Processing Mwes: Neurocognitive Bases of Verbal Mwes and Lexical Cohesiveness Within Mwes

Total Page:16

File Type:pdf, Size:1020Kb

Processing Mwes: Neurocognitive Bases of Verbal Mwes and Lexical Cohesiveness Within Mwes Processing MWEs: Neurocognitive Bases of Verbal MWEs and Lexical Cohesiveness within MWEs Shohini Bhattasali Murielle Fabre Cornell University Cornell University / Ithaca, NY, USA Ithaca, NY, USA INSERM-CEA / Paris-Saclay, France [email protected] [email protected] John Hale Cornell University / Ithaca, NY, USA DeepMind / London, UK [email protected] Abstract Multiword expressions have posed a challenge in the past for computational linguistics since they comprise a heterogeneous family of word clusters and are difficult to detect in natural lan- guage data. In this paper, we present a fMRI study based on language comprehension to provide neuroimaging evidence for processing MWEs. We investigate whether different MWEs have distinct neural bases, e.g. if verbal MWEs involve separate brain areas from non-verbal MWEs and if MWEs with varying levels of cohesiveness activate dissociable brain regions. Our study contributes neuroimaging evidence illustrating that different MWEs elicit spatially distinct pat- terns of activation. We also adapt an association measure, usually used to detect MWEs, as a cognitively plausible metric for language processing. 1 Introduction This study focuses on how Multiword Expressions are processed in the brain and provides a functional lo- calization of different facets of MWEs using neuroimaging data. If MWEs are indeed non-compositional, then perhaps their comprehension proceeds through a single, unitary retrieval operation, rather than some kind of multistep compositional process. If we assume a single retrieval operation for these MWEs, how do the differences in their grammatical category affect their processing? Are they observable on the neuronal level? Proceeding from this general hypothesis, this paper investigates the neural substrates of different types of MWEs and MWEs with different levels of compositionality. Firstly, verbal MWEs are distinguished from non-verbal MWEs and the neural bases of each are compared. Additionally, to model lexical co- hesivenss of MWEs we use Pointwise Mututal Information, PMI (Church and Hanks, 1990), which is an association measure and traditionally used to identify MWEs. This gradient metric of cohesiveness within MWEs is correlated with brain activity to illustrate whether MWEs with varying degrees of com- positionality evoke different patterns of activation in the brain. In this way, we provide further insight about MWE processing during natural language comprehension. 2 Background 2.1 Previous MWE Processing studies MWE comprehension has been shown to be distinct from other kinds of language processing. For in- stance, it is well-established at the behavioural level that MWEs are produced and understood faster than matched control phrases due to their frequency, familiarity, and predictability (Siyanova-Chanturia and Martinez, 2014), in accordance with incremental processing (Hale, 2006). This would follow if MWEs were remembered as chunks, in the sense of Miller (1956) that was later formalised by Laird, Rosenbloom and Newell (1986; 1987). This work is licensed under a Creative Commons Attribution 4.0 International License. License details: http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/. 6 Proceedings of the Joint Workshop on Linguistic Annotation, Multiword Expressions and Constructions (LAW-MWE-CxG-2018), pages 6–17 Santa Fe, New Mexico, USA, August 25-26, 2018. , Eye-tracking and EEG work further documents this processing advantage across a wide range of MWE sub-types, e.g. • Binomials (Siyanova-Chanturia et al., 2011b), • Phrasal verbs (Yaneva et al., 2017), • Complex prepositions (Molinaro et al., 2013; Molinaro et al., 2008), • Nominal compounds (Molinaro and Carreiras, 2010; Molinaro et al., 2012), • Lexical bundles (Tremblay and Baayen, 2010; Tremblay et al., 2011), • Idioms (Underwood et al., 2004; Siyanova-Chanturia et al., 2011a; Strandburg et al., 1993; Laurent et al., 2006; Vespignani et al., 2010; Rommers et al., 2013). For example, Siyanova-Chanturia et al. (2011b), found their eye-tracking results illustrate that bino- mial MWEs such as bride and groom are processed faster that the reversed three-word phrase groom and bride, due to the high-frequency nature of the former expression. However, previous work has focused on a particular subtype of MWEs and to our knowledge, none of them have implemented a fMRI study of MWEs within a naturalistic text to either contrast between dif- ferent categories of MWEs or model the cohesiveness within them. Recent computational work (Savary et al., 2017; Cholakov and Kordoni, 2016; Gharbieh et al., 2016; Uresova et al., 2016) has focused on verbal MWEs in order to identify them within a corpus, rather than study how they are processed in a naturalistic setting. 2.2 MWEs and Compositionality The name MWE loosely groups a wide variety of linguistic phenomena including idioms, perfunc- tory greetings, character names, and personal titles. What unifies cases of MWEs is the absence of a wholly compositional linguistic analysis; they are “expressions for which the syntactic or semantic prop- erties of the whole expression cannot be derived from its parts” (Sag et al., 2002). The naturalistic story used as a stimulus in this study includes various types of MWEs and some examples from the stimulus, The Little Prince are given below. The bold expressions were identified using a MWE analyzer, ex- plained further in x4.2. Over half of the attestations in the text are headed by a verb and can be labelled as VPs (see x5.2.1). These encompass verb participle constructions, light verb constructions, and verb nominal constructions among others. The remaining attestations are a mixture of nominal compounds, greetings, personal titles, character names, and complex prepositions. (1) So I thought a lot about the adventures of the jungle and in turn, I managed with a coloured pencil to make my first drawing. (2) My little fellow, I don’t know how to draw anything except boa constrictors, closed and open. (3) When I drew the baobabs, I was spurred on by a sense of urgency. (4) ‘What are you doing there?’, he said to the drinker who he found sitting in silence in front of a number of empty bottles and a number of full bottles. (5) You must see to it that you regularly pull out the baobabs as soon as they can be told apart from the rose bushes to which they look very similar to when they are young. (6)“ Good morning”, said the little prince politely, who then turned around, but saw nothing. However, MWEs cannot be strictly binarized as compositional and non-compositional. These expres- sions fall along a graded spectrum of compositionality. To capture the varying degrees of compositional- ity within MWEs, we use an association measure, known as Pointwise Mutual Information (PMI). While PMI scores are commonly used in computational linguistics to identify MWEs as ngrams with higher scores are likely to be MWEs (Evert, 2008), in this study they are utilized as a gradient predictor to de- scribe the the lexical cohesiveness of MWEs. Intuitively, its value is high when the word sequence under 7 consideration occurs more often together than one would have expected, based on the frequencies of the individual words (Manning et al., 1999). More formally, PMI is a log-ratio of observed and expected counts: 1 c(wn) PMI = log2 1 (1) E(wn) MWEs can receive positive or negative PMI scores which indicate cohesion or repulsion respectively between the words in a sequence (Church and Hanks, 1990). MWEs that receive a higher PMI score are seen as lexically more cohesive, which is interpreted as more noncompositional (less compositional). Thus, these scores are repurposed in this study to describe the cohesive and noncompositional aspect of MWEs and utilized to obtain a quantifiable metric to correlate with the fMRI signal. Krenn (2000) also suggests that association measures such as PMI and Dice’s coeeficient (Dice, 1945; Sørensen, 1948; Smadja et al., 1996) are better-suited to identify high-frequency collocations whereas other association measures such as log-likelihood are better at detecting medium to low frequency collocations. Since MWEs are inherently high-frequency collocations, we chose PMI as a metric to describe the strength of association between these word clusters. 3 Research Questions To summarize, this study investigates the following: • Are the differences between the grammatical categories of MWEs observable at the cerebral level? Does processing of verbal MWEs implicate separate brain areas from non-verbal MWEs? Specif- ically, if the strong relationship between verbs and their arguments are encoded in different brain areas compared to non-verbal MWEs featuring no argumental structure? (c.f. Analysis 1 in x5.2.1 ) • Do MWEs with varying levels of cohesiveness tap into different cognitive resources? For example, are MWEs with higher PMI scores processed differently from MWEs with lower scores? Do they activate dissociable brain regions? (c.f. Analysis 2 in x5.2.2 ) 4 fMRI study 4.1 Method We follow Brennan et al., (2012) in using a spoken narrative as a stimulus. Participants hear the story over headphones while they are in the scanner. The sequence of neuroimages collected during their session becomes the dependent variable in a regression against word-by-word predictors, derived from the text of the story. 4.2 Stimuli & MWE Identification The audio stimulus was Antoine de Saint-Exupery’s´ The Little Prince, translated by David Wilkinson and read by Nadine Eckert-Boulet. It constitutes a fairly lengthy exposure to naturalistic language, comprising 15,388 words and lasting over an hour and a half. Within this text, 742 MWEs were identified using a transition-based MWE analyzer (Al Saied et al., 2017). Al Saied et al. use unigram and bigram features, word forms, POS tags and lemmas, in addition to features such as transition history and report an average F-score 0.524 for this analyzer across 18 different languages which reflects robust cross-linguistic performance. For an illustrated example of the MWE identification process with this analyzer, please see the Appendix.
Recommended publications
  • Incidental Vocabulary Acquisition from an Authentic Novel: Do Things Fall Apart?
    Reading in a Foreign Language April 2010, Volume 22, No. 1 ISSN 1539-0578 pp. 31–55 Incidental vocabulary acquisition from an authentic novel: Do Things Fall Apart? Ana Pellicer-Sánchez and Norbert Schmitt University of Nottingham United Kingdom Abstract Nation (2006) has calculated that second language (L2) learners require much more vocabulary than previously thought to be functional with language (e.g., 8,000–9,000 word families to read independently). This level is far beyond the highest graded reader, and would be difficult to explicitly teach. One way for learners to be exposed to mid- frequency vocabulary is to read authentic materials. The original A Clockwork Orange study (Saragi, Nation, & Meister, 1978) showed impressive amounts of incidental vocabulary learning with first language (L1) readers, but subsequent studies with L2 learners (using graded readers or simplified materials) showed only modest gains. This study explores the degree to which relatively advanced L2 readers can acquire spelling, word class, and recognition and recall of meaning from reading the unmodified authentic novel Things Fall Apart. After more than 10 exposures, the meaning and spelling could be recognized for 84% and 76% of the words respectively, while the meaning and word class could be recalled for 55% and 63%. Keywords: vocabulary acquisition, incidental learning, English as a foreign language (EFL), extensive reading, word knowledge In a landmark study, Nation (2006) calculated the amount of vocabulary necessary to function in English, using a 98% coverage figure (Hu & Nation, 2000). He found that it took much more vocabulary than previous estimates: 6,000–7,000 word families for spoken discourse and 8,000– 9,000 families for written discourse.
    [Show full text]
  • Language Difficulty and Prior Learning Influence Foreign Vocabulary Acquisition
    languages Article Language Difficulty and Prior Learning Influence Foreign Vocabulary Acquisition Sayuri Hayakawa *, James Bartolotti, Aimee van den Berg and Viorica Marian Department of Communication Sciences and Disorders, Northwestern University, Evanston, IL 60208, USA; [email protected] (J.B.); [email protected] (A.v.d.B.); [email protected] (V.M.) * Correspondence: [email protected] Received: 4 November 2019; Accepted: 22 December 2019; Published: 27 December 2019 Abstract: When learning a foreign language, words that are the hardest to learn are often the easiest to forget. Yet, there is also evidence that more challenging learning contexts can lead to greater long-term retention. Here, we investigate the effect of language difficulty on vocabulary retention by teaching participants novel words that varied in both imageability and similarity to a known language over a period of four weeks. We found that easier words (high-imageability and familiar) were generally retained better than harder words (low-imageability and unfamiliar). However, when words were fully learned during training, the more difficult unfamiliar words were later recalled with higher accuracy than easier familiar words. The effect of language difficulty on vocabulary retention therefore varies depending on how well words were initially encoded. We conclude that greater challenges can reap greater long-term rewards so long as learners establish a strong foundation during initial acquisition. Keywords: foreign language acquisition; desirable difficulties; imageability; language similarity; vocabulary 1. Introduction When learning a new skill, whether it be computer programming, tennis, or a foreign language, many of us have experienced a ‘honeymoon period,’ where the transformation from complete novice to competent newcomer prompts a rush of excitement and confidence.
    [Show full text]
  • An Introduction to Applied Linguistics This Page Intentionally Left Blank an Introduction to Applied Linguistics
    An Introduction to Applied Linguistics This page intentionally left blank An Introduction to Applied Linguistics edited by Norbert Schmitt Orders: please contact Bookpoint Ltd, 130 Milton Park, Abingdon, Oxon OX14 4SB. Telephone: (44) 01235 827720. Fax: (44) 01235 400454. Lines are open from 9.00 to 5.00, Monday to Saturday, with a 24-hour message answering service. You can also order through our website www.hoddereducation.co.uk If you have any comments to make about this, or any of our other titles, please send them to [email protected] British Library Cataloguing in Publication Data A catalogue record for this title is available from the British Library ISBN: 978 0 340 98447 5 First Edition Published 2002 This Edition Published 2010 Impression number 10 9 8 7 6 5 4 3 2 1 Year 2014, 2013, 2012, 2011, 2010 Copyright © 2010 Hodder & Stoughton Ltd All rights reserved. No part of this publication may be reproduced or transmitted in any form or by any means, electronic or mechanical, including photocopy, recording, or any information storage and retrieval system, without permission in writing from the publisher or under licence from the Copyright Licensing Agency Limited. Further details of such licences (for reprographic reproduction) may be obtained from the Copyright Licensing Agency Limited, of Saffron House, 6–10 Kirby Street, London EC1N 8TS. Hachette UK’s policy is to use papers that are natural, renewable and recyclable products and made from wood grown in sustainable forests. The logging and manufacturing processes are expected to conform to the environmental regulations of the country of origin.
    [Show full text]
  • Second Language Word Association: Processes, Methodologies, and Models
    Second Language Word Association: Processes, Methodologies, and Models John Paul Racine A thesis submitted to Cardiff University in fulfilment of the degree of Doctor of Philosophy 2018 DECLARATION This work has not been submitted in substance for any other degree or award at this or any other university or place of learning, nor is being submitted concurrently in candidature for any degree or other award. Signed …………………………………………………… Date: September 28, 2018 STATEMENT 1 This thesis is being submitted in partial fulfillment of the requirements for the degree of Ph.D. Signed ………………………………………….………… Date: September 28, 2018 STATEMENT 2 This thesis is the result of my own independent work/investigation, except where otherwise stated, and the thesis has not been edited by a third party beyond what is permitted by Cardiff University’s Policy on the Use of Third Party Editors by Research Degree Students. Other sources are acknowledged by explicit references. The views expressed are my own. Signed ……………………………………….……….…… Date: September 28, 2018 STATEMENT 3 I hereby give consent for my thesis, if accepted, to be available online in the University’s Open Access repository and for inter-library loan, and for the title and summary to be made available to outside organisations. Signed ……………………………………………..…..….. Date: September 28, 2018 For Moka May you dream big and always believe in yourself Acknowledgements I wish to express my deepest thanks to my supervisors, Professors Alison Wray and Tess Fitzpatrick, and Dr. Michelle Aldridge-Waddon. Without their wisdom and guidance, this dissertation would not have been possible. My research has also benefitted greatly from discussions with many other distinguished academics over the years.
    [Show full text]
  • Challenging Learners in Their Individual Zone of Proximal Development Using Pedagogic Developmental Benchmarks of Syntactic Complexity
    Challenging Learners in Their Individual Zone of Proximal Development Using Pedagogic Developmental Benchmarks of Syntactic Complexity Xiaobin Chen and Detmar Meurers LEAD Graduate School and Research Network Department of Linguistics Eberhard Karls Universitat¨ Tubingen,¨ Germany {xiaobin.chen,detmar.meurers}@uni-tuebingen.de Abstract and identify reading material individually chal- lenging learners, essentially instantiating the next This paper introduces an Intelligent Com- stage of acquisition as captured by Krashen’s con- puter Assisted Language Learning system cept of i+1 (Krashen, 1981) or relatedly, but em- designed to provide reading input for lan- phasizing the social perspective, Vygotsky’s Zone guage learners based on the syntactic com- of Proximal Development (ZPD; Vygotsky, 1976). plexity of their language production. The In terms of structure of the paper, we first locate system analyzes the linguistic complexity our approach in terms of the Complexity, Accu- of texts produced by the user and of texts racy, and Fluency (CAF) framework in SLA re- in a pedagogic target language corpus to search. Then we review approaches adopted by identify texts that are well-suited to foster earlier studies in developmental complexity re- acquisition. These texts provide develop- search, including problems they pose for a peda- mental benchmarks offering an individu- gogical approach aimed at offering developmental ally tailored language challenge, making benchmarks. We propose and justify a solution, ideas such as Krashen’s i+1 or Vygotsky’s before presenting the architecture and functional- Zone of Proximal Development concrete ity of the SyB system. and empirically explorable in terms of a broad range of complexity measures in all 2 Development of Syntactic Complexity dimensions of linguistic modeling.
    [Show full text]
  • Polish and Finnish Teenagers' Motivation to Learn
    MOUTON EuJAL 2016; 4(2): 277–300 Janina Iwaniec* and Riikka Ullakonoja Polish and Finnish teenagers’ motivation to learn English: The role of context DOI 10.1515/eujal-2015-0034 Abstract: Language learning motivation has been investigated in a number of contexts (Dörnyei and Ushioda 2009). However, there has been only one attempt (Taguchi et al. 2009) at a systematic comparison of models of motivation to learn English by students from different countries. The aim of this study is to investigate the role of context by comparing motivational characteristics and SEM (Structural Equation Modelling) models of language learning motivation of 15–16 year-old students from Finland and Poland. The data from both countries was collected using the same motivational questionnaire with a total of 351 students completing it. A number of differences were identified between the two contexts. Finnish learners scored significantly higher on scales of the ideal L2, intrinsic motivation, knowledge orientation, motivated behaviour and peer pressure, whereas Polish learners reported higher levels of international orientation and parental encour- agement. The comparison of models of language learning motivation shows differences in the role of parents and peers, endorsement of goals between the two cohorts. Finally, we discuss the potential reasons behind the differences in motivation between the two contexts and conclude that contexts not only affect motivational characteristics but also the structure of language learning motiva- tion. Keywords: motivation, Polish context, Finnish context Zusammenfassung: Sprachlernmotivation wurde bereits in unterschiedlichen Kontexten untersucht (Dörnyei und Ushioda 2009). Allerdings gab es bisher lediglich eine Untersuchung (Taguchi et al., 2009) eines systematischen Ver- gleichs, in der versucht wurde, Motivationsmodelle beim Spracherwerb der eng- lischen Sprache von Lernern aus verschiedenen Ländern zu vergleichen.
    [Show full text]
  • An Analysis of Lexical Errors in the English Compositions of Thai Learners
    An analysis of lexical errors in the English compositions of Thai learners SAENGCHAN HEMCHUA – Srinakharinwirot University, Bangkok NORBERT SCHMITT – The University of Nottingham ABSTRACT The importance of vocabulary in second language (L2) writing is widely accepted, but there has been relatively little research into the lexical errors learners produce when writing in their second language. Moreover, the error categorisation frameworks used in some previous studies have addressed only a relatively limited number of lexical error categories. In this paper the authors used a more comprehensive error taxonomy based on James (1998), with some additions from Leech’s semantics (1981), to analyse Thai third-year university students’ English compositions for lexical errors. The analysis revealed that (a) ‘near synonyms’ were the most numerous errors, followed by ‘preposition partners’ and ‘suffixes’, (b) the students had more difficulty with semantics than the forms of words, and (c) the identified sources of errors were mainly from L2 intrinsic difficulty rather than the first language (L1) transfer. The findings from the Thai students’ written lexical errors have impli- cations for L2 vocabulary teaching and learning, which should be of interest to wider English as a Second Language (ESL)/English as a Foreign Language (EFL) contexts. Research into lexical errors The language produced by foreign language (FL) learners almost inevitably contains errors of various types. This is the process of learning a language. Empirical evidence suggests that lexical errors are the most frequently occurring category of errors in written English (for example, Grauberg 1971; Meara 1984; Lennon 1991). They significantly affect the quality of academic writing (Astika 1993; Ellis, R 1994; Engber 1995) and native speakers consider them the most irritating (Santos 1988).
    [Show full text]
  • 090Ad 1376381204.Pdf
    ! ! " ! " ! # ! ! ! #$ ! % ! # " !! ! ! ! &' (# ) ! #" !! &' # # ! !!) * ) + ,- ) #. /0" // # # !! ) - + " #. " # ( # !! "# ! "# ! ! " # $ !% ! & $ ' ' ($ ' # % % ) %* %' $ ' + " % & ' ! # $, ( $ - . ! "- ( % . % % % % $ $ $ - - - - // $$$ 0 1"1"#23." "0" )*4/ +) * !5 !& 6!7%66898& % ) - 2 : ! * & /- ;9<8="0" )*4/ +) "3 " & 9<8= Cohesive Argumentative Writing Through Controlled, Guided, and Free writing Activities Abbas Ali Zarei, Assistant professor, Imam Khomeini International University, Qazvin, Iran Ramin Rahmany Assistant professor, Islamic Azad University, Takestan, Iran Maryam dodangeh M.A., Islamic Azad University, Takestan, Iran TABLE OF CONTENTS LIST OF TABLES............................................................................................5 LISTOFGRAPHS……………………………………………………………..6 CHAPTER I: Preliminaries 1.1 Introduction………………..……………………………………………...7 1.2 Statement of the Problem..........................................................................10
    [Show full text]
  • Vli.V08.1.2187-2759 VLI Editorial Team
    Vocabulary Learning and Instruction Volume 8, Number 1, March 2019 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.7820/vli.v08.1.2187-2759 VLI Editorial Team Editorial Board: Jeffrey Stewart, Raymond Stubbe, Aaron Gibson Reviewers: Paul Meara, Norbert Schmitt, John A. Read, Stuart Webb, John P. Racine, Tomoko Ishii, Tim Stoeckel, Phil Bennett, Dale Brown, Jon Clenton, Stuart McLean, Peter Thwaites, Tatsuya Nakata, Kiwamu Kasahara, Masumi Kojima, James Rogers, Yuko Hoshino, Vivienne Rogers, Yu Kanazawa, Stephen Cutler, Ian Munby, Mark Maby, Brandon Kramer, Makoto Yoshii, Anna C. S. Chang, Brent Culligan Copy Editors: Alex Cameron, Andrew Gallacher, Peter Harold, Mark Howarth, Linda Joyce, Tim Pritchard, Andrew Thompson, Alonzo Williams, Ian Mumby, Pete Westbrook, Melanie González, Stephen Cutler, Michael McGuire, Shusaku Kida, Jenifer Larson-Hall, Shin Ishikawa, Magda Kitano. The Editorial Team expresses a sincere thank you to Mana Ikawa, who designed the cover for the print version of VLI. Copyright © 2019 Vocabulary Learning and Instruction, ISSN: Online 2187-2759; Print 2187-2767. All articles are copyrighted by their respective authors. Vocabulary Learning and Instruction Volume 8, Number 1, March 2019 doi: http://dx.doi.org/10.7820/vli.v08.1.2187-2759 Table of Contents Articles Page Letter from the Editor iv Raymond Stubbe Seventh Annual JALT Vocabulary SIG Symposium Morning Session: Vocabulary and Corpus Linguistics A Reconsideration of the Construct of “A Vocabulary for Japanese Learners of English”: A Critical Comparison of the JACET Wordlists and New General Service Lists 1 Shin’ichiro Ishikawa Validating the Construct of Readability in EFL Contexts: A Proposal for Criteria 8 Geoffrey G. Pinchbeck Toward Written Error Correction with a Japanese-English Parallel Corpus: Data-Driven Learning in the Japanese EFL Classroom 17 Michael McGuire Evaluating Corpora with Word Lists and Word Difficulty 29 Brent A.
    [Show full text]
  • Mouton De Gruyter | IRAL 49-1 (2011) | 1St Proofs Scott Crossley and Thomas Lee Salsbury / [email protected], [email protected]
    Mouton de Gruyter | IRAL 49-1 (2011) | 1st proofs Scott Crossley and Thomas Lee Salsbury / [email protected], [email protected] The development of lexical bundle accuracy and production Please note: page- and linebreaks are temporary; overlong lines, orphans/widows, and split examples will be dealt with after your corrections have been incorporated; there is no need to mark split examples or bad page breaks. Typesetter’s notes/queries /1/ Article adapted to IRAL style sheet. There is a huge number of minor changes and corrections, please carefully check the file for errors. Preliminary page and line breaks 1-iral-49-1 — 2011/1/29 18:46—1— #2—ce Mouton de Gruyter The development of lexical bundle accuracy and production in English second language speakers SCOTT CROSSLEY AND THOMAS LEE SALSBURY Abstract Six adult, second language (L2) English learners were observed over a period of one year to explore the development of lexical bundles (i.e., bigrams) in nat- urally produced, oral English. Total bigrams produced by the L2 learners over the year of observation that were shared with native speakers were compared using a frequency index to explore L2 learner’s accuracy of use. The results of the study support the notion that bigram accuracy increases as a function of time spent learning English. This finding lends credence to the notion that the production of lexical bundles by L2 learners begins to develop in paral- lel with the frequency of lexical bundles used by native speakers. Like native speakers, L2 learners also begin to produce lexical bundles that serve prag- matic and syntactic functions.
    [Show full text]
  • Challenging Learners in Their Individual Zone of Proximal Development Using Pedagogic Developmental Benchmarks of Syntactic Complexity
    Challenging Learners in Their Individual Zone of Proximal Development Using Pedagogic Developmental Benchmarks of Syntactic Complexity Xiaobin Chen and Detmar Meurers LEAD Graduate School and Research Network Department of Linguistics Eberhard Karls Universitat¨ Tubingen,¨ Germany fxiaobin.chen,[email protected] Abstract and identify reading material individually chal- lenging learners, essentially instantiating the next This paper introduces an Intelligent Com- stage of acquisition as captured by Krashen’s con- puter Assisted Language Learning system cept of i+1 (Krashen, 1981) or relatedly, but em- designed to provide reading input for lan- phasizing the social perspective, Vygotsky’s Zone guage learners based on the syntactic com- of Proximal Development (ZPD; Vygotsky, 1976). plexity of their language production. The In terms of structure of the paper, we first locate system analyzes the linguistic complexity our approach in terms of the Complexity, Accu- of texts produced by the user and of texts racy, and Fluency (CAF) framework in SLA re- in a pedagogic target language corpus to search. Then we review approaches adopted by identify texts that are well-suited to foster earlier studies in developmental complexity re- acquisition. These texts provide develop- search, including problems they pose for a peda- mental benchmarks offering an individu- gogical approach aimed at offering developmental ally tailored language challenge, making benchmarks. We propose and justify a solution, ideas such as Krashen’s i+1 or Vygotsky’s before presenting the architecture and functional- Zone of Proximal Development concrete ity of the SyB system. and empirically explorable in terms of a broad range of complexity measures in all 2 Development of Syntactic Complexity dimensions of linguistic modeling.
    [Show full text]
  • Eurosla 27 – Reading, 30Th August
    Version 31st July 2017 Eurosla 27 – Reading, 30th August – 2nd September 2017 Wednesday 30th August doctoral workshops Palmer Building Room 1.02 9.00-9.30 Linda Evenstad Emilsen Acquisition of gender in Norwegian L2 9.30-10.00 Ting Yan Rachel Kan Acquisition of Cantonese classifiers in majority and minority language contexts 10.00-10.30 Shi Zhang The effect of L2 exposure on processing and interpreting reflexive binding and aspect marking in Mandarin-English bilinguals break 10.45-11.15 Eloi Puig Mayenco Investigating the grammatical knowledge of Negative Quantifiers and Negative Polarity Items in L3 English 11.15-11.45 George Pontikas, Ian Processing of English wh-questions by Cunnings, Ianthi Maria sequential bilingual children: a visual-world Tsimpli and Theo paradigm study Marinis 11.45-12.15 Gabriella Notarianni Tense and aspect in the acquisition of Italian Burk as a foreign language Palmer Building Room 1.03 9.00-9.30 Vincent DeLuca Changing our Brains and Minds: Exploring the effect of the bilingual experience on cognition, neural structure and function. 9.30-10.00 Kim Collewaert Referential mechanisms in oral narratives of learners of Spanish 10.00-10.30 Anders Agebjörn The form and function of (double) definiteness in second language Swedish break 10.45-11.15 Xuemei Chen Don’t you find this funny? Humour appreciation among Chinese L2 users of English 11.15-11.45 Audrey Bonvin Discourse cohesion in a Romance-Germanic languages contact environment: lexical and syntactic preferences for additive linking in Switzerland 11.45-12.15
    [Show full text]