. The Cooperative Research Study also evaluated the relative effectiveness of conventional basal materials and the phonics-first reading series. Chairs analysis of this series indicated thatit differs from conventional basal materials primarily in its approach to teaching and practicing new words. In other aspects of the instructional program. however. Chall found that the phonics-first basal system did not differ greatly from conventional basal readers. The comparative effectiveness of these programs in terms of first-grade and second-grade reading and spelling achievement was evaluated. All of the performance measures at either testing point favored the code-emphasis phonics-first program. Code-emphasis pupils were superior in the word recognition and comprehension skills involved in silent reading alter one year and two years of instruction. They were also better spellers at both testing points. Information regarding reac"ng rate, oral word pronounciation, and accuracy of oral reading is available only at the end of the first grade.wherealldifferencesfavoredthephonics-firstcode- emphasis group. Furthermore, practically all of the differences reportedfor thisparticular comparison of code-emphasis and meaning-emphasis programs were statistically significant( Bond and Dykstra: 1967: Dykstra. 1967 ). Pupils comprising the sample for the first-grade reading measures numbered 191, while the sample used for evaluating first and second grade silent reading and spelling totaled 1013 and 441 respectively.

DISCUSSION Data from the Cooperative Research Program in First-Grade Reading Instruction tend to support. Chairs conclusion that code- emphasis programs produce better over-all primary grade reading and spelling achievement than meaning-emphasis programs. This superiority is especially marked with respect to pronouncing words orally in isolation, spelling words from dictation, and identifying words in isolation on a silent reacting test.It is apparent that concentrated teaching of the alphabetic code is associated with improved initial ability to encode and decode words. This evidence

. 259 254 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

reinforces the view that pupils can be helped to learn sound-symbol relationships. It is difficult to make conclusions about the relative effective- ness of analytic and synthetic phonics programs. The relatively successful code-emphasis programs utilized both types of instruc- tion. Evidence is also inconclusive about the relative effectiveness of unlocking a new word by sounding out the word and blending it together versus spelling the word letter-by-letter as advocated by the Bloomfield-Barnhart and Fries materials. The code-emphasis pro- grams differed on this point. yet were relatively successful as a group in producing pupils with above-average word recognition skills. For further information on this point it would be well to look at individual studies which evaluated separately each of the types of code-emphasis programs or which compared two or more code- emphasis programs( Schneyer. 1967: Sheldon. 1967: Ruddell, 1967; Tanyzer, 1966: Hayes. 1967). The relative effectiveness of code-emphasis and meaning- emphasis programs in influencing ability in reading comprehension is still somewhat ambiguous. Taken as a group pupils who learned to read by means of conventional basal readers were slightly superior in silent reading comprehension to pupils whose initial instruction had been in linguistic materials. However, this finding was reversed in the comparison of conventional basals with phon- ics-first basal materials. In this latter comparison all differences favored the code-emphasis phonics-first basal program. It should be noted that Chall in her analysis of various types of instructional programs found the phonics-first program to differ little from conventional basals in its emphasis on comprehension. follow-up activities for a lesson, teacher guidance in reading. and similar variables. Perhaps this indicates that itis essential to direct the beginners' attention to a variety of reading tasks and to stress understanding of what is read in addition to developing the ability to decode words. Evidence seems to indicate that some direct early instruction in the more mature aspects of reading behavior may be helpful. Conclusions regarding the influence of code-emphasis and meaning-emphasis programs on rate of reading are likewise ambig- uous. At the end of the first grade pupils in linguistic programs were slower oral readers than pupils in basal programs. By the end of grade two, however, this finding was reversed. Pupils in the phonics-first code-emphasis program read at a higher rate than conventional pupils at the end of grade one. but no evaluation of rate of pupils in these programs was reported at the end of grade cen DYKSTRA 255

two. On the basis of the limited information available, there appears to be little reason for concern that first-grade and second-grade pupils in code-emphasis programs become slow, halting readers. Longitudinal data are necessary to test the long-range consequences of the two types of programs. Similar problems exist in drawing conclusions from the data on accuracy of connected oral reading. Basal pupils read more accurately than linguistic pupils at both testing points. However, phonics-first basal pupils read more accurately than conventional basal pupils at the end of grade one. Here again the evidence can best be termed conflicting. It is likely that other variables peculiar to certain code-emphasis programs account for the lack of unani- mity in the findings. NEEDED CLARIFICATION Although this study supports, in general. Chall's conclusions concerning the superiority of code-emphasis programs in beginning reading, a note of caution is in order. There is no clear evidence that the early emphasis on code per se is the only or even the primary reason for the relative effectiveness of the code-emphasis programs. The major types of programs which were compared differed in a number of respects in addition to the varying empha- ses on code and meaning. The possibility exists that some other characteristic of these programs (higher expectations of pupil achievement, for example) may be a more crucial element in determining pupil achievement than the emphasis on code-break- ing. It is also possible that some particular combination of factors within the code-emphasis programs accounted for their effective- ness. There is some evidence for this conjecture in that the various code-emphasis programs did not appear to be equally effec- tive. Unfortunately, studies of the nature discussed in this report compare one complex of instructional factors with another com- plex of instructional factors, thereby making it impossible to isolate the single characteristic ( if indeed there is one) which makes one program More effective than another. Researchers interested in this question will likely have to turn to laboratory investigations.

REFERENCES Bond. C. L.. and Dykstra. R. Final report of the coordinating center for first. grade regaling instruction. (USOE Project X -001) Minneapolis: University of Min- nesota. 1967. Bond. G. L.. and Dykstra. R. The cooperative research study in first-grade reading instruction. Regaling Research Quarterly, 1967. 2 (4), 9-142. Chall. Jeanne. Learning to read. New York: McCrawHill, 1967. P. 137. Dykstra. R. Final report of the continuation of the ccardinating center for 256 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

first -grude reading instruction programs.USOE Project 6-1651) Minneapolis: Uni- versity of Minnesota. 1967. Hayes. R. B.. and Wuest. R. C. 1.t.a. and three other approachesto reading in the first gradeextended into second grade.The Reading Teacher.1967. 20, (8), 694-697; 703. Ruddell. R. Reading instruction in first grade withvarying emphasis on the regularity of grapheme - phoneme correspondences and therelation of language structure to mea g--- extended into second grade. TheReading Teacher,1967, 20. ( 8 ). 730-736. Sheldon. W. DNichols. Nancy. and Lashinger. D. R. Effect of first grade instruction using basal readers, modified linguistic materials, andlinguistic readers extended into second grade.The Reading Teacher,1967. 20, (8), 720-725. Schneyer. J. W. Reading achievement of first grade childrentaught by a lin- guistic approach and a basal readers approachextendedinto second grade.The Reading Teacher. 1967.20. (8). 704-710. Tanyzer. H. J.. and Alpert. 11. Three different basal readingsystems and first grade reading achievement.The Reading Teacher,1966. 19, (8), 636-642. Linguistics and Reading Problems at the Junior High School Level

CHARLES C. FRIES*

Al+ol'TTI R F. years ago the follow- school loci. One can. of (muse. blame ing headline appeared in The Neu. the textbooks and demand that the books I'm.k Times. attached to an article con- be brought to thelc el of the pupil's cerning the New York City School Sys- reading competence. If. hi Avec cr. such .1 tern. realistic reading testing does not occur at 10,000 in 7th Grade Found the junior high school le% elitis simply 4 Years Behind in Reading postponed to a later time andmore dif- In other words, a tremendous number of ficult body of materialto the senior hiLh boys and girls of twelve years of age can- school, to the college, or to the reading not read well enough to do the studying level required for a job other than that required beyond the third g:acif... of unskilledLth r.Potential ''dropouts'' Two years later a new superintendent often can be identified by such reading ofthatsameschoolsysteminsisted, difficulties at the beginning of the junior "There is just no sense at all in pretend- high school level. ing to teach world history and biology The remedy proposed in the quotations to a tenth grade student who reads at the seems limited to setting up smaller classes fourth grade level.- And he proposed, for reading instruction and to doubling for "the child who slips behind in learn- the time given to these classes. There ing to read,- to "put him in a class half seems to have been no questioning of the as big, and double the time spent on read- materials used as the content of the teach- ing ...because hehas to learn toread." ing for these smaller classes and doubled These quotations seem to indicate first, time. a school situationthat may be a more It is of these two matters--the realistic general and continuing problem, and, sec- testing situation at the junior high school ond, the belief that more teachers and level and the practical preparation of the more time to do more of what is nowpupils to meet it successfully that I would being done constitutes the only remedy. bring a consideration of the knowledge It is true, I believe, that the child whoand understanding achieved in the field has been in school for six years, enters,of linguistics. at the junior high school level, academical- The real test of whether the elementary ly, as well as physically and emotionally, school pupil has actually learned to read an especially critical period. Here, often during gradesIto .6 must ultimately be for the first time, he is faced with the the efficiency with which he meets the necessity of serious study-reading of achallenge of the serious study-reading of considerable range of subject matter con- the junior high school subject-matter con- tent. For this subjectmatter content hetent. His "readiness'' for the kind of read- must produce results of knowledge and ing required at this level is the function understanding derivedsolelyfromhis of the reading teaching that precedes. In reading. other words, the beginning steps of ele- Inadequaciesinhisreading compe- mentary reading and the whole of the tence will inevitably show themselves at reading activities of the "transfer" stage this time and make it impossible for him must be analyzed, planned, programed, to meet the reading demands of this and measured in terms of the competencies Reading and Inanity, IRA Proceedings,Vol. 10, 1965, 2.15-247.

263 258 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

essential for the kind of reading required The first sentence specifies the mean- for full success at the junior high school ing of the word Nat by identifyingit level. with the well-known animal, cat. For us Some specific examples from our actual this identification is that Nat is the cat's classroom materials -developed upon lin- name. guistic principles" for the teaching of be- Nat is a cat. ginningreadingthroughthetransfer The second sentence adds to the mean- stage may help to put more precise mean- ing by asserting that this particular cat, ing content into these general statements. Nat, has a special physical feature to he These examples should also show that the described as fat. goals set and the series of small steps to- Nat is fat. ward their attainment .ts set forth in these The third sentence adds more to the materials do net require any -forced feed- meaning by bringing the description and ing. The materials have been used with the identification together in one summary hundreds of -slow learners.- These ex- sentence. amples should also serve to eontradict the Nat is a fat cat. many assertions that our linguistic ap- The three sentences are tied together proach- is-segmental method- with the into a sequence by the repetition of the -Same basic aim as the phonic method of word Nat. teaching reading:- the assertion thatit To be a "sequence- of sentences in our is a -phonemic approach to reading- with use of the term there must always be in -word lists .ts the basic learning material:- the sentences some type of "sequence sig- the assertion that it applies the -principle nals." Here the repetition of the name of matching letters with sounds:- and the Nat serves the purpose. A discourse is a assertion thatit"ignores meaning- and sequence of sentences tied together lin- centers the attention of the pupil on for- guistically by "sequence signals." mal and mechanical features.- Each of the two sentences after the As .1 matter of Lk t the pimar) objec- firstadds something to the "growing" tie of our materials built upon linguistic meaning. The lastsentence bringsto- understanding is Ilk abilit) to real for gether what we have called the "cumula- mcauings. Reading for meanings requires tive- meaning of this unit. All sequences not only that the pupil must grasp the of sentences that belong to a unit as meanings of the words he reads and the shown by sequence signals have not only meanings attachedtothe grammatical individual sentence meaning but also this structures in which these words are placed. "growing accumulation of meanings." but also and especially that he must build Only through continued and consistant up as he proceeds all the situational mean- practice in the reading of "sequences of ings that come from the succession of sentences" in units of growing size and sentences in each story unit. Even from complexity does the pupil build up re- the very beginning of the first reading les- sponses to these cumulative meanings as son with only three content words, rat, they develop, and make maximum prog- fat, Nat, (and the two structures words ress toward what we have called "pro- is and unstressed a as in a cat) there is ductive- reading. Real reading is never some meaning added to the whole with a passive receptive process of recognizing each sentence. The pupil must grasp this written words and the grammatical struc- cumulative meaning. tures in which these words occur. It is not The very first reading unit is the fol- even an active responding to all the sig- lowing. nals of meaning actually represented in Nat is a cat. the writing. Real reading demands that Nat is fat. the reader build up and carry along such Nat is a fat cat. a complete understanding of the cumu- ,

FRIES 259 lative meaning of the sequences of sen- groups, do not stimulate for them any tences that he habitually supplies,fills recognition responses. The case for .1 con- in, the patterns of tone (intonation), the siderable amountofproperlydirected special stresses, and the pauses of group- and properly used oral reading from the ing that the live language of speech uses very beginning rests primarily on the need but which are either not indicated at all to develop the habits of "productive" by our writing system or only crudely reading. hinted at by our present system of punc- One other matter deserves attention in tuation. Real reading is thus -productive" respect to developing in the pupils the reading. ability to read for meanings. The most Oral reading with -expression- is not commonly used Preprimers, Primers, and only the avoiding of a monotone; nor are First Readers for teaching the first stages the variations of pitch in their sequences of learning to read have large pictures* haphazard and lawless. They fit into cer- on practically every pagepictures that tain major patterns characteristic of the show the situations of each "story." These English language. These patterns of in- pictures give meaning to the words and tonation are not solely related to the sep- sentences in the readitv; text. In fact On arate sentences, but also and especially to many pages of the pre-primers the words the relations between sentences as they and sentences have no real meaning with. follow one another in a discourse. out the pictures. From allthe reading Children do not need to be taught to hooks of our linguistic series pictures have use the intonation patterns of English. been excluded as a nutter of principle in As a matter of fact, fitting into the tone order to compel the pupils to build the patterns of the intonation sequences of habit of seeking the meaning, even the English is perhaps the very first thing the situational meaning, in the words and child learns of the language. Most normal sentences of the written text alone. The children of five speak their language using usual use of pictures in the teaching of all the intonation patterns of their lin- beginning reading builds the habit of guisticcommunity.Usually,however, lookingfirstatthepictures andthen neither the pupils nor the teachers(if guessing at the identification of the words they have not had contact with the formal used. We would emphasize the necessity study oflinguistics or of speech)are of completely reversing the process--ha%- aware of what the major English intona- ing the pupils read for the meanings. and tionpatternsarc.Problems arisewith then, as a means of strengthening their pupils because, at the beginning of learn- vivid imaginative realization of the situa- ing to read, in their struggle to recognize tion, having them draw their own pic- the written representations of the words, tures to depict parts of the situation or they often develop habits of pronouncing some of the persons involved as they in- the words as separate items rather than dividually imagine them to be. Many of as words in the sequences of utterances the very slow learners do very well with in real "talk.- such exercises and certainly do not need Children will use the patterns of in- to have ready made pictures .is crutches. tonation naturally whenever they realize At this point an objection will prob- the meaning of what they are saying. Oral ably arise. With some justice one may reading must become the telling of the insist that the immediate problem for the meanings which they have received from junior high school is the great number of the written sequences of sentences. To put seventh graders who canreadatthe it another way, their intonation will dem- onstrate whether they have really realized The Scott.1;ite,mattSerie. --The threePre- Pn"".MO thePI IMO 11:1Ve t"LII 3::1 vividly the meanings or are simply pro- with apicture on everypage. The Fitt Reader. nut of a total of 187 page. ha. 171 page. with picture. nouncing words which, as a succession 01 and 1G without pictures.

G5 260 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

fourth, or fifth, or even sixth grade level step will be greatly reduced and should but who tanniit read well enough to meet progressisel decrease aspupilsrealize the realistic- test of the serious studyread- more clearly the steps of their own grow- ing tlt.ttthe carious subject-matter texts ing skill. This realization must come from require.Ile will then point outthat I the ,b/iii' itself, not from attempts of the lust: been concerned with the child at theteacher to /a/ him about what he is doing. beginning stage of reading and the ma- Second. asI !use insisted elsewhere, a terials and teaching practice that. for be-person Lan read insofar as Ile can re- ginning readers has sersed to lead up to spond''to the language signals of his the mature reading habits and eompetan- mune language code when these signals ciesrequired. and that normal -begin- arerepresented by patterns of graphic ning readers.' require atle.tstfive years shapes ,ii fully as he can to these same of reading practice to read' this level. language signals represented by patterns I offer two comments and suggestions, of soca! sounds. Stress the words "respond based on a limited experience with small as full.- for the most significant base groups of these twelse and thirteen year from which to measure reading ability olds. must he the receptive language control of First,thesepupil!:cannotprofitably the person being measured. Reading abil break into this series of readers in the its. must he evaluated against.t Nrticultr middle or near the end. The sequence of language ability. Any junior high school the significant contrasts embedded in the pupil's reading competence must he esal- materials has been -programed- in small milted in terms of that pupil's receptive steps so that the responses at each level ability in the English language-- --his ability depend on the habits established earlier. to respond to the -talk- addressed to him. To be effective the pupil must start at the All reading at any level in the schools beginning and proceed through the ma- should he evaluated in terms of progress terials as organized. For seventh graders toward active -productive- reading and the amount of time necessary for each some type of vivid imaginative realization.

266 Vowel Sounds in VCC Words

PAC IA Frio*

IN The Reading Teacher FOR APRIL 1966, David R. Stone states thatthe function of double letters [consonants] is to guide the reader to a preceding vowel sound ( the short c in letter, forexam- ple).- He says thatthis function has been obscured, however, and is rather unreliable...particularly... when all short vowels are considered. The word 'defective...would under the above rule be spelled *defecctive'(pp. 503-504). The present author has had considerable success teaching remedial reading pupils in junior high school to decipher long words by using a similar, but more general rule. The pupilswere taught to attack words like "defective" in the followingmanner: 1] Look for any common word parts you can recognize (de... tive).2] Look at the rest of the word. The chief question is how the vowel sound should be pronounced.3] The vowel is a single vowel. Since it is followed by two consonants, it is probably short. Say the short sound of e. Try it in the word: de-fec-tive, defective. This method by-passes the complicated rules of syllabication and makes use of whatever scattered information the remedial pupils have. Upon reading Dr. Stone's remarks, however. itoc- curred to this investigator that success with the vcc rule might have been a result of the fact that the procedure directed pupils'atten- tion to the middles of words, the part that remedial pupils tend most to neglect in deciphering long words. The present study was therefore done to investigate the frequency with which single vowels that immediately precede pairs of consonants actually have short sounds. By "pairs of consonants- is meant either double consonants (-tt-) or dissimilar consonants (-st-). The words selected for analysis are those "introduced in five of the most widely used basal reading series. together with the Dolch (1942) and Fry (1960) lists" (Stone and Tracy, 1963). There are 1450 words on this list, which extends from pre-primer to high third reader. The average number of words introduced in each series by the end of third grade is 1628. This leavesan aver- age of only 178 words in each series unaccounted for, since accord- ing to Stone, all of the words in the list appear inevery one of the series. The "Basic Word List from Basal Readers"may therefore be considered a reasonable sample of all the wordsa child is

The Beading Teacher, 21 (1:tin uary 11168), 1-12-14.1.

267 262 ARTICLES PARTIALI.Y RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

expected to be able to read by the end of third grade. For this study, all the words that contain a single vowel fol- lowed by two consonants (vcc words) were checked off. This com- bination was found in 613 words (42 per cent of the words on the list). After the vcc words were identified, the investigator identified those which had a short vowel immediately preceding the two consonants and those which did not. Two criteria were used: 1] The sound was unquestionably short in the author's dialect; and 2] For a pupil to identify it as short before he knew the word would not, in the author's opinion, tend to prevent recognition of the word by distorting it excessively. If a word had'a short vowel before the two consonants, it was concidered to be evidence in favor of the rule. If it had any other vowel sound, it was considered to be evidence against the rule. The rule would be considered useful if it held in a proportion of cases substantially exceeding the likelihood of a single vowel being short in any position at all. An estimate of the likelihood of a single vowel being short was obtained by counting the number of short vowel sounds in all the words on the list from pre-primer through the first half of second. There were 203 short vowel sounds in 531 syllables. The likelihood of getting a short vowel sound in any posi- tion is .38. Results Of the 613 vcc words, 431 contained a short vowel before two consonants. Thus, 70 per cent of the vcc words tended to favor the usefulness of the rule. When words of five or more letters were considered separately (in general, words of more than one syllable plus short words ending in -dge), the percentage of conformity was 75. For the rest of the words (three or four letters) the percentage was 67. Statistical tests showed that1] the propor- tion of words that have a short vowel before two consonants is significantly greater than is expected by chance, and 21 that longer words are likely to he more consistent than shorter words in this respect. (Both findings were significant at the 1 per cent level.) These results seem generally consistent by grade level, particularly for the longer words. There appear to be more irregular short vcc words in readers at the primer level than regular words, but the proportion of regular vcc words approaches the proportion for the entire sample by first reader. Of the long vcc words that did not have short vowel sounds where expected, 17 per cent contained -ar and 10 per cent con- tained -or. About 9 per cent contained -on, sounding like -un (won- 68 FULD 263 derful). The only other exceptions to the rule that occurred more than twice involved -other (mother ), -al (almost), and -u (bullet). The shorter words appeared to involve similar exceptions.

Table 1Number of regular and irregular vcc words by grade levels Total number of Reader Regular Irregular words on list Total number of Level vcc words vcc words at each level syllables

Preprimer 2' 1' 58 59 2" 1" Primer 5 9 78 90 4 0 First 18 12 115 139 12 3 2' 36 22 188 243 19 7 2' 45 21 206 256 17 2 3' 58 42 ?33 1 3' 87 35 84 33 801 1,076

TOTALS: 431 182 1.450 *Top figure for each level is for words of four or fewer letters. *Bottom figure for each level is for words of five or more letters. Per cent of conformity to vcc rule Long words: 75 per centShort words: 67 per cent Composite: 70 per cent Long words are significantly different from short words at the 1 per cent level. and all of the observed percentages are significantly different from the chance expectation of 38 per cent at the I per cent level.

Implications for teaching Itis interesting to note that the first words children are expected to learn do not seem to conform to the general pattern. This means that the vcc rule would not be useful at thevery begin- ning of reading instruction. By the second half of first grade, however, 60 per cent of the short vcc words and 80per cent of the long vcc words fit the rule. Therefore, it would probablyprove helpful in deciphering about 30 per cent of all words. By the third grade, the vcc rule would be confirmed 73per cent by long vcc words and 71 per cent by.short vcc words. Spelling rules and syllabication rules could be taughtas special cases of the vcc rule; they conform to it, but theyare more restricted. Since the rule seems more consistent for long words than for short, it should be particularly helpful for remedialcases. Older 264 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

pupils do not like to be confined to "baby" words. If they are encouraged to tackle long words with the help of this.rule, they have a chance to make use of what "advanced" knowledge of common word parts they may have.

Reference Stone, D. R., and Tracy. Vilda B. A basic word list from basal readers. Elementary English, 1963, 420427. Reading Achievement and Word Recognition Skills

BEN i I. HACKNEY.JR.*

DESPITE CONSIDERABLE RESEARCH. one still knows relatively little about how individual children learn to read. The current literature is in:It reement that reading is not composed of one skill, but a large number of interrelated skills which are developed over a period of years. Research indicates that children learn to recognize words by different methods and that no one method is best for all children.It cannot be assumed that all children develop equal facility with each skill. Individuals may be good readers and still be deficient in one or more of the basic word recognition skills. The purpose of this study was to determine which of the word recogni- tion skills normally taught in a basal reading series program has been acquired by a random sample of fourth grade students in the public schools of North Carolina. The skills analyzed were the basic word recognition techniques taught in the basal reading series program in the primary grades of the public schools in North Carolina. The fourth grade level of students was chosen for study because all of the word recognition skills have been introduced in the primary reading program. The study identified the word recog- nition skills possessed by a random sample of fourth grade students and attempted to answer the following questions: I. Is there a basic pattern of word recognition skills acquired by students in designated reading achievement levels? 2. Are there word recognition skills which are commonly taught in a basal reading series program that do not contribute to reading achievement? In completing this study. the primary null-hypothesis was the following: A random sample of fourth grade students in the public schools of North Carolina who score differentially in achievement on the California Reading Test (Tiegs and Willis, 1957) do not differ significantly in their word recognition skills as determined by the Doren Diagnostic Reading Test of Word Recognition Skills 1956). The Doren Test which is a test designed specifically to measure. in a group situation, the word recognition skills presented in the basal reading series was used to measure the word recogni- tion skills of the sample. The California Reading Test was used to determine the reading achievement level. Patterns of skills in rela- tionship to achievement may indicate the need for adjusting priority in the instructional emphasis on certain word recognition skills. °The. Reading Teacher, 21 (March 1968) , 515-518.

271 266 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENk.nL

Design The study involved the selection of a random sample of 1,711 fourth grade students by utilizing a probability sampling technique developed by the NEA Research Division Staff (1960 ). The random sample was of sufficient size to give an accuracy of plus or minus live percentage points with a level of confidence of 98 per cent. This means the chances should be at least ninety-eight in 100 that the scores obtained on the standardized tests from the random sample of fourth grade students in the public schools of North Carolina do not vary more than five percentage points from the true test scores of all the fourth grade students in the public schools of North Carolina. The random sample of students was selected from a total population of 106.617 fourth grade students during the school year of 1963-1964. The sample was divided into three differential reading achieve- ment level groups. Percentile norms for the total reading score of the California Reading Test were used as reference points in defining the limitation of each. A student was assigned to a group on the basis of his total percentile score obtained from the Cali- ! ornia Reading Test. Percentile scores included in each group were the following: Group One 99.98,95,90, and 80 Group Two 70.60,50.40, and 30 Group Three 20,10,5,2, and 1 California Reading Test percentile norms are not inclusive of all possible percentile scores, but are limited to the major percentiles used in dividing the sample. For general interpretation, group one, consisting of 676 students was above average in reading achieve- ment; group two, consisting of 809 students was average in reading achievement; and group three, consisting of 226 students was below average in reading achievement. A t test of difference be- tween means verified the significant differences among the three groups on the total percentile scores of the California Reading Test. The level of significance was .01. Total reading percentile scores of the California Reading Test for group one had a mean of 89.142 with a standard deviation of 6.935; group two had a mean of 53.449 with a standard deviation of 13.670; and group three had a mean of 12.185 with a standard deviation of 7.123. The Doren Diagnostic Reading Test of Word Recognition Shill was administered to all students. By utilizing scores obtained from this test, the three groups were compared. Eleven specific null- hypotheses were stated concerning each of the eleven measured 272 HACKNEY 267

word recognition skills:. letter recognition, beginning sounds, whole words, words within words, speech consonants, ending sounds, blending. rhyming, vowels, sight words, and discriminate guessing. Treatment of data The primary null-hypotheses and each of the eleven specific null-hypotheses were accepted or rejected on the basis of the deter- mination of the existence of significant differences between the means of the respective Doren Test scores of the groups when com- pared one group with another. The three possible group com- parisons were: group one with group two: group one with group three; and group two with group three. The ttest of difference between means was utilized to determine if there were significant differences at the .01 level of significance. Data obtained from the analysis were used to reject the primary and all eleven specific null - hypotheses. Results Groups composed of students high in reading achievement made better scores on each of the eleven word recognition skills tests than did groups composed of students who were considered average or below average in reading achievement. The average did better than the below average group. The pattern was consistent in that the lowest scores on the word recognition tests were made by the below average group in reading achievement. Good students in reading seemed to possess better word recognition skills than poor readers in each of the eleven word recognition skills measured. Apparently. all of the eleven word recognition skills measured contributed to reading achievement. If there had been a comparison in which a low achievement group made high scores on one or more word recognition tests. or a high achievement group made low scores on one or more word recognition tests, there would have been doubt as to the contribution of this skill to reading achieve- ment. In all group comparisons of scores obtained from the word recognition tests. the scores of the lower reading achievement level group never equaled or surpassed the word recognition scores of the group higher in reading achievement. The t tests for differences between means of the Doren Test scores for the three groups when compared one group with another revealed significant differences. The means of the test scores for each unit and total score of the Doren Test were significantly different when compared with the mean scores of the other groups. The level of confidence was .01 with 1,708 degrees of freedom. 268 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED Olt VERY GENERAL

A standard deviation was obtained for each unit score and total score of the Doren Test for each of the three groups. The standard deviations obtained from group one scores were lower than the standard deviations obtained from group two scores. With no exceptions. the standard deviations on the Doren Test scores for group two were lower than standard deviations obtained from group three scores. This meant that the lower a group was in reading achievement as measured by the California Reading Test, the greater was the spread of scores obtained from the Doren Test. The higher a group scored on reading achievement as measured by the California Reading Test. the less was the spread of scores of the Doren Test. Conclusions In answer to the two questions raised at the beginning, an interpretation of the data revealed a consistent pattern of word recognition skills for students in reading achievement level groups. The interpretation ofof the data collected in the study implied that each of the eleven word recognition skills measured by the Doren Test contributed to reading achievement. Because of the sampling technique. it is felt that the findings of this study would likely be found true for the total population of fourth grade students in the public schools of North Carolina. Additional research is

needed for I rther studs to determine the extent to which these eleven word recognition skills contribute to reading success.

References Doren. Margaret. Doren diagnostic reading test Of word rceognition shills. Minneapolis, Minnesota: Educational Test Bureau. 1956. . Small sample techniques. The N.E.A. Research Bulletin. 1960. 38, 99.104. - Tiegs. E. W.. and Clark. W. W. California reading test. elementary grades 4.5-6, form A. Del Monte. California: California Test Bureau. 1957.

21 4 Phoneme-Grapheme Correspondences in Monosyllabic Words

RctiARD E. How: Es° is Tins STUDY the spellings ofall tlonosyllabic words aretherefore monosyllabic words in the core vocab- characterizedbytheirfunctionality ulary of American English were ana- andtheir structural sunplicity, and lyzed in order to determine the degree bothofthesecharacteristicscause towhich the orthography approxi- monosyllabic words to be quite useful mates the alphabetic principle (i.e., a in beginning spelling and reading pro- one-to-one correspondence between a grams. Each characteristic,infact, phoneme and a singlegraphemic can be used as the most basic ration- repiesentation) in this set of words. ale for the selection of words for be- Most beginning spelling and reading ginning spellers and readers. programs, for a number of reasons, In terms of functionality, it is gener- typically use vocabularies comprised ally assumed that the child's introduc- largely of monosyllabic words.With tion to written language should begin the proliferation of linguistically or- with the study of words he will most iented spelling and reading programs likely be acquainted with and which inwhich pupils arc guided toward are useful to him in verbal communica- gaining an understanding of the alpha- tion.Numerous studies, the Rins land betic structure of our writing system, study for example (4), have attempted it seems important to describe the or- to determine children's uses of words thography of those words which are and these studies have, in turn, been the basis for the child's introduction to important sources for word selection. a structural analysis of American En- Because monosyllabic words tend to be glish orthography. among the most frequently used words of the American English core lexicon, Monosyllabic words and the itis easy to see why these types of orthography words predominate in beginning spell- Monosyllabic words constitute a rich ing and reading programs. and functional part of our language. Structural simplicity, on the other Indeed. 340 of the first five hundred hand, refers to the idea that the words most frequently used American En- a child initially encounters in learning glish words have monosyllabic struc- to spell and read should be controlled tures (5). Why is there a predilec- in terms of their structural complexi- tionto employ monosyllabic words ties. Monosyllabic words, of course, with high frequency in our language? do not necessitate that the child con- It seems likely that it is simply easier cern himself with such linguistic fac- to remember and to pronounce mono- tors as syllabicationand pffixation. syllabic words than other words hav- Monosyllabic words are, in short, the ing more complex structures.This simplest structural forms of the lexicon "principle of least effort" (6), in tact, and therefore are assumed to be the probablyaccountsforsuchhighly logical starting points for subsequent functional monosyllabic words as structural analysis of written language. phone, zoo, ad, lab, walk and bus that Structural simplicity, however, has a areclippedformsofpolysyllabic second important consequence, particu- words. larly in light of recent linguistic in- 'Forging Ahead in Reading, IRA Proceedings, Vol. 12, Part I, 1968, 560.566. A

270 ARTICLES PARTIAI.I.Y RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

sights into thenature of American (speech sounds) in oral language.A English orthography. In order to un- thirty-phoneme language. for example, derstand this consequence. we should would use only thirty distinct graphic first examine briefly the notion of an al- symbols to represent these phonemes phabetic orthography,since this is the in writing.Theoretically, at least, an principle upon which American En- individual should be able to spell and glish orthography is based. read correctly any written representa- An alphabetic orthography is based tion of a spoken word once he has upontheideathattheconstituent masteredthe abilitytodifferentiate speech sounds of oral languagethe speech sounds and has mastered their phmteinesof a languageeach have graphic representations. individual graphic representationsin American English orthography is al- writing.The majority of the world's phabeticallybased. Ituses alphabet writing systems are of this type chiefly letters and their combinations to repre- because alphabetic orthographies are sent the phonemes of American En- more efficient than other types of or- glish speech. We are all keenly aware, thographies. "Word" writing systems however, that our writing system does (technically, such systems are called not attain the idealized state of a pure logographicorthographies), for exam- alphabetic orthographybut it is based ple, employ graphic symbols to repre- upon this principle, and for this rea- sent the morphemes. or meaning ele- son, linguists and others propose that a ments, of a language. These writing basic, indeed crucial, aspect of a child's systems do have the advantage of cir- understanding of written language in- cumventing various language forces, volves his understanding the nature of :melt as sound change and dialect, that theAmericanEnglish orthography. Most important, too, it is argued that a caninterfereinthetranslationof speechinto writing and vice versa. knowledge of the alphabetic structure But logographic writing systems also of the orthography can be applied as require their risers to master a prodi- importantcomponentsofmastering gious number of graphic symbols if written American English. they are to use this type of writing But, because American English or- system, for each word of oral language thography does not truly reflect the al- requires its own graphic symbol. phabetic principle, we cannot rely with A second type of drthography is complete assurance upon the knowl- called a syllabary. In such writing sys- edge that a given phoneme is spelled a tems, graphic symbols are devised to certain way.In our orthography we represent the syllables that, in combi- are forced to play odds: we are obliged nation, make up the words of a lan- to determine which graphemic repre- guage. Syllabicwritingisan im- sentation of a phoneme most often rep- provement overlogographicwriting resents that phoneme in words, and because there are fewer syllables than we into also masterthoseexcep- words ina language and therefore tional phoneme- grapheme correspond- fewer graphic symbols that must be ences which exist in the orthography. mastered in order to spell and read. The hpAc reason underlying our or- Yet,syllabically-basedorthographies thogrn.Ws disparity with the alphabetic still require substantial "memory principle lies not only in the fact that loads." we have but twenty-six letters to rep- An ideal alphabetic orthography, on resent some three dozen or so pho- the other hand, employs only as many nettles but that we use these lelters in graphic symbols as there are phonemes various combinations, with the Comic- '46 HODGES 271 queuce that we have farmoregraphic The distinction, while useful for ana- symbols than necessary. lytic purposes, also has important edu- In any case, the notion of structnral cational consequences. simplicity, in linguistic terms, also re- fers to the need tocontrolthe selection Phoneme-grapheme correspondences of words for beginning spelling and itt monosyllabic words reading such that the dominant The study reported here involves the sound-to-letter correspondences are encoding process of spelling.Its pur- mastered initially by children before pose is not particularly to identify edu- they progress toward more complex cationalimplicationsforbeginning and less productive orthographic char- spelling and reading programs but to acteristics. Such linguistically based describe the orthographic structures of reading programs at the Bloomfield- monosyllabic words that are or could Banihardt materials typify this condi-be used in such programs and to dem- tion. onstrate a technique that might be use- ful in examining the orthography for Differences between spelling reading purposes. and reading processes A recently completed .studyof The discussion to this point has been phoneme-grapheme correspondences in directed toward an understanding of AmericanEnglishorthography(1) the nature of American English ortho-analyzed the spellings of over 17,000 graphy.But. particularly in terms ofdifferent words to determine the de- the study reported here, it should also gree to which this core vocabulary of be noted that the processes of spelling American English approximates the and reading differ in an alphabetic or-alphabetic principle.The findings of thography.At the risk of oversimpli- this research have been reported else- fication, spelling is anencodingopera- where in the literature (1, 2, 3); but tion.The speller's task is to translate thesefindings may be summarized speech sounds into writing, a task thatquickly by noting that the researchers necessitatesthedevelopmentof an found that most phonemes are typically ability to select from a set of possible represented at least 80 percent of the graphic representations of a phonemetime by a particular graphic symbol ei- that graphic symbol which correctlyther anywhere in a syllable or in par- represents the phoneme in the word toticular positions in syllables.In other be spelled.Reading processes, on thewords, recalling that in our imperfect other hand, involvedecodingopera- alphabetic orthography we must rely tions.. Reading an alphabetic ortho-on odds that a graphic symbol will rep- graphy, in short, requires an individualresent a phoneme, American English to determine which of several possibleorthographyisalphabetically based; phonemes is represented by a givenbut we must utilize cues in addition to graphic symbol in a word. simple sound-to-letter correspondence In sum, spelling involves sound-to-to spellcorrectly American English letter correspondences while reading words, using alphabetic principles. involvesletter-td-soundcorrespond- The Stanford Research Project did ences.In a pure alphabetic orthogra-not isolate monosyllabic words for crit- phy these processes arereciprocal. ical analysis; rather, all words in the But in an imperfect alphabetic ortho- core American English orthography graphy such as the American English were examined, each word being bro- writing system, spelling and reading ken down into syllables, with the result have their own unique complexities.that monosyllabic words were treated

9 rift., 272 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

as syllables rather than as discrete certain how some phonemes are spelled words. The present study isolated the in our alphabetically based orthogra- 3,428 monosyllabic words of the earlier phy.This classification was termed study, and these words were subjected positional constraints. to an examination of their phoneme- By rank-ordering the spellings of grapheme characteristics. each phoneme at each of these levels of A phonemic classification was de- analysis it was therefore possible to as- vised, a system. that employed forty- sess how nearly each phoneme in mon- eight phonemestwenty-eight conso- osyllabic words approximates the al- nants and twenty vowels. Conventional phabetic principle.In this way, a de- phonemic systems, of course, use fewer termination could be made of the odds numbers orphonemes to classify Amer- that a given phoneme would be repre- ican English phonology. But, it was sented by a particular graphic symbol felt appropriate for spelling purposes to at least 80 percent of the time in mon- include phonemic notations for the oc- osyllabic words or in some position in currence of vowel phonemes before /r/, them.The 80 percent criterion is a as well as for the consonant clusters useful measure of the degree to which /kw/ and /ks/, which have unique the orthography approximates the al- orthographic characteristics. phabetic principle, since it means that The possible graphemic representa- anyphoneme-graphemecorrespond- tions of phonemes used in American ence achieving this criterion could be English orthography were then de- applied to the spelling of monosyllabic rived from the previous Stanford Re- words and the resulting spelling would search,with somereclassifyingof those graphic symbols that include a be correct at least four times out of final letter c as in bake or spoke. five. Computer technology was then em- Findings ployed to analyze the phoneme-gra- pheme correspondenCes in monosylla- In terms of the phonological charac- bic words in two ways: teristics of monosyllabic words as com- 1. To examine them in terms of pared to polysyllabic words,itwas how a given phoneme is spelled any- found that place in monosyllabic words.This ex- 1. All phonemes of the general lexi- amination was intended to describe the con are not present in monosyllabic orthography of monosyllabic words as words.The neutral vowel /4, oc- though the orthography were ideally curring in unstressed syllables, for ex- alphabetic.No factors were consid- ample, does not occur in monosyllabic ered, other than how is a sound spelled words since, by definition, all monosyl- whenitappears in a monosyllabic labic words, when isolated from run- word.This classification was termed ning speech, have primary stress. simple phoneme-grapheme correspond- 2. More important for spelling pur- ence. poses, however, is the apparent fact 2. To examine them in terms of that the number of spellings of certain how a given phoneme is spelled in par- phonemes inthe general lexicon as ticular positions in monosyllabic classified in the Stanford Research are words; that is, initial, medial, and final considerably reduced in numbers when position of monosyllabic words.This monosyllabicwordsareseparately examination thus described the ortho- classified.In the general lexicon, con- graphy of monosyllabic words using sonant phonemes employ 141 graphic additional cues that are needed to as- symbols, while in monosyllabic words HODGES 273

89 graphicsymbolsareemployed. tions to the alphabetic principle of This is also true of vowel phonemes. the total phonemic system used inthis In the general lexicon. 234 graphic study as well as the separate symbols are employed consonant torepresent and vowel classifications. vowel phonemes. while in monosyllabic This table words 156 graphic symbols are used. also points out how position affects the In short. 130 fewer graphic symbols numbers of phonemes that are used in respectivepositionsinmonosyllabic are used in monosyllabic words than in words. the general lexicon. Findings such as the foregoing pro- Such reductions in the number of vide us with general information about graphemes used to represent phonemes the orthography. in monosyllabic words as compared But they do not help to us determine the conditions that char- the generallexicon would seem to inferthat acterize American English orthogra- phoneme-grapheme corre- phy's departure from the alphabetic spondences in monosyllabic words principle. more nearly approximate the alphabetic 5. Twenty-four of the twenty-eight principle in these words than in all consonant phonemes classified in this words of the general lexicon.But, is study exceed the 80 percent criterion this the case? atthe level of simple phoneme-gra- 3. At the level of simple phoneme- pheme correspondence. The four pho- grapheme correspondence.American nemes that do not are /j/. /k/, /ng /, English phonemes in the general lexi- and /z/. However. all consonant pho- con fail to achieve the 80 percent cri- nemes exceed this criterion when posi- terion (73.13 percent) while phonemes tional constraintsarc considered. in monosyllabic words only exceed this That is, even the four errant phonemes criterion (81.36 percent).Mean per- which fail to exceed the 80percent cri- centage tabulations, however, obscure terion at the level of simplecorre- the important information concerning spondence do so in sonic position in consonant and vowel phonemes, re- monosyllabicwords. Clearly,then, spectively, as well as individual pho- consonant phonemes as a group are not primarily responsible for American nemes. Consonant phonemes as a English orthography's departure from group increase in approximations to the alphabetic principle. thealphabeticprinciple from 83.99 6. On the other hand, onlyseven of percent to 88.35 percent at the level of the twenty vowel phonemes classified simple phoneme-grapheme correspond- in this study exceed the 80percent cri- ence. Vowel phonemes as, a group terion at the level of simple phoneme- also increase, but froni 62.27 percent grapheme correspondence, these pho- to 64 percent, well below the 80 per- nemesbeingtheso-called"short cent criterion. vowel" phonemes and two vowelpho- 4. In terms of positional constraints, nemes occurring before/r/.Even consonant phonemes further approxi- when positional constraintsare consid- mate the alphabetic principle in each ered, vowel phonemesas a group do position of monosyllabic words as com- not appreciably increase in approxima- pared to their occurrences in the total tions to the alphabetic principle,al- lexicon. Vowel phonemes, onthe though /01/,foul, and Alt/(the other hand, also approach this princi- vowel sound in one pronunciationof ple although in no position do vowel law) exceed thr 80 percent criterionin phonemes exceed the 80 percent cri- sonic position in monosyllabic words. terion. Table I summarizes approxima- In sum, this examination ofthe ortho-

- 279 A 274 ARTICLES PARTIALI.Y RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

TABLEI SUMMARY TABLE OF PERCENTAGES OF APPROXIMATION TO ALPHABETIC PRINCIPLE OF PHONEMES IN MONOSYLT.ABIC WORDS 48 Phoneme 28 Consonant 20 Vowel Phonological Level: Classification Classification Classification No. of No. of No. of Phonemes Plumemes Phonemes Used (1sed Used Simple Phoneme- Grapheme Corres. 48 81.36 28 88.35 20 64.00 Position Factors Initial 43 95.17 24 96.00 19 73.77 Medial 39 80.37 19 98.08 20 68.48 Final 33 81.46 23 83.15 10 59.66 Mean % 84.92 91.71 68.05

graphy of monosyllabic words reveals :4re obviously comprised of other _pho- that the phonemes largely responsible nemes Irving less consistent spellings. for the orthography's failureto ap- What might be done about them ? proximate the alphabetic principle in One alternative is to present lists of monosyllabic words are the so-called words, selected for their functionality "long vowel" phonemes and certain in writing, in which particular pho- diphthongs. neme-grapheme correspondences,al- though failing to exceed the 80 percent Discussion criterion. occur in large numbers of Such seeming disparities with the al- words. This "spelling pattern" ap- phabetic principle should not require proach, for example, could be used to demonstrate one of the several spell- that we "throw the baby out with the ings of the "long" vowel /ow/, the so- bath water." For the fact of the matter called"longo" soundspelledmi. isthat American English orthogra- Although this spelling of /ow/ occurs phyisalphabeticallybased and al- only 17 percent of the time in mono- though as a system is more compli- syllabic words. 45 of the 51 words in cated than it might be, contains many whichitoccurs are extremely func- sound -lo- letter correspondences that, if tionalwords. A number of these learned, can ease the burden of master- words are board, boat. cloak, coach. ing our writing system.The conso- coal.coast,coat. croak, foam, goat, nant and "short vowel" phonemes, for groan, load,loaf.loan. road, roam, instance, are rich sources for helping roar. roast, soak, soap, and throat. children to understand the nature of In short. one possible solution is to the orthography andto applythis present individual phoneme-grapheme knowledge in core ectly spelling words. correspondences in terms of the func- In fact, it is precisely these phoneme tionalwordsinwhich theyoccur. classifications that are so heavily uti- Rare spellings of phonemes would, in lized in linguistically-based beginning this instance. be treated as exceptions spelling and reading programs and and learned by whatever instructional are often used toinitiate traditional methods appear to be appropriate. phonics-oriented reading programs. A-second alternative suggests itself, American English words, however, however, when we review the degree HODGES 275

to which phonemes approximate the tions.What is suggested here is that alphabeticprincipleinmonosyllabic balanced spelling programs mustcon- words as compared to their spellings in tend with both criteria: that is, if we the generallexicon,particularly thewish to provide pupils with spelling spellings of those phonemes which de- content that accurately describes the part farthest from the alphabetic princi- alphabetic structure of the orthography ple in monosyllabic words. The vowel and has utility for them as well. phoneme /ey/, for example, (theso- The purpose of this study, however, called "long a" sound) in monosyllabichas not been to search for curriculum words does not achieve the 80 percent implications per se but to providea criterion at the level of simple pho-description of the alphabetic nature of neme-grapheme correspondence nor in the orthography of American English any position in these words.But, in words and to illustrate a research de- the general lexicon, this phoneme is sign and methodology that has poten- spelled a approximately 81 percent of tialusefulness for further studies of the time in the final position of sylla- the orthography in terms of reading. bles that arc not word final. Studies such as this, it is hoped, con- Examples of this observationare able, tribute to a better understanding of the crater.lady,radio,acorn,flavor, structure of American English ortho- major, table.baby,gravy, bacon, graphy: and with suck understanding paper. and cable. may well emerge more effective and Inother words, should we wish to accurate spelling and reading program. do so, an earlier introduction of the factor of syllabication and affixation REFERENCES into beginning spelling and reading 1.Hanna, PaulR., etal. Phoneme- Grapheme Correspondences as Cues to programs would enable children better Spelling Improvement. Washington, D. C., to understand the alphabetic principle U. S. Dept. of Health, Education and underlying our orthography and to in- Welfare, Office of Education, 1966. duce useful sound. to-letter correspond- 2. Hodges, RichardE. "The Casefor ences that pertain to the total lexicon. TeachingSound-to-LetterCorrespond- ences inSpelling," TheElementary Such an alternative, of course,neces- School Journal (Mar. 1966). sitates that words be selected withpre- 3.Hodges, Richard E., andE. Hugh Ru- cision and with a concern for their dolf."Searching Linguistics for Cues functionality. Buts the concepts of func- for the Teaching of Spelling,"Elemen- tionality and tary English, 42 (May 1965), 527-533. structural simplicity4. Rinsland, Henry D. A Basic Vocabulary which were discussed at the outset of of Elementary School Children.New this report are not either-or proposi- York: Macmillan Co., 1945. Conference on Perceptual and Linguistic Aspects of Reading

EVAN K EISLER*

ACONFERENCE on Perceptual andthere would be a total of perhaps Linguistic Aspects of Readingforty letter-to-sound relationships for was held on October 31 andNovem-the beginning reader to learn. In ber 1-2, 1963 at the Center for Ad-English, however, a single letter may vanced Study in the Behavioral Sci-represent many different sounds, and ences in Stanford, California,withit is largely for this reason that alpha- twenty-seven participants represent-bet revision is advanced as an aid in ing specialties in linguistics, psycholo-reading instruction. The proposals for gy, education, and sociology.Thealphabet revision presented at the meeting was sponsored by the Com-conference were offered primarily as mittee on Learning and the Educa-transitional orthography in reading; tional Process of the Social Sciencebeginning readers would first en- Research Council and was plannedcounter printed materials in the re- by Sub-Committee Chairman Elea-vised alphabet system, switching over nor J. Gibson of CornellUniversity.to traditional orthography after some The meeting was made possible bydegree of reading fluency had been funds granted by the Carnegie Cor-attained. poration of New York. The confer- John Downing (2) reported on ence focused upon the acquisition andthe Initial Teaching Alphabet (i.t.a., transfer of beginning reading skills,formerly the Augmented Roman Al- and a central issue was the way inphabet), which uses forty-three char- which the teaching of reading shouldacters, of which twenty-four are tra- be guided by the correspondences be-ditional. The nineteen new characters tween written and spoken English.have a marked resemblance to tradi- It was generally agreed that, if onetional forms to aid in transition. considers the relation only of individ- In a study begun in 1961 in Eng- ual letters to their sounds in Englishland, over 2,000 four- and five-year- speech, there is not one-to-one corre-old children have been learning to spondence between graphemes andread from primers printed in i.t.a. phonemes. Divergent views were ex-Each child moves through a graded pressed with respect to the signifi-series of readers at his own pace and canceofthisfactforreadingis transferred to books printed in tra- instruction. ditional orthography (t.o.) whenever the teacher decides that he is ready to Alphabet Revision Proposals and make the shift. A control group of Field Studies children has been learning to read With an "ideal" writing systemunder similar teaching techniques

The Reading Teacher, 18 (October 1961). 43.19.

r-oo 40-A KEISLER 277

from the same books printed int.o. pity for beginning readers who,ac- A serious but unavoidable weaknesscording to Malone, shift easilyto 1.0. in the design of the study resulted Edward Fry (3) presenteda third from the fact that participation byscheme for beginning readers that the schools had to be entirely volun-makes use of diacritical marksto tary; the administration of each co-indicate how each of the letters in operating school decided whether itstraditional orthography is pro- school would join the experimentalornounced. The orthographyuses only the cilium! group. characters found on a standardtype- Preliminary findings, covering thewriter and provides foran almost first eighteen months for a limitedperfect correspondence between let- sample, show that children in theters and sounds. After the young i.t.a. classes have reached much morereader attains a certain proficiency, advanced books than children in con-he shifts to materials in which Ilia- trol classes.Relevant data, so farc ritical marks appear on fewer and from only a few classes, show thatfewer words. No empirical evidence them: experimental children read t.o.has yet been obtained. with greater accuracy and compre- Considerable surprise wasappar- hension titan children in the controlent among the conference partici- classes, in which t.o. was. usedex-pants at the ease with which children clusively. reportedly moved from the transition- John Malone ;8) presentedan-al alphabets to t.o., anda number of other proposed alphabet, UNIFON.questions were raised in discussion, This isomorphic spelling system, us-among them the following: Does ex- ing :1different letter for each pho-perience with a more regular alpha- neme, was designed as a permanentbet lead the child to conceive of writ- replacement of traditional orthogra-ing as a code and thus help himto phy. UN WON uses forty capital let-attack even traditional orthography terstwenty-two of them from themore effectively? Is an i.t.a. experi- traditional alphabet ),all of whichence helpful simply because it allows have the same maximum height andthe child to produce the sound of the width, but lower case letters are alsoword for himself without waiting for available. Apart from regularizingassistance? Should therenot be a English spelling, a number of othercareful empirical study of the dis- :.ilvantages are claimed for this sys-criminability and utility of all letter tem: that it is effective for electronicforms proposed for anew orthogra- wailing and writing devices, that itphy before the system is tried out? fi:,:ilitates the learninit of English as 1Vhentheexperimental groupis a set and language. md that withcalled upon to engage in writing and minor additions it could be used asaspelling later, will interfering effects phonetic alphabet for many otherof exposure toi.t.a.be revealed? languages. The system has been triedWere the readingpassages in the informally as a transitional orthogra-criterion tests restricted toa narrow r,pq 278 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

range of English words with regularthe Philippine Achievement Test bat- spellings, thus failing to expose possi-tery. Data were available for a thou- ble inadequacies of the experimentalsand pupils. On no section of the test group? was there a significant difference be- How much do phonetic methodstween the two groups; what small of trashing help when the writtendifferences did appear tended to favor langtge already hasthedesired the combination method. The results amount of regularity? Frederick Da-also suggested that the phonic meth- vis (I) reported a study conductedod might be less effective for pupils with Emperatriz Tensuan in Luzonof low mental ability. in teaching beginning reading in Pili- A variety of questions were raised pino, a language that exhibits a highabout the Davisreport. Did the degree of one-to-one grapheme-pho-teachers in the two groups actually neme correspondence.* The experi-conduct their classes very differently? ment compared two methodsof Isitpossible that the criterion test teaching reading. One of these, thetapped semantic and syntactic levels cartilla method, is essentially a phonic that had not been adequately ex- approach. After learning to "sound"ploited in the cartilla method? out each letter, the child develops skill in blending the sounds, thus pro-Linguistic and Perceptual nouncing unfamiliar words. The sec-Approaches to Reading ond approach, called a combination Linguists have consistently pointed method, involves having the childout that the correspondence between associate written words or phraseswritten and spoken English is weak with their counterparts inspeech. only if one attempts to relate individ- The pupil then identifies these fa-ual letters and sounds. If the graph- miliar written words in the readingemic units chosen arc letter patterns, material. He subsequently examineswords, and word groups, a high de- the correspono,es between graph-gree of correspondence may be found. mes and phonemes in the context ofThis suggested to some participants visually familiar words, and thenthat the teaching of beginning read- learnsto pronounce unfamiliaring is unlikely to be advanced by at- words. temptstobring about individual Efforts were made to equate the letter - to - sound correspondence twogroups,involvingthirty-ninethrough modification of the alphabet classes in all, with respect to school,system. It was argued that the begin- teachers, and reading materials. Thening reader should instead be helped workbook cxeicises related to the two to use the correspondence rules al- methods were, of course, different.ready present in English. After two years of instructionin Can children make use of such grades I and 2. children were givenhigher-order correspondence rules as they learn to read? Or arc such rules study is reported in this is.tte of 'fur: REAHING Tr. smut. merely a convenient way for adult

2.84 KEISLER 279

scientists to describe structural rela-who had spontaneously learnedcon- tionships between speech and writtenstituent correspondences showed ex- language? What behavioral processescellent transfer. on the part of the child arc involved Do readers actually behave inac- as he begins to read? Useful answers cordance with grapheme-phoneme to questions about the acquisition ofcorrespondences as formulated by a reading skill may be found in the linguisticanalysis? The prediction directions suggested by Eleanor Gib-that readers do not proceed on a son, who summarized a series of herlater-by-letter basis is supported by investigations (4,5,6). She analyzedempirical evidence on visual percep- the reading process in terms of three tion of both skilled and early readers. kinds of learning tasks to be mastered The smallest unit of encoding can more or less in sequence: learning tobe termed a spelling pattern, a unit differentiate graphemes, learning to of one or more letters which is related decode graphemes to phonemes, and to a specified pronunciation. For ex- forming higher units. ample, ck at the end of a word al- Learning to discriminate one letterways has the sound found in duck. from another is not an automatic 'I hese spelling-to-sound correspond- process. Children at the age of fourence rules have been defined for all begin to respond to critical dimen-vowel and consonant clusters up to sions of letter-like shapes; by eightfive letters, in part as a result of re- they easily discriminate between un- cent analyses carried out by com- familiarletterforms differing on puters.In one experimentitwas these dimensions. Part of the child'sfoundthatfirstgradechildren task appears to be the discovery of showed asignificant difference in what the critical dimensions of differ- intheir visual perceptionof pro- ence are, such as "curve-straight"nounceableandunpronounceable and "obliqueness." three letter words but not longer ones. Decoding may be studied by ob- The evidence supports the notion that serving the behavior of skilled readersas children mature they perceive as a as they attack an unfamiliar lan-unit longer and more complex spell- guage. In one investigation half ofing patterns. Little is known about the subjects learned the sounds ofthe formation of higher units in read- individual Arabic letters without evering.Both syntactic and semantic seeing them in words; the other halfstructure as well as spelling patterns learned to say words composed of theundoubtedly determine to some ex- same letters without being told howtent the nature of the larger percep- the individual letters sounded. Whentual units so important in the reading subjects were required to sound out process. new words constructed with the same Joachim Wohlwill pointedout letters, the "letter" group was on the that reading is essentially a transfer averagesuperiortothe "word" problem: the child has to learnto group, but those of the "word" groupgive to a new set of stimuli verbalre-

4Th, p Kok.)t) 280 ARTICI.ES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

spouses already in his repertoire. Re-specificstimulusresponse associa- viewing the literature on transfer in tions. General transfer, as contrasted children, he noted that transfer proc-with specific transfer, involves a range esses could be conceptualized as vary-of stimulus and response equivalence ing along a continuum from thosethat cannot be predicted on the basis fairly specific in nature to altogetherof identical elements. However, spe- nonspecific ones, such as perceptualcific associative processes in the form sets, concepts and rules, and learningof horizontal and vertical mediation- sets. al chains may be responsible for gen- Harry Levin (7) viewed beginningeral transfer effects. reading as a process of learning and In the discussion of these papers a using a code, suggesting that childrendifference in the definition of read- be introducedto reading by firsting was apparent. Is it effective to teaching them the nature of codes inregard beginning reading as a decod- writing systems; children, for exam- ing process by %vhich appropriate ver- ple. might invent a coding system of balresponsesaretransferredto their own before they start formalprinted materials? Or is it mom fruit- reading instruction. Since a singleful to view beginning reading as a grapheme isoften associated withprocess of gaining meaning from the more than one phoneme, should theprinted page and applying it? Both beginning reader, as Bloomfield sug-definitions could be translated into gests, master one association thor-operational terms which might yield oughly before coming across a con-quite different results when used as flicting one? Or is it better to havecriterion tests. Does the first definition the child, early in his schooling, en-clarify an important intermediate ob- counter graphemes with alternativejective in initial reading instruction, pronunciations? According to Levin'sor is it better to focus on the long- experiments,itis more difficult torange goals from the start? teach variable or conflicting associa- What is the most useful way of tions in the first place, but on new talkingabout grapheme-phoneme tasks there is greater transfer. Thiscorrespondences? Should these rules suggests that the child should be ex- be treated as a spelling -sound diction- posed early to a certain amount ofary, or should they be viewed as in- irregularity in the correspondencesformation which permits the reader between written and spoken forms ofto generate appropriate responses in the language, though not necessarilynew reading contexts? More impor- the total range of variation. tant, apparently, is the question of Leo Postman, while agreeing withfunctional stimulus-response units in the need to analyze what is learneddescribing the code. Many considera- and how the essential skills are ac-tions, including that of difference in quired, questioned the basic assump-dialect, led one participant to suggest tion that success in reading dependsthat the most appropriate response on rules cr principles more than onunit may be the morpho-phoneme

286 KEISLER 281

rather than the phoneme. solved. Investigators have failed to Some doubt was expressed regard-define reading in nreasurable terms, ing the value of spelling patterns inand comparisons have often been the teaching of reading. On the othermade between results from studies in hand, it was pointed out that there%Illicit the definitions of reading were Las been no empiricaltest of thequite different. Controls have often utilityofproperlyteaching such been so poor that superior teachers or grapheme-phoneme correspondencesmore advanced books have been as- in reading instruction. Field tests ofsigned to one teaching method. Com- linguistic approaches, as distinct from mercial aspects frequently have over- -phi uric- methods, are currently be-ridden %drat is known about reading; ing conducted in the schools of Phila-for example, pictures frequently are delphia and Nliqurni in Pennsylvania.included in readers not to serve al, The view was expressed that even the educational function but to sell the linguistically oriented reading mate-books to adults. midis which are now becoming com- Nlan deplored the cleavage that mercially available may not reflecthas existed between those interested the recent advances in the field ofin classroom investigations of read- linguistics. Perhaps important varia-ing and those concerned with basic bles will be found in the prompting theory and experimentation. Since it and reinforcementconditions pro-k difficultto generalize from the vided during reading instruction. Itusual short-range laboratory studies was suggested that it might be fruit- to classroom practices, there is a need ful, for example, to focus attentionfor well-controlled intermediate-range on having the pupil produce appro-studies of several weeks' or months' priate verbal responses tinder effec-duration. Laboratory methods may tive reinforcement conditions. be adapted to study various facets of the more complex classroom teaching Research in the conditions. The behavioral processes Teaching of Reading of children in reading need to be On the final day of the conferencestudied under different instructional a panel, consisting of Arthur Status, sequences. Jeanne Chall, Ruth NVeir, and Lee Every investigation of a reading Cronbach, considered the directionssystem should involve an analytic de- in which research in reading mightscription of behavior so that points profitably move. It was pointed outof difficulty and failure can be iden- that studies in reading have not beentified. Long-term follow-up is essen- closely related to form a cumulativetial not only because findings from systematic attack. In many experi-short-range studies are frequently re- ments, treatments have been givenversed in time, but because a new labels, such as the "phonics" method,approach may require modifications without an adequate description of in subsequent grades to capitalize on what teaching procedures arein- its effects.

- 287 282 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

The interaction of teaching meth- sis in reading. Far more remains to od with individual differences de-be learned about how a beginning serves attention. There is no singlereader enlarges his apprehension of method which is likely to work besthigher units, from spelling patterns for all children, Furthermore, differ-to words to syntax. ent methods arc likely to require quite References different materials and training of I.Davis, Frederick B. "The Phonic Method teachers. vs. the Combination Method in Teach. In summarizing the three-day ses- ing Beginning Reading." In Ralph C. Staiger and CI: 'bred, Y. Melton, New sion, John Carroll indicated wide in- Developments in Programs and terest in the efforts of those who arc Procedures for CollegcAdnit Reading. Twelfth Yearbook of the National Read- attempting to introduce transitional ing Conference, Milwaukee, Wisconsin: alphabets in spite of the fact that National Reading Conference, Inc., Nlar queue University. 1963. linguisticinnovations,historically, °.Downing, John. "The Augmented Ro- have always met considerable resist- man Alphabet for Learning to Read." Balding Teacher, 16 (March 1963) , 325 ance. The reports that children move :136. easily from reading in the modified 3.Fry.Edward. "A DiacriticalMarking S stem for Beginning Reading Instruc- alphabet to traditional orthography tion."ElementaryEnglish, 1 (May should cause us to look at our theories 1961) . 526-529. 4. Gibson, Eleanor J.Gibson. J. J., Pick. of transfer more closely. A. D., and Osscr. II. "A Developmental Criticism of research in the field Study of the Discrimination of Letter- Like Forms," Journal of Comparative of reading should be tempered by the and Physiological Psychology, 55.'(1962) . fact that studies in the 1920's and 897-906. 5. Cibson, Eleanor J., Osser. H., and Pick. 1930's were carried out with a mini- A. I). "A Study in the Development of mum of resources. Some of the re- Grapheme Phoneme Correspondence," Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal search was of excellent quality even Behavior, 2 (1963), 142-146. though misquoted and misapplied in 6. Gibson. Eleanor J.Pick, A. D., Osser, 11.. and Hammond, M. "The Role of practice.Resources now available GraphemePhonane Correspondence in offer opportunities for far better re- thePerception of Words." American Journal of Psychology, 75 (1962) , 554 search in reading. Among the ques- 570. tions which deserve far more study is 7.Levin. Harry. and Watson, John. "The the relevance of grapheme-phoneme LearningofVariable Grapheme - to - Phoneme Correspondence: andThe correspondence for the teaching of Learningof Variable Grapheme - to Phoneme Correspondence: Varia sin reading, especially in the light of the the Initial Consonant Position." In A reports that young children have been BasicResearch Program on Reading, CornellUniversity,1963(Cooperative taught to read with little regard for Research, Project No. 639, U. S. Office of such rules. Research must explore Educat ion) . )1.Malone. John R. "The l.arger Aspects more fully the relative importance of of Spelling Reform," Elementary Eng- the functions of synthesis and analy- lish. 39 (1962), 435.415.

`488 Word Recognition Practice: Basal vs. Phonics Programs

GEonci.: E. MASON*

TOE TEACOEICS manuals and work- readingseries thesoundsare books for eight leading series of basal usually presented within spoken readers and seven well-knownsets of wordsand theletterswithin phonics teaching publications Were studied printed words. in order to compare suggested practices. (4)Initial consonant substitution and The level at which each understanding of syllabicdivisionWere theonly phonics. structural analysis. or dictionary techniques suggested for teaching usage was first introduced, as well as the by every basal reading series. Phon- amount of practice suggested, wasre- icsprogramsteachmost com- corded. The information was then tabled monly (a) the finding of letters or depicted infigures for comparisons. (in printed words) representing The original plan was to compare basal dictated sounds, (b) writing let- reading series,to compare phonics ma- ters representing dictated sounds, terials, and finally compote the two types. and(c)pronouncing Words by "sounding-out" their letters. Findings (5)The greatestagreement among The differences in bel of introduction basal reading series was on the and sequence of introduction of skills and structuralskills and understand- understandings were more numerous than ings. All suggested teaching inflec- anticipated. The major findings of the tional endings, compound words, study were the extensive differences among contractions,prefixes,roots, and programs of either We. Few generaliza- root changesinderivedforms, tions could be drawn. Therefore compari- Phonics programs were completely son of the Iwo I ypes of programs was lim- agreed only on the teaching of pre- ited to a very few areas. fixes and suffixes (but not on the The followinglist incorporates those same ones). generalizations possible: (6) Every basal reading series included (I) Some phonics programs were more a program for developing diction- similar to basal reading programs aryskills. No phonics program than they were to other phonics provided as many activities as any progrmns, add vice versa. single basal reading series for dic- (2)Basal reading programs were more tionary skill development. aptto teachdiscriminationof (7) The phonics programs suggested. rhyming and alliteration prior to . ingeneral, more letter-naming. introduction of sound-letter rela- They directed attention to more tionships than were phonics ma- letters and to more specific sounds terials. than did basal reading programs. (3)Consonantletter-soundrelatioh- The latter presented more total ships were more apt to be taught printed and spoken wordsfor first than Were vowel letter-sound practice. relationships in all the programs (8) In basal reading series, calling at- studied. However, in many phonics tention to a word's meaning is books isolated letters and sounds done by pointing out its use in are presented, whereas inbasal context, or by providing an incom- Improvement of Itetutiug through ClawoomPractice, IRA Procalings. %'ol. 9. !!Hi 309-310.

289 284 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

plete sentence into which it may troduction and extent to which practice is be inserted. In phonics programs, suggested. The sane differences are evi the child is usually asked to lily or dent among phonics and manuals. Pro- write the word in a sentence.' grams of either type are different in so many ways that the isolation of the differ- Summary ences in instruction which might be re- Differences exist in levels at which skills sponsible for the differences in achieve- and understandingsar;;introducedin meat seems nearly impossible. basal programs, and in sequences of in-

290 A Study of the Techniques of Word Identification

H.ALAN ROBINSON THE PURPOSE of this study was tothe words are unfamiliar more de- determine the relative effective-tailed analysis seems necessary (5). ness of various techniques for theIt appears that children who are still identificationofunfamiliar wordslearning how to read are more influ- met in reading. The following hy-enced by significant parts of words potheses were proposed: than are mature adult readers (10), 1. Context clues alone will not bealthough this tendency may be re- sufficient for the successful identifica-lated to the techniques they have tion of unfamiliar words by averagelearned rather than to natural inclin- fourth grade readers. ation. According to studies of word 2. Word configurations, in addi-perception, when analysis does take tion to context clues, will not addplace, the first part of the word ap- materially to the fourth grader's suc- pearsto be more significantfor cess in identifying unfamiliar words.word perception than the end (6). 3. Phonic and/or structural ele- This study endeavored to discover ments in initial positions added towhat the fourth grade reader actually context and configuration clues willdoes in attacking unfamiliar words. enable fourth graders to identify un-Toward this end it became necessary known words. to devise two instruments, one for 4. When context, configuration,ascertaining the words unfamiliar to and phonic and/or structuralele-a given population, and the other for ments in initial positions are not ofthe presentation of those words in a sufficient help, the addition of phonicreading framework in which the sub- and/or structural elements infinaljects would attempt to identify the positions will permit successful identi-words through a variety of identifica- fication of the unfamiliar words. tion techniques. 5. When context clues plus con- figuration and phonic and structuralMaterials analyses fail, analysis of the words in 1.Sight Word Test. The test their entirety will result in successfulwords were chosen from those words identification. familiar to 50 per cent or more of A review of the literature indicatespupils in grade six, based on the that mature readers use context andDale-Eichholz study (3). This test configurationcluesinperceivingwas designed to screen out those words (4). When the words are fa-pupils who could identify. two-thirds miliar, these clues suffice, but whenor more of twenty-two test words. With the aid of Carol Hostetter, re-The twenty-twotestwords were search assistant. randomly scattered through a list of The Reading Teacher, 16 (January 1963), 238.242. 286 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL forty-five words making up the Sightfor pupils raiding at fourth grade Word Test. levels. The actual study was con- 2: Tat Passage. Five forms of aducted in May 1962 with fourth selection designed to measuregraders in a nearby suburban school the ability of fourth grade subjects tosystem where the population was identify the twenty -two test wordscloser to "average" than in a private through different techniques of wordschool. identifical.ion. The passage was below fifth grace reading level excludingProcedure the test words, and approximately I. The words on the Sight Word sixth level including the test words,Test were presented in rapid suc- based on the Da le-Chall readabilitycession to each of eighty-eight sub- formula. jects.Sixty-one subjectsfailedto Form I of the test passage usedpronouncecorrectlytwo-thirdsor blank spaces instead of the twcnty-more of the twenty-two tcst words two words in order to assess the sub-and, as a result, were chosen as the jects' use of context clues alone. subjects for the experiment. They Form H of the same test passagereturned the following week to read used configurations of the twenty-the five forms of the Test Passage. two words to assess the use of con- 2. Each of the sixty-one subjects text plus configuration.* individually read Form I silently, or Form III of thc same test passageorallyifthe subject desired, and used context and configuration cluesstated the words he thought should plus beginning dements of each ofbe placed in each of the twenty-two the twenty -two unfamiliar words. blank spaces. Subjects were helped to Form IV of the test passage con-pronounce any words other than the tainedcontextandconfigurationtest words if they asked for such help. clues plus beginning elements andThey were encouraged to think about final dements of each of the twenty-what they were reading. two unfamiliar words. 3. Subjects were then given the Form V of the same test passagesubsequent forms, one at atime. contained each of the twenty -twnThey were asked if they wanted to words completely exposed. alter any of their decisions or suggest words for those they had not re- Preliminary Investigation sponded to at all. In order to validate the tests, the 4. The number ofcorrcctre- investigator and a research assistantsponses for each subject on each of conducted a pilot study with twenty-the Test Passages were totaled, and fivepupils in a private school inmeans and standard deviations were Chicago. The tests appeared suitablecomputcd for forms III through V. Ittpresentingthcconfigurationsof Correct responses for forms I and II words alone each letter in the word was filled in so that the actual Tellers were rep-were so few that statistical treatment resettled but could not be identified as such. would have been meaningless. ROBINSON 287

5. The number of corrcct responseswithdrew estimates they had made for each of the twenty-two words wasusing context clues alone. Of the tabulated, and percentages of words1,342 possible responses, there wcrc corrcct on each form wcrc computed.185 meaningful responses including the three corrcct identifications. Findings Hypothesesthreethroughlive Hypotheses one and two may bemust be rejected for this population. accepted as valid for this population. Although the subjects were more Hypothesisoneproposedthatsuccessful in identifying unfamiliar average fourth grade readers wouldwords when using phonic and/or rot find context clues alone sufficientstructuralelementsandthrough fci, the successful identification of un-analyses of the whole word, no single familiarwords.Throughcontextsubject succeeded in identifying all clues alone about one-seventh of1 the words. Percentages of corrcct re- per cent of the test words wcrc idcn-sponses for each of the twenty-two tificd and only about 14 per cent ofwords ranged from 0 to 21 per cent the responses were meaningful syno-(M, 6.4)on Form III(context- nyms. Two of the sixty-one subjectsconfiguration-initial elcmcnts); 2 to correctly identified one word each,36 per cent (M, 17.8) on Form IV while fifty of the subjects mentioned(context - configuration - initialele- meaningful synonyms. Of the 1,342ments- final elcmcnts); 10 to 61 per responses passible (numbcr of sub-cent (M, 35.8) on Form V (context jects 61 times numbcr of wordsand entire word). 22), there were 188 responses, Hypothesisthreeproposed that including the two corrcct identifi-the addition of phonic and/or struc- cations. tural cicmcnts in initial positions to Hypothesis two statedthat thecontextandconfiguration clues presence of word configurations, inwould enable the fourth graders to addition to contcxt clues, would notidentify unknown words. Correct re- add materially to the fourth graders'sponses on Form III ranged from 0 successinidentifyingunfamiliarto 6 per cent with a mean of 1.41 words. 1Vhen word configuration wasand a standard deviation of 1.66. added to context clues, about one-Obviously, out of a possible twenty- fifth of 1 per cent of the test wordstwo words corrcct, the addition of wcrc idcntificd, and again about 14initial elcmcnts to context and con- per cent of the responses wcrc mean-figuration resulted in little success for

ingful synonyms. Three of the sixty- .this group of subjects or any one one subjects correctly identified onesubject in the population. word each, although two of them Hypothesis four stated that when had idcntificd the same words cor-context,configuration, and phonic rectly using only context clues. Forty-and/or structural cicmcnts in initial eightofthesubjectsmentionedpositions were not of sufficient help, meaningful synonyms; two subjectsthe addition of phonic and/or struc-

. 23 288 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL turalelementsinfinalpositionsto fourth grade subjects. Since the would permit successful identificationresults of a study of word identifica- of the unfamiliar words. Correct rc-tion ought to be cumulative, that is, sponses on Form IV ranged fromeach word identification technique 0-16 with a mean of 3.93 and aneeds to build on and incorporate the standard deviation of 3.28. Althoughpreceding one, .another limitation of two subjects were able to identify 50this study is the single ordering of per cent or more of the words, thetechniques. This study started with addition of final elements did notcontext, added configuration, then result in successful identification forbeginning elements, then final ele- the total population. ments, andfinallypresentedthe Hypothesis five proposed that ifwhole word. Perhaps different results context clues plus configuration andwould be achieved if beginning ele- phonic and structural analyses failed,ments were presented before the total analysis of the words in their entiretyconfiguration of the word, or final would result in successful identifica-elements before beginning, et cetera. tion. Correct responses on Form V Actually, a case could be built for ranged from 1-19 with a mean ofdevoting more attention to any one 7.95 and a standard deviation ofaspect. It appears that these subjects, 4.58. Approximately one-quarter offor instance, would profit from in- the sixty-one subjects could identifystruction in the use of context clues. 50 per cent or more of the wordsEven when confronted with context when viewing the totalword inclues alone and no word form, the context. selections of words which could com- plete meaning were meager and in- Discussion accurate. When parts of the word An obvious conclusion is that noform were presented, most subjects word identification technique wasseemed to concentrate all effort on used very successfully by this groupsounding the parts without regard to ofsubjects. No subjectcorrectlycontext. On the other hand, it was identified all the words. On the otherimpossible to determine the role of hand, the study had certain limita-context when subjects correctly iden- tions. The test words were in thetified words in the final test passage listening vocabularies ofthepilotwhere the whole word was viewed in population, but this information wasthe contextual setting. not ascertained for the final test pop- Nevertheless, it does seem reason- ulation. The school in which theable that these subjects reading at pilot study was conducted is attendedapproximatelyfourthgradelevel by an exceptionally able group ofshould meet with a greater degree of pupils. A limitation might also lie insuccess in the identification of un- the test passage, which may not havefamiliar words than was reflected in been constructed well enough to pro-this study. Perhaps average fourth vide the kinds of context clues helpfulgrade readers need an intensification

2c14 ROBINSON 289 of instruction in the synthesizing of Reading. New York:Ronaldl'ress. 1952. Chapter 5. word identification techniques. Since 2.Dale.Edgar. an Chall.JeanneS. it appears that most success in identi- "A Formula for Predicting Readability," Ethical' IResearchBulletin, Ohio ficationoccurs when many tech- State University. 27 (Jan. 1918). 11-20. niques are used, pupils may need to 3.Eichholi. Gerhard. and Dale. Edgar. Children's Knowledge of Words. learn how to marshall these forces. embus:Bureau of Educational Re- Perhaps an additional step needs search and Service, Ohio Slate [Myer- thy. 1960. to be taken in thc designing of pro- I.Gray, William S. "Reading" in En. grams aimed at helping pupils iden- cyclopediaofEducationalResearch - (edited by Chester W. Harris). New tify words. After the pupils havr York: Macmillan. 1960. Pages 1098.99. learned to apply the techniques of 5. Hamilton.Francis. ThePerceptual Factors in Reading. Columbia Univer- identification to familiar words, we sity Contributions to Philosophy, Ps), should not assume that pupils will on chology. and Education. XVII. New York: Science Press. 1907. their own transfer this skill to un- 6. Huy. Edmund B. The Psychology and familiar words. Probably many pu- Pedagogy of Reading. New York: Mac- millan. 1908. Chapters 4 and 5. pils similar to the subjects involved 7.Porter,Douglas. "The Instrumental in this study need guidance and di- Value of Sound Cues in Rending." Un- published paper presented at Annual rection m applying word identifica- Meeting of American Educatinnal Re tion techniques to unfamiliar words. search Association, Feb. 17. 1960. 9. Tinker. Miles A. "Visual Apprehension Further study of average fourth and Perception inReatliinT. Psycho. grade readers using the tests of this logical Bulletin, 26 (Apr. 19211. 2°3-40. 9. Vernon. M. D. Backwardness in Read. experiment in a number of permuta- ing. Cambridge: Cambridge Universit tions could add interesting and valu- Press. 1957. Chapters 2 ani 3. 10.Vernon. M. D. A Further Study of able evidence to the present results. Pima Perception. Cambrkge: Cam- Additional experimentation of this bridge University Press. 1954. II. Vernon. M. D. The Experimental Study kind with readers at a variety of ofReading. Cambridge:Cambridge grade levels might help to explain the UniversityPress.1931. Chapters 5.7. 12. Woodworth, Robert S.Experimental results of this study. This small piece Psychology. New York:Holt. 1938. ofresearch only offers atesting Pages 23 and 28. method and results for a specific population. Furthcr research along COMMENT these lines, or even replications of Robinson's rcport is of particular thisstudy, might eventuallycon-intcrcst in view of the current reas- tribute to the development of sig-sessment of the relative emphasis nificantinsightsintoinstructionalwhich should be given to the various practices and the construction of in-aspects of thc word recognition pro- structional materials. gram. The study is exploratory and should stimulate further experimen- References tation in thc use of various tcch- I.Anderson, Irving IL, and Dearborn. nique of word recognition.T.C. Walter F. The Psychology of Teaching Visual Discrimination of Words and Reading Readiness

CAROL ANN SHEA* Introduction THIS STUDY WAS DESIGNED to develop a test of visual discrimination of words and to determine which of the following testsThe Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form &the Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test, or the Test of Visual Discrimination of Words was the best predictor of reading achievement at mid-year in first grade. Failure in first grade is primarily the result of lack of ability to read. This fact suggests that it would be valuable if valid meas- ures of a child's readiness to begin formal reading instruction could be obtained early in the pupil's school life. De Boer and Dallman (1960)stated that if an attempt is made to teach a child to read before he is ready, many undesirable results may occur. In some cases, a deep seated aversion toward reading may develop. Heilman (1961)contended that when a child is expected to decipher word symbols before he is visually ready to do so, he is likely to fail. In1922,Gates made the first investigation of importance that was concerned with visual perception. He concluded that the ability to detect small differences in words showed a fairly high correla- tion with reading, whereas the ability to see differences in pairs of objects had no association with reading. Sister Mary of the Visitation(1929)in a study similar to that of Gates, verified many of his conclusions. She concluded that the "ability to discriminate minute details in series of actual words and also in groups of unrelated letters are pre-eminent factors in reading." Purpose of the investigatiivi It was the purpose of this study:1] to develop a test using words that were similar in configuration in order to measure visual discrimination; 2]to determine whether such a test administered at the beginning of first grade would enable teachers to determine more accurately than The Metropolitan Reading ReadinessTest, Form R, which children were ready for formal reading activities at that time and which children needed more readiness 'work;3] to ascertain if the test of visual discrimination of words was a pre- dictor of reading achievement at mid-year in first grade; and 41 to determine which of the following tests. The Metropolitan Read- The Refl.?, ig Teacher, 21 (January 1968), 361-367. SHEA 291

ing Readiness Test, Form R, the Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test, or the Test of Visual Discrimination of Words was the best pre- dictor of reading achievement at mid-year. Test construction A visual discrimination test was constructed by the investi- gator. This consisted of two sections. The first section was com- posed of only letters. There were fifty items and two sample items. All of the upper and lower case letters were used as stimuli. The second section was composed of actual words that were similar in configuration and letter components. There were fifty items and two sample items. The length of the words increased from two; letters to seven-letters. The stimulus word or letter was printed on individual nine by twelve cards. Each card was held up by the examiner and the pupils were allowed to look at it. The card was exposed five seconds for each of the letter items and ten seconds for each of the word items. Examples of the Letter Test 1.P S B R H Stimulus B 15. m u n w r Stimulus m Examples of the Word Test 4.pay hay gay guy gap Stimulus gay 6.ran sun nun run rut Stimulus run The Visual Discrimination Test, in its original form, was administered to 134 kindergarten children in the town of Hamden, Connecticut. During this same period The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R was administered to these children. The results of these tests were used to establish the reliability and validity of the Visual Discrimination Test. The difficulty level of the items in the letter portion of the Visual Discrimination Test was computed. Because 82 per cent of the items on the letter section of the test were in the range of difficulty of 80 to 100 per cent, it was concluded that information about the relative differences among the subjects could not be obtained. Therefore, the letter section was not included in the final form of the test. The level of difficulty was computed for each item in the word section of the Visual Discrimination Test. The results showed that 96 per cent of the items were in the range of difficulty of 20 to 89 per cent. It was felt that with this range of difficulty, information

27. 292 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

about the differences in visual discrimination among the subjects could be obtained. In order to establish the validity of the Visual Discrimination Word Test, the reading readiness section of The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R was used as an independent measure of the criterion. A Pearson Product r was computed be- tween the Visual Discrimination Word Test and the reading readi- ness portion of The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R. The correlation between the experimental test and the criterion measure was .59. To further establish the validity of the Visual Discrimination Word Test the matching section of The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R was used as a criterion measure. A Pearson Product r was computed between Ow tt tests. The correlation between the experimental test and t4. As criterion measure was .51. A Kuder-Richardson reliability curfkient was computed for the Visual Discrimination Word Test. The reliability coefficient was .91. Sample and measuring instruments The first grade children in the Dunbar Hill School in Hamden, Connecticut, were used for the study. There were thirty-four boys and forty-two girls in this group. The ages of the seventy-six children ranged from seven years seven months to five years nine months. The mean age was six years two months. The Visual Discrimination Word Test, the reading readiness portion of The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R, and the Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence TestLevel 1, Form A were administered to the children during the month of September. In January, after approximately four months of reading instruction, each child was tested individually on a word recognition test. The word recognition test included all the words in the pre-primer and primer vocabularies of the Macmillan Company Series and of the Ginn and Company Series. Statistical procedures and results 1. A frequemy distribution was made for each of the follow- ing tests t Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test, Form R (reading section), the Visual Discrimination Word Test, and the Lorge- Thorndike Intelligence Test. 2.Quartile points for each of the frequency distributions were found and the distributions were divided into quarters. 3. The scores of each pupil for each of the three tests were 208 SHEA 293

analyzed and classified according to quarters. Tables 1, 2 and 3 show the total number of cases for each quarter in the row designated "number of cases predicted." 4.Quartile points were found for the frequency distribution of the word recognition test and the distribution was divided into quarters. 5. The scores of each pupil on the word recognition test were analyzed and classified according to quarters. 6. A comparison was made for each pupil to determine if their respective scores on each of the three tests designated were in the same quarter as their scores on the word recognition test. If their score on the word recognition test was in the same quarter as originally predicted by either The Metropolitan Reading Readi- ness Test, the Visual Discrimination Word Test, or the Lorge- Thorndihe Intelligence Test, they were included in the total number of cases listed in Tables 1, 2, 3 in the row designated "number of cases that agreed with prediction." 7. The relative per cent of accuracy of each test and each quarter was determined by comparing the number of cases pre- dicted by that test for each quarter with the number of cases that fell within the predicted quarter on the word recognition test. For example: On the Visual Discrimination Word Test, there were twenty-one pupils with scores in the lowest quarter of the distribution.. On the word recognition test, sixteen of the twenty-one pupils were in the lowest quarter. Therefore, the Visual Discrimina- tion Word Test predicted accurately for seventy-six per cent of the cases. The Visual Discrimination Word Test predicted for the high quarter 11 per cent better than the Lorge-Thorndihe Intelligence Test and 27 per cent better than the Metropolitan reading readiness section. For the high middle quarter the Metropolitan was the best predictor of word recognition ability. It predicted 35 per cent of the time for the high middle quarter. It predicted 2 per cent better than the Lorge-Thorndike and 10 per cent better than the Visual Discrimination Test. Chi square with one degree of freedom was computed for the lowest quarter of the Metropolitan and the Visual Discrimination Test to determine whether these results merely reflected the opera- tion of chance or whether their appearance probably resulted from a significant difference. With the hypothesis H:P, = P2 if 13, = .76, the percentage of cases predicted accurately for the lowest quarter

9119 294 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

of the Visual Discrimination Test (see Table 2) and P., = .52, the percentage of cases predicted accurately for the lowest quarterof the Metropolitan (see Table 1), the hypothesis can be rejected at about the .15 level of confidence. In comparing the results of the entire group on the Visual Discrimination Test and the Lorge-Thorndike with the hypothesis H:P, = P, if P, = .50, the percentage of cases predicted accurately for the entire group by the Visual Discrimination Test (seeTable 2) and P. = .37, the percentage of cases predicted accurately for the entire group by the Lorge-Thorndike (see Table 3) the hypoth- esis can be rejected at about the .10 level of confidence. In order to determine which combination of tests would provide the best estimate of readiness to begin formal reading instruction, multiple correlations were performed. Table 4 shows the intercorrelations of these tests and Table 5 the multiple correlations. In an analysis of the intercorrelations, the Lorge -Thorndike and the Visual Discrimination Test had the lowest intercorrelation (.46) but, the combination of these tests had the highest correla- tion with the criterion, the word recognition test (.76).Therefore, the Lorge-Thorndike and the Visual Discrimination Test measure different aspects of reading readiness and a combination of both would contribute substantially to prediction. Table 1Distribution of cases according to quarters for The Metropolitan Reading Readiness Test

LowestSecond Third HighestTotal quarterquarterquarterquartergroup AgreeN 10 9 7 1 21 DisagreeN 19 32 20 5 16 % of agreement 52% 28% 35% 20% 36%

Table 2Distribution of cases according to quarters for the Visual Discrimination Word Test

Lowest Second ThirdMghestTotal quarterquarterquarterquartergroup AgreeN 16 6 5 11 38 DisagreeN 21 12 20 23 16 % of agreement 76% 50% 25% 41% 50% On the other hand, the Metropolitan and the Lorge-Thorndike had the highest intercorrelation (.86) but, the combination of these tests had the lowest correlation with the criterion, the word SHEA 295

Table 3Distribution of cases according to quarters for the Lorge-Thorndike Intelligence Test

Lowest Second Third HighestTotal quarterquarterquarterquartergroup AgreeN 8 9 7 4 28 DisagreeN 17 27 21 11 76 % of agreement 47% 33% 33% 36% 37%

Table 4Intercorrelaticins among four variables, including one index word recognition and three predictive indices

Variable X. X, X, X, X2 Visual discrimination .49 .46 .65 X, Metropolitan .49 .86 .61 X, Lorge-Thorndike .46 .86 .66 X, Word recognition .65 .61 .66

Table 5Multiple correlations between the three predictive tests and the word recognition test

'1.34 '1.23 .73 '1.24 .76 '1.234 .66

1 equals criterion, Word Recognition Test 2 equals predictive test, Visual Discrimination Word Test 3 equals predictive test, Metropolitan Reading Reariness Test 4 equals predictive test, Lorge-Thorndihe Intelligence Test

recognition test (.66). Therefore, the Metropolitan and the Lorge- Thorndike measure much the same thing and the inclusion of both, instead of either one or the other, would contribute little to the prediction achieved. With an r of .65 there was substantial correlation between achievement on the Visual Discrimination Test and achievement on the word recognition test and with an r of .66 an almost iden- ticalrelationship existed between achievement on the Lorge- Thorndike and the word recognition test. The substantial relation- ship existing between achievement on the Visual Discrimination Test and the word recognitiort test confirmed the theory that the ability to discriminate visually words was an indicator of readiness to begin formal reading instruction when the sight method of 296 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

instruction was employed. Schools using different teaching ap- proaches might yield different results. In addition the statistical analysis indicated that the ability to discriminate words visually is particularly significant in the upper and lower quarters. It also confirmed the theory of other investigators that intelligence was a factor in determining when a pupil was ready to learn to read. Neither the ability to make visual discriminations between words nor intelligence is the sole factor to be used to determine readiness for formal reading instruction. The multiple correlations showed that the combination of the Lorge-Thorndike and the Visual Dis- crimination Test was a better predictor of readiness to read than the combination of the Metropolitan and the Lorge-Thorndike or the Visual Discrimination Test and the Metropolitan. These results lend support to the theory that the lack of capa- bility to discriminate visually between words is a significant factor in retarding or limiting tile, reading success of children. This in turn leads to the assumption that children in the crucial lower quarters on the Visual Discrimination Word Test should be given extensive training in visual perception before formal reading in- struction is started.

References De Boer, J. J., and Dallmann, Martha. The teaching of reading. New York: Holt, Rinehart & Winston, 1960. Gates, A. The psychology of reading and spelling with special reference to disability. New York: Bureau of Publications, Teachers college, Columbia Univer- sity, 1922. Heilman, A. W. Principles and practices of teaching reading. Ohio: Merrill, 1961. Sister Mary of the Visitation. Visual perception in reading and spelling: a statistical analysis. Educational Research Bulletins, 1929, 4 (1 ). Washington, D. C.: The Catholic Education Press. What Do Teachers Know about Phonics and Syllabication?

GEORGE D. SPACHE and MARY E. BAGGETT THE EXTENT TO WHICH teachersclaims of teachers that they faithfully can and do teach their pupilsfollow such guides (3, 5) can we be- variousphonicsandsyllabicationlieve that phOnics is well taught by skills is, of course, deperident uponteachers who don't understand the their own knowledge of the underly-basic principles? ing principles and conventions. Sev- eral previous authors have exploredProcedures this question by administering vari- All students enrolled in the senior ous phonics tests to groups of teachersauthor's workshops and classes dur- and graduate students in education.ing the academic year 1963-64 were In general, the results have not beengiven a revised version of Aaron's very encouraging. Gagan (2) foundtest of phonics principles and infor- that teachers knew about 68 per centmal tests of syllabication and syllabi- of the items in the informal Rogerscation principles. Aaron's test was Test of Phonic Ability. Ramsey's and modified by groupingall .related Schubert's studies (4, 6) give similaritems in consecutive groups. A sam- results on their own phonics tests.ple item of this fifty-item test is: Aaron's( / )group of prospective 5. The u in dru' fuel has the same and experienced teachers answered sound as: only 57 per cent of his 60-item test. (I)u in true,(2) u in rut.(3) u in He also found that scores tended to urn, (4) u in nature. (5) of these increase with teaching experience, The syllabicationtestaskedre- implying that the scores of teachersspondents not to mark the syllables tended to depend on their classroombut to write the number of syllables experience rather than on the ade .present. Samples ofthesixty-five quacy of their pre-service training.items arc: Educational assignment was not as- 2. cyclic 61.pleottasitt sociated with performance on the lattice 65.sisal test, contrary to the logical expecta- 37. diatonic tion that primary teachers, who com- The twelve-item test on rules for monly present this content, would besyllabication was a multiple-choice most familiar withit.Collectivelyarrangement. A sample item is:

these studies raise grave doubts re- .1.A consonant between two vowels garding the adequacy of the teaching usually goes with the vowel it when the vowel in front is accented and of phonics in our classrooms. Despite short. the emphasis upon these skills in the a) before manuals and guides accompanying h) following c)neither basalreadingmaterials,and the d) either before or after ' The Reading Teacher, 19 (November 1965)96.99. 298 ARTICLES PARTIAI.LY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL

Subjects can they teach pupils a skill which Six of the respondents were full-they can do well but do not clearly time graduate students without pre-understand? vious teaching experience other than Aaron made comparisons within their internships. The remainder ofhis group of prospective and in-serv- 93 were in- service teachers pursuingice teachers to discover the influence graduate credit in the area of read-of teaching experience and assign- ing. Aaron's groups were composedment upon knowledge of phonics of 65 per cent of inexperienced teach-principles. Years of experience were ers in contrast with our group of 6found to be associated with perform- per cent. ance, but educational assignment was not related. Apparently teachers at Results various gradc levels have about the The mean scores of the 99 studentssame proficiency in applying phonics on the three tests and the respectiveprinciples, for what they know has ranges of scores were: Phonies Prin-been learned gradually in the class- ciples, mean 45.0, range 18-60; Syl-room, not intraining. Using the labication, mean 59.6, rangc 30-65;same categories of cxperiencc and Syllabication Rules, mean score 8.21,assignment in our group yielded thc range 1-11. The rangc of thc presentdata of Tab lc1 bclow. group in thc Phonics test is similar to TABLE 1 that in Aaron's study, 13-59, but the tic( atES ON POONIIS PRINCIPLE%.11,_tir mean is greater than his of 34.22. A. According to Yeats of Teaching Since years of experience were found Experience to be positively related to success in Over this test, the differences in thc means Scores ()Yrs. 1.5 Yrs.6.10 Yrs.10 Yrs. may well reflect the greater tcaching Mean 35.00 39.5 44.8 cxpericncc of our group. Range 16.55 16.60 2640 26-60 Thesc means indicatc that thc Number 6 31 22, 40 group answered correctly 75 per cent R. According to Grade Groups of the phonics, 91 per cent of the Other syllabication, and 68 per cent of the Scores Grs. 1.3Grs.:1.6Grs. 7.12Grades syllabicationrulestests.Although M eat t 6.3 47.6 39.6 46.9 Range 16.60 31.60 26.55 16.60 competent in the act of syllabication, Nnuther 30 27 25 17 thc group appcars no morc knowl- cdgeablc in the principles of syllabi- Tcachcrs with ten or more years of cation than in phonics. It is qucs-experience or sixten years of experi- tionablc whcthcr those teachers, par-ence know significantly morc about ticularly of intcrmcdiatc grades, whophonicsthanbeginningteachers. arc expected to teach syllabicationThese differences arc significant at can do this competently whcn thcythc .01 and .02 levels, respectively. are so weak in the underlying phonicAs in Aaron's group, thc test was ad- and syllabicationprinciples. Howministcredearlyinthegraduate

'4r 4 SPACHE 299

course, prior to discussion-or instruc-cncc or assignment is significant. Ap- tion in this area. Thus the scores re-parently,syllabicationaccuracyis flect upon the adequacy of the pre -learned before entering teaching, and servicc training of these groups. does not materially improve with In the present study the teachersclassroom teaching experience at any assigned to junior and senior highparticular level. Some explanation of school were significantly poorer inthe failure to improve in syllabica- knowledge of phonics, at the .01tion, despite the experience involved level, than all other groups. This isin teaching this skill to pupils, may in contrast to the lack of differenceslie in teacher knowledge of the prin- among such groups in Aaron's study.ciples underlying this skill, as shown Several factors such as size of sample,by the data of Table 3. average length of experience, and TABLE 3 the nature of the tcaching assign- SCORES ON SYLLAIIICA TION RULES ESI ments in this deviant group may ac- A. According to Years of Teaching count for this result. If, for example, Experience a large proportion of our secondary Over teachers were science, mathematics, Scores 0 Yrs. 1-5 Yrs.6.10 Yrs.10 Yrs. business - education teachers, or the Mean 7.3 8.0 8.6 8.4 like, they might well be expected to Range 4.11 2-11 1.11 4.11 Number 6 31 22 40 be less familiar with phonics princi- B. According to Grade Groups ples. The data on the remaining tests Other ScoresGrs. 1.3Grs. l -6Grs.7-12Grades ofsyllabicationandsyllabication rules are summarized in Tables 2 and Mean 8.5 9.07 7.6 7.47 Range 4-11 6-11 4.11 1-11 3 below. Number 30 27 23 17 TABLE 2 SCORES ON SYLLABICATION I'ESI Comparisons among the groups accordingtoteachingexperience A. According to Years of Teaching Experience yield nosignificantdifferences. Over Length of teaching experience dots Scores 0 Yrs. 1.5 Yrs.6.10 Yrs.10 Yrs. not result in increased knowledge of NIcan 61.6 60.5 58.2 59.8 syllabication rules. Comparisons Range 55.65 50.63 35.62 30.64 among the grade groups, however, do Number 6 31 22 40 indicate that the level .of teaching B. Accordi Ig to Grade Groups assignment is significantly related to Other knowledge of such rules. Teachers of ScoresOrs. 1-3Grs. 4-6Grs. 7-12Grades Grades 1-3 are more knowledgeable klean .60.0 60.8 59.3 58.3 than those of Grades 7-12 at the 5 Range 35.65 50.65 1.64 32-63 Number 30 27 25 17 per cent level; and more knowledge- able than the Other group (a mixed None of the comparisons betweengroup of administrators, supervisors, groups according to teaching experi-reading specialists, and adult educa- . 3r 300 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL tors) at the 5 per cent level. Inter-seem in even grcatcr need of such mediate grade teachers score signifi-instruction, in Florida at least. cantly better at the 1 per cent level Pre-service training in syllabication than secondary teachers or the mixedis apparently effective and results in group. Level of teaching assignmentreasonable teacher skill in this area. does promote grcatcr learning of syl- Knowledge of the underlying rules labicationprinciplcs among those forsyllabication,however,isnot teachers who are commonly respon-strong at any teaching level and only sible for instruction in this area. comparatively better for primary and Before offering the implications ofintermediate grade teachers. Other these results, an evaluation of thestudies of the values of syllabication interrelationships among these threerules (as compared to phonics prin- tests of word skills is appropriate. Theciples) are necessary at varying edu- correlation between Phonics Princi-cational levels. ples and Syllabication was .657, a If knowledge of syllabication rules marked relationship, again implyingdces not markedly influence success the continuity and interdependencein applying phonics principles or in of these two skills. In contrast, theactual syllabication, of what value correlation between the Phonics Prin-are such rules? Should more train- ciples and Syllabication Rules testsing in phonics and its principles be was .370, implying that abilitytosubstituted for the timepresently apply phonics principles does notspcnt on syllabication rules? contribute greatly to similar knowl- edge of syllabication rulcs. The cor- References relation of .256 bctwccn Syllabica- I.Aaron, Ira E. "What Teachers and Pro- spective Teachers Know About Phonic tion and Syllabication Rules raises Generalizations," Journal of Educational questions about the value of knowl- Research, 53 (May 1960), 323.330. 2. Gagon, Glen Scott. "A Diagnostic Study edge of syllabication principles for of the Phonic Abilities of Elementary success in syllabication. This result is Teachers in the State of Utah." Doc- toraldissertation, Colorado State Col- confirmed in an earlier unpublished lege, 1960. study of these tests by an r of .230 3.Purcell, Barbara A. "Methods of Teach. ing Reading: A Report of a Tri-State among 69 college freshman students. Survey," Elementary School Journal, 58 (May 1958) ,449.453. Implications 4. Ramsey, Z. Wallace. "Will Tomorrow's Teachers Know and Teach Phonics?" The replication of Aaron's study Reading. Teacher, 15(Jan. 1962), 241. 245. confirms the need for pre-service in- 5.Russell, David H. "Teachers' Views on struction for primary and intermedi- Phonics." Ektriettlary English, 32 (Oct. 1955), 371.375. ate teachers in phonics and its prin-6. Schubert, Delwyn G. "Teachers and ciples. Secondary teachers whose in- Word Analysis Skills," Journal of Devel- opmental Reading 2 (Summer 1959), structional areas touch upon this skill 62.64. Developing Advanced WordPerception Skills

DIANA UMSTATIba

,California, Georgia, Utah, New enable them to read the material at hand, TEXASTEXAS,New Jersey, Pennsylvania... for the specific purpose ofthemoment. New Hampster," said Patty as she quickly The amounts of informational intake, of read a list of the names of the states. To cognitive strain, and of risk will vary with, the last response the teacher whispered, the student, the topic, the kind of material, "New Hampshire," and hurried on to the and the specific purpose. We shall con- next student. Then the teacher stopped. sider this balance of benefits to the stu- Here it was again, "New Hampfler." The dents as we discuss the development of second student, and also a third one, pro- advanced word perception skills. nounced it the same way. Why the errors ? What must the teacher do? The Advanced Word Perception Skills The Need An inspection of the teacher's manuals of a few series of basic readers indicates Boys anti girls at the upper elementary that, by consensus, the major advanced level are interested in learning about hun- word perception skills arc: phonetic skills, dreds of different things. They seek much structural analysis skills, syllable phonics, of their information from printed mate- dictionary skills, and abilities to use cer- rials. This, in turn, demands reading skills tain contextual clues. that are efficient for their many needs and The methods of teaching these several interests. It requires word perception skills skills and the status given each one varies which will enable them to read, with case, somewhat from one series of readers to about dozens and dozens of topics,in another. In short, the authors plan to use various kinds of materials, and for nu- a balance of informational intake, cogni- merous purposes. It means that the class- tive strain, and risk that is conducive to room teacher must teach perception skills the development of their major purposes. that will meet these challenges. The ratio of the components of a given program is in agreement with the respec- A Balance Theory tive definitions of reading. In Jerome S. Brunces book, A Study of The Development of the Skills Thinking,' he reported that the capable successful subjects in his experiments It is not our purpose to discuss the skills se- program of any series of basic readers nor lectedstrategiesforoperationwhich would enable them to maintain a balance the philosophy of the author(s) of any of informational intake, cognitive strain, series. Instead, we want to observe Patty and risk, commensurate with the task. It and her friends as they read to meet their seems to us that this is also true of suc- numerous needs andinterests. Which cessful, ambitious readersin the upper skills do they use, and under what circum- elementary grade levels. They want to stances? Are certain skills high on their "look at" and to "think about" words just priority lists, or do they shift freely from one skill long enoughto getthatinformation to the other as the demands which, if combined with other available change? information about the same words, will First, let us return to Patty and the list of names of the states. She knew Califor- `JeromeS. Bruner, Jacqueline J. Goodnow, and George A. Austin. A Study ofThinking. New nia, Pennsylvania, and Utah, but she said York: Science Editions, Inc., 1962. New Hampster instead of New Hamp- Readingand Inquiry, IRA Proceedings, Vol. 10, 1965, 30.32.

3.7 7 302 ARTICLES PARTIALLY RELATED OR VERY GENERAL shire. Why? to the quality of thinking that they must Patty recognized the categorystates in do. They used allthe word perception the United States. She was familiar with skills as they read, but were versatile in the the names, but did not recognize them as timing of the use of each. They consistent- sight words. She rapidly used a combina- ly matched personal strengths and weak- tion of skills to make initial attacks upon nesses with the specific task at hand. the words. Her background of informa- Some children may learn to shift from tion hastened her completion of the iden- skill to skill freely, without direct guid- tification of the words, with one exception. ance. Others are like Wayne, who needed Why did she say "New Hampster?" We much help. Wayne's teacher assisted him believe that she had been hearing it that in reading arithmetical story problems by way, and our hunch was supportedby her teaching him at which points precision two peers who made similar errors. The was essential and at which pointsgeneral- balance of benefits of a particular strategy izations would be acceptable. For example, was at work, and the informational intake he substituted names of friends for the was insufficient to compensate for an un- proper names but carefully analyzedall known risk, an error in aural perception other words within the questions. in an earlier experience. A new strategy In Summary had to be selected and a new balance had to he maintained for the identification of We recognize that children are faced New Hampshire. Patty was equal to the with voluminous amounts of printed ma- task. She knew where to add a step and terials. There is neither time nor energy how to fitit into her sequence of deci- for reading all of it. Yet they must learn sions. Her word ,perception skills weremuch of the content. This demands that adequate forthe identificationof the they encounter many new words each time word. they shift to a different topic or a different As we observed Patty and her friends material. New words demand efficient from day to day in various circumstances word perception skills. We have discussed and with many materials we noticed the efficiency in terms of pupils, needs, mate- preparation that went into their reading. rials, and purposes. We have also dis- They were constantly aware of their pur- cussed efficiency in terms of a balance of poses. They also took into account all the benefits which the reader receives from information which they could bring to theparticularstrategies heuses. The the situation. They anticipated words and choice of strategy may well be considered clues for recognizing them. They consid- as the foundation of the developmentof ered the type of material to be encoun- adequate word perceptionskillsatthe tered. Last. but not least, they were alert upper elementary level.